The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at www.emeraldinsight.com/1751-1348.htm JMH 18,2 Women issues to Wonder Woman Contributions made by the students of Hugo Munsterberg 166 Leon C. Prieto College of Business Administration, Savannah State University, Savannah, Georgia, USA Abstract Purpose – This article seeks to depict the pivotal role Hugo Munsterberg, the great pioneer in industrial psychology, played in the lives of his students, some of whom were feminists regardless of his own chauvinistic opinions. The article aims to examine the contributions made by Mary Calkins, Ethel Puffer, and William Marston, all former students of Munsterberg, who went on to make valuable contributions in psychology, women’s issues, the polygraph, and the creation of the first and most famous comic book super heroine. Design/methodology/approach – Synthesizing articles from history journals, writings about the figures of interest, published works by the figures themselves and other resources, this paper illustrates how Hugo Munsterberg impacted the scholarly careers of Calkins, Puffer, and Marston who all made valuable contributions to academia and popular culture. Findings – This paper concludes that Munsterberg’s influence was evident in the works of Calkins, Puffer, and Marston in areas as diverse as the psychology of beauty to the detection of deception. Despite his own chauvinistic views Munsterberg had an amicable and productive relationship with the aforementioned students, which sometimes extended beyond a professional relationship. Consequently, they initiated a research agenda that was greatly influenced by Dr Munsterberg. Originality/value – This article highlights Dr Hugo Munsterberg’s influence on Calkins, Puffer, and Marston, who made valuable contributions in women’s issues, as well as the development of DISC theory, and the super-heroine Wonder Woman. Keywords Mary Calkins, William Marston, Hugo Munsterberg, Ethel Puffer, Wonder Woman, Influence, Students, Psychology, Women Paper type General review Journal of Management History Vol. 18 No. 2, 2012 pp. 166-177 q Emerald Group Publishing Limited 1751-1348 DOI 10.1108/17511341211206834 Dr Hugo Munsterberg was probably the most prodigious, industrious, and provocative psychologist in the world at the turn of the twentieth century (Porfeli, 2009). The Harvard philosopher-psychologist William James had recruited him in Germany in 1892, when Munsterberg was only 29, to take over Harvard’s pioneering work in experimental psychology (Business Week, 1966, p.20). In a letter William James wrote to his brother, he described Munsterberg as the “Rudyard Kipling” of psychology, because like Kipling, Munsterberg was a prodigy (Porfeli, 2009). At 28 years old, Munsterberg was regarded as one of the most accomplished psychologists in Germany ( James, 1920). Munsterberg was arguably among the most productive psychologists of his era, with a vita including more than 20 books and numerous articles produced during a 17-year period (Hale, 1980; M. Munsterberg, 1922). Despite his brilliance, Munsterberg was criticized by many because of his impetuous personality and moral rectitude that antagonized many of his colleagues (Spillman and Spillman, 1993, p. 327). Professor Munsterberg was also disliked for his views on women. He felt that it was unfortunate that American women controlled the higher culture and ideals because men were too preoccupied with their occupations (New York Times, 1910, p. 5). Munsterberg also felt that if an artificial equality was kept up through women’s higher development, American intellectual work will be kept down by the women (Munsterberg, 1901, p. 163). Munsterberg also managed to enrage suffragists when he reported that he would be satisfied with the jury system as long as women were kept out of it. He came up with those conclusions during an experiment he conducted with students from Harvard and Radcliffe in which he tried to determine what the essential mental processes were during trial by jury (Munsterberg, 1922, p. 435). Despite his pre-eminence, his abrasive personality, his view on women, and unpopular politics (his support of Germany in the First World War) led to his work being largely ignored in the twentieth century (Porfeli, 2009). Dr Munsterberg, despite his views on women, still managed to attract a plethora of female and male students (Munsterberg, 1922, p. 61). The focus of this paper will be on three of his students, namely Mary Calkins, Ethel Puffer Howes, and William Moulton Marston, who were all enthusiastic about learning under the German professor. The writer will discuss the irony in which a male chauvinistic professor had a significant impact on the professional careers of the three aforementioned students who contributed to the field of psychology and women’s rights in their own particular way. The writer will first discuss Mary Calkins, her professional and cordial relationship with Munsterberg, and the influence he had on her work. Second, the writer will discuss Ethel Puffer and the contributions she made in the advancement of women. Third, the writer will discuss William Moulton Marston, the influence of Munsterberg on Marston’s psychological work, the polygraph, and Marston’s interest in the feminism which played a factor in him creating the well known super-heroine, Wonder Woman. Mary Calkins Mary Calkins (1863-1930) was born in Hartford, Connecticut, the eldest of five children, and grew up in Buffalo, New York, where her father was a protestant minister. Calkins left home in 1882 to attend Smith College in an era when higher education for women was still an unproven experiment (O’Connell and Russo, 1990, p. 58). Her father Wolcott Calkins arranged for Mary to have an interview with Wellesley’s president, Alice Freeman. Mary Calkins subsequently joined the faculty. Later, when Mary sought a PhD in psychology from Harvard, which did not admit women, Wolcott petitioned the Harvard Corporation to admit his daughter as a special visitor (Palmieri, 1983, p. 198). Calkins and Munsterberg Mary Calkins considered Hugo Munsterberg to be one of her great teachers, the others being, William James and Edmund Sanford (Calkins, 1930, pp. 31-33). In Munsterberg’s early years at Harvard he began his cordial relations with Calkins, in whose career he had a lifelong interest and for whose creative work he had a great admiration; and he spent many evenings over at Miss Calkins’s parents (Munsterberg, 1922, p. 63). There was once an incident at Harvard where Dr Munsterberg had planned a lunch meeting of the Committee of the American Psychological Association, in which Calkins was a member, at the Harvard Union, but the head waiter denied them entrance stating that women were not allowed, but Munsterberg almost by main force gained his point and the Committee its lunch (Calkins, 1930, p. 34). Calkins admired Munsterberg who she noted as a man of deep learning, high originality, and astounding versatility, interested alike in systematic psychology, in Women issues to Wonder Woman 167 JMH 18,2 168 the setting and solution of experimental problems. She worked with him for three years in the Psychology Laboratory of Dane Hall (Calkins, 1930, p. 33). In 1894, she published the first of a series of reports on work she had done in Munsterberg’s laboratory: experiments on the pair associate technique, which she had invented. In October 1894, Munsterberg wrote to the president and fellows of Harvard College inquiring if there was a chance that Calkins might be admitted as a candidate for the PhD. The following is an excerpt from his letter: With regard to her ability, I may say that she is the strongest student of all who have worked in the laboratory in these three years. Her publications and her work here do not let any doubt to me that she is superior also to all candidates of the philosophical PhD during the last years. More than that: she is surely one of the strongest professors of psychology in this country. The Harvard PhD attached to the name of Mary W. Calkins would mean not only a well deserved honor for her, but above all an honor for the philosophical department of Harvard University (Furumoto, 1979, p. 352). The Harvard Corporation records for 29 October 1894 note that Munsterberg’s request was considered and refused (Furumoto, 1979, p. 352). In the spring of 1895, Calkins presented her thesis entitled “An experimental research on the association of ideas” to the Department of Philosophy at Harvard. The thesis was approved by the members of the department, and after conducting an informal and unauthorized PhD examination of the candidate; her examiners forwarded a communication to the president of Harvard College (Hanus et al., 1993). These efforts failed and there remained the possibility that Radcliffe College, as the women’s college equivalent to Harvard, with only Harvard instructors on its faculty, should confer the degree of philosophy on her who had won it within the Harvard walls. Not until May, 1902, when three other women had also passed their PhD exams, was it decided that the Radcliffe doctorate was actually the form of the Harvard degree for women. Munsterberg tried his best to persuade Calkins to accept the PhD at Radcliffe College and he wrote (Munsterberg, 1922, p. 76): We are all very anxious that you do so, as just by the cooperation by you four prominent women with you as acknowledged leader, the new degree would command at once highest respect in the whole academic world, certainly superior to the degree or similar degrees. It will be the Harvard degree. Of course it will be too late for correspondence. You must cable a word to Radcliffe; be sure and cable: “Yes!” However, Calkins remained firm and refused. Her graduate studies successfully accomplished, except for the lack of degree, she returned to Wellesley College in the autumn of 1895 as Associate Professor of Psychology and Philosophy. Calkins’ professional work Over the next five years, there appeared in the psychological literature a steady stream of studies from the Wellesley College psychological laboratory communicated by Calkins; she was a passionate educator and believed that psychology, in the hands of a teacher who lays stress on verbal definitions and on traditional groupings, may become a mere text book subject, a memorizing of verbal statements and uncomprehended schedules (Calkins, 1907, p. 674). She also published some papers reporting the results of her work in Munsterberg’s laboratory, including the work that would have been her doctoral thesis. In 1900, there appeared the first series of papers in which she developed her ideas about psychology as a science of self, and the following year, her first book, An Introduction to Psychology, was published (Furumoto, 1979, pp. 352-353). Her publications of the 1890s, primarily reports of experimental work, were supplanted after 1900 by theoretical and philosophical papers. For the period after 1900, Calkins’s major contribution to psychology was the development of a system of self psychology where she was concerned that the self was often left out of psychology on the ground that scientific introspection has failed to discover it (Calkins, 1915, p. 495). Calkins was also concerned in showing the possibility of such a psychology of selves by considering, in the first place, the essential distinction between philosophy and science (Calkins, 1900, p. 491). She published prolifically in both psychology and philosophy; four books and well over a hundred papers are divided evenly between the two disciplines. Her work in psychology tends to cluster in the first half of her career, whereas her concern with philosophy was a continuing thread. The shift in emphasis is reflected in her election to the presidencies of the American Psychological Association in 1905 and the American Philosophical Association in 1918 (Furumoto, 1979, p. 353). Calkins acknowledged the influence of Hugo Munsterberg on her conception of the double standpoint in psychology, which is the theory that every experience may be treated alike from the atomistic and from the self psychological standpoint (O’Connell and Russo, 1990, p. 61). Concluding notes on Calkins In the 1890s, Calkins challenged the work of a colleague, Joseph Jastrow. In his study, he asked college students, both male and female, to write down 100 words as fast as possible. He found “that women repeat one another’s words more than men” and “there is less variety among women than among men” (Furumoto, 1979, p. 353). After analyzing these lists he concluded, “that the feminine traits revealed are an attention to the immediate surroundings, to the finished product, to the ornamental, the individual, and the concrete; while the masculine preference is for the more remote, the constructive, the useful, the general, and the abstract” (Furumoto, 1979, p. 353). Calkins was infuriated by his findings and responded on the impossibility of making valid distinctions between masculine and female intellect when one cannot eliminate the effect of the environment. Mary Calkins had a long and distinguished career as a psychologist and educator. She overcame the obstacles faced by women to receive a higher education and won the respect of her professors, especially Munsterberg, who acknowledged her as one of his most outstanding students. In 1891, she founded one of the earliest psychological laboratories in the United States at Wellesley College, where she served on the faculty for 40 years. Her contributions to psychology include the invention of the paired-associate technique for studying memory and the development of self-psychology. She was elected to the presidency of both the American Psychological Association and the American Philosophical Association, the first woman to be accorded this honor by either organization (O’Connell and Russo, 1990, p. 57). Ethel Puffer Howes Ethel Puffer (later Howes) (1872-1950) was born in the eastern Massachusetts town of Framingham (Furumoto and Scarborough, 1987, p. 75). On both sides of her family, several generations of her ancestors had been native New Englanders (Furumoto and Scarborough, 1987, p. 75). Her father George attended business school in Boston, where he worked as a railroad station master. After completing high school, Ethel Puffer Women issues to Wonder Woman 169 JMH 18,2 herself traveled across the state of Massachusetts to attend Smith College, graduating in 1891, when she was not quite 19 (Furumoto and Scarborough, 1987, p. 75). Puffer took a teaching job at a high school in New Hampshire as an instructor of Mathematics. When she was at Smith College, she became very interested in psychology, and, in 1895, Puffer set out for Germany (Furumoto and Scarborough, 1987, p. 75). 170 Puffer and Munsterberg In Puffer’s classes, she met a young Canadian who had completed a PhD in psychology at Harvard while Hugo Munsterberg was there on a three year appointment, and through him, she was able to arrange an interview with Munsterberg, who was at the time teaching at the University of Freiburg. She was interested in aesthetics, one of Munsterberg’s areas of specialization, and had attended a course of lectures on the topic in Berlin (Furumoto and Scarborough, 1987, p. 78). Much to Puffer’s delight, Munsterberg showed real interest in her, offering to direct her research and to let her work in his private laboratory. Munsterberg even assured Puffer that his wife would help make her stay in Freiburg a pleasant one. Adopted into the Munsterberg family, Puffer thrived on the holidays, parties, cultural events, and outings that were an integral part of upper middle class life in southern Germany (Furumoto and Scarborough, 1987, p. 79). Puffer was a diligent worker in Munsterberg’s laboratory where he invested his time and attention on her, individually supervising her research. Working on a problem in the psychology of beauty, she investigated the role played by symmetry in making a work aesthetically pleasing. For her study, Munsterberg prepared about a thousand pictures to test and measure for symmetry, and they spent mornings together working on her project. To allay any concerns her mother might have about the propriety of this arrangement, Puffer wrote: “Don’t be alarmed. Mrs Munsterberg paints at the other end of the room and inquires when she does not understand our meaning” (Furumoto and Scarborough, 1987, p. 80). Munsterberg considered Puffer’s research so promising that he encouraged her to pursue it as a dissertation topic and to submit the work already completed in the competition for a graduate fellowship to be awarded by the Association of Collegiate Alumnae (ACA), which was a group started in Boston to raise standards for women’s higher education. Ethel Puffer took Munsterberg’s advice, applied for an ACA fellowship, and won it. Munsterberg submitted a letter of recommendation and stated that Puffer was the only American woman he had ever met whose abilities matched those of Mary Calkins. The award provided her with support for a year of work on her dissertation (Furumoto and Scarborough, 1987, p. 80). Awarded the ACA’s American fellowship for the academic year 1897-1898, Puffer returned to New England, accompanying the Munsterberg family. Hugo Munsterberg had decided to return permanently to Harvard, where Puffer continued her dissertation work with him. She completed the requirements for the PhD by the end of the year and found herself in the same degree limbo as Mary Calkins, whose work had been finished three years earlier. Her situation was different from Calkins’s, however, in that by the time Puffer began studying at Harvard, Radcliffe College existed and Puffer was officially a Radcliffe student. She received only a certificate from the Philosophy Department, documenting that she had successfully carried out work equivalent to that of candidates for the Harvard PhD. Copies were sent also to Radcliffe and the Harvard Corporation. After waiting three years, Puffer wrote to the dean of Radcliffe inquiring whether the college was willing to confer the PhD. As a result of Puffer’s overture, four women who had completed graduate work at Harvard over the previous several years were offered the Radcliffe PhD in spring 1902. They were divided, however, among themselves about the wisdom of setting this precedent. Ethel Puffer was one of the two women who accepted the degree; Mary Calkins was one of two who declined (Furumoto and Scarborough, 1987, p. 81). Women issues to Wonder Woman 171 Puffer’s professional life and work on women issues For a decade after her doctoral studies were completed, Puffer remained in the Boston area, dedicating herself to a career in psychology. During this time, she held positions at Radcliffe, Wellesley, and Simmons colleges and published a book, The Psychology of Beauty in 1905, based on her research in aesthetics; she credited Munsterberg for his philosophical theories and scientific guidance, which largely influenced her thought (Puffer, 1905, p. vii). She was also being courted by a young graduate of the Massachusetts Institute of Technology who was a native of Keene, New Hampshire, where they met when Puffer taught high school there. In August 1908, then in her mid-thirties, Ethel Puffer married Benjamin Howes, an event that brought her career in psychology to a halt (Furumoto and Scarborough, 1987, p. 82). Puffer lived in an era where academic hiring practices viewed married women as inappropriate candidates for teaching positions. In 1922, Puffer published two articles in the Atlantic Monthly, spelling out what she saw as an inherent contradiction in the notion that a woman could combine marriage and a career (Howes, 1922a, 1922b). Puffer claimed that for years the reality of the lives of educated married women had been generally ignored (Furumoto and Scarborough, 1987, p. 84). Job discrimination was only one of several obstacles confronting the married professional woman. Even if every woman had the right to marry and go on with her job, Puffer maintained, she would still face several stumbling blocks to success in a career. She believed that what was beyond the grasp of the young, married professional woman was the possibility of mental concentration, of long-sustained intensive application, of freedom from irrelevant cares and interruptions, which every professional man knows is a dire necessity, if he is to touch success (Howes, 1922a, p. 446). Puffer felt that women are both inevitably impelled to and interdicted from, marriage, children, careers (Howes, 1922a, p. 452). As long as the culture defined careers as all consuming, and as long as women were assigned all the responsibility for the nurturing of children, there could be no solution to the dilemma (Furumoto and Scarborough, 1987, p. 87). In her second Atlantic Monthly article “Continuity for women”, Puffer rejected the suggestion that the talents of educated women could find adequate expression within the home, asking whether an entomologist would find the full expression of his science in keeping his household free from insect pests. Would he continue to be an entomologist at all if that were the extent of his activity? (Howes, 1922b, p. 733). Puffer challenged women to forego careers in a traditional sense in favor of professional work that could be accomplished on a flexible timetable as their primary responsibility of mothering permitted (Furumoto and Scarborough, 1987, p. 87). A grant from the Laura Spelman Rockefeller Fund in 1925 financed the Institute for the Coordination of Women’s Interests at Smith College, with Puffer as director. During the three-year grant period, while her two children were still in elementary school, Puffer commuted from her home in New York City to western Massachusetts. In the JMH 18,2 172 three-year grant period, aided by the institute staff, Puffer carried out an impressive array of projects. One of these was a survey of 500 Smith alumnae, identified by the institute as successfully coordinating family and work responsibilities (Furumoto and Scarborough, 1987, pp. 87-89). Another project included developing a cooperative nursery school and a food service that delivered hot meals to families. These demonstration programs were designed to reduce women’s domestic responsibilities, thus freeing them for other work (Furumoto and Scarborough, 1987, pp. 87-89). The institute also conducted research to identify occupations that would meet women’s needs for flexible schedules, singling out among others, free-lance journalism and domestic and landscape architecture (Furumoto and Scarborough, 1987, pp. 87-89). While working with Gertrude Lane and Myra Reed Richardson, editors of Woman’s Home Companion, Puffer launched a popular campaign for women’s home service clubs in 1923 that outdid all of the Ladies’ Home Journal’s previous efforts to promote kitchen-less houses and community kitchens in 1919 and 1920. Puffer first visited cooperatives such as the Evanston Community Kitchen, and wrote about them in the Woman’s Home Companion and she and her colleagues encouraged readers to tell them about everyday problems, resulting in over two thousand letters about housewives’ isolation, overwork and depression. Throughout these campaigns Puffer warned women that commercial labor saving devices were not a solution to their problems and she identified women’s larger need, for true and substantial happiness that had been the avowed aim of the Seneca Falls Convention (women rights convention) in 1848. The Institute for the Coordination of Women’s Interest became the base camp for Puffer’s broad campaign in favor of socialized domestic work between 1926 and 1931 and she marshaled historians to research the experience of managing careers and homes, career guidance specialists to design new strategies for conquering employers’ prejudice against women, a housing expert to study the architectural implications of employed women’s needs, and home economists and child care experts to demonstrate the feasibility of services to assist employed mothers (Hayden, 1981, pp. 269-271). Puffer even brought in Lillian Mollner Gilbreth as a visitor to the institute to explain how housewives could “efficiently” do all their own work at home (Hayden, 1981, p. 275). Yet, despite the productivity of the institute and the fact that it appeared to be addressing a matter of real concern to many women, its funding was not renewed; as the 1920s drew to a close, it was phased out of existence (Hayden, 1981, p. 275). The Rockefeller Fund did not renew its grant. Presumably, this occurred because, in the foundation’s view, the institute had devoted its efforts too much to applied research and not enough to developing a theoretical framework for coordinating women’s interest (Hayden, 1981, p. 275). In her late 50’s when the project was terminated, Puffer turned her attention to other interests, ending her professional involvement with the issue of coordinating educated women’s needs for intellectual as well as personal fulfillment. In her Atlantic Monthly articles of 1922, when she expressed the inherent contradiction for women in combining marriage and motherhood with careers, Puffer’s way of resolving the dilemma was to redefine careers for women, rather than to suggest redefining marriage and motherhood (Furumoto and Scarborough, 1987, pp. 89-90). In her last public utterance on the marriage versus career issue in 1929, Puffer did seem to be calling for just that, a redefinition of women’s traditional role to eliminate forever what she referred to as the “intolerable choice” (Howes, 1929, p. 6). William Moulton Marston William Moulton Marston (1893-1947) was born in Saugus, Massachusetts. Over the course of his career, he wore many hats and was known as psychologist, a feminist theorist, an inventor, and comic-strip writer. He obtained an AB from Harvard in 1915 and then a law degree in 1918 and a PhD in psychology in 1921. He began working on his blood pressure approach to deception in 1915 as a graduate student under the direction of Hugo Munsterberg in the Harvard Psychological Laboratory (Moore, 2003, p. 292). Marston and Munsterberg Hugo Munsterberg had an impressively broad spectrum of interests, including psychotherapy, pedagogy, philosophy, aesthetics, mysticism and cultural psychology, but it was the detection of deception that one of his undergraduate students, William Moulton Marston, found most fascinating (Bunn, 1997, p. 95). The ambitious young psychologist would extend and develop Munsterberg’s scientific lie detection research program, and would do so in a manner reminiscent of his teacher’s exuberant style (Bunn, 1997, p. 95) that allowed some to view Marston as the illegitimate heir of Munsterberg. Having received an LLB in 1918 and a PhD three years later, Marston immediately set about manufacturing his reputation as the inventor of the lie detector (Bunn, 1997, p. 95). Marston, the lie detector and other professional work According to Marston, deception may be tested by means of the measurement of the systolic blood pressure of a suspect while he is testifying and the success of this method was reported by him working under Professor Munsterberg in 1915 (Marston, 1921, p. 552). Having performed experiments with the instrument, and after testing it on suspected spies during the First World War, in 1923, Marston unsuccessfully attempted to get the lie detector admitted as evidence in a court of law (Bunn, 1997). During the early 1920s, Marston devoted himself to empirical research on the detection of deception and the measurement of systolic blood pressure (Bunn, 1997). In 1924, he traveled to New York City to work with the National Committee for Mental Hygiene, and then to Texas, where he analyzed every prisoner, male and female, in the state penitentiaries according to his theory of emotions. The latter half of the decade saw him develop his theoretical ideas (Bunn, 1997). He advanced an unusual psychonic theory of consciousness, promoted materialism and vitalism with equal vigor, and speculated on the relationship between primary colors and primary emotions (Bunn, 1997). After the First World War, Marston moved for ten years from one academic post to another, including stints at American University, Columbia University, New York University, and Tufts University (Moore, 2003, p. 292). It was during this period that Marston developed his theory of emotions, borrowing from related literature, and developed his own personality test to measure four important personality factors (Moore, 2003, p. 292). The factors he chose were called dominance, influence, steadiness, and compliance, from which the DISC theory takes its name. In 1926, Women issues to Wonder Woman 173 JMH 18,2 174 Marston published his findings in a book entitled The Emotions of Normal People, which included a brief description of the personality test he had developed (Moore, 2003, p. 292). Then, in 1929, he left academia, and traveled to Universal Studios in California, where he spent a year as director of public services (Moore, 2003, p. 292). Like Munsterberg, Marston was quite a publicist and he appeared in magazine photo-stories attempting to resolve marital difficulties with his lie detector, and he even appeared in a 1938 Gillette advertisement, which used a lie detector test to discover men’s “true” feelings about various shaving aids (Saturday Evening Post, 1938). In 1940, when he was serving as an educational consultant for Detective Comics, Inc (now known as DC Comics), Marston asked why there was not a female hero. Max Charles Gaines, then head of DC Comics, was intrigued by the concept and told Marston that he could create a female comic book hero – a “Wonder Woman” – which he did, using a pen name that combined his middle name with Gaines’s: Charles Moulton (Moore, 2003, p. 295). Marston, feminism and Wonder Woman Marston did not regard dominance and submission as strictly being masculine and feminine categories. In fact he thought it quite legitimate for men and women to adopt either dominant or submissive behaviors depending on the particular circumstances (Bunn, 1997, p. 104). In a 1937 New York Times interview, Marston predicted that within 100 years the country will see the beginning of a sort of Amazonian matriarchy; within 500 years a definite sex battle for supremacy would occur, and after a millennium, women would take over the rule of the country politically and economically. Marston felt that men would eventually lose the sex battle for supremacy because they adhered to the misguided doctrine of material success, whereas happiness was the female criterion for success. Although his academic psychology and his popular psychology had both attempted to win the battle for womanhood, Marston’s most enduring contribution towards the creation of a sort of Amazonian matriarchy was a comic book character (Bunn, 1997, pp. 105-106). Wonder Woman first appeared in a nine-page center spread in the December-January 1941 issue of All Star Comics (Moore, 2003, p. 295). Then, in January 1942, she debuted in Sensation Comics number one, with a full version of her origin and her first adventure, armed with her bulletproof bracelets, magic lasso, and her Amazonian training (Moore, 2003, p. 295). Wonder Woman’s magic lasso (a device that compelled all encircled by it to tell the truth) is her most notable possession and a link to the original and modern myth of the invincibility of the polygraph (Moore, 2003, p. 295). In many ways, Marston was the perfect designer of the first female comic book superhero (Bunn, 1997, pp. 106-107). He had feminist ambitions, an enthusiastic and populist sensibility, and an expertise that lent credibility to the project (Bunn, 1997, pp. 106-107). But whereas Wonder Woman provided him with far more powerful opportunities to promote his ideas about feminine equality than psychology ever had, he nevertheless drew heavily on psychology for inspiration (Bunn, 1997, pp. 106-107). The super-heroine embodied many of the psychological themes Marston had developed throughout his career, but above all, Wonder Woman was informed by the meta-principle that had fascinated him for 20 years: dominance-submission (Bunn, 1997, pp. 106-107). Because the Amazon universe as Marston conceived of it was a place of female freedom that was ultimately dependent on the absence of men, it was a freedom defined by submission (Bunn, 1997, p.107). Like the philosophy expounded by Wonder Woman herself, freedom was not an absolute quality but was rather subject to essential restrictions. Wonder Woman was therefore an expression of Marston’s philosophy of freedom through servitude (Bunn, 1997, p.107). The route to male freedom was through constraint too: because masculine power was destructive, it must be held in check by feminine “love allure”, which is the power women possess in dominating men by using their feminine wiles (Bunn, 1997, p.107). Marston’s family life Marston’s personal life was every bit as unconventional as his ideas about matriarchy; if nothing else, the details make one wonder about his fixation on liberated women (Gillespie, 2001). In 1915, the same year he graduated from Harvard, Marston married a Mt Holyoke graduate named Elizabeth Holloway, who went on to earn an MA in Psychology (Radcliffe College) and law degree (Boston University), and to assist him in his psychological research. In the late 1920s, when teaching at Tufts University, Marston met a student named Olive Richard, who moved in with him and his wife (Gillespie, 2001). Marston had two children by each woman and he and his wife formally adopted his children by Richard. “It was an arrangement where they all lived together fairly harmoniously”(Gillespie, 2001). A business associate vouched for Marston’s offbeat arrangement, remembering him as “the most remarkable host, with a lovely bunch of kids from different wives all living together like one big family – everybody very happy and all good, decent people” (Gillespie, 2001). These living arrangements, unusual now, and extraordinary in Marston’s day, may have accounted for some of his career changes (Daniels, 2000). Few colleges would have countenanced a professor who was living with two women and having children with both of them, so Marston may have sacrificed his academic opportunities out of affection for these two women, who apparently were friendly enough to name their kids after each other (Daniels, 2000). Unfortunately, Marston was unable to enjoy his happy home life for long, as he first contracted polio and then succumbed to cancer in 1947, reportedly continuing to write from his deathbed (Lyons, 2006). After Marston’s death, his widows continued to live together for another four decades until Olive’s death in the late eighties (Lyons, 2006). As Byrne Marston (son of William and Olive) described it, “It’s kind of crazy, but it worked out and they got along quite well (Lyons, 2006). They were just a pair from then on until they died.” Elizabeth Marston died in 1993, at the age of 100 (Lyons, 2006). Marston’s legacy continues The legacy of William Moulton Marston will continue to live on even though time has relegated him to near obscurity. His contributions to science and popular culture are still very much present even though different writers have re-imagined and re-tooled Wonder Woman because of their lack of understanding or interest in his views of dominance-submission. As the creator of the systolic blood pressure test used to detect deception, Marston’s pioneering work has become one of the components of the modern polygraph. Women issues to Wonder Woman 175 JMH 18,2 176 Conclusion Hugo Munsterberg was a complex individual; even though he demonstrated that he was a bit of a chauvinist, probably due to the masculinist German culture that he was raised in. The difficulties caused by Munsterberg’s pro-German attitudes and behavior were compounded by his personal style (Landy, 1992). He was condescending to all but Germans and had made his position clear from almost his first day in America (Landy, 1992). At the time of his death Munsterberg was an object of public scorn and was well on the way to professional ostracism due to his controversial views and his persona (Landy, 1992). However, he still exhibited many admirable qualities. To the students who came to him for training in new methods of experimental psychology, he gave freely of time and interest, and his fertile mind supplied many and varied problems for investigation (Science, 1917, p. 82). Virtually all the students who came in contact with him idolized his creativity, warmth towards them, and energy (Landy, 1991). Some of those students were Calkins, Puffer and Marston, who ironically all possessed feminist ambitions, but nevertheless had a amicable and productive relationship with their professor, which sometimes extended beyond a professional relationship, but a cordial one. Munsterberg’s influence was evident in each of their works, in areas as diverse as the psychology of beauty and the conception of the double standpoint, to the detection of deception. Even though the names of Calkins, Puffer, Marston, and Munsterberg are somewhat lost in the shadows of time, they are still admired by the writer for illuminating the world with their presence by the contributions they made. References Bunn, G.F. (1997), “The lie detector, Wonder Woman and liberty: the life and work of William Moulton Marston”, History of the Human Sciences, Vol. 10 No. 1, pp. 91-119. Business Week (1966), “Milestones of management”, p. 20. Calkins, M.W. (1900), “Psychology as science of selves”, Philosophical Review, Vol. 9 No. 5, pp. 490-501. Calkins, M.W. (1907), “Psychology: what it is about?”, Journal of Philosophy, Psychology, and Scientific Methods, Vol. 4 No. 25, pp. 673-83. Calkins, M.W. (1915), “The self in scientific psychology”, The American Journal of Psychology, Vol. 26 No. 4, pp. 495-524. Calkins, M.W. (1930), “Mary Whiton Calkins”, in Murchison, C. (Ed.), A History of Psychology in Autobiography, Vol. 1, Clark University Press, Worcester, MA, pp. 31-62. Daniels, L. (2000), Wonder Woman: The Complete History (The Life and Times of the Amazonian Princess), Chronicle Books, San Francisco, CA. Furumoto, L. (1979), “Mary Whiton Calkins (1863-1930) fourteenth president of the American Psychological Association”, Journal of the History of Behavioral Sciences, Vol. 15 No. 4, pp. 346-56. Furumoto, L. and Scarborough, E. (1987), Untold Lives: The First Generation of American Women Psychologists, Columbia University Press, New York, NY. Gillespie, N. (2001), “William Marston’s secret identity: the strange private life of Wonder Woman’s creator”, Reason Magazine, available at: www.reason.com Hale, M. (1980), Human Science and Social Order, Temple University Press, Philadelphia, PA. Hanus, P.H., James, W., Munsterberg, H., Palmer, G.H., Royce, J., Santayana, G. and Siegfried, C.H. (1993), “1895 letter from Harvard Philosophy Department”, Hypatia, Vol. 8 No. 2, pp. 230-3. Hayden, D. (1981), The Grand Domestic Revolution: A History of Feminist Designs for American Homes, Neighborhoods, and Cities, MIT Press, Boston, MA. Howes, E.P. (1922a), “Accepting the universe”, Atlantic Monthly, Vol. 129, pp. 444-53. Howes, E.P. (1922b), “Continuity for women”, Atlantic Monthly, Vol. 130, pp. 731-9. Howes, E.P. (1929), “The meaning of progress in the woman movement”, Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, Vol. 143 No. 1, pp. 14-20. ed in James, H. (1920), “The Letters of William James”, Vol. 1, The Atlantic Monthly Press, Boston, MA. Landy, F.J. (1991), “A conversation with Harold Burtt”, The Industrial-Organizational Psychologist, Vol. 28, pp. 73-5. Landy, F.J. (1992), “Hugo Munsterberg: Victim or visionary?”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 77 No. 6, pp. 787-802. Lyons, C. (2006), “Suffering Sappho!: a look at the creator and creation of Wonder Woman’s creator”, CBR News, available at: www.comicbookresources.com Marston, W.M. (1921), “Psychological possibilities in the detection tests”, Journal of the American Institute of Criminal Law and Criminology, Vol. 11 No. 4, pp. 195-214. Moore, M.H. (2003), The Polygraph and Lie Detection, National Academies Press, Washington, DC. Munsterberg, H. (1901), American Traits from the Point of View of a German, Houghton, Mifflin and Company, Boston, MA and New York, NY. Munsterberg, M.A.A. (1922), Hugo Munsterberg, His Life and Work, D. Appleton and Co., New York, NY. New York Times (1910), “Finds our culture in women’s hands”, January 29, p. 5. O’Connell, A.N. and Russo, N.F. (1990), Women in Psychology: A Bio-bibliographic Sourcebook, Greenwood Press, New York, NY. Palmieri, P.A. (1983), “Here was fellowship: a social portrait of academic women at Wellesley College, 1895-1920”, History of Education Quarterly, Vol. 23 No. 2, pp. 195-214. Porfeli, E.J. (2009), “Hugo Munsterberg and the origins of vocational guidance”, Career Development Quarterly, Vol. 57 No. 3, pp. 225-36. Puffer, E.D. (1905), The Psychology of Beauty, Houghton, Mifflin and Company, Boston, MA. Saturday Evening Post (1938), “Now! Lie detector”, March, p. 67. Science (1917), “Minute on the life and services of Hugo Munsterberg”, Vol. 45 No. 1152, pp. 73-96. Spillman, J. and Spillman, L. (1993), “The rise and fall of Hugo Munsterberg”, Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, Vol. 29 No. 4, pp. 322-38. About the author Leon C. Prieto, MBA, PhD, is an Assistant Professor of Management at Savannah State University. His research and teaching interests include social entrepreneurship, human resources, organizational behavior, entrepreneurship, personality and management history. To purchase reprints of this article please e-mail: reprints@emeraldinsight.com Or visit our web site for further details: www.emeraldinsight.com/reprints Women issues to Wonder Woman 177 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.