Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes xxx (2014) xxx–xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/obhdp Responses to normative and norm-violating behavior: Culture, job mobility, and social inclusion and exclusion Jennifer Whitson a,⇑, Cynthia S. Wang b, Joongseo Kim b, Jiyin Cao c, Alex Scrimpshire b a The University of Texas at Austin, Department of Management, McCombs School of Business, Austin, TX 78705, United States Oklahoma State University, Department of Management, Spears School of Business, Stillwater, OK 74078, United States c Stony Brook University, Department of Management, College of Business, Stony Brook, NY 11794, United States b a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 13 January 2013 Accepted 11 August 2014 Available online xxxx Accepted by Michael Morris, Ying-yi Hong and Chi-yue Chiu Keywords: Culture Job mobility Social inclusion Social exclusion a b s t r a c t Research has demonstrated the effects of culture and mobility on the utilization of monetary rewards and punishments in response to norm-related behaviors (e.g., honesty and dishonesty), but less is known about their effects on the utilization of social inclusion and exclusion. Three experiments found that individuals in high job mobility contexts were more likely to exclude dishonest actors than those in low mobility contexts; job mobility did not affect the level of social inclusion. Experiment 1 demonstrated cultural differences in the utilization of social inclusion/exclusion versus monetary rewards/punishments, with perceived job mobility as an underlying mechanism. Experiment 2 provided a behavioral measure of social inclusion/exclusion. Experiment 3 manipulated job mobility and found that the perceived difficulty of social exclusion mediated the relationship between job mobility and social exclusion. This paper illustrates critical boundary conditions for past findings and provides insight into responses to norm-related behavior across different cultures. Ó 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Introduction When employees uphold or break norms, organizations often have formal systems of responding, and research has shown that rewards and punishments are critical in creating environments that encourage good and deter bad behavior (e.g., Chen, 2012; Fuster & Meier, 2010; Podsakoff, Bommer, Podsakoff, & MacKenzie, 2006; Wayne, Shore, Bommer, & Tetrick, 2002). Practically, rewards and punishments are effective in altering behavior (Kazdin, 2001), and organizations design financial incentive systems (e.g., performance bonus systems; financial sanctions) to reinforce positive norms and to increase employee motivation. Research has demonstrated the prominent use of both monetary rewards (Wang, Galinsky, & Murnighan, 2009) and punishments (Gray, Ward, & Norton, 2012), and a meta-analysis established that both increase cooperation in social dilemmas (for a review, see Balliet, Mulder, & Van Lange, 2011). However, whereas monetary rewards and punishments offer clear tools for managers to respond to employees who uphold or break norms, they are less widely available to most individuals. For example, coworkers generally cannot provide financial bonuses and sanctions to peers. ⇑ Corresponding author. In this paper, rather than focusing on monetary responses to normative (e.g., honest) and norm-breaking (e.g., dishonest) behavior, we draw attention to a form of response widely available to organizational members: social inclusion (i.e., the act of including someone in interpersonal relationships) and social exclusion (i.e., the act of excluding someone from interpersonal relationships; Abrams, Hogg, & Marques, 2005). Decisions to include normupholders or exclude norm-violators likely see more widespread use, because they can be made by almost everyone in social, group, or organizational contexts (Abrams et al., 2005). Importantly, our proposal moves beyond monetary responses to normative and norm-breaking behavior, delineating them from other responses, particularly social inclusion and exclusion. To date, little is known about how individuals from different cultural contexts choose to socially include and exclude others. We posit that the usage of these norm-related responses will differ across cultural contexts and that a critical socioecological factor driving these differences is the level of job mobility, i.e., the degree to which individuals can change jobs and professions within a given environment (Chen, Chiu, & Chan, 2009). The current research makes important contributions to theory on culture and responses to norm-related behaviors by lending insight into the crucial function that job mobility plays in different responses within and between cultures. This is theoretically http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.08.001 0749-5978/Ó 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Please cite this article in press as: Whitson, J., et al. Responses to normative and norm-violating behavior: Culture, job mobility, and social inclusion and exclusion. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.08.001 2 J. Whitson et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes xxx (2014) xxx–xxx compelling because social inclusion and exclusion and monetary incentives may be utilized very differently, depending on the socioecological context in which they occur. We next provide an overview of the effects of social inclusion and exclusion. We then theorize how perceptions and experiences of job mobility may lead to differences in social inclusion and exclusion within and between cultures. The effects of social inclusion and exclusion The need to belong is a fundamental human motivation (Baumeister & Leary, 1995) and being accepted as a member of a group is an integral human need (Maslow, 1968; Ryan & Deci, 2000). Social inclusion leads to a multitude of emotional and health benefits. For example, when people’s need to belong was satisfied, they exhibited higher intrinsic motivation (Ryan & Deci, 2000), reported greater well-being (Tay & Diener, 2011), and experienced increased self-esteem (Heppner et al., 2008). These effects also occur at societal levels: not only did individuals with more and higher-quality relationships have higher self-esteem, but countries with inhabitants who regularly interacted with friends had higher nationwide self-esteem levels (Denissen, Penke, Schmitt, & van Aken, 2008). Individuals also can socially exclude others. William James once noted that being excluded is an extremely aversive experience, writing, ‘‘No more fiendish punishment could be devised, were such a thing physically possible, than that one should be turned loose in society and remain absolutely unnoticed by all the members thereof’’ (James, 1890, p. 293). In fact, the experience of exclusion can be so intense that socially excluded individuals exhibit neural activity similar to that caused by physical pain (Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Williams, 2003; MacDonald & Leary, 2005). Social exclusion engenders anxiety (Baumeister & Tice, 1990; Leary, 1990), loneliness (e.g., Peplau & Perlman, 1982), anger (e.g., Williams, Shore, & Grahe, 1998), impaired self-regulation (Baumeister, DeWall, Ciarocco, & Twenge, 2005), and self-defeating behavior (e.g., Twenge, Catanese, & Baumeister, 2002). Given the powerful psychological and behavioral ramifications, social inclusion and exclusion serve as effective ways to enforce normative behavior. For example, including helpful individuals and excluding unhelpful individuals shapes group socialization and norms over time (Levine, Moreland, & Hausmann, 2005). Congruently, the threat of ostracism is considered a fundamental mechanism of establishing a norm (Ouwerkerk, Kerr, Gallucci, & Van Lange, 2005) and actual ostracism has been shown to promote cooperation in groups (Feinberg, Willer, & Schultz, 2014). These findings suggest these strategies can be effective in promoting normative behavior, thus, influences on their utilization are crucial topics of study. Despite these findings, there is a paucity of research about their utilization across contexts with different levels of job mobility. In the next section, we discuss why culture and job mobility might hold such an influential position in the use of social inclusion and exclusion. Culture and job mobility Mobility is a socioecological construct (Oishi & Graham, 2010) that takes into account the macro-environment (e.g., the structural characteristics of cities; the economic and political landscape of a community) and its psychological and behavioral ramifications. Mobility has been studied in the context of residential mobility (the degree to which people can change their residence within a given environment; Oishi, 2010), relational mobility (the degree to which people can change relationship partners within a given environment; Schug, Yuki, Horikawa, & Takemura, 2009) and job mobility (the degree to which people can change jobs and professions within a given environment; Chen et al., 2009; Yuki et al., 2007). Recent research suggests that mobility is deeply tied to many features of a given culture. For example, residential mobility alters the qualities preferred in associates and friends (Lun, Oishi, & Tenney, 2012) and influences self-construals (Oishi, Lun, & Sherman, 2007). Cultural differences in relational mobility affect the level of similarity between friends (Schug et al., 2009) and monetary reward and punishment decisions (Wang & Leung, 2010). Finally, cultural differences in job mobility are associated with different worldviews (Chen et al., 2009), with individuals in high mobility contexts less likely to endorse a belief in a fixed world. Our reasons for focusing on job mobility are twofold. First, job mobility has important implications for interpersonal interactions within organizations. Second, mobility is a critical driver of cultural differences, and perceived job mobility in particular influences cultural patterns of judgment and behavior (Chiu & Chen, 2004; Stryker, 2007). Phenomena that occur at these societal levels are powerful predictors of cross-cultural difference (Bahns, Pickett, & Crandall, 2012; Chen et al., 2009; Falk, Heine, Yuki, & Takemura, 2009; Heine & Renshaw, 2002; Schug et al., 2009; Schug, Yuki, & Maddux, 2010; Yuki et al., 2007), and therefore valuable factors to be aware of in the life of an organization. Importantly, the frequency that employees change jobs and professions throughout their work careers varies significantly by country (Borghans & Golsteyn, 2012). For example, Americans change jobs quite frequently, with the average American holding approximately ten jobs over a lifetime (Bialik, 2010; Topel & Ward, 1992). In an 11country comparison (Borghans & Golsteyn, 2012), college graduates in the United States changed jobs most often, with only 19% of graduates still holding their first job after three years. In contrast, the Korean Employment Information Service (2009) reports that the average Korean holds only 4.1 jobs over a lifetime. Job mobility can also differ within countries (Chen et al., 2009); for example, the 2007–2009 Great Recession resulted in a significant decrease in job mobility in multiple countries (Meriküll, 2011; Moscarini & Postel-Vinay, 2013). We argue that the employment of social inclusion and exclusion is influenced by job mobility. Culture, job mobility, and the utilization of social inclusion and exclusion Recent research on cultural differences in monetary responses to norm-related behaviors suggests that mobility plays a crucial role. Mobility critically influenced monetary responses, such that, Americans (who tend to be higher in job and relational mobility) rewarded honest actors more and punished dishonest actors less, than East Asians (Wang & Leung, 2010; Wang, Leung, See, & Gao, 2011). The same effects were found when job mobility was manipulated rather than measured (Wang et al., 2011). The authors posited that the strength of ties drove these effects (Wang & Leung, 2010; Wang et al., 2011). The looser networks in high mobility societies increased rewards as individuals attempted to strengthen ties with more relationally-distant honest others. In contrast, the tighter-knit networks generally found in low mobility societies increased feelings of obligation to maintain order via punishment. These mobility-driven differences in the strength of ties may also influence choices to include and exclude. Specifically, we predict that job mobility, with its critical function in determining strength of ties, will influence how individuals choose to socially include and exclude, with these effects dramatically differing from monetary responses. Unlike studies involving monetary incentives, social inclusion and exclusion are inherently Please cite this article in press as: Whitson, J., et al. Responses to normative and norm-violating behavior: Culture, job mobility, and social inclusion and exclusion. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.08.001 J. Whitson et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes xxx (2014) xxx–xxx tied to the contextual affordances of mobility. As we discuss below, the ease with which one’s context affords the maintenance and deepening of ties will influence social inclusion in response to normative behaviors. Further, the ease with which one’s context affords the weakening of ties will influence social exclusion in response to norm-violating behaviors. Social inclusion can be about forming, maintaining, or strengthening ties with an individual. Congruently, social exclusion can be about weakening or dissolving ties with an individual (Abrams et al., 2005; Baumeister & Leary, 1995). Within organizations, employees rarely have control over who is introduced into or ejected from their social contexts (i.e., hired or fired). As a result, their choices to socially include will be based largely around decisions to maintain and deepen (rather than form) ties with individuals who share the same context as they do, whereas choices to socially exclude will be based on decisions to weaken (rather than dissolve) ties. Due to our emphasis on organizational contexts, this paper therefore focuses on the maintenance, strengthening, and weakening of ties. In terms of social inclusion, low mobility environments provide affordances that make it easier to maintain ties. Relationships with others require communication and contact with one another (Hays, 1985). It may be easier to maintain ties in low mobility contexts because the stable, dense networks in those contexts will help to sustain them (Bian & Ang, 1997). Indeed, relationships in low relational mobility societies tend to be more stable and resilient to shocks or neglect (Wiseman, 1986; Yuki & Schug, 2012). Moreover, individuals in low mobility contexts may be motivated to deepen existing ties, because those ties last longer and serve multiple functions (Lun et al., 2012; Oishi & Kesebir, 2012). Research supports this proposition, as individuals in low residential mobility contexts feel less lonely (Oishi et al., 2013), and have deeper relationships with others that span multiple activities and preferences (Oishi, 2010). This suggests that individuals in an environment with low job mobility will be more likely to have interconnected and stable networks, making ties much easier to maintain and deepen (Dess & Shaw, 2001; Lewis, Belliveau, Herndon, & Keller, 2007), therefore increasing social inclusion. In a low-turnover organization, one can imagine coworkers will come to know each other’s preferences and find it easier to invite each other to events. However, in a highturnover organization, the sparse and volatile nature of interpersonal networks would make it more difficult to do so. Thus, because individuals in low mobility contexts perceive maintaining and deepening bonds with others as easier, we expect they will be more likely to socially include honest actors than individuals in high mobility contexts. Moreover, we test cultural differences in two countries: the United States and South Korea. We speculate that Koreans are more likely than Americans to use social inclusion, because South Korea has a lower level of job mobility than the United States. Hypothesis 1, Social Inclusion (H1SI): Individuals in low job mobility contexts are more likely than individuals in high job mobility contexts to socially include an honest actor. Hypothesis 2, Social Inclusion (H2SI): South Koreans perceive themselves as possessing lower job mobility than Americans, which translates into increased social inclusion. The contextual affordances of high job mobility allow individuals to more easily weaken bonds with norm-violators, and social exclusion is an opportune method to punish them and clean up the social environment simultaneously. Conversely, low job 3 mobility contexts provide fewer affordances for weakening undesirable relationships, making social exclusion more difficult to employ in response to dishonest, norm-violating actors. Indeed, research suggests that Americans express a greater desire to avoid dishonest actors than do East Asians (Wang & Leung, 2010), implying that social exclusion may be a more prominent option in high mobility contexts than in low mobility contexts. In essence, social exclusion cannot be accomplished when one is continuously brought into contact with a norm-violator, and it is more feasible to exclude a norm-violator in a high mobility environment where social networks are less dense. For example, an employee in a low-turnover organization who wishes to exclude a dishonest actor from social events will have a hard time of it, as that actor is likely to have ties with many other individuals in the organization, and thus their exclusion would require the coordination of many. Conversely, exclusion would be easier in the sparser networks of a high-turnover organization, and indeed, social exclusion is more likely to be used in situations when the cost is low, for example, when there is less interdependence between individuals (Robinson, O’Reilly, & Wang, 2013). Thus, we suggest individuals in high job mobility environments will exclude more than those in low job mobility environments. We also test whether cultural differences exist between the United States and South Korea. We predict that Americans are more likely than South Koreans to use social exclusion as a response to dishonest actors, because the United States has a higher level of job mobility than South Korea. Hypothesis 1, Social Exclusion (H1SE): Individuals in high job mobility contexts are more likely than individuals in low job mobility contexts to socially exclude a dishonest actor. Hypothesis 2, Social Exclusion (H2SE): Americans perceive themselves to possess higher job mobility than South Koreans, which translates into increased social exclusion. Overview of experiments Three experiments test the effects of job mobility on social inclusion and exclusion. Experiment 1 examines how social inclusion and exclusion, as well as monetary reward and punishment, are employed by Americans (high job mobility) and South Koreans (low job mobility). We test perceived job mobility as the mechanism underlying our proposed effects. Experiment 2 measures perceived job mobility and utilizes a behavioral measure of social inclusion and exclusion. Experiment 3 manipulates perceived job mobility and tests whether social inclusion and exclusion decisions are influenced by the perceived difficulty of doing so. Experiment 1: Culture, job mobility, and responses to honesty and deception Experiment 1 measured the perceived job mobility (Chen et al., 2009) of Americans (high job mobility) and South Koreans (low job mobility). We compared the effects of national culture and perceived job mobility on two different strategies of norm enforcement: monetary reward or punishment (using the measure from Wang et al., 2009) and social inclusion or exclusion. We included monetary responses to replicate previous findings (Wang & Leung, 2010; Wang et al., 2011) and to provide comparison points for the social inclusion and exclusion findings. Please cite this article in press as: Whitson, J., et al. Responses to normative and norm-violating behavior: Culture, job mobility, and social inclusion and exclusion. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.08.001 4 J. Whitson et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes xxx (2014) xxx–xxx Table 1 Descriptive statistics and variable inter-correlations, Experiment 1. Variables Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. .34 .51 .19 3.60 3.78 22.05 .62 .47 .50 2.29 1.86 1.35 3.64 .49 1.00 .03 .51** .00 .34** .42** .08 1.00 .03 .07 .17* .01 .05 1.00 .01 .09 .28** .02 1.00 .06 .02 .06 1.00 .13 .05 1.00 .05 1.00 Culture (0 = US; 1 = South Korea) Behavior (0 = Honesty; 1 = Dishonesty) Job mobility Monetary response Social inclusion or exclusion Age Sex (0 = Male; 1 = Female) Note: N = 221. * Correlation is significant at p 6 .05. ** Correlation is significant at p 6 .01. Method Participants and design.1 Two-hundred and twenty one undergraduate students from a US southwestern university and a South Korean university responded to a scenario as part of a class exercise. The American sample included 146 students (94 females and 52 males; 70 Caucasians, 48 Asians, 19 Hispanics, 6 African Americans, and 3 other races; mean age = 20.95, SD = 1.82, range = 19–31) and the South Korean sample included 75 students (42 females and 33 males; all Asian; mean age = 24.19, SD = 5.10, range = 18–56). The design was a 2 (behavior: honest, dishonest) 2 (culture: American, Korean) between-participants design (see Table 1 for descriptive statistics and correlation matrix). Procedure. Following Wang et al. (2009), American and Korean participants received scenarios in their respective national languages (English in the US and Korean in South Korea) in which an actor was honest or dishonest. The instructions, scenarios, and questions were back-translated from Korean to English by independent translators to ensure accuracy. To measure perceived job mobility (Chen et al., 2009), participants indicated the extent to which they agreed with four statements (from 5 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree; e.g., ‘‘Compared to other societies, it is relatively easy for people in our society to change from one profession to a totally different profession.’’). We dropped one reverse-coded item (‘‘In our society, even when people change professions, the business network they have established in their professions will still be very useful for their new profession.’’) due to low correlations with the other items ( .29, .10, and .06; Kim & Mueller, 1978) and low reliability (Hinkin, 1995). The final three items were averaged (a = .63), with higher numbers reflecting higher levels of perceived job mobility. In the dishonest condition, participants read: ‘‘Imagine the following scenario: You recently completed some work with another individual. You just found out that the individual was dishonest about some key information regarding the interaction. As a result, you only received $100. You would have received 50% more if the other individual had given you honest information.’’ Thus, in the dishonest condition, participants expected $150 and suffered a $50 loss. In the honest condition, participants were told they received $100 because the other person was honest; they expected $50 and enjoyed a $50 gain. These scenarios ensured that the gain 1 We used three consistent criteria for excluding participants: (1) participants whose responses indicated that they were not paying attention (e.g., answered all 5’s or 5’s across all scale items, even though items were reverse coded), (2) participants who completed the survey too quickly or too slowly, given the suggested time allotment and mean completion times (2.5 standard deviations above and below the mean), and (3) participants who failed the experimental manipulation checks. Twelve participants were excluded in Experiments 1 and 3 because of their lack of attention. Six participants were excluded in Experiment 2 because the survey was not completed within the time allotted. Finally, 9 participants were excluded from Experiment 2 because they failed the manipulation check of whether the other person acted honestly or dishonestly in Gneezy’s Deception Game. from the honesty and the loss from the dishonesty was equivalent at $50. To ensure that Korean participants understood the monetary stake, the US dollar amount was also presented in an equivalent amount of Korean Won, based on the approximate exchange rate ($1 = 1000 Korean Won; adapted from Wang & Leung, 2010). For example, a monetary portion of the dishonest scenario stated, ‘‘As a result, you only received $100 (approximately 100,000 Korean Won).’’ Finally, participants were asked about two types of responses to the honest or dishonest actor. They were told as they answered each question to imagine that response as the only one they had available to them. The appearance of each question was counterbalanced. Social inclusion or exclusion. Participants indicated the degree to which they would (in the honest condition) socially include the individual in or (in the dishonest condition) socially exclude the individual from their social circle (1 = not at all to 6 = extremely). Monetary reward or punishment. Following Wang and her colleagues (Wang et al., 2009, 2011; Wang & Leung, 2010), respondents could spend hypothetical money to reward (in the honest condition) or punish (in the dishonest condition) at a personal cost set at one-tenth of the amount the individual would be rewarded or punished. This information was presented in $20 increments, from $0 to $100 (e.g., participants who punished $20 selected, ‘‘Punish the individual $20 (at a cost of $2).’’ For Korean participants, the dollar amount was also translated into Korean Won. Results and discussion The effect of culture on responses to honesty and deception. The dependent variables were subjected to a 2 (behavior: honest versus dishonest) by 2 (culture: American versus Korean) by 2 (strategy: social inclusion/exclusion versus monetary reward/punishment) mixed-model analysis of variance (ANOVA). The third factor was measured as a within-participant factor.2 A three-way interaction emerged, F(1, 217) = 64.00, p < .001, suggesting the differential effects of culture and behavior on the type of response utilized. To explore the contrasts, we analyzed the effects of culture and behavior on each type of response separately. Social inclusion or exclusion. Participants were more likely to include honest actors (M = 4.02, SD = 1.20) than exclude dishonest actors (M = 3.56, SD = 1.46), F(1, 217) = 18.76, p < .001, d = .34. Also, Americans (M = 4.11, SD = 1.22) were more likely than Koreans (M = 3.15, SD = 1.39) to include and exclude the actors, F(1, 217) = 28.94, p < .001, d = .73. An interaction emerged, 2 A correlation emerged between perceived job mobility and participant age, r(219) = .28, p < .001. To ensure that participant age was not driving our job mobility effects, we performed the analyses controlling for participant age and the results remained consistent. Therefore, the results reported do not include participant age as a control. Please cite this article in press as: Whitson, J., et al. Responses to normative and norm-violating behavior: Culture, job mobility, and social inclusion and exclusion. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.08.001 J. Whitson et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes xxx (2014) xxx–xxx Fig. 1. Effect of perceived job mobility (mean ± one standard deviation) and behavior on social inclusion and exclusion, Experiment 1. F(1, 217) = 29.90, p < .001, such that Americans (M = 4.21, SD = 1.21) were more likely than Koreans (M = 2.37, SD = 1.08) to exclude dishonest actors, t(217) = 7.85, p < .001, d = 1.60; Americans (M = 4.01, SD = 1.22) and Koreans (M = 4.03, SD = 1.18) did not differ in the extent to which they included honest actors, t(217) = .06, p = .95, d = .02. Monetary response. An interaction emerged, F(1, 217) = 32.60, p < .001. Americans (M = 2.99, SD = 2.06) monetarily punished less than did Koreans (M = 4.35, SD = 1.23), t(217) = 3.99, p < .001, d = .80. In contrast, Americans (M = 4.21, SD = 1.89) monetarily rewarded more than did Koreans (M = 2.74, SD = 1.01), t(217) = 4.09, p < .001, d = .97. These results replicate those by Wang and Leung (2010). No main effects emerged for culture, F(1, 217) = .04, p = .84, d = .002, or for rewarding and punishing, F(1, 217) = .61, p = .43, d = .14. The effect of perceived job mobility on responses to honesty and deception. We ran separate linear regressions for each dependent variable, with perceived job mobility and behavior as independent variables. Social inclusion and exclusion. A Job Mobility Behavior interaction emerged for the utilization of social inclusion or exclusion, b = .18, SE = .08, t(217) = 2.25, p = .03. As perceived mobility increased, exclusion increased, b = .14, SE = .05, t(217) = 2.53, p = .01. However, perceived mobility did not influence inclusion, b = .04, SE = .06, t(217) = .70, p = .49 (see Fig. 1). Monetary response. A Job Mobility x Behavior interaction emerged for the amount of monetary response, b = .43, SE = .11, t(217) = 4.06, p < .001. As perceived mobility increased, the amount of monetary punishment decreased, b = .21, SE = .07, t(217) = 2.89, p = .004, but the amount of monetary reward increased, b = .22, SE = .08, t(217) = 2.85, p = .005 (see Fig. 2). Moderated path analyses. We hypothesized that cultural differences in perceived job mobility explain why Americans and Koreans differ in their responses. To test this hypothesis, we ran two moderated path analyses, one with social inclusion/exclusion and one with monetary reward/punishment as the dependent variable. For each moderated path analysis, we utilized the Second Stage and Direct Effect Moderation Model (Edwards & Lambert, 2007). In this model, moderation occurs between the mediator and the dependent variable (i.e., second stage moderation) and the independent variable and the dependent variable (i.e., direct effect moderation). Our hypothesis was tested via two multiple regression models (see Table 2 for regression results). The first regression 5 Fig. 2. Effect of perceived job mobility (means ± one standard deviation) and behavior on amount of monetary reward and punishment, Experiment 1. tested and confirmed that Americans (M = .64; SD = 2.07) perceive themselves to possess higher job mobility than Koreans (M = 1.84; SD = 1.77), t(219) = 8.86, p < .001, d = 1.29. The second regression tested whether the actor’s behavior influences the extent to which perceived job mobility and culture affect the utilization of each norm enforcement strategy. We discuss the results for each dependent variable and the overall path analysis significance testing below. Social inclusion and exclusion. We predicted that Americans socially exclude more and include less than Koreans, and that these relationships are mediated by levels of perceived job mobility. We regressed social inclusion/exclusion on culture (independent variable), job mobility (mediator), behavior (moderator), the culture behavior interaction, and the job mobility x behavior interaction. A bootstrap procedure with 5000 samples tested the magnitude of the direct, indirect, and total effects at each level of the moderator (honest versus dishonest). We first consider results in the dishonest condition. The direct effect of culture on exclusion emerged, with a 95% bias-corrected confidence interval (CI) of [ 2.41, 1.46] not overlapping with zero. Although trending, the indirect effect, which tested job mobility as a mediator between culture and exclusion, did not emerge, with a 90% CI of [ .07, .32]. However, the indirect effect, when combined with the direct effect, produced a total effect, with a 99% CI of [ 2.40, 1.24]. These findings provide support for H1SE and H2SE: Americans perceived themselves to possess greater job mobility than Koreans, which translated into increased social exclusion. In the honest condition, the CIs of the direct [ .79, .57], indirect [ .23, .50], and total effects [ .50, .47] all overlapped with zero. Therefore, H1SI and H2SI were not supported: no differences in inclusion emerged by culture, and perceived job mobility did not mediate the effects of culture on social inclusion. Monetary response. We replicated Wang and Leung (2010)’s study by regressing monetary reward/punishment on culture (independent variable), job mobility (mediator), behavior (moderator), the culture behavior interaction, and the job mobility behavior interaction. We again utilized the bootstrapping method by Edwards and Lambert (2007). In the dishonest condition, we found a direct effect of culture on monetary punishment with a 95% CI of [.39, 1.84]. Although the indirect effect through perceived job mobility did Please cite this article in press as: Whitson, J., et al. Responses to normative and norm-violating behavior: Culture, job mobility, and social inclusion and exclusion. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.08.001 6 J. Whitson et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes xxx (2014) xxx–xxx Table 2 Second stage and direct effect moderated path analysis results for social inclusion/exclusion and monetary reward/punishment, Experiment 1. Perceived job mobility Mediator variable model b Constant Culture (0 = US; 1 = South Korea) .84 2.48 Dependent variable model Conditional indirect effects Honesty Direct Indirect Total Dishonesty Direct Indirect Total p <.001*** <.001*** .16 .28 Social inclusion/exclusion b Constant Culture (0 = US; 1 = South Korea) Job Mobility Behavior (0 = Honesty; 1 = Dishonesty) Job mobility Behavior Culture Behavior SE Monetary reward/punishment SE 4.07 .14 .06 .17 .01 1.80 Boot effect .14 .14 .002 1.94 .12 1.82 .15 .30 .06 .21 .08 .40 p b <.001*** .64 .37 .41 .93 <.001*** 4.14 1.28 .07 1.08 .17 2.41 Conf. interval Boot effect SE p .22 .44 .09 .31 .12 .58 <.001*** .004** .46 .001** .16 <.001*** Conf. interval [ .79, .57] [ .23, .50] [ .50, .47] 1.28 .17 1.44 [ 2.05, .43]* [ .63, .29] [ 1.97, .84]* [ 2.41, 1.46]* [ .07, .32] [ 2.40, 1.24]** 1.13 .26 1.39 [.39, 1.84]* [ .15, .70] [.75, 1.98]* Note: N = 221. Unstandardized regression coefficients are reported. Bootstrap sample size = 5000. * p 6 .05, ** p 6 .01, *** p 6 .001. + p 6 .10. not emerge [ .15, .70], the indirect and direct effects combined to produce a total effect [.75, 1.98]. In the honest condition, a direct effect of culture on monetary reward emerged [ 2.05, .43]. The indirect effect did not emerge [ .63, .29], but the total effect of culture on monetary reward did [ 1.97, .84]. Experiment 1 replicated findings by Wang and Leung (2010) that Americans were less likely than Koreans to monetarily punish a dishonest actor, but were more likely to monetarily reward an honest actor. Importantly, decisions to socially exclude versus include portrayed a considerably different pattern of effects. As predicted, Americans were more likely than Koreans to exclude dishonest actors, with this effect driven by higher perceived job mobility. However, contrary to our predictions, Americans and Koreans did not differ in their levels of social inclusion. Experiment 2: Behavioral social inclusion and exclusion Experiment 1 demonstrated clear cultural patterns and so Experiments 2 and 3 focused on the proposed mechanism, job mobility, to clarify its effects. Experiment 2 moved beyond the scenario used in Experiment 1 by placing individuals in a context that allowed behavioral choices. We used a modified version of Gneezy’s (2005) Deception Game, in which one player sends a truthful or misleading message and the other player chooses whether to believe it, with their decisions jointly determining their payoffs. Participants also played Cyberball (Williams, Yeager, Cheung, & Choi, 2012), in which they chose to include or exclude others in an on-line ball tossing game. This paradigm was designed to duplicate inclusion and exclusion decisions that occur in face-to-face group interactions (Williams & Sommer, 1997) and has demonstrated similar effects as face-to-face paradigms (Williams, 2009). We believe this paradigm has implications for organizational settings in which group members often engage in interconnected work tasks and choose if and when to interact with others. Method Participants and design. One-hundred nineteen undergraduate participants from a US southwestern university completed this study for extra credit (80 females and 39 males; 50 Caucasians, 41 Asians, 24 Hispanics, and 4 other races; mean age = 29.61, SD = 8.53, range = 18–40) and were randomly assigned to a dishonest or honest condition. We also measured participants’ perceived job mobility (see Table 3 for descriptive statistics and correlation matrix). Procedure. We used the same job mobility scale as in Experiment 1 (Chen et al., 2009), and dropped the same item because of decreased reliability and low correlations with the other items (.20, .05, and .002). The three items were averaged (a = .60), with higher numbers reflecting higher levels of perceived job mobility. Stage 1. Participants were told they would be completing two stages during the study. In Stage 1, participants took part in the modified version of Gneezy’s (2005) Deception Game. Participants were informed that they would be assigned at random to play as Player A or Player B with another randomly chosen participant. In reality, every participant played as Player B, and we manipulated Player A’s choices. Participants were told that only Player As would have the payoff matrix with two options (Options A and B) based on a fictitious currency (MAXs). Participants (Player Bs) could not see the payoff matrix, but were asked to choose Option A or B; depending on the option selected, they would earn more or less than Player A. The instructions also indicated that, prior to their option choice, Player As would send participants one of two messages: ‘‘Option A earns you more than Option B’’ or ‘‘Option B earns you more than Option A.’’ The instructions made it clear that only one of these messages was accurate. Because we controlled Player As’ choices, the message always indicated that participants would do best by choosing Option A. Research (e.g., Gneezy, 2005) indicates that a large majority of participants believe the message and choose Option A. Please cite this article in press as: Whitson, J., et al. Responses to normative and norm-violating behavior: Culture, job mobility, and social inclusion and exclusion. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.08.001 7 J. Whitson et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes xxx (2014) xxx–xxx Table 3 Descriptive statistics and variable inter-correlations, Experiment 2. Variables Mean SD 1 2 3 3a 3b 3c 3d 4 5 1. Job mobility 2. Behavior (0 = Honesty; 1 = Dishonesty) 3. Total number of passes to prior player a. Round 1 b. Round 2 c. Round 3 d. Round 4 4. Age 5. Sex (0 = Male; 1 = Female) .35 .54 1.41 .39 .20 .34 .49 29.61 .67 2.09 .50 .92 .49 .40 .47 .50 8.53 .47 1.00 .09 .03 .11 .08 .13 .01 .02 .11 1.00 .29** .25** .09 .02 .23* .01 .01 1.00 .60** .39** .28** .68** .11 .08 1.00 .10 .24** .43** .17 .04 1.00 .003 .01 .02 .18* 1.00 .20* .01 .07 1.00 .01 .04 1.00 .05 1.00 Note: N = 119. * Correlation is significant at p 6 .05. ** Correlation is significant at p 6 .01. After choosing Option A or B, participants in the honest condition were told that Player A’s message was true, that they would receive 40 MAXs in Stage 1, and that ‘‘you would have received 50% less if you had chosen the other option.’’ In the dishonest condition, participants were told that Player A’s message was not true, that they would receive 40 MAXs in Stage 1, and that ‘‘you would have received 50% more if you had chosen the other option.’’ Thus, their payoffs were identical, but were framed as either an equivalent gain or a loss resulting from a truth or a lie, respectively. Stage 2. After completing Stage 1, participants then moved on to Stage 2, in which they played an adapted version of Cyberball (Williams et al., 2012). Participants were informed that they would be interacting with three other participants who were simultaneously online during the study, one of whom was the honest or dishonest actor from Stage 1. On the computer screen, participants saw images of four figures. The participant was labeled as ‘‘You (Player 2)’’ and the player from Stage 1 as ‘‘Prior Player 1.’’ The two new players were labeled, ‘‘New Player 3’’ and ‘‘New Player 4’’, respectively. We controlled the actions of the other three players at all times. Participants started the game in possession of the ball and were asked who they wanted to throw it to: ‘‘Prior Player 1’’, ‘‘New Player 3’’ or ‘‘New Player 4’’. We measured participants’ tossing behavior, with participants’ choices providing a measure of inclusion or exclusion. To make tossing decisions more salient, emphasis was placed on participants mentally visualizing the ball-tossing between players; the figures and ball were animated on the computer, with the animation reflecting the participants’ tossing decisions and the other players’ subsequent reciprocation. To standardize tossing decisions, after the participant tossed the ball, the receiver tossed the ball back to the participant (e.g., if the participant tossed the ball to New Player 3, New Player 3 would return the ball to the participant). Each back and forth tossing exchange was coded as one round. Because passes were pre-programmed to be returned to the participant, the game only included four rounds to enhance the realism and reduce suspicion of the experience. Results and discussion Number of passes to Prior Player 1. We began by analyzing each round separately. For Round 1, a binomial logistic regression analysis resulted in a Job Mobility x Behavior interaction, b = .41, SE = .21, Wald = 3.75, p = .05 (see Fig. 3). As perceived job mobility increased, the odds that the ball would be passed to the dishonest actor decreased, b = .41, SE = .18, Wald = 5.57, p = .02, suggesting that the level of job mobility was positively associated with the usage of social exclusion. Following honesty, however, perceived job mobility did not influence the odds of the ball being passed to the honest actor, b = .005, SE = .12, Wald = .002, p = .97. Separate logistic regression analyses were conducted for Rounds 2, 3, Fig. 3. Effect of perceived job mobility (mean ± one standard deviation) and behavior on the probability of passing the ball to the prior player, Experiment 2 (Round 1). and 4. The Job Mobility x Behavior interaction did not emerge across the final three rounds (all p’s > .39). To further explore the effects, we aggregated the number of passes participants made to the Prior Player 1 across all four rounds, with higher number reflecting higher inclusion/less exclusion, e.g., tossing the ball to Prior Player 1 in all four rounds was coded as 4. We tested the effect of perceived job mobility and behavior on the amount of social inclusion or exclusion. Participants tossed the ball to honest players more than to dishonest players, b = .53, SE = .17, t(115) = 3.19, p = .002. However, no interaction emerged, b = .02, SE = .08, t(115) = .24, p = .81. Overall, we found no differences in social inclusion across the levels of mobility. Conversely, individuals from high mobility contexts were more likely to exclude dishonest actors in the initial Cyberball round than individuals from low mobility contexts, with the effects dissipating in later rounds. It is possible that the later rounds attenuated the effects of perceived job mobility because a multi-round interaction with the same individuals would, over time, reduce a participants’ sense of mobility within their current set of interpersonal interactions. We discuss this possibility more extensively in the general discussion. Experiment 3: The difficulty of socially excluding Experiment 3 aimed to accomplish several goals. First, as Experiments 1 and 2 measured perceived job mobility, Experiment 3 manipulated job mobility to test causality. Although Experiments 1 and 2 suggest that job mobility will not influence social inclusion, we nonetheless measure it to be comprehensive. Second, we Please cite this article in press as: Whitson, J., et al. Responses to normative and norm-violating behavior: Culture, job mobility, and social inclusion and exclusion. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.08.001 8 J. Whitson et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes xxx (2014) xxx–xxx improved the single-item measures of social inclusion and exclusion employed in Experiment 1 by using multiple-item measures so as to better test the validity of our findings. Third, we ensured that all aspects of the design were organizationally-related: our participants were working adults, our scenarios situated participants within a working context, and our manipulation of mobility was job-specific. Finally, we examined potential mechanisms underlying our effects. In particular, we proposed that social exclusion will be more prevalent in high mobility environments because it is less difficult to implement. Procedure. Participants were randomly assigned to a high versus low job mobility condition. We manipulated job mobility by asking participants to read a scenario containing job statistics for a hypothetical country (Chen et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2011). Participants in the low job mobility condition read: ‘‘Due to its government’s labor policies, the job mobility in Country X is extremely low. According to the research statistics provided by the National Academy of Social Sciences, the majority of the people in Country X have worked in only 1 or 2 jobs in the same occupation throughout their lifetime. About one third of the people who are older than 50 years of age still remain in their first job.’’ Participants in the high job mobility condition read a scenario in which the job mobility in Country X was extremely high, with statistics showing that people in Country X had worked in 3–6 jobs and that the percentage of people who remain in their first job decreased after the age of 30. Two supporting graphs were presented with each condition. Participants in both conditions were asked to imagine how they would prepare for the job market if they were a citizen of Country X and describe that preparation in as much detail as possible. Social inclusion or exclusion. After the job mobility manipulation, participants received the same scenario as in Experiment 1. Participants indicated the degree to which they would socially exclude (in the dishonest condition) or socially include (in the honest condition) the actor. Social exclusion (inclusion) was measured using the following three questions (adapted from Ferris, Brown, Berry, & Lian, 2008): ‘‘After the incident, to what extent would you ignore (include) the individual at work?’’, ‘‘. . .would you leave (stay in) the area when the individual entered?’’, and ‘‘...would you be unresponsive (responsive) to the individual’s greeting at work?’’, 1 = not at all to 6 = extremely. These items were averaged to form a measure of social exclusion (a = .86) and social inclusion (a = .81). Perceived difficulty of social inclusion or exclusion. Participants were then asked to rate how difficult it would be to implement these strategies using four items, e.g., ‘‘How hard would it be for you to do these things?’’, 1 = not at all to 6 = extremely. These items were averaged to form a difficulty measure (a = .91). Perceived effectiveness of socially including or excluding. Participants also rated eight items measuring the effectiveness of these social exclusion (inclusion) strategies, e.g., ‘‘How likely would the individual be to repeat the dishonest (honest) act if you did these things?’’, ‘‘How strong are these punishments (rewards)?’’, ‘‘How negatively (positively) affected would the individual have been by these things?’’, 1 = not at all to 6 = extremely. These items were averaged to form an effectiveness measure (a = .93). To confirm that difficulty and effectiveness were two independent constructs, the two measures were analyzed via a principal components analysis (PCA) with varimax rotation with kaiser normalization. Results revealed a two-factor structure (eigenvalues greater than 1.0) explaining 73% of the variance and supporting the discriminant validity of difficulty and effectiveness. All the Hypothesis 3. Individuals in high job mobility contexts perceive social exclusion as less difficult than individuals in low mobility contexts, which translates into increased social exclusion. An alternative explanation for why low job mobility was associated with decreased social exclusion in Experiments 1 and 2 involves the perceived effectiveness of social exclusion. As individuals in a low mobility environment are more likely to have collective self-construals (Oishi, Lun et al., 2007), it is not surprising that their social exclusion causes greater fear and pain (Kim & Markman, 2006) and is perceived to be a more effective social control (Hashimoto & Yamagishi, 2013) than social exclusion for individuals in a high mobility environment. Furthermore, theory suggests that people are aware that, ceteris paribus, punishments are felt more intensely than rewards (Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Finkenauer, & Vohs, 2001), and so tend to use them more sparingly (Wang et al., 2009). If this is the case, it logically follows that as social exclusion becomes more excruciating for those excluded (as in low mobility contexts), individuals may feel they need to use this method less extensively. In short, an important question is whether this rationale could also explain the social exclusion findings from Experiments 1 and 2: that is, whether individuals from low mobility contexts are more moderate in their use of social exclusion than those from high mobility contexts because they believe that not as much exclusion is necessary to be an effective deterrent. We therefore included a measure of perceived effectiveness to test this alternative explanation. Method Participants and design. One-hundred fifteen working adults in the United States (45 females and 70 males; 90 Caucasians, 9 Asians, 6 Hispanics, 5 African Americans, and 5 other races; mean age = 31.35, SD = 11.66, range = 18–67) were recruited via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk), an online survey program which has been proposed to provide a diverse and representative sample while yielding data that are equal to or exceed the psychometric standards in established research (see Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011). The design was a 2 (job mobility: high, low) 2 (behavior: honest, dishonest) between-participants design (see Table 4 for descriptive statistics and correlation matrix). Table 4 Descriptive statistics and variable inter-correlations, Experiment 3. Variables Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. .51 .55 2.50 3.62 3.99 31.35 .39 .50 .50 1.31 1.11 1.52 11.66 .49 1.00 .08 .10 .06 .13 .04 .15 1.00 .33** .47** .70** .08 .05 1.00 .12 .44** .06 .12 1.00 .62** .05 .10 1.00 .04 .13 1.00 .43** 1.00 Manipulated job mobility (0 = Low; 1 = High) Behavior (0 = Honesty; 1 = Dishonesty) Difficulty Effectiveness Social inclusion or exclusion Age Sex (0 = Male; 1 = Female) Note: N = 115. * Correlation is significant at p 6 .05. ** Correlation is significant at p 6 .01. Please cite this article in press as: Whitson, J., et al. Responses to normative and norm-violating behavior: Culture, job mobility, and social inclusion and exclusion. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.08.001 J. Whitson et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes xxx (2014) xxx–xxx Table 5 First stage and direct effect moderated path analysis results for social inclusion/ exclusion, Experiment 3. Difficulty Mediator variable model b SE p Constant Job mobility Behavior Job mobility Behavior 1.83 .36 1.37 1.03 .26 .34 .33 .46 <.001*** .29 <.001*** .03* Dependent variable model b SE p Constant Job mobility Behavior Job mobility Behavior Difficulty 5.71 .12 2.20 .55 .24 .26 .29 .30 .40 .08 <.001*** .68 <.001*** .17 .004** Conditional indirect effects Boot effect Conf. interval Social inclusion/exclusion Honesty Direct Indirect Total Dishonesty Direct Indirect Total .12 .09 .21 .43 .16 .59 [ .49, .26] [ .28, .02] [ .59, .20] [ .07, .97] [.003, .47]* [.06, 1.15]+ Note: N = 115. Unstandardized regression coefficients are reported. Bootstrap sample size = 5000. * p 6 .05, ** p 6 .01, *** p 6 .001. + p 6 .10. items were significantly loaded on the expected factors with no cross-loadings. Results and discussion Social inclusion and exclusion. The likelihood of socially including/excluding the actor was submitted to a Job Mobility x Behavior between-participants ANOVA. Participants were more likely to include an honest actor (M = 5.16, SD = .73) than to exclude a dishonest actor (M = 3.03, SD = 1.31), F(1, 111) = 111.31, p < .001. An interaction emerged, F(1, 111) = 3.84, p = .05. High mobility participants (M = 3.33, SD = 1.38) were more likely than low mobility participants (M = 2.75, SD = 1.20) to socially exclude a dishonest actor, t(111) = 2.16, p = .03; mobility did not influence social inclusion decisions (high mobility: M = 5.07, SD = .77 versus low mobility: M = 5.28, SD = .67), t(111) = .69, p = .49. Moderated path analyses. We hypothesized that difficulty, but not effectiveness, would explain the effect of job mobility on social exclusion. To test this hypothesis, we used two First Stage and Direct Effect Moderation Models (Edwards & Lambert, 2007), one with difficulty as the mediator, the other with effectiveness as the mediator. In this model, moderation occurs between the independent variable and the mediator (i.e. first stage moderation) and the independent variable and the dependent variable (i.e., direct effect moderation). Difficulty. To test H3, we ran the moderated path analysis utilizing two regressions (see Table 5). The mediator variable model tests the interactive effect of manipulated job mobility and behavior on difficulty.3 A Job Mobility Behavior between-participants ANOVA yielded an interaction, F(1, 111) = 4.97, p = .03, with participants in the high job mobility condition (M = 2.53, SD = 1.32) 3 We reported ANOVA results for ease of interpretation. As shown in Table 5, the moderated path analysis utilized OLS regressions. 9 viewing social exclusion as less difficult to implement than those in the low job mobility condition (M = 3.20, SD = 1.43), t(111) = 2.15, p = .03, d = .49. Differences did not emerge for social inclusion (high mobility condition (M = 2.19, SD = 1.06) versus low mobility condition (M = 1.83, SD = .92)), t(111) = 1.06, p = .29, d = .36. Participants also viewed it as more difficult to exclude a dishonest actor (M = 2.88, SD = 1.41) than to include an honest actor (M = 2.03, SD = 1.01), F(1, 111) = 13.86, p < .001, d = .69. The main effect of job mobility did not emerge, F(1, 111) = .42, p = .52, d = .21. The dependent variable model tests whether the increased difficulty of implementing inclusion/exclusion strategies decreased their utilization. The more difficult participants thought it was to include/exclude, the less likely they were to do so, b = .24, SE = .08, t(110) = 2.98, p = .004. As with Experiment 1, we used the bootstrapping method with 5000 samples and constructed bias-corrected CIs. In the dishonest condition, indirect effect was significant [.003, .47] using a 95% CI. The indirect effect combined with the direct effect [ .07, .97], produced a total effect [.06, 1.15] using a 90% CI. This analysis marginally supported H3: individuals in the high job mobility condition viewed exclusion as less difficult to implement than those in the low job mobility condition, which translated into increased social exclusion. In the honest condition, CIs of the indirect [ .28, .02], direct [ .49, .26], and total effects [ .59, .20] overlapped with zero, indicating that difficulty did not mediate the relationship between job mobility and social inclusion. Effectiveness. We tested whether participants in the low job mobility condition utilized less social exclusion because they perceived it as more effective. Participants perceived inclusion (M = 4.19, SD = .95) to be more effective than exclusion (M = 3.15, SD = 1.02), F(1, 111) = 30.75, p < .001, d = 1.06. However, the Job Mobility Behavior interaction, F(1, 111) = .31, p = .58, and the main effect of job mobility, F(1, 111) = .05, p = .83, did not emerge. Due to the absence of these effects, we conclude that effectiveness did not mediate the effects of job mobility on social exclusion, ruling out the alternative hypothesis. Experiment 3 confirmed a causal relationship between job mobility and social exclusion. Participants in the high mobility condition were more likely to exclude dishonest actors than participants in the low mobility condition. Furthermore, the relationship between mobility and exclusion was mediated by the difficulty of exclusion, but not by effectiveness. No effects were found for social inclusion. General discussion Across three experiments, using different cultural samples (Americans, South Koreans), research methods (correlational, experimentally manipulated), and measures of social inclusion and exclusion (scenarios, behavioral), we consistently found that individuals in high perceived job mobility contexts were more likely to socially exclude dishonest actors than those in low perceived job mobility contexts, and that perceived job mobility did not affect levels of social inclusion. Experiment 1 demonstrated clear cultural differences in the utilization of social inclusion/exclusion versus monetary rewards/ punishments. Specifically, when employing monetary incentives, Americans punished a dishonest actor less and rewarded an honest actor more than South Koreans; however, Americans socially excluded a dishonest actor more than South Koreans, whereas no differences by job mobility emerged for the social inclusion of an honest actor. Importantly, perceived job mobility was a contributing underlying mechanism. Experiment 2 provided a behavioral measure of social inclusion and exclusion. It found that as perceived job mobility increased, the likelihood of tossing the ball to Please cite this article in press as: Whitson, J., et al. Responses to normative and norm-violating behavior: Culture, job mobility, and social inclusion and exclusion. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.08.001 10 J. Whitson et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes xxx (2014) xxx–xxx a dishonest actor in the initial round of the game decreased, suggesting increased social exclusion. Experiment 3 examined the proposed theoretical mechanism and found that the perceived difficulty, but not the perceived effectiveness, of socially excluding a dishonest actor mediated the relationship between job mobility and social exclusion. It also determined causality by showing that manipulated perceptions of job mobility increased social exclusion. As in Experiment 1, job mobility did not affect social inclusion decisions in Experiments 2 or 3. In sum, these experiments provide support for an emergent socioecological perspective that sheds insight into how culture influences responses to norm-violating behavior. Critically, this research goes beyond past work that has focused on cultural comparisons of monetary reward and punishment responses. Our findings illustrate a new cultural pattern of results and a boundary condition for previous work. While previous work on reward and punishment behaviors showed that East Asians were more monetarily punitive than Westerners, our results show that Westerners can be just as punitive when utilizing a different form of response. An individual’s choice to punish may depend on the situational affordances of their culture and context. We hope that this research will stimulate investigation into different forms of responses, thereby expanding understanding of culture, mobility, social norms, and responses to normative and norm-violating behaviors. This research offers important practical implications for individuals in organizational contexts. An employee working abroad, or even those who choose to move to a new company, should be aware that differing contextual levels of job mobility may lead to organizational cultures whose responses to norm-violating behavior may differ dramatically from what the employee is used to. For instance, a manager who is hired from a stable (i.e., low mobility) organization with clearly defined sanctioning systems may gain a reputation in a more quickly changing entrepreneurial (i.e., high mobility) organization for being unnecessarily punitive. Instead, members of these entrepreneurial organizations may be used to responding to norm-violating behaviors more informally, for example, by excluding others. In sum, our research implies that managers responding to bad behavior risk violating cultural expectations if they do not take into account the contextual mobility of their organization. Future directions The findings that job mobility or national culture are not associated with social inclusion warrants further discussion. Because participants’ choices to socially include were based largely around decisions to maintain or deepen, rather than form, ties (e.g., in Experiment 1, keeping an existing member of a social group; in Experiment 2, passing a ball to another player; in Experiment 3, social inclusion of a coworker), this may be why no differences emerged. It may be easy for individuals in both high and low job mobility contexts to maintain a relationship with honest actors. Future studies could go beyond inclusion strategies based on ‘‘friendly defaults’’ to develop stronger manipulations or measures to capture social inclusion strategies that require more effort (e.g., tie formation). Our findings in Experiment 2 suggest that social exclusion decisions are contextually sensitive and individuals are likely to adjust their reactions once the environmental mobility changes. For example, whereas participants high in perceived job mobility were less likely to toss the ball to the dishonest player in the initial round of Cyberball, this effect dissipated over the next three rounds. One possibility is that the norm of inclusion accentuated by the Cyberball game resulted in an increase in participants’ inclusionary behavior over time, regardless of their respective levels of job mobility. Another possibility is that multiple rounds with the same three players engendered a perception of low mobility in which participants felt inextricably tied to the other players. A further analysis using a median split to form high and low mobility groups suggests that these explanations may indeed have played a part: participants in both low and high mobility contexts in the final two rounds responded in a manner similar to participants in low mobility contexts in the initial round (i.e., in the first round, only 14% of participants in high mobility contexts tossed the ball to the dishonest actor, as compared to 37% of the participants in low mobility contexts; in the final two rounds, between 30% and 43% of both groups tossed the ball to the dishonest actor). Future research should examine this dynamic to better understand the malleability of perceived mobility, and when the immediate situational context may override more stable measures of socioecological mobility (e.g., the job mobility scale utilized in Experiments 1 and 2). A more fine-grained examination of various types of mobility may also be worthwhile to explore. For example, whereas measures of job and relational mobility are often based on a perceived societal mobility (i.e. how easy it is to change careers or relationships in a given environment; Chen et al., 2009; Schug et al., 2009, 2010); measures of residential mobility are often based on objective statistics (i.e. the number of residential moves people have made; e.g., Oishi, Ishii, & Lun, 2009; Oishi, Lun et al., 2007; Oishi, Rothman et al., 2007; Oishi & Schimmack, 2010; Yuki et al., 2007). Oishi (2014) proposed that in contrast to most psychological research that examines how perceived societal mobility affects cognition, emotion, and behavior, socioecological psychology can also employ environmental data (e.g. sex ratios in a given environment, population density) to investigate how the objective environment influences individual psychology. Future research should compare the relative influences of perceived societal mobility versus objective mobility’s influence on norm-related reactions. Moreover, it would be valuable to explore whether the effects produced by job mobility also result from residential and relational mobility, or whether these different forms of mobility differently influence the same phenomena. These three types of mobility are correlated, but conceptually distinct, so may exhibit notable differences in both their pattern and magnitude of effects. Perceived job mobility, for example, may exhibit a stronger effect on behaviors related to organizational contexts. In Experiment 1, we also measured perceived relational mobility (Yuki et al., 2007; 12 items, from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree; e.g., ‘‘They have many chances to get to know other people’’). A correlation between relational and job mobility emerged, r(211) = .24, p < .001. However, the results suggested relational and job mobility are distinct constructs: while controlling for relational mobility, the reported effects for job mobility still hold. Moreover, whereas the pattern of effects of relational mobility on social inclusion/exclusion mirrored that of job mobility, the effects were not as robust. The interaction between perceived relational mobility and behavior on the amount of social inclusion or exclusion was in the same direction as the job mobility data but not significant, b = .37, SE = .28, t(217) = 1.33, p = .19. This may have occurred because the scenario specified a work-based relationship between the target individuals, thus resulting in a stronger effect for the more contextually pertinent construct of job mobility. It is also worthwhile to explore whether individuals react to social inclusion and exclusion differently depending on the level of job mobility in their environment. Although Experiment 3 found that the level of perceived effectiveness of social inclusion and exclusion was the same in low versus high job mobility environments, our measurement only captured the perception of the Please cite this article in press as: Whitson, J., et al. Responses to normative and norm-violating behavior: Culture, job mobility, and social inclusion and exclusion. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.08.001 J. Whitson et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes xxx (2014) xxx–xxx initiator, but not the recipient, of social inclusion and exclusion. One possibility is that when one individual decides to exclude another, recipients in high mobility contexts will be less affected than those in low mobility contexts; for with their higher mobility, the recipients can simply opt out of the situation, making the exclusion less of a threat and ultimately, less of a punishment. In this case, less threatening punishments may be balanced by the increased willingness of individuals in high mobility contexts to exclude. Thus, the overall impact of the exclusion may in fact be the same across contexts. Future research should try to examine this other side of these interactions. Conclusion More tangible responses to norm-relevant behavior, such as monetary incentives, generally occur via formal institutional or organizational channels. Social inclusion and exclusion, on the other hand, thrive in both formal arenas and in the informal webs of human connection that lie just beneath the surface of any organizational chart. This research explores these latter forms of responses to normative and norm-violating behaviors. Job mobility does not alter responses to norm-relevant behavior via social inclusion, but does via social exclusion, both within and across cultures. This provides greater insight into the relationship between culture, mobility and the utilization of responses to normative and norm-violating behaviors. These clashes of organizational and national culture are not uncommon, and understanding how different cultures choose to respond is an important step towards reducing misinterpretations and helping improve cross-cultural organizational dynamics. References Abrams, D., Hogg, M. A., & Marques, J. (2005). The social psychology of inclusion and exclusion. New York: Psychology Press. Bahns, A. J., Pickett, K. M., & Crandall, C. S. (2012). Social ecology of similarity: Big schools, small schools and social relationships. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 15(1), 119–131. Balliet, D., Mulder, L. B., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2011). Reward, punishment, and cooperation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 137(4), 594–615. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323–370. Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Ciarocco, N. J., & Twenge, J. M. (2005). Social exclusion impairs self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(4), 589–604. Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497–529. Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (1990). Point-counterpoints: Anxiety and social exclusion. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9(2), 165–195. Bialik, C. (2010). Seven careers in a lifetime? Think twice, researchers say. The Wall Street Journal. <http://online.wsj.com/news/articles/SB10001424052748704 206804575468162805877990> . Bian, Y., & Ang, S. (1997). Guanxi networks and job mobility in China and Singapore. Social Forces, 75(3), 981–1005. Borghans, L., & Golsteyn, B. H. (2012). Job mobility in Europe, Japan and the United States. British Journal of Industrial Relations, 50(3), 436–456. Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(1), 3–5. Chen, J., Chiu, C.-Y., & Chan, F. S.-F. (2009). The cultural effects of job mobility and the belief in a fixed world: Evidence from performance forecast. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(5), 851–865. Chen, M. L. (2012). The effect of leader reward and punishment behaviors on subordinates’ budget reports. Engineering Economist, 57(1), 41–54. Chiu, C.-Y., & Chen, J. (2004). Symbols and interactions: Application of the CCC model to culture, language, and social identity. In S.-H. Ng, C. Candlin, & C.-Y. Chiu (Eds.), Language matters: Communication, culture, and social identity (pp. 155–182). Hong Kong: City University of Hong Kong Press. Denissen, J. J., Penke, L., Schmitt, D. P., & van Aken, M. A. (2008). Self-esteem reactions to social interactions: Evidence for sociometer mechanisms across days, people, and nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(1), 181–196. Dess, G. G., & Shaw, J. D. (2001). Voluntary turnover, social capital, and organizational performance. Academy of Management Review, 26(3), 446–456. 11 Edwards, J. R., & Lambert, L. S. (2007). Methods for integrating moderation and mediation: A general analytical framework using moderated path analysis. Psychological Methods, 12(1), 1–22. Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302(5643), 290–292. Falk, C. F., Heine, S. J., Yuki, M., & Takemura, K. (2009). Why do Westerners selfenhance more than East Asians? European Journal of Personality, 23(3), 183–203. Feinberg, M., Willer, R., & Schultz, M. (2014). Gossip and ostracism promote cooperation in groups. Psychological Science, 25(3), 656–664. Ferris, D. L., Brown, D. J., Berry, J., & Lian, H. (2008). The development and validation of the Workplace Ostracism Scale. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(6), 1348–1366. Fuster, A., & Meier, S. (2010). Another hidden cost of incentives: The detrimental effect on norm enforcement. Management Science, 56(1), 57–70. Gneezy, U. (2005). Deception: The role of consequences. The American Economic Review, 95(1), 384–394. Gray, K., Ward, A. F., & Norton, M. I. (2012). Paying it forward: Generalized reciprocity and the limits of generosity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 143(1), 247–254. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0031047. Hashimoto, H., & Yamagishi, T. (2013). Two faces of interdependence: Harmony seeking and rejection avoidance. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 16(2), 142–151. Hays, R. B. (1985). A longitudinal study of friendship development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(4), 909–924. Heine, S. J., & Renshaw, K. (2002). Interjudge agreement, self-enhancement, and liking: Cross-cultural divergences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(5), 578–587. Heppner, W. L., Kernis, M. H., Nezlek, J. B., Foster, J., Lakey, C. E., & Goldman, B. M. (2008). Within-person relationships among daily self-esteem, need satisfaction, and authenticity. Psychological Science, 19(11), 1140–1145. Hinkin, T. R. (1995). A review of scale development practices in the study of organizations. Journal of Management, 21(5), 967–988. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Holt. Kazdin, A. E. (2001). Behavior modification in applied settings. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Kim, J.-O., & Mueller, C. W. (1978). Factor analysis: Statistical methods and practical issues (Vol. 14). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Kim, K., & Markman, A. B. (2006). Differences in fear of isolation as an explanation of cultural differences: Evidence from memory and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(3), 350–364. Korean Employment Information Service (2009). <http://eng.keis.or.kr/eng/ network/employ_info.jsp>. Leary, M. R. (1990). Responses to social exclusion: Social anxiety, jealousy, loneliness, depression, and low self-esteem. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9(2), 221–229. Levine, J. M., Moreland, R. L., & Hausmann, L. R. M. (2005). The social psychology of inclusion and exclusion. In D. Abrams, M. A. Hogg, & J. M. Marques (Eds.), Managing group composition: Inclusive and exclusive role transitions (pp. 137–160). New York: Psychology Press. Lewis, K., Belliveau, M., Herndon, B., & Keller, J. (2007). Group cognition, membership change, and performance: Investigating the benefits and detriments of collective knowledge. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 103(2), 159–178. Lun, J., Oishi, S., & Tenney, E. R. (2012). Residential mobility moderates preferences for egalitarian versus loyal helpers. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(1), 291–297. MacDonald, G., & Leary, M. R. (2005). Why does social exclusion hurt? The relationship between social and physical pain. Psychological Bulletin, 131(2), 202–223. Maslow, A. H. (1968). Toward a psychology of being (2nd ed.). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand. Meriküll, J. (2011). Labour market mobility during a recession: The case of Estonia. Working Papers of Eesti Pank, No 1/2011. Moscarini, G., & Postel-Vinay, F. (2013, May). Did the job ladder fail after the great recession? Paper Presented at NBER Project on the Labor Market in the Aftermath of the Great Recession, Cambridge, MA. Oishi, S. (2010). The psychology of residential mobility implications for the self, social relationships, and well-being. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(1), 5–21. Oishi, S. (2014). Socioecological psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 581–609. Oishi, S., & Graham, J. (2010). Social ecology lost and found in psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(4), 356–377. Oishi, S., Ishii, K., & Lun, J. (2009). Residential mobility and conditionality of group identification. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(4), 913–919. Oishi, S., & Kesebir, S. (2012). Optimal social-networking strategy is a function of socioeconomic conditions. Psychological Science, 23(12), 1542–1548. Oishi, S., Kesebir, S., Miao, F. F., Talhelm, T., Endo, Y., Uchida, Y., et al. (2013). Residential mobility increases motivation to expand social network: But why? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(2), 217–223. Oishi, S., Lun, J., & Sherman, G. D. (2007). Residential mobility, self-concept, and positive affect in social interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(1), 131–141. Oishi, S., Rothman, A. J., Snyder, M., Su, J., Zehm, K., Hertel, A. W., et al. (2007b). The socioecological model of procommunity action: The benefits of residential stability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 831–844. Oishi, S., & Schimmack, U. (2010). Residential mobility, well-being, and mortality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(6), 980–994. Please cite this article in press as: Whitson, J., et al. Responses to normative and norm-violating behavior: Culture, job mobility, and social inclusion and exclusion. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.08.001 12 J. Whitson et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes xxx (2014) xxx–xxx Ouwerkerk, J. W., Kerr, N. L., Gallucci, M., & Van Lange, P. A. (2005). Avoiding the social death penalty: Ostracism and cooperation in social dilemmas. In K. D. Williams, J. P. Forgas, & W. von Hippel (Eds.), The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying (pp. 321–332). New York: Psychology Press. Peplau, L. A., & Perlman, D. (1982). Perspectives on loneliness. In L. A. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness: A sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy (pp. 1–20). New York: Wiley. Podsakoff, P. M., Bommer, W. H., Podsakoff, N. P., & MacKenzie, S. B. (2006). Relationships between leader reward and punishment behavior and subordinate attitudes, perceptions, and behaviors: A meta-analytic review of existing and new research. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 99(2), 113–142. Robinson, S. L., O’Reilly, J., & Wang, W. (2013). Invisible at work: An integrated model of workplace ostracism. Journal of Management, 39(1), 203–231. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1177/0149206312466141. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 68. Schug, J., Yuki, M., Horikawa, H., & Takemura, K. (2009). Similarity attraction and actually selecting similar others: How cross-societal differences in relational mobility affect interpersonal similarity in Japan and the USA. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 12(2), 95–103. Schug, J., Yuki, M., & Maddux, W. (2010). Relational mobility explains between-and within-culture differences in self-disclosure to close friends. Psychological Science, 21(10), 1471–1478. Stryker, S. (2007). Identity theory and personality theory: Mutual relevance. Journal of Personality, 75(6), 1083–1102. Tay, L., & Diener, E. (2011). Needs and subjective well-being around the world. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(2), 354–365. Topel, R. H., & Ward, M. P. (1992). Job mobility and the careers of young men. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 107(2), 439–479. Twenge, J. M., Catanese, K. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Social exclusion causes selfdefeating behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(3), 606–615. Wang, C. S., Galinsky, A. D., & Murnighan, J. K. (2009). Bad drives psychological reactions, but good propels behavior: Responses to honesty and deception. Psychological Science, 20(5), 634–644. Wang, C. S., & Leung, A. K.-Y. (2010). The cultural dynamics of rewarding honesty and punishing deception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(11), 1529–1542. Wang, C. S., Leung, A. K.-Y., See, Y. H. M., & Gao, X. Y. (2011). The effects of culture and friendship on rewarding honesty and punishing deception. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(6), 1295–1299. Wayne, S. J., Shore, L. M., Bommer, W. H., & Tetrick, L. E. (2002). The role of fair treatment and rewards in perceptions of organizational support and leadermember exchange. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(3), 590–598. Williams, K. D. (2009). Ostracism: A temporal need-threat model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 275–314. Williams, K. D., Shore, W. J., & Grahe, J. E. (1998). The silent treatment: Perceptions of its behaviors and associated feelings. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 1(2), 117–141. Williams, K. D., & Sommer, K. L. (1997). Social ostracism by coworkers: Does rejection lead to loafing or compensation? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(7), 693–706. Williams, K. D., Yeager, D. S., Cheung, C. K. T., & Choi, W. (2012). Cyberball (version 4.0) [Software]. <https://cyberball.wikispaces.com>. Wiseman, J. P. (1986). Friendship: Bonds and binds in a voluntary relationship. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 3(2), 191–211. Yuki, M., & Schug, J. (2012). Relational mobility: A socioecological approach to personal relationships. In O. Gillath, G. E. Adams, & A. D. Kunkel (Eds.), New directions in close relationships: Integrating across disciplines and theoretical approaches. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Yuki, M., Schug, J., Horikawa, H., Takemura, K., Sato, K., Yokota, K., et al. (2007). Development of a scale to measure perceptions of relational mobility in society. (CERSS Working Paper No. 75), Center for Experimental Research in Social Sciences, Hokkaido University. Please cite this article in press as: Whitson, J., et al. Responses to normative and norm-violating behavior: Culture, job mobility, and social inclusion and exclusion. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.08.001