Neurophysiologie clinique 34 (2004) 33–39 www.elsevier.com/locate/neucli ORIGINAL ARTICLE Fear-conditioned cues of impending pain facilitate attentional engagement Stefaan Van Damme a,b,*, Jürgen Lorenz c, Christopher Eccleston d, Ernst H.W. Koster a, Armand De Clercq e, Geert Crombez a,b a Department of Psychology, Ghent University, Henri Dunantlaan 2, B-9000 Ghent, Belgium Research Institute for Psychology and Health, P.O. Box 80.140, 3508 TC Utrecht, The Netherlands c University of Hamburg, Institute of Neurophysiology and Pathophysiology, Martinistrabe 52, 20246 Germany d Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Claverton Down, Bath BA2 7AY, UK e Department of Applied Mathematics and Computer Science, Ghent University, Krijgslaan 281 (S9), B-9000 Ghent, Belgium b Received 7 August 2003; revised and accepted 17 November 2003 KEYWORDS Selective attention; Experimental pain; Classical conditioning MOTS CLÉS Attention sélective ; Douleur expérimental ; Conditionnement classique Abstract Aims of study. – Selective attention to signals of impending pain allows the avoidance of bodily harm. In order to identify the attentional components involved in the selection of pain signals over competing demands, we used an emotional modification of an exogenous cueing task. Methods. – Fifty-two pain-free volunteers detected visual targets of which the location was correctly or incorrectly predicted by a spatial cue. Cues were emotionally modulated using differential classical conditioning. The conditioned cue (CS+) was sometimes followed by an electrocutaneous stimulus (UCS), thus becoming a pain signal, whereas the UCS never followed the other cue (CS–), referred to as safety signal. Results. – Analyses of response times showed that pain signals facilitated the directing of attention to their location in comparison to safety signals. In contrast, pain signals did not impair disengagement of attention from their location in comparison to safety signals. Conclusion. – It is concluded that attention is more strongly engaged to a signal of impending pain compared with a cue signalling its absence. We explore why disengagement from the pain signal is not impaired compared to the safety signal. The findings are discussed in terms of the defensive importance of pain anticipation. © 2003 Elsevier SAS. All rights reserved. Résumé Objectifs. – L’attention sélective dirigée vers un signal de la douleur imminente permet d’éviter les dommages physiques. Afin d’identifier les composantes attentionnelles impliquées dans la sélection des signaux de la douleur, nous avons utilisé une version émotionnelle de la tâche d’indiçage. Méthodes. – Cinquante-deux sujets volontaires sains avaient pour tâche de détecter des cibles visuelles, dont la localisation spatiale était annoncée — de façon correcte ou incorrecte — par un indice spatial. La valence émotionnelle de l’indice était induite à * Corresponding author. Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Ghent University, Henri Dunantlaan 2, 9000 Ghent, Belgium. Tel.: +32-9-2649105; fax: +32-9-2649149. E-mail address: Stefaan.VanDamme@UGent.be (S. Van Damme). © 2003 Elsevier SAS. All rights reserved. doi: 10.1016/S0987-7053(03)00102-3 34 S. Van Damme et al. l’aide d’une procédure de conditionnement classique. Lors de certains essais, l’indice conditionné (CS+) était suivi d’un stimulus électrique nociceptif (UCS), devenant alors un signal de douleur. En revanche, aucun UCS ne suivait l’indice non conditionné (CS–), qui devenait alors un signal de sécurité. Résultats. – L’analyse des temps de réaction a montré que le signal de douleur facilitait la direction de l’attention vers sa localisation spatiale, en comparaison au signal de sécurité. En revanche, le signal de douleur ne détériorait pas le désengagement de sa localisation spatiale, par rapport au signal de sécurité. Conclusion. – Nous concluons que l’attention est attirée plus fortement par un signal de douleur imminente en comparaison à un signal annonçant l’absence de douleur. Nous explorons pourquoi le désengagement du signal de douleur n’est pas détérioré en comparaison au signal de sécurité. Les résultats sont discutés en termes de caractère défensif de l’anticipation de la douleur. © 2003 Elsevier SAS. All rights reserved. Introduction The question of how pain attracts attention over competing demands has become a substantial research topic over the last decade. There is already a significant amount of research documenting the effects of pain on attention. The idea that pain demands attention, interrupts ongoing activities, and interferes with present demands has been supported by numerous studies in both clinical and non-clinical populations. These studies feature a variety of research methods, such as behavioural tasks [2,5,12,29,33], psychophysiological measures [9,10,19,20], and functional brain imaging [1,8,24]. Eccleston and Crombez [13] developed a cognitive-affective model in which they argued that the interruption of attention by pain is a normal and evolutionary adaptive process. According to these authors, pain has a processing priority by activating a primitive defensive system that urges escape from somatic threat. Although pain can occur without warning, the learning of signals of impending pain significantly contributes to this protective function. For example, when we hear the humming of a wasp while eating a fruit, our attention will be immediately drawn to the threat of a painful sting, and we will attempt to escape from this situation. The idea that pain anticipation subserves an important protective function has been supported by neurophysiological research in both clinical and non-clinical populations. For instance, in a number of functional brain imaging studies, changes in activity of cortical nociceptive networks were measured during anticipation of pain and during actual painful stimulation. It was found that cortical networks involved in the processing of pain itself were already activated during anticipation of pain, suggesting that these locations are directly affected by cognitive factors such as attention [25,26]. However, little is known about the mechanisms by which pain signals capture attention, facilitating the detection of pain itself. Dawson et al. [6] investigated the allocation of attention to signals of impending pain. They asked participants to concentrate on the differential conditioning of visual stimuli. The conditioned stimulus (CS+) was frequently followed by an electrical shock, thus becoming a pain signal. The other stimulus (CS–) was never followed by an electrical shock. Furthermore, participants were asked to perform an auditory reaction time task as a secondary task. The authors found that reaction times to auditory stimuli were significantly slower during CS+ presentation than during CS– presentation, suggesting an increased allocation of cognitive resources to pain signals [6]. Because this study measured general attentional deployment through the interference of task performance by pain signals, a more detailed understanding of the attentional sub-components involved was not possible. In general, three components of attention to all forms of threat can be distinguished [14,15,23,33]: (1) an initial transient shift of attention to a threatening stimulus, (2) a sustained engagement with the threatening stimulus, and (3) disengagement from the threatening stimulus. In order to differentiate these attentional components during the processing of pain signals, we used an emotional modification of an exogenous cueing paradigm, originally developed by Posner et al. [27]. In an exogenous cueing paradigm, participants are asked to detect a visual target presented at the left or right side of a fixation cross. Targets are preceded by a visual cue at the same spatial location (validly cued trials) or at the opposite location (invalidly cued trials). Presenting a spatial cue facilitates or inhibits target detection in comparison with a condition in which no spatial cues are presented: the presentation of a valid cue typically leads to response time benefits (due to attentional engage- Fear-conditioned cues of impending pain facilitate attentional engagement ment at the correctly cued location), whereas the presentation of an invalid cue leads to response time costs (due to attentional disengagement from the incorrectly cued location), described as the cue validity effect [27]. In the present study, spatial cues are emotionally modulated by a differential classical conditioning procedure. A nociceptive stimulus (UCS) sometimes follows the conditioned cue (CS+), rendering it a pain signal. In contrast, the UCS never follows the other cue (CS–), which we refer to as the “safety signal” according to Seligman’s hypothesis [28]. Using this paradigm we hypothesize that two components of attention to signals of impending pain will be differentiated: First, we expect facilitated attentional engagement to pain signals, indicated by stronger response time benefits when the CS+ in comparison with the CS– is used as a valid spatial cue. Second, difficulty disengaging from pain signals should be indicated by stronger response time costs when the CS+ in comparison with the CS– is used as an invalid spatial cue. Until now, most studies investigated the effect of pain on attention. The present study is one of the first to investigate the effect of pain anticipation on spatial attention. Furthermore, the present study differs from previous work in providing a detailed examination of the underlying attentional components. Method Participants Fifty-two undergraduate psychology students (eight males and 44 females; mean age = 19.04 years) participated to fulfil course requirements. All participants gave their informed consent and were free to terminate the experiment at any time. Each person had normal or corrected-to-normal eyesight. Experimental duration was approximately 30 min. Apparatus and test material The exogenous cueing task was programmed and presented by the INQUISIT Millisecond software package (Inquisit 1.32, 2001) on a S710 Compaq Deskpro computer with a 72 Hz, 17-inch color monitor. INQUISIT measures response times with a millisecond accuracy [7]. Target stimuli consisted of black squares (1.1 by 1.1 cm), presented on a white background. Two colour slides (green and pink; 4.8 cm high × 6.5 cm wide) were used as spatial cues for the location of 35 the targets. Each trial began with a fixation cross in the middle of the screen (duration of 1000 ms). Cues were presented 9.2° from the fixation cross for a duration of 200 ms. Target onset followed immediately after cue offset. On half of the test trials, cue location correctly predicted target location (validly cued trials). On the other half of the test trials, cue location incorrectly predicted target location (invalidly cued trials). Participants were seated 60 cm from the computer screen. They were instructed to respond to the left targets by pressing the ‘q’ key with the left index finger and to the right targets by pressing the ‘5’ key with the right index finger on a standard AZERTY computer keyboard. A trial ended when a participant responded or 2000 ms had elapsed. In order to control for responses to cues instead of targets, a number of trials were presented, in which the cue was not followed by a target (catch trials). Furthermore, in order to ensure that participants maintained gaze at the middle of the screen, a number of control trials were presented. In these trials, the fixation cross was followed only by a randomly selected digit between 1 and 9 for a duration of 50 ms (digit trials). Participants were instructed to report the digit aloud. If participants were not able to report the digits (correctly), this indicated that they did not maintain gaze at the fixation cross. Cues were emotionally modulated by a differential classical conditioning procedure. The conditioned cue (CS+) was on one third of the presentations followed by a nociceptive stimulus (UCS). The other cue (CS–) was never followed by an UCS. The UCS was a transcutaneous electrocutaneous stimulus, delivered by a Digitimer constant current stimulator (Digitimer DS7A, 1998). Intensity of the electrocutaneous stimulus was selected individually by each participant (see below). Electrocutaneous stimuli had an instantaneous rise and fall time, and a duration of 750 ms. The stimuli were delivered by two lubrificated Fukuda standard Ag/AgCl electrodes (1 cm diameter) attached to the external side of the right ankle. The skin at the electrode sites was first abraded with a peeling cream (Nihon Kohden) to reduce skin resistance. Which colour was CS+ or CS– was counterbalanced across participants. The CS+ and CS– were presented equally often, in a fixed random order with no more than three consecutive presentations of one cue. Procedure Participants were tested individually in a soundattenuated room designed for psychophysiological experiments. 36 S. Van Damme et al. Figure 1 Schematic illustration of the four trial types. On each trial, a fixation cross was presented for 1000 ms. This was immediately followed by the presentation of a cue (200 ms duration). Immediately after cue offset the target was introduced. Targets were presented at the same location of cues (validly cued trials), or at the opposite location of cues (invalidly cued trials). The CS+ was followed by the UCS in one third of the trials. Preparation phase Participants were informed that an electrocutaneous stimulus would be used during the experiment. They were further informed that this stimulus “stimulates the pain fibres and that most people find this kind of stimulation unpleasant”. After this, participants gave informed consent. To familiarise the participants with the electrocutaneous stimuli, they were given a series of stimuli with increasing intensity. They were asked to select a stimulus which they found aversive but tolerable, and which required a certain effort to deal with. The electrocutaneous stimulus chosen by the participant was set as the intensity used during the experiment. Practice phase Participants were seated in front of a computer to perform the exogenous cueing task. All instructions were presented on the computer screen. Participants were instructed to respond to the targets as quickly as possible, without sacrificing accuracy. Participants were informed about cues and targets, and they were asked to respond only to the location of the targets by pressing on the corresponding key. Participants were told that cues would predict target location on some of the trials. Furthermore, they were informed that there would be trials on which only a cue but no target would appear and they were asked not to respond on these trials. They were told that there would also be trials in which only a digit would appear which they had to report aloud. The practice phase consisted of 10 trials: four validly cued trials, four invalidly cued trials, one catch trial, and one digit trial. Participants were made aware of the fact that no electrocutaneous stimuli would be presented during this phase. Experiment phase Participants were informed that one type (colour) of cue would sometimes be followed by an electrocutaneous stimulus, and that the other type of cue would never be followed by an electrocutaneous stimulus. Participants did not know in advance which cue would be followed by the electrocutaneous stimulus. The experiment phase consisted of 165 trials: 72 validly cued trials, 72 invalidly cued trials, 12 catch trials, and nine digit trials. One third of the CS+ presentations was followed by an electrocutaneous stimulus (UCS). These trials were not analysed in order to make sure that the effect of the CS+ was not confounded by the effect of the UCS. The experiment phase started with two buffer trials in which the CS+ was followed by an UCS in order to facilitate the differential conditioning. Fig. 1 shows a schematic illustration of the four trial types used in the study. Post-experiment phase As a method check, participants rated to what extent they expected an electrocutaneous stimulus following the CS+ and the CS– on an 11-point nu- Fear-conditioned cues of impending pain facilitate attentional engagement merical graphic rating scale (anchored zero = not at all to 10 = very strongly). On a similar scale, participants also rated how aversive they found the electrocutaneous stimulus. Statistical analyses A 2 (cue validity: valid, invalid) × 2 (signal: CS+, CS–) ANOVA with repeated measures was performed upon the mean response times (RT). Responses faster than 150 ms were considered as responses to cues instead of targets, and were removed from the RT analyses. Greenhouse-Geisser corrections (with corrected degrees of freedom) are presented where the sphericity assumption is violated (Mauchly’s test of sphericity; P < 0.05). Results Participants rated the electrocutaneous stimulus as aversive (M = 6.38, S.D. = 1.51). The differential conditioning procedure was successful. Participants expected the electrocutaneous stimulus significantly more after the presentation of a CS+ (M = 6.94, S.D. = 2.22) than after the presentation of a CS– (M = 0.71, S.D. = 1.64), t(51) = 14.72, P < 0.001. The mean percentage of error rates was rather small for responses to validly cued CS+ trials (0.62%), validly cued CS– trials (0.44%), invalidly cued CS+ trials (2.96%), and invalidly cued CS– trials (1.18%). Participants responded (erroneously) to 3.37% of the catch trials. They complied with gaze fixation, consistently reporting the digits. All error trials (1.31%) and outliers (RTs less than 150 ms or greater than 750 ms; 1.03%) were removed from the RT analyses. Also the trials in which the CS+ was followed by an electrocutaneous stimulus (UCS) were removed from the analyses, in order to prevent that the effect of the UCS would confound the effect of the CS+. The mean RT was then calculated for each participant as a function of each factor in the design. A 2 (cue validity: valid, invalid) × 2 (signal: CS+, CS–) ANOVA with repeated measures showed a significant main effect of cue validity, F(1.51) = 30.59, MSE = 548.24, P < 0.001, indicating that responses to validly cued targets were faster than responses to invalidly cued trials (331 vs. 349 ms). The main effect of signal was not significant, F(1.51) = 2.34, MSE = 238.13 ns. Of particular importance for this study was the significant interaction effect of cue validity × signal, F(1.51) = 5.93, MSE = 214.93, P < 0.05. This interaction was further analysed in terms of engagement and disengagement effects. 37 The engagement component relates to response times on validly cued trials. In particular, we tested whether the presentation of the CS+ as valid cue facilitated target detection compared to the presentation of the CS– as a valid cue. The disengagement component covers response times on invalidly cued trials. We tested whether the presentation of the CS+ as an invalid cue inhibited target detection compared to the presentation of the CS– as an invalid cue. All tests were done using t-tests for dependent samples. We found that the presentation of pain signals as spatial cues affected the engagement component but not the disengagement component. Responses were significantly faster on trials with the CS+ as a valid cue (M = 327 ms, S.D. = 41 ms) compared to trials with the CS– as a valid cue (M = 336 ms, SD = 46 ms), t(51) = 2.52, P < 0.05. However, response times did not differ on trials with the CS+ as an invalid cue (M = 350 ms, S.D. = 47 ms) compared to trials with the CS– as an invalid cue (M = 349 ms, S.D. = 49 ms), t(51) = 0.65 ns. Discussion The main objective of this study was to investigate the mechanisms by which pain signals capture attention. We characterized pain signals as stimuli that signal the occurrence of pain, whereas we considered stimuli signalling the absence of pain as safety signals. We used both of these stimuli in an emotional modification of the exogenous cueing paradigm and hypothesized that two attentional components could be identified: (1) Pain signals may facilitate attentional engagement by accelerating covert orienting to the target location when compared with safety signals (engagement component). (2) Pain signals may inhibit attentional disengagement by slowing covert orienting to targets in the opposite location when compared with safety signals (disengagement component). First, we replicated the cue validity effect: Presenting a valid spatial cue produced covert orienting of attention to the cue location and accelerated target detection [27]. Notably, we obtained a robust cue validity effect although we used an equal number of correctly and incorrectly cued trials. In original experiments, valid and invalid cues had occurrence probabilities of 80% and 20% of the trials, respectively [27]. Second, and of most importance to this study, we observed a differential modulation of attentional engagement and disengagement by the signal value of the cues: the pain signal induced stronger attentional engagement relative to the safety signal, whereas both pain and 38 safety signals were equally difficult to disengage from. The differential effect of pain signals on engagement and disengagement is intriguing and needs further consideration. First, it appears reasonable that the attentional engagement component in the exogenous cueing task benefits from threatening cues due to their greater perceptual salience and induction of more arousal than neutral cues. However, a limitation of the present study was that no physiological measures were included, which could have clarified the role of arousal. Second, strong engagement might generally be facilitated by simple warning signals that allow rapid processing. Consistent with this view, LeDoux [18] argued that the simplicity of conditioned stimuli contributes to the efficiency of recruitment of the brain network underlying fear-conditioning. Thus, it appears that the phasic engagement process benefits from the simple and alerting character of the pain signal. The biological value of this function may be seen in the heightened spatial awareness of a potential threat that needs to be rapidly defended. In contrast, disengagement involves a release from an ongoing tonic process. The pain signal, due to its short-lived duration and due to the brevity and low cognitive demand of the electrocutaneous stimulus that it signals, may have failed to sufficiently intensify this tonic process to render disengagement impaired. This would explain why attentional disengagement is affected by more complex cognitive cues such as pictures or words, as has been demonstrated in a number of recent studies using a crossmodal cueing paradigm [33,34]. Further support for a dissociation of attentional engagement and disengagement is provided by neuroimaging studies. A cortical network consisting of frontal, parietal, and cingulate regions has been described to play a key role in identifying and shifting attention to salient features of the sensory environment [11]. By rendering the cue a pain signal, this network might have been more strongly recruited, facilitating target detection within the cued location. In contrast, holding of attention over prolonged periods of time has been suggested to rely on sustained activity in the basal ganglia and prefrontal cortex [3,4]. These regions have been described to be active during sustained painful stimulation in humans [21]. More particularly, the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex has been suggested to be specifically involved in the disengagement from affective pain processing [22]. Our findings are partly in contrast with previous studies using an emotional modification of the exogenous cueing paradigm. One reason for discrepancy may be that most studies used threatening S. Van Damme et al. words [15,32] or pictures [15,16,35] as cues, which involve more complex cognitive and emotional processing, influencing disengagement (see above). Another reason may be that none of these studies used paradigms with pain stimuli or conditioning. More similar to our study are exogenous cueing paradigms that have used a fear-conditioning procedure with aversive noise [30,31]. However, in these studies, the exogenous cueing task was not performed during acquisition as in our study, but only during extinction. The apparent discontinuity between acquisition and extinction might have accounted for the contrasting results in these studies [17]. It can be concluded that attention is more strongly engaged to a signal of impending than to a cue signalling the absence of pain. From an evolutionary perspective, one can argue that this process may subserve the function of locating the potential risk and adopting protective behaviours before an injury occurs. However, it may become maladaptive in situations of chronic pain where there is no behavioural escape from pain. In the general context of attention to the threat of pain and the initiation of avoidance behaviour, it is therefore necessary to disentangle the diverse components of attentional shift, engagement and disengagement [14,15,23,33]. Our study is one of the first that allows such detailed investigation of the underlying processes involved in spatial attention to pain signals. One limitation of the present study is that we did not use control trials with non-nociceptive stimuli. Further studies may use a non-painful somatosensory stimulus of similar salience [34] in order to investigate whether our effects are unique to pain. In future research, it may also be useful to create experimental paradigms that allow a direct investigation of the link between spatial attention to pain signals and avoidance behaviour. Finally, our modulation of an attentional cueing task by a fear-conditioning procedure may be combined in future experiments with neurophysiological measures (EEG or fMRI) to foster our understanding of the differential anatomical representation of attentional engagement to and disengagement from pain. Acknowledgements This study was supported by a research grant (G.0107.00) of the Fund for Scientific Research, Flanders (Belgium) to Geert Crombez. The authors wish to thank Jan De Houwer and Bruno Verschuere for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript, and Valéry Legrain for helping with the French summary. Fear-conditioned cues of impending pain facilitate attentional engagement References [1] [2] [3] [4] [5] [6] [7] [8] [9] [10] [11] [12] [13] [14] [15] [16] [17] Aziz Q, Schnitzler A, Enck P. Functional neuroimaging of visceral sensation. J Clin Neurophysiol 2000;17:604–12. Bushnell MC, Duncan GH, Dubner R, Jones RL, Maixner W. Attentional influences on noxious and innocuous cutaneous heat detection in humans and monkeys. J Neurosci 1985; 5:1103–10. Corbetta M, Miezin FM, Dobmeyer S, Shulman GL, Petersen SE. Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed. J Neurosci 1991;11:2383–402. Coull JT, Frith CD, Buchel C, Nobre AC. Orienting attention in time: behavioural and neuroanatomical distinction between exogenous and endogenous shifts. Neuropsychologia 2000;36:808–19. Crombez G, Baeyens F, Eelen P. Sensory and temporal information about impending pain: the influence of predictability on pain. Behav Res Ther 1994;32:611–22. Dawson ME, Schell AM, Beers JR, Kelly A. Allocation of cognitive processing capacity during human autonomic classical conditioning. J Exp Psychol 1982;111:273–94. De Clercq A, Crombez G, Roeyers H, Buysse A. A simple and sensitive method to measure timing accuracy. Behav Res Meth Ins C 2003;35:109–15. Derbyshire SWG, Vogt BA, Jones AKP. Pain and stroop interference tasks activate separate processing modules in anterior cingulate cortex. Exp Brain Res 1998;118:52–60. Dowman R. Attentional set effects on spinal and supraspinal responses to pain. Psychophysiol 2001;38:451–64. Dowman R, Shell S. The pain-related negative difference potential: a direct measure of central pathway activity or of interactions between the innocuous somatosensory and pain pathways. Neurophysiol Clin 1999;29:423–42. Downar J, Crawley AP, Mikulis DJ, Davis KD. The effect of task relevance on the cortical response to changes in visual and auditory stimuli: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 2001;14:1256–67. Eccleston C. Chronic pain and attention: a cognitive approach. Br J Clin Psychol 1994;33:535–47. Eccleston C, Crombez G. Pain demands attention: a cognitive-affective model on the interruptive function of pain. Psychol Bull 1999;125:356–66. Eysenck MW. Anxiety: the cognitive perspective. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1992 195 p. Fox E, Russo R, Bowles R, Dutton K. Do threatening stimuli draw or hold attention in subclinical anxiety? J Exp Psychol 2001;130:681–700. Fox E, Russo R, Dutton K. Attentional bias for threat: evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces. Cogn Emot 2002;16:355–79. Hugdahl K. Cortical control of human classical conditioning: automatic and positron emission tomography data. Psychophysiology 1998;35:170–8. 39 [18] LeDoux J. Emotion: clues from the brain. Annu Rev Psychol 1995;46:209–35. [19] Legrain V, Guérit J, Bruyer R, Plaghki L. Attentional modulation of the nociceptive processing into the human brain: selective spatial attention, probability of stimulus occurrence, and target detection effects on laser evoked potentials. Pain 2002;99:21–39. [20] Lorenz J, Bromm B. Event-related potential correlates of interference between cognitive performance and tonic experimental pain. Psychophysiology 1997;34:436–45. [21] Lorenz J, Cross DJ, Minoshima S, Morrow TJ, Paulson PE, Casey KL. A unique representation of heat allodynia in the human brain. Neuron 2002;35:393–393. [22] Lorenz J, Minoshima S, Casey KL. Keeping pain out of mind: the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in pain modulation. Brain 2003;126:1079–91. [23] Mogg K, Bradley BP. A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behav Res Ther 1998;36:809–48. [24] Peyron R, Laurent B, Garcia-Larrea L. Functional imaging of brain responses to pain. A review and meta-analysis. Neurophysiol Clin 2000;30:263–88. [25] Ploghaus A, Tracey I, Gati JS, Clare S, Menon RS, Matthews PM, et al. Dissociating pain from its anticipation in the human brain. Science 1999;284:1979–81. [26] Porro CA, Baraldi P, Pagnoni G, Serafini M, Facchin P, Maieron M, et al. Does anticipation of pain affect cortical nociceptive systems? J Neurosci 2002;22:3206–14. [27] Posner MI, Inhoff A, Friedrich FJ, Cohen A. Isolating attentional systems: a cognitive-anatomical analysis. Psychobiology 1987;15:107–21. [28] Seligman MEP. Chronic fear produced by unpredictable shock. J Compar Physiol Psychol 1968;66:402–11. [29] Spence C, Bentley DE, Phillips N, McGlone FP, Jones AKP. Selective attention to pain: a psychophysical investigation. Exp Brain Res 2002;145:395–402. [30] Stormark KM, Hughdahl K. Peripheral cueing of covert spatial attention before and after emotional conditioning of the cue. Int J Neurosci 1996;86:225–40. [31] Stormark KM, Hughdahl K, Posner MI. Emotional modulation of attention orienting: a classical conditioning study. Scand J Psychol 1999;40:91–9. [32] Stormark KM, Nordby H, Hughdahl K. Attentional shifts to emotionally charged cues: behavioural and ERP data. Cogn Emot 1995;9:507–23. [33] Van Damme S, Crombez G, Eccleston C. Retarded disengagement from pain cues: the effects of pain catastrophizing and pain expectancy. Pain 2002;100:111–8. [34] Van Damme S, Crombez G, Eccleston C, Goubert L. Impaired disengagement from threatening cues of impending pain in a crossmodal cueing paradigm. Eur J Pain [in press]. [35] Yiend J, Mathews A. Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures. Q J Exp Psychol A 2001;54:665–81.