Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives in Marketing Practices1 Stephen M. Colarelli Joseph R. Dettmann Central Michigan University RUNNING HEAD: Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 1 Portions of this paper are drawn from Colarelli (2003) and Colarelli, Alampay, and Canali (2002a). 1 Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 2 Abstract Firms spend considerable sums of money on marketing, and they continue to do so because marketing works. However, marketing can only work if marketers have a reasonably accurate view of human nature. We argue that many consumer products and advertisements reflect an implicit view of human nature that is compatible with the tenets of evolutionary psychology. Implicit theories of human nature that are out of synch with reality sell few products. We provide an overview of the evolutionary perspective and the examine connections between marketing practices and evolved adaptations, including kin selection, prestige seeking, preferences for salt, sweets, and fat, and savanna-like landscapes. We also examine adaptations that differ by gender and how they are mirrored in marketing. Finally, we discuss how evolutionary ideas about variation are reflected in marketing practices. Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 3 Although the relationship between academic psychology and marketing has a long and mutually beneficial history (DiClemente & Hantula, 2000), the practice of marketing – particularly advertising and product development – suggests a curious paradox. For most of the 20th century an evolutionary perspective was almost absent in academic psychology. Yet during the same period, many consumer products and appeals used in advertising reflected an implicit view of human nature s similar to that articulated by evolutionary psychology, which emerged only recently. The purpose of this article is to draw attention to connections between an evolutionary perspective and marketing practices. We hope that this will pique scholars’ and professionals’ interest in evolutionary perspectives, encourage more formal applications of an evolutionary perspective in marketing, and generate a greater interplay between marketing and an evolutionary-oriented psychology. Finally, we hope that drawing attention to these connections may stimulate evolutionary psychologists to look more closely at marketing practices as a valuable source of data. Unlike Saad and Gill (2000) – who examined explanatory paradigms used in the marketing literature and argued that few marketing scholars use an evolutionary psychological framework – we are concerned with implicit views of human nature, how they mirror evolutionary psychological thinking, and how marketing practices reflect these views. We begin by providing a brief overview of the evolutionary perspective. We then examine connections between marketing practices and evolved human adaptations, such as prestige seeking and preferences for sweets, fat, and salt. Finally, we discuss how some marketing practices reflect evolutionary ideas about variation. Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 4 An Evolutionary Perspective Evolutionary theory views the development of biological and social systems as occurring through a process of variation, selection, and retention—occurring through a slow process of small incremental improvements, rather than through a priori design (Dennett, 1995). It is primarily concerned with "why" and "function" questions. Why do humans prefer foods that are salty, sweet, and have a high fat content? What are the possible adaptive functions of those preferences? The primary processes in biological and social evolution are natural selection and sexual selection. Darwin’s three postulates, which we modified slightly so that they are applicable to both biological and cultural evolution, express the basic processes of natural selection. The first postulate is that without constraints, populations can expand indefinitely; however, because resources are limited, the capacity of an environment to support any population is limited. The second is that the units in a population, such as organisms or social structures, vary; that is, they differ in slight ways from one another. These variations affect the ability of units to survive and replicate. The third postulate is that many, but not all, variations are transmitted from generation to generation. Variations that produce successful consequences, regardless of initial intentions, are usually retained. Darwin also proposed a theory of sexual selection, although refinements and implications were not developed until the latter half of the twentieth century (Cronin, 1991). The difference between natural and sexual selection is the reason for the evolution of a trait. With natural selection, traits evolve because they are directly related to survival and reproduction—the osprey’s talons, the rabbit’s running speed, nurturing behaviors that female mammals exhibit towards their young. With sexual selection, the Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 5 criteria for the evolution of a trait is attractiveness to the opposite sex (Cronin, 1991). A classic example is the peacock’s tail. It does not directly help the peacock survive and reproduce; in fact, it is an encumbrance when attempting to escape from predators. However, the peacock’s tail is attractive to peahens, who are more likely to mate with peacocks with large colorful tails than those with smaller, drab tails. Sexual selection can also occur in social systems, where the criterion for replication of a social process or artifact is its capacity to attract members of the opposite sex (Miller, 2000). Sexually selected social practices might include displays of musical talent, athletic prowess, poetry, repartee, and the latest sartorial fashions. The evolutionary perspective in the social sciences contains two broad subperspectives: evolutionary psychology and socio-cultural evolution. Evolutionary psychology. Evolutionary psychology is a synthesis of modern psychology and evolutionary biology. It uses the logic of natural selection to examine human mental processes and behavior. Two assumptions of evolutionary psychology are: (1) that the human mind is modular, consisting of numerous psychological mechanisms and (2) that these mechanisms are adaptations. An adaptation is any feature of an organism that has been important to increasing inclusive fitness, that is, the propagation of genes into future generations, either directly by mating and caring for offspring or indirectly by helping kin survive and reproduce (Williams, 1966, 1996). Adaptations serve a purpose – that is, a function – reflected in special design (Williams, 1966). The function of wings on most birds is flight. The function of the eye is to enable an organism to see. The distinguishing features of special design are: functionality, reliability, complexity, and good design. Psychological mechanisms are heritable, Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 6 content-specific psychological processes that are activated in specific contexts associated with solving adaptive problems—such as identifying and competing for fertile mates. They are Darwinian algorithms that allow the mind to process information efficiently in contexts where doing so has paid off in the currency of survival and reproduction (Gigerenzer and others, 1999). Evolutionary psychology is relatively new field; not surprisingly, there is a healthy debate about the nature and number of psychological mechanisms. It is not uncommon to see multiple interpretations of psychological studies with an evolutionary perspective (de Waal, 2002). However, these debates are having the salutary effects of improving theory and evidentiary standards for identifying psychological mechanisms (Andrews, Gangestad, & Matthews, in press; de Waal, 2002). Keeping these caveats in mind, Table 1 provides a provisional list of psychological mechanisms. _____________________ Insert Table 1 About here _____________________ The mind of present-day humans was shaped during 1.2 million years of the Pleistocene era, what evolutionary psychologists refer to as the environment of evolutionary adaptation (EEA). It is, therefore, not necessarily adapted to industrial and post-industrial society. Our current mental mechanisms evolved over hundreds thousands of years, and during most of that time humans lived in small hunter-gatherer bands on the African savanna. In addition, changes in our mental mechanisms occur much slower than changes in culture. Behavioral patterns that are deeply rooted in evolutionary history cannot be quickly altered by cultural or technological change. Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 7 Although psychological mechanisms probably influence a range of human behavior, people are not necessarily conscious them. Common misconceptions about evolutionary psychology are that it implies that behavior is “genetically programmed” and that it downplays the role of learning. To the contrary, a fundamental premise of evolutionary psychology is the interaction between human nature and the environment. For example, all normally developing humans acquire language. The developmental regularities of language acquisition across cultures are evidence that language is an instinct in humans (Pinker, & Bloom 1992). However, the content of language – whether a person speaks French or English – is learned and is a product of culture. Evolution has shaped the ease with which behaviors are learned, the parameters of learning, and the contexts in which they are most readily acquired (Geary, 1995). Similarly, many adaptations are facultative—the phenotype varies depending on environmental conditions to which the organism is exposed (Gaulin & McBurney, 2001). Socio-Cultural Evolution. Socio-cultural evolution is a process whereby social systems and social practices develop from variations in form and trial and error, rather than strictly through deliberate design (Campbell, 1975; Colarelli, 1998). Acquired characteristics – such as behaviors and values – are socially selected and retained (Boyd & Richerson, 1985; Campbell, 1975). Variations can arise through pure chance, improbable combinations, imperfect imitation, and errors; but they can also occur through guided evolution, where analysis plays an important role (Boyd & Richerson, 1985). Through cultural selection mechanisms (e.g., imitation) social systems select some variations and they usually retain those that are functional (Colarelli, 1998). The efficiency of a new social practice does not ensure that social systems will select it and Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 8 use it. Circumstances must favor its inclusion into a pool of variations, selection mechanisms must capitalize on it, and conditions must favor its retention. Social units can “learn” to use a practice that is invalid for its intended purpose because it produces other functional consequences or is compatible with evolved human preferences. The evolution of social practices is often a complex interaction of psychological mechanisms, ecological conditions, and cultural transmission. An over-simplified model might look like this: psychological mechanisms influence human decision rules and preferencesī these in turn influence choices and the development of artifacts and traditionsī the effects of traditions and artifacts on survival and reproduction influence the rate of cultural transmission of artifacts and traditions. For example, like many other animals, humans forage for food and other resources. Given the regularities in foraging behavior among hunter-gatherers, it is a reasonably hypothesis that psychological mechanisms evolved in humans that guide foraging behavior (Rajala & Hantula, 2000; Winterhalder & Smith, 1981). As the consumer society developed, new venues emerged for foraging—shopping areas in cities, malls, and web pages. To some extent, ancient psychological mechanisms that guided the foraging behavior of our ancestors also influenced the shape of new foraging ecologies. Over time, those ecologies that produced beneficial consequences were more likely to be replicated. This brief example suggests how the development of cultural artifacts (e.g., shopping centers) and cultural practices (shopping) emerged from a complex interaction of psychological mechanisms, ecological conditions, culture, and cultural transmission. Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 9 Intuitive Evolutionary Thinking in Marketing Companies spend considerable sums of money on marketing—$320 billion globally in 2000 (Kloss, 2001). They do so because marketing works. Developing new products that meet consumers’ needs can substantially improve a firm’s fortunes. Effective advertising can make the difference between whether consumers ignore a product or buy it. However, for marketing to work, marketers must base their activities on a reasonably accurate view of human nature. What model might they be using? The content of many advertisements and the nature of many consumer products suggest to us that marketing practitioners are using a model of human nature that is implicitly based on evolutionary psychology. With the exception of some sex differences, humans are fundamentally alike in their basic psychological mechanisms. Natural selection sculpted a universal human nature consisting of a multitude of adaptations that were useful for survival and reproduction under the social and ecological conditions found during the EEA. Since culture changes much more rapidly than biology, humans continue to exhibit preferences and responses based on ancient mechanisms. Consumption is a bio-social activity (Sherry, 1991). Psychological mechanisms have played an important role in the development of preferences for many cultural products and practices, including consumer products and marketing practices (Burnham & Phelan, 2000). As the consumer culture emerged marketers tapped into psychological mechanisms in developing consumer products. An advertisement that appeals to an evolved preference is more likely to produce effective consequences and be adopted. For example an ad appealing to young males that features young and attractive females is more likely to get their attention and Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 10 develop a positive emotion towards the product than, say, an ad featuring older, matronly women. Consumer products and advertisements are likely to tap into evolved psychological mechanisms when they provide information that is similar to that which was provided in the original contexts of the adaptive problems that gave rise to the evolution of a particular psychological mechanism. Interestingly, John B. Watson was one of the first psychologists to recognize the importance of emotional appeals in advertising, and he used them in his work in advertising at the J. Walter Thompson agency (Buckley, 1982). This is not to say that all advertisers consciously engineered advertisements based on evolutionary principles; rather, many advertising appeals emerged from an interaction of psychological mechanisms, ecological conditions, cultural factors, and consequences. We begin by looking at how advertising and product development map onto psychological mechanisms. We dichotomize these mechanisms into those that are common among humans and those that are characteristic of males or females. Whenever possible, we bring three converging sources of evidence: contemporaneous, historical, and cross-cultural. If a practice is evident over time and across cultures, then it is more likely that the practice is based on an evolved psychological mechanism than on an idiosyncratic feature of a culture or history. After drawing connections between marketing practices and psychological mechanisms, we look at how marketing practices reflect the evolutionary principle of variation. Some Common Adaptations Some psychological mechanisms, such as the preferences for savanna-like landscapes or a fear of snakes, are less adaptive to people living in post-industrial Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 11 consumer societies than to hunters and gatherers during the EEA. Others, such as kinbased altruism and status seeking, continue to be adaptive. Still others, such as cravings for fats and sweets, are mismatched to the modern environment because they lead to over-consumption and attendant health problems in resource-rich modern environments. Yet most of our psychological mechanisms elicit strong responses, and these in turn influence our susceptibility to consumer products and advertising. Kin selection. One of the most powerful psychological mechanisms is kinselection (Hamilton, 1964). Kin selection is the tendency to behave altruistically to kin, and increasingly so as relatedness increases. Among group-living species, kin selection is a fact of life. The biological puzzle of altruism was solved by William D. Hamilton, in what later become known as Hamilton’s Rule: natural selection will favor individuals who incur costs to help another when it is in their genetic self-interest. For example, natural selection will favor individuals to be twice as altruistic to someone who shares 50% of their genes (say, a child or a sibling) than to someone who shares 12.5% of their genes (a first cousin). Numerous studies have found that genetic relatedness is correlated with altruistic behavior. People are more likely to bequeath their estates to kin than nonkin (Smith, Kish, & Crawford, 1987). Parents typically invest more resources – such as food and health care expenses – in genetic offspring than step, adopted, or foster children (Case, Lin, & McLanahan,1999, 2000; Case & Paxton, 2001). Kin selection probably influences the purchase of many products that people use to help relatives. These might include toys, child safety products, insurance products, and financial planning services. Life insurance is an example par excellence of kin selection. The primary purpose of life insurance is to financially assist the insured person’s designated Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 12 beneficiaries, who are typically spouses, offspring, and other kin. Life insurance, particularly term life insurance, provides no tangible benefit to the insured person during her lifetime. Life insurance emerged as a viable consumer product concomitantly with modernization—as extended families diminished, and as people became increasingly dependent on money and consumer products for survival. In pre-modern and nonindustrialized countries, if an adult with children died prematurely, other family members were expected to take care of the surviving spouse and children. For example, in some traditional societies it is customary that an unmarried brother marry his deceased brother’s widow (Wilson, 1950/1964). Life insurance is relatively unknown in traditional societies and in developing countries where extended families and limited geographical mobility are the norm. Ninety-six percent of all the life insurance sold in 1997 was sold in Europe, North America, and Asia, while four percent of world sales were in Latin America, Africa, and Oceania (American Council of Life Insurance, 1999). Life insurers begin targeting consumers when they have their first baby (Grimm, 2002), and advertising copy for life insurance – directed at a wide variety of ethnic groups – typically has a strong emphasis on family and providing for one’s offspring (Barney, 2002; Marketing News, 1986). In 1997, 87% of full-time employees in medium and large private firms in the US received life insurance as part of their benefits package (US Dept. of Labor, Bureau of Labor Statistics, 1997); in 1998 married couples with children were almost twice as likely to own life insurance as people who were unmarried (LIMRA, 2000). The connection between life insurance and kin selection is apparent, too, in new in how changing demographics drive new approaches to selling life insurance. As Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 13 women become co-breadwinners or sole providers for their offspring, insurance marketers increasingly target women (Myers, 1996). Saving and paying for children’s college expenses is another prominent area of consumer behavior related to kin-selection. In countries such as the United States parents contribute substantially to children’s college expenses. For most intact families with children under 18, saving for their children’s college education is their most important savings goal (DeVaney & Chien, 2002). Parents, along with some help from the students themselves, contribute about half of the cost of a child’s college education (Gladieux & Hauptman, 1995). Over a typical four-year stint in college, parents in the US contribute about $21,800 for a child attending a public university and $47,300 for a child attending a private university (Vail, 2002). The financial planning and mutual fund industry offers a variety of products for parents who plan on helping pay for their children’s expenses. Many of these are associated with Section 529 of the Internal Revenue code, which provides parents a tax break for college savings. All 50 states have some sort of 529 plan (either a prepaid type plan or a savings plan), and parents and relatives have already saved $21 billion in such plans (Feigenbaum, 2002; Collegesavings.org, 2002). Advertising copy for college savings plans acknowledges the power of kin selection by appealing to parents’ or grandparents’ desires to help with their children’s education. For example, the ad copy for the Michigan Educational Savings Program emphasizes that “parents, grandparents, [and] relatives…may open an account and contribute…on behalf of a beneficiary” (MESP, 2002, p. 1; emphasis added). Consistent with a kin-selection hypothesis, families with two genetic parents contribute more towards college expenses of their offspring than do families with step Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 14 children. Zvoch (1999) found that step parents began financial planning for college later, set less money aside for college, and spent less per year on educational expenses than two genetic parents. Anderson, Kaplan, and Lancaster (1999) found that compared to stepchildren, genetic offspring of two married parents were more likely to attend college, more likely to receive money for college, and more likely to receive more money. Genetic children of current mates received an average of $4,293, while step children of current mates received an average of $1,828 (in one year in 1990 dollars). Xhosa men in South Africa also contribute significantly more money to their genetic children’s educational expenses than they do towards their stepchildren’s educational expenses (Anderson, Kaplan, Lam, & Lancaster, 1999). The educational attainment of genetic offspring is also higher than step, foster, or adopted children (Case, Lin, & McLanahan, 2001). Status and prestige. Status and prestige have nearly universal appeal among humans. Status is associated with rank in a hierarchy and greater resource entitlement, which in turn is associated with greater survival and reproductive advantages (Betzig, 1986; Perusse, 1993). Status can be attained by control of economic resources, political or military power, legitimate authority, and by having valued skill and knowledge. Prestige is a particular type of status—the quality of having attained status by skill in a particular domain (Barkow, 1989; Henrich & Gil-White, 2001; Treiman, 1977). Prestigious individuals, therefore, are regarded as possessing socially useful skills and knowledge; they receive deference and entitlements accordingly. Prestige is valued because it is associated with skills that allow people (both males and females) to compete for resource acquisition, which it turn enhances their ability to reproduce and care for Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 15 offspring (Barkow, 1989). On the other hand, people can attain high status but have relatively low prestige. In this section we will focus on the connection between prestige and marketing because prestige is valued and sought by both males and females. Because status – that is, entitlement to reward based on rank in a hierarchy – is more actively pursued by males, we examine the connection between status and marketing in a later section where we look at adaptive differences between males and females. People acquire skills and knowledge in a variety of ways – books, lectures, seminars, training programs – but the oldest method has been that of social learning, by imitating skilled individuals. Long before widespread literacy and commercial vocational training, people acquired skills by observing and working with master practitioners (Colarelli, 2003). Even today, one of the most effective methods of acquiring a skill is for an individual to become a member of a community of practice in which he or she can observe and work with more skilled individuals (Colarelli, 2003). In addition to imitating skills, people typically imitate other behaviors of prestigious individuals (Henrich & Gil-White, 2001). The psychological mechanism that predisposes people to imitate high-prestige individuals evolved because it helped to solve the adaptive problem of efficiently identifying and gaining useful skills, values, and beliefs (Henrich & Gil-White, 2001). Because it may not always be possible to identify precisely which behaviors contribute to skill acquisition and prestige, it may have been adaptive to err on the side of excess and copy as many behaviors as possible of prestigious individuals. Ownership and displays of prestigious consumer goods (i.e., status symbols) are, to some extent, cultural equivalents of the physical traits that biologists call “honest signals.” Honest signals are traits that are difficult to fake. Conspecifics (i.e., members Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 16 of the same species) use honest signals as evidence of a potential mate’s health, robustness, and genetic quality (Zahavi, 1975). The male lion’s mane, for example, is associated with indicators of health, such as lower parasite counts; female lions preferentially mate with males with full, lustrous manes (Withgott, 2002). Thus, the display of prestigious consumer goods can be a type of honest signal of mate value. A man who wears expensive clothes is signaling to potential mates (and male rivals) about his resources and skills at acquiring wealth; such men are generally perceived as more attractive by females (Ellis, 1992). Women displaying prestigious consumer goods are also sending signals about their resources and expectations for resources. Consumer goods, however, can be fallible honest signals. A person of reasonably modest means with access to credit can give the impression of having more status than he actually has by going into debt or spending all of his discretionary money on status symbols. The appeal of prestige is widely used in marketing. Mainstays are products that enhance skills. Self-help books on everything from cooking to romance to management are staples in the marketplace. More than 1,700 business titles were published in 1996 alone (Chadderdon, 1998). Approximately 102 million cooking and crafts books were sold in the United States, accounting for over 10 percent of national book sales (American Booksellers Association, 2002). Books in the “psychology” and “recovery” categories (e.g., family life, love, sex, marriage, diet, health, exercise, and personal grooming) accounted for 6.3 percent of US book sales in 1998. Normally one achieves prestige or status through years of hard work. Marketers, however, promote products to create the illusion that consumers can bypass time and effort. They do this with status symbols—artifacts that signal an individual’s prestige or Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 17 status. Marketers use existing status symbols as well as cultivate new ones. Advertisements co-opt the prestige-imitating mechanism by associating a product with prestige—by portraying a functional relationship between a product and a skill, by associating a product with prestige without a functional relationship, or by cultivating an image that the product is expensive. Portraying a functional relationship involves associating the product with skillfulness in a particular domain. A classic strategy is to portray celebrities, known for a particular skill, using the product. IBM has an advertising campaign that portrays celebrity intellectuals using IBM’s laptop computers (Morgan, 2002). Nike ads show celebrity athletes wearing Nike shoes and clothing. The implicit message is that if the consumer uses the product she can be more skillful or at least create a first-impression that she is skillful in a particular domain. Uniliver has successfully used the celebrityassociation strategy for Lux soap for years and in many cultures. Among the female celebrities that have appeared in ads for Lux are Joan Collins, Jane Fonda, Audrey Hepburn, Sophia Loren, Elizabeth Taylor, and Raquel Welch (Rijkens, 1992). Lux is Unilever’s premier international brand, and it is a world leader in sales. The imitation mechanism can also be co-oped by associating a product with a prestigious person even though there is no functional relationship between the product and a skill. Associating a prestigious person with a product creates the illusion that the product is prestigious. This can be seen in celebrity endorsement of products unrelated to the celebrity’s skill, such as a famous opera singer wearing a Rolex watch. Industry observers attribute the surge in demand for premium cigars to the advertising strategy of associating cigar smoking with an upscale lifestyle (Frank, 1999). “The link is promoted Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 18 by magazines like Cigar Aficionado, whose covers portray cigar-smoking celebrities such as Arnold Schwarzenegger, Madonna, and Demi Moore” (p. 31). However, cigar makers were using prestige appeal to sell cigars as early as the 19th century (Jones, 1979). For example, one advertisement in 1892 for General Arthur Cigars portrays a young woman dancing in a formal gown, surrounded by men in tuxedos tipping their top hats to her (Jones, 1979, p. 58). Prestige appeal is also common cross culturally. A good example is After Eight chocolate mints. In the 1960’s After Eight sought to internationalize its product. It used a marketing strategy that appealed to prestige (depicting upper class people marveling over the product), and the campaign apparently worked, with the mints grew in popularity throughout Germany, Holland, Italy, and France (Rijkens, 1992). Price also signals prestige. Cultivating an image that a product is expensive can increase prestige appeal. Owning and displaying an expensive product can signal that the owner has the surplus resources to buy the product and therefore the skills to acquire substantial resources. Over a century ago the economist Thorstein Veblen (1899/1953) adumbrated the connection between consumer goods and prestige appeal when he coined the term conspicuous consumption—the “specialized consumption of goods as an evidence of pecuniary strength.” (p. 60). He also anticipated the notion that a psychological mechanism (“a heritage from the past”) may be responsible for conspicuous consumption (p. 165). Products that are commonly marketed as status symbols are ornaments that are readily displayed, such as jewelry or watches. A $40 Timex wristwatch keeps time just as well as a $14,000 Patek Philippe, but the Patek Philippe signals that its owner has resources and (possibly) the skill to acquire resources. Advertising such status symbols is Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 19 anything but novel in the realm of marketing. Upscale watches have been marketed as status symbols for decades (Jones 1979) and this continues unabated, as a cursory glance through the pages of most upscale periodicals will attest. Preferences for sweets, fat, and salt. People across the world share preferences for sweets, salt, and fat; they share aversions for foods with a bitter, rancid, or putrid taste (Rozin, 1976). During the approximately 1.2 million years that humans lived as huntergatherers, people evolved a preference for foods with a high nutritional value (Buss, 1995). Protein, carbohydrates, and minerals are essential for nutrition and health. Foods high in fat were the most efficient form of calories and often had high concentrations of protein. Foods that were sweet, such as ripe fruits, generally indicated high levels of nutrients, especially carbohydrates. A salty flavor indicated high mineral content— particularly sodium chloride, a mineral that humans cannot live without (Pocock & Richards, 1999). People who preferred those tastes would be more likely to survive and reproduce than people who had preferences for bitter and putrid foods, which are associated with poisons and rotten food. People’s preferences would motivate them to seek out such foods, but their relative scarcity and difficulty of acquiring meant that people did not overdo their intake. In the modern era, however, fats, sweets, and salt are readily available. These evolved preferences have played a significant role in the development and advertising of consumer food products. Salt was one of the first commodities and consumer products. Records show that salt was used in trade and in food preparation by the Chinese and Egyptians over 4000 years ago, and salt has remained an essential flavoring and preservative (Kurlansky, 2002). Although people do not experience a Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 20 craving for salt, they notice its absence and prefer foods with salt. The modern snack food industry capitalizes on humans’ need for salt by creating an array of salty snacks— potato chips, salted nuts, tortilla chips. In 1999, consumers ate 6 billion pounds of salty snacks, with annual sales reaching $19 billion. Potato chips lead the pack with $4.7 billion in sales (Howell, 2000). Snack foods with a low-salt content do not sell well. Sweets and fats are also staples of the consumer food market. Consumers in the United States spend approximately 21.5% of their total food expenditures on oils, fats, sugar, confectioneries, and soft drinks, and while not as high in other countries, expenditures on these food items still remain a significant portion of total food expenditures (Euromonitor International, n.d.). One study of television food advertisements directed to children found that 63% were for foods with a high concentrations of fat and/or sugar (Wilson, Quigley, & Mansoor, 1999). Coca-Cola, first mixed in 1866, is an historical and global example of the success of a product that has co-opted the human preference for sweets. It is one of the most (arguably the most) recognized product brands in the world. In 1998 Coca-Cola ranked 8th on the list of global advertisement spenders and was first in product global presence, being present in 69 countries (Kloss, 2001). People’s preferences for fat, sweets, and salt contributed to the growth of fast food restaurants. If people did not like the taste of greasy hamburgers and salty french fries, these restaurants would have quickly gone out of business (see, e.g., Kramer, 1997). The abundance of junk food is taking a toll on human health. In 1999, 60% of Americans adults were classified as overweight and 27% as obese. Twenty five percent of American children are overweight and 10% are obese (Economist, 2002). While sedentary lifestyles and lack of exercise contribute, the excessive consumption of fats and Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 21 sweets plays a significant role. One strategy to combat the problem is to reduce the amount of fat in food or to develop fat substitutes. However, given our evolved preferences for fats and sweets, such strategies have been ineffective. Consumers do no like the taste of low fat foods (Hamilton, Knox, Hill, & Parr, 2000); unfortunately, finding a fat substitute that tastes like the real thing has proven illusive (Taylor & Linforth, 1998). Preferences for savanna-like landscapes. Psychological mechanisms for landscape preferences evolved because landscapes with certain features were more conducive to survival and reproduction than others. This why most people still regard certain types of landscapes and landscape features as more beautiful, and hence, attractive than others (Kaplan, 1992; Orians & Heerwagen, 1992). This is consistent with Thornhill’s definition of beauty: “beauty is the moving experience associated with information processing by aesthetic judgment adaptations when they perceive information of evolutionarily historical promise of high reproductive success” (1998, p. 557). Most people instinctively find pictures of clear, flowing water more beautiful than a pool of stagnant water, grassy landscapes with scattered trees and lakes more attractive than arid, treeless landscapes, and mountains or hills in the distance more attractive than flat terrain. Clear, flowing water usually has fewer potentially harmful bacteria than stagnant water; savanna-like landscapes tend to have higher concentrations of resourceproviding potential for land-dwelling, omnivorous primates (Orians & Heerwagen, 1992); mountains and hills convey a sense of mystery and a desire to lean more about a landscape (Kaplan, 1992). Empirical studies have found that people have strong preferences for such ancestral cues as “water sources, oasis, flowers, ripe fruits, savanna Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 22 (open forests that give easy visual access), growth and leaf patterns of healthy savanna trees, closed forest canopy (shelter), caves (with easy access to outside landscape), and mountains” (Thornhill, 1998, p. 562). These evolved preferences are commonly used in advertising. Marketers use attractive landscapes to evoke positive emotional associations. Landscape features have often been used, in our culture and in others, to distinguish among brands and to impart desirable associations with them (Gould & Minowa, 1994; Goodrum & Dalrymple, 1990). The sense of mystery and adventure associated with mountains has been a stable feature of marketing sport utility vehicles (SUVs; Messaris, 1997). Landscapes appealing to evolved preferences for verdant savanna-like landscapes, mountains, water, and flowers are also prominent in marketing resorts (e.g., Steinauer, 2002; Successful Meetings, 2001). To anyone who has spent any time in a hotel or resort, it is painfully obvious that landscape features influence the price of a room. Rooms with a view of mountains or lakes are inevitably more expensive than rooms with a view of a parking lot. Interestingly, even products that are harmful to human health are given a healthy patina through clever associations with beautiful landscapes. Advertisement for Kool cigarettes superimpose a pack of Kools over a sparkling river surrounded by lush greens and wooded patches (Leiss, Kline, & Jhally, 1986, p. 196). Alcoholic beverages, such as Coors Beer, are frequently marketed in a similar manner, with the product superimposed over a mountain stream running through a beautiful landscape. Adaptive Differences Between Males and Females Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 23 Men and women differ in some evolved mechanisms. This is due to different adaptive problems faced by the different sexes as well as to differences in traits favored by sexual selection (Buss, 1994; Miller, 2000). Because of the physiological and psychological demands associated with gestation, birth, lactation, and attachment, human females provide more parental investment in offspring than males (Andersson, 1994). Parental investment is the effort and resources devoted to an offspring that improves its chances of survival and that also limit the parent’s ability to invest in other offspring (Trivers, 1972). The welfare of children is more dependent on the mother staying alive than the father (Högberg & Broström, 1985). Therefore, natural selection favored women who were less prone to risky and aggressive behavior. Although contextual factors (e.g., threats to offspring) can trigger aggressive behavior in women, women are, on average, significantly less aggressive, more fearful, and less prone to take risks than men (A. Campbell, 1999). Women’s greater parental investment also helps to explain why women are more nurturing and have a greater interest in children than do men (Geary, 1998). Women are also the primary arbiters in mating relationships (Geary, 1998). Their greater parental investment motivates women to be more selective in their choice of short-term and long-term mates. It is in females’ genetic interests to mate selectively with “high quality” males. Women attend to a larger number of attributes of potential mates and are more discriminating in their choice of mating partners. Females favor males with traits associated with resource acquisition, health, good genes, and kindness. They prefer males who exhibit signs of high social status and wealth (expensive ornamentation, age) or who have the ability to gain high social status (intelligence, social Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 24 dominance, competitiveness, height), are in good health and have good genes (height, firm musculature, athleticism), and display a temperament compatible with being a good husband and father (kindness; Ellis, 1992). Recent studies are providing evidence of the reproductive consequences of some of these preferences. Tall males father more children than shorter males (Pawlowski, Dunbar, & Lipowicz, 2000); women are more attracted to men with masculine facial features during that part of their monthly cycle when the probability of conception is highest (Penton-Voak, & others, 1999). It is in male’s genetic interests to mate with a wide variety of healthy, fertile females. Therefore, males pay attention to fewer attributes of potential mates than women do; men attend primarily to physical cues related to health and fertility. These include youthful appearance, a waist to hip ratio of about .70 (fertility and health), smooth skin (youth, absence of parasites), lustrous hair (youth), full lips (high estrogen level), and a bouncy gait (youth, energy). When seeking out long-term mates, males are also attentive to traits that signal that a woman would be a good mother and sexually faithful (Buss, 1994). Members of each sex engage in intrasexual competition to compete for access to desirable members of the opposite sex. In most primate species, including humans, malemale competition is more intensive than female-female competition. In male-male competition, males compete with one another for status, resources, and social dominance. They do this ultimately to gain access to sexually receptive females (Geary, 1998). Female-female competition involves competition for high quality males for marriage partners. Women typically compete with one another for resources by presenting themselves in ways that are viewed as attractive to men, ways that “mirror the attributes Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 25 associated with male choice of mating partners, such as physical attractiveness” (Geary, 1998, p. 137). Marketing and Adaptive Differences between Males and Females Personal advertisements, where people advertise themselves in periodicals and on the Internet in hopes of attracting a short- or long-term mate, are a particularly revealing arena to examine the interplay of evolved sex differences and marketing. Males are more likely to advertise their professional and financial status, while women typically advertise attributes related to physical beauty (Greenlees & McGrew, 1984); older women are less demanding in what they require in a mate, whereas older men are more demanding (Campos, Otta, & Sequeira, 2002). Height, education, resources, and age of male advertisers are positively related to the number of responses from females, while age, weight, and education were negatively related to female hit rates (Pawlowski & Koziel, 2002). Much of traditional marketing mirrors evolved sex differences. Product development and advertising reflect the constellation of evolved sex differences which various psychologists have grouped under the categories of communal (concern for others, interpersonal harmony) and agentic (task emphasis, competition) orientations. Women are more communal and men more agentic (Archer, 1996). Evolved preferences. Marketing practices reflect females’ greater interest in children and safety. Advertisements for child-care products are much more evident in publications and programming directed at women (Reda, 1994; Shermach, 1994). Products related to personal and child safety are also directed more towards females (Garfield, 1999). One reason that women became a major market for SUVs is their ostensible safety features— Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 26 larger size, four-wheel drive (Scotti, 1998). Women’s preference for romantic involvement over short-term sexual liaisons is reflected in the marketing and sales of romance novels and products related to weddings. Women are the primary consumers of romance novels, which account for almost 50% of all paperbacks books sold (Discount Store News, 1996). Harlequin, a major publisher of romance novels, sold 205 million books in 1992 in 108 countries and 26 languages (Economist, 1993). Magazines, services and products related to weddings are almost exclusively aimed at young women (Fine, 2000). There are plenty of magazines for brides; we are not aware of any for grooms. The development and marketing of toys is largely gender specific. Girls’ toys typically reflect females’ greater interest in beauty, infants, and nurturing, while boys’ toys reflect a greater preference for competition, rough and tumble play, and mechanical objects (Fitzgerald, 1993). Gender specificity with children’s toys has paid off handsomely. Mattel’s Barbie doll accounted for $1 billion of Mattel’s $1.6 billion in total sales in 1991 (Brandweek, 1993). Mattel’s “He- Man: Masters of the Universe” – an aggressive, muscle-man action toy – was one of the most successful boys’ toys ever introduced (Mandese, 1990). Gender-based toy preferences appear to be due more to innate propensities than to socialization. Alexander and Hines (2002) found that young male and female vervet monkeys exhibited gender-typical preferences for toys. Male monkeys spent more time than females playing with a toy car and females spent more time playing with a doll. Women also tend to form closer ties with same-sex friends than men and female groups are less hierarchical than male groups (Colarelli, Alampay, & Canali, 2002b; Fisher, 1999). Marketers have taken advantage of intensive, flat female networks by Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 27 engaging women to directly sell to their friends. Tupperware parties are a classic example. Approximately 72% of American women have attended a Tupperware party; with 34% of American women attending over six. Tupperware parties have also been successful across cultures. Tupperware has sales consultants in over 100 countries, with 80% of sales occuring overseas (Verburg, 2002). The economic success of women’s micro-industries financed by the Grameen Bank in Bangladesh is an example of a consumer services based on the nature of female relationships (Yunus, 1999). Ninety-five percent of the Grameen Bank’s borrowers are women (Wahid, 1999). The Grameen Bank originated in 1976 (and was chartered as a bank in1983) in Bangladesh to provide credit without requiring collateral to poor, primarily female, villagers. New borrowers are required to join a small group of established borrowers. Obtaining a loan is based on the approval of peers in the group. Default by any individual in the group results in all group members having their credit line suspended or drastically reduced. The Grameen Bank is one of the most successful Third World economic development programs to date. By 1998, it established 1,118 branches, providing service to 38,766 villages. It has 2.3 million borrowers, $2.5 billion in cumulative loans, $185 million in savings, and a 98% loan collection rate. One of the strongest evolved preferences among males is the desire for young, fertile females. The adage “sex sells” is a truism in marketing. Beautiful women have been used in ads to sell everything from cars to scientific equipment. Using females as sex object in advertisements is routine in many countries (Caudle, 1994). It is also an example of the use of classical conditioning in advertising (cf. Gorn, 1982). Beautiful women are prominently placed in print ads to attract readers’ attention and are, for most Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 28 men, an unconditioned stimulus. They stimulate a pleasant, sexually arousing response in sexually mature males. Marketers take advantage of this evolved mechanism by pairing beautiful women (unconditioned stimulus) with a product (conditioned stimulus). The goal is to evoke the same response in males who view the product (conditioned response) as they exhibit when viewing an attractive female (unconditioned response). That ads using attractive women to sell merchandise appear routinely even in highbrow newspapers attests to the power of this mechanism. The appeal of beautiful females to males is also used in the development of products that appeal to males, particularly pornography. Erotica has existed throughout history, is found across cultures, and has been a minor staple of literature and the visual arts (Loth, 1961). The pornography business caters primarily to a male audience; for example, the audience of Internet porn sites is approximately 84% male (Weiss, 2001). The appeal of pornography is also become particularly evident with advent of the Internet. Internet porn is one of the most successful dot-com industries. About 30% of the online population visits a porn site each month. There are approximately 50,000 porn sites in countries throughout the world (Gruenwedel, 2000). One study reports that almost $1 billion was spent by on-line porn users in 1998 (see Gruenwedel, 2000); in 1999, porn web accounted for 70% of the online paid content market (Kavanagh, 1999). Porn is also a mainstay of the pay-per-view cable TV market, accounting for 69 percent of sales (Gruenwedel, 2000). Same-sex Competition. People engage in same-sex (intrasexual) competition to make themselves more attractive to the opposite sex than their competitors. A central arena of female-female competition is physical attractiveness. This no doubt helps to Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 29 account for the appeal of cosmetics to women and the success of the cosmetics industry: the function of cosmetics is to enhance physical beauty. Women are the primary consumers of cosmetics; men account for only 7% of the market share (www.cosmeticmarket.com, 1996). Cosmetic advertisements date back (at least in America) to the 1750s (Goodrum & Dalrymple, 1990). Newspaper ads portrayed attractive women using a variety of cosmetics—soaps, make-up, and hair products. Although advertisements have changed to reflect the times, their core themes have remained constant—slowing the aging process, beautifying one’s hair, and enhancing a body part. Many cosmetics ads portray attractive women as young and fertile. An ad for Palmolive soap, appearing in 1928, is illustrative. It depicts a young female holding a bouquet of flowers and reads: “He remembered that schoolgirl complexion. Youth is charm, and youth lost is charm lost, as every woman instinctively realizes” (Goodrum & Dalrymple, 1990, p. 130). Much of male-male competition involves competition over status, resources, and reputation (Buss, 1994, 1999). Not surprisingly, marketers have used this to their advantage in the development of products and ads that appeal to males. Young males in the US gain status and reputation primarily through athletic prowess (Coleman, 1965). Male-targeted sports magazines that illustrate male-male competition and promise the improvement of the reader’s competitive ability at sports are among the most circulated in the US. Of the hundreds of magazines circulating in the U.S. each year, Sports Illustrated was 16th in 2001 in annual average circulation (3.22 million). Nearly all universities in the United States spend proportionately more on male than female sporting programs (www.chronicle.com, 2002). For many universities, money spent on male Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 30 teams more than doubles that which is spent on female sporting teams. As males age, male-male competition centers more on work and money than athletics. Not surprisingly, the readership of the major management and finance periodicals – Fortune, Forbes, and Business Week – is primarily male (Corazza, 1984). The two best-selling men’s magazines, Playboy and Sports Illustrated, appeal to men’s interest in beautiful young women and male-male competition. These are the only two male-oriented magazines in the 25 top-selling magazines in the US, Sports Illustrated and Playboy (ranked 16th and 17th, respectively, in average annual circulation in 2001). The two best-selling women’s magazines (ranked sixth and seventh) are Better Homes and Gardens and Family Circle, which feature articles on home, children, food, and relationships. Men’s Health, a hybrid men’s magazine that features articles on relationships, food, as well as male-male competition ranks only 44th. (www.magazine.org, 2002). Products that assume no or little sex differences between males and females on status competition or sexuality tend to fail in the marketplace. The magazine, Sports Illustrated Women, a female-oriented version of Sports Illustrated featuring articles on female athletic competition and adventure sports, printed its last issue in December 2002, after only two years on the market (Colford, 2002). Playgirl, a female version of Playboy, featuring sexually explicit pictures of men, had to revamp its content and format to resemble more traditional women’s magazines to stay viable (Calvacca, 1998). Variation Evolution by natural selection occurs through small incremental changes, and variation is a critical element in change (Colarelli, 2003; Dawkins, 1986). Small Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 31 variations – mutations, imperfect imitations, mistakes, new ideas – are the stuff from which new biological and social adaptations evolve. Sufficient variation helps a system cope with uncertain futures by improving the probability that the variation will enable the system to adapt to unpredictable changes in the environment. Systems typically develop structures that maintain requisite – neither too much nor too little – variation (Colarelli, 2000; Williams, 1996). Sexual reproduction and incest avoidance are classic examples of biological and psychological mechanisms that perpetuate genetic variation, and assortative mating (mating with partners who share similar characteristics) helps to keep variation in check. A good deal of variation, however, is genetic and cultural noise (Tooby & Cosmides, 1990). Variation tends to manifest itself more frequently on relatively unimportant traits, such as eye or hair color. Most of these surface variations have little relevance to survival and adaptation. On the other hand, there is less variation in traits that are adaptive—almost all humans have two eyes and opposable thumbs. Adaptations vary less because, over the long term, they are important to survival and reproduction. Variation and Marketing Evolutionary conceptions of variation are relevant to marketing in at least two ways: (1) change and adaptation and (2) appealing to individuals. Variation in consumer products and advertising helps to insure that useful products and advertisements will be available to meet changing consumer demands. Although humans share many adaptations, they also differ (Medawar, 1981). So, although most people enjoy eating sweets, they also vary in their preferences for particular flavors. Some people prefer grape jam; others prefer strawberry. Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 32 Continual variation is the lifeblood of the popular cultural marketplace. Fads and fashions come and go with amazing regularity in popular culture. New talent is the lifeblood of the rock and roll industry. Most new bands never make it, but a few do (Hire, 1986). Therefore, variation is a better strategy than trying to predict which bands will take off. Introducing variation and establishing mechanisms for nurturing variation in product development have become important strategies in large, mainstream corporations. These strategies include hiring out-of-the-box thinkers, providing architectural and social structures that facilitate interaction among different groups of employees, and allowing employees freedom to develop ideas and products (Leifer, McDermott, Colarelli-O’Connor, Peters, Rice, & Veryzer, 2000). Continual variation in advertising also helps to keep old products current. Classic Coca-Cola varied little as a product since its conception; however, the marketing of Coca-Cola has been characterized by continual variation in advertising strategies (Goodrum & Dalrymple, 1990). Variation is useful in market research. A fundamental notion in evolutionary theory is that predictions about the future are problematic. The large number of variables, interactions, and environmental contingencies inherent in biological and social systems preclude precise prediction (Colarelli, 2003). Thus, it is not surprising that traditional marketing research has a spotty record of predicting what consumers will buy (Zangwill, 1992, 1993). Some marketers, however, use research strategies that are driven by variation. Rather than trying to predict what consumers want through extensive consumer surveys and focus groups, they introduce variations of a product into the market and wait to see what consumers prefer. Based on the feedback from sales, firms Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 33 learn what consumers really want, and then they revise their products accordingly. Zangwill (1992) describes how some of the most successful consumer product companies use this approach. Sony, Sanyo, Seiko, and Citizen often develop a platform of a product from which several variations can be easily produced. They then put several (sometimes hundreds) of variations on the market and see which ones sell best. Sony introduced over 160 models of its Walkman into the market to discover which models consumers preferred. Many marketers are aware of the importance of understanding individual consumer needs (Lee & Johnson, 1999), and recent technological innovations in manufacturing and communications have allowed mass marketing to achieve a more personalized feel. Major companies that at one time could not meet individual consumer needs can now more readily do so. Building customization of and variation in products is not only a marketing strategy but an organizational philosophy of a number of companies. Dell Computer’s mission statement states that Dell is committed to “meeting customer expectations of flexible customization capability” (www.dell.com, 2002). General Motors (GM) also integrates marketing and manufacturing to allow customers to personalize products. In fact, GM was a pioneer in personalizing products on a mass scale. It was one of the first automobile manufacturers to give customers a choice in the color and style of automobiles (Chandler, 1964). This was a marked contrast to Ford’s strategy of almost complete standardization, and GM’s responsiveness to customer preferences was a major factor in GM surpassing Ford in market share. Today, G M has a feature on its web site allowing consumers to “build your own vehicle” from the ground Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 34 up (www.gmc.com, 2002). The consumer can choose from hundreds of variations, including, color, seat cover type, equipment accessories, and engine size. Conclusions The applied social sciences exist in two worlds. One is applied research and the other is practice. Sometimes these two worlds are in agreement about social interventions; often they are not. The applied research world frequently criticizes the world of practice—for ignoring its recommendations, for failing to use the latest research findings, and for relying on common sense and rules of thumb. The idea that a practice proved its mettle by withstanding the test of time is usually an anathema to applied researchers. Yet, it often turns out that the world of practice was not wrong and that the world of applied research was in error (Lindblom, & Cohen, 1979). For decades, applied researchers in personnel psychology criticized human resource practitioners using the employment interview—arguing that their research showed that the employment interview was ineffective in predicting performance. After a half a century of criticizing the employment interview, studies now suggest that the interview is a good predictor of performance after all (McDaniel, Whetzel, Schmidt, & Mauer, 1994) and that it serves useful functions in addition to predicting job performance (Adams, Elacqua, & Colarelli, 1994; Colarelli 1996). It is possible that marketing practitioners have a more veridical conception of human nature than marketing researchers. Practitioners appear to use an intuitive evolutionary perspective in developing marketing practices, whereas most researchers are still adhere to the SSSM (Pinker, 2002; Saad & Gill, 2000). Consumer products and advertisements that tap into evolved psychological mechanisms seem to have a robust Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 35 appeal to consumers. Similarly, practices that produce adaptive consequences are likely to be perpetuated, regardless of the originators’ intentions or whether they understood why the practices worked. An implication of the evolutionary perspective for marketing research is reverse engineering of successful practices. Marketers may profit by studying products and ad campaigns that have been successful over the years and across cultures and using them as templates. A second implication is the utility of explicitly incorporating evolved psychological mechanisms into product development and marketing. Some people may want little boys to be less competitive or they may want people to eat less fat and fewer sweets. But who is going to have more success in the marketplace—firms that appeal to young males’ propensity to behave competitively with one another or those that appeal to males as nurturers, firms sell sweet snacks with a high fat content or those that sell low fat and non-sweetened snacks? However, in a complex world in which individuals and cultures are in some ways unique, making precise predictions about the effects of interventions is a risky business. Therefore, a third implication of the evolutionary perspective for marketing is to embrace variation and uncertainty, working with them rather than trying to engineer them out. This includes individualizing products and advertising, introducing multiple prototypes, and emphasizing quick-response production. Successful marketing appeals to our evolved human nature, which, alas, is a mixed bag. People can be compassionate, generous, and cooperative; humans are frequently altruistic, moral, and restrained. And marketers develop products and provide information about products that appeal to the noble side of our nature—college savings plans, environmentally friendly products, services to care for the poor and elderly. Yet, Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 36 people can also behave aggressively; they can be selfish and vindictive; people’s desires can spin out of control into gluttony and hedonism; they are not above deceiving others when it is to their advantage. Marketers are often criticized for selling products that appeal to people’s baser side—pornography, junk food, wasteful status symbols, and products that harm the environment. Indeed, a recurring problem in, and criticism of, marketing is deception in advertising. While difficult to condone, deception will probably be a staple of advertising because, as Dawkins (1976/1989) reminds us, deceit is an inherent attribute of animal (including human) communication. Given the realities of human nature and the criteria by which the success of marketing efforts are inevitably judged (i.e., sales), appeals to our baser side will remain a staple of marketing practices. Unfortunately, this may become increasingly problematic because of the mismatch between our evolved nature and the nature of modern society. It is now relatively easy to design and sell products that hijack our evolved mechanisms and induce people to over-consume and consume harmful products. A better understanding of human nature is certainly a necessary, but not sufficient, condition for mitigating harmful excesses. It is also necessary that markets remain free and competitive. The conflict of competing interests and ideas is perhaps the most effective restraint on unbridled excess. Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 37 References Alexander, G. M., & Hines, M. (2002). Sex differences in response to children’s toys in nonhuman primates (Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus). Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 467-479. Adams, G.A., Elacqua, T.C., & Colarelli, S.M. (1994). The employment interview as a sociometric selection technique. Journal of Group Psychotherapy, Psychodrama & Sociometry,47, 99-113. American Booksellers Association (2002). Retrieved October 20, 2002, from the World Wide Web: http://www.bookweb.org/research/stats/387.html American Council of Life Insurance. (1999). ACLI life insurance fact book 1999. Washington, DC: American Council of Life Insurance. Anderson, K G., Kaplan, H., Lam, D, & Lancaster, J. (1999). Paternal care by genetic fathers and stepfathers II: Reports by Xhosa high school students. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 433-451. Anderson, K G., Kaplan, H., & Lancaster, J. (1999). Paternal care by genetic fathers and stepfathers I: Reports from Albuquerque men. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 404-431. Andersson, M. (1994). Sexual selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Andrews, P. W., Gangestad, S. W., & Matthews, D. (in press). Adaptationsim— How to carry out an exaptationist program. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. www.bbsonline.org/Preprints/Andrews/ Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 38 Archer, J. (1996). Sex differences in social behavior: Are the social role and evolutionary explanations compatible? American Psychologist, 51, 909-917. Barkow, J. H. (1989). Darwin, sex, and status: Biological approaches to mind and culture. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. Barney, L. (2002, September 30). New York Life Insurance Launches $43M Campaign. Mutual Fund Market News. Retrieved October 8, 2002, from http://0web.lexis-nexis.com.catalog.lib.cmich.edu/universe/form/academic/s_indnews.html Betzig, L. L. (1986). Despotism and differential reproduction: A Darwinian view of history. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine. Boyd, R., & Richerson, P J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Brandweek. (1993, February 8). Mattel’s Barbie’s got a new billion-dollar figure, 32. Buckley, K. W. (1982). The selling of a psychologist: John Broadus Watson and the application of behavioral techniques to advertising. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 18, 207-221. Burnham, T., & Phelan, J. (2000). Mean genes. Cambridge, MA: Perseus. Buss, D. M. (1994). The evolution of desire. New York. Basic Books. Buss, D. M. (1995). Evolutionary psychology: A new paradigm for psychological science. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 1-30. Buss, D. M. (1999). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 39 Calvacca, L. (1998, February 1). Service, with a smile. Folio: The Magazine for Magazine Management, 27, 27-28. Campbell, A. (1999). Staying alive: Evolution, culture, and women’s intrasexual aggression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 203-252. Campbell, D. T. (1975). On the conflicts between biological and social evolution and between psychology and moral tradition. American Psychologist, 30, 1103-1126. Campos, L. S., Otta, & Sequeira, J O. (2002). Sex differences in mate selection strategies: Content analyses and responses to personal advertisements in Brazil. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 395-406. Case, A., Lin, I., & McLanahan, S. (1999). Household resource allocation in stepfamilies: Darwin reflects on the plight of Cinderella. American Economic Review Papers and Proceedings, 234-238 Case, A., Lin, I., & McLanahan, S. (2000). How hungry is the selfish gene? Economic Journal, 110, 781-804. Case, A., Lin, I., & McLanahan, S. (2001). Educational attainment of siblings in stepfamilies. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 269-289. Case, A., & Paxson, C. (2001). Mothers and others: who invests in children’s health? Journal of Health Economics, 20, 301-328. Caudle, F. M. (1994). National boundaries in magazine advertising: Perspectives on Verbal and nonverbal communication. In B. G. Englis (Ed.), Global and multinational advertising (pp. 117-140). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 40 Chadderdon, L. (1998, September). Blurb buddies. Fast Company, 20, 54. Retrieved May 25, 2001, from the World Wide Web: http//www.fastcompany.com/ online/20/cdu.html. Chandler, A. D. (Ed.) (1964). Giant enterprise: Ford, General Motors, and the automobile industry. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World. Colarelli, S. M. (1998). Psychological interventions in organizations: An evolutionary perspective. American Psychologist, 53, 1044-1056. Colarelli, S. M. (2000). Evolution, the criterion problem, and complexity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 151-152. Colarelli, S. M., Alampay, M. R., & Canali, K. G. (2002a). Letters of recommendation: An evolutionary perspective. Human Relations, 55, 315-344. Colarelli, S. M., & Alampay, M. R., & Canali, K. G. (2002b). Sex composition and influence in small groups: An evolutionary perspective and review of the literature. Manuscript submitted for publication. Colarelli, S. M. (2003). No best way: An evolutionary perspective on human resource management. Greenwich, CT: Praeger. Coleman, J. S. (1965). Adolescents and the schools. New York: Basic Books. Colford, P. D. (2002, October 17). Time Inc. to pull plug on Sports Illustrated Women Magazine. Daily News (NY). Collegesavings.org. (2002). All states offer Section 529 state college savings plans. www.collegesavings.org/All50.htm. Corazza, K. (1984). Small Business Boom Boon to Publishers. Advertising Age, 70, 48-49. Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 41 Cosmetic Market (2002). Retrieved October 15, 2002, from the World Wide Web: http://www.cosmeticmarket.com Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Cognition, 31, 187-276. Cronin, H. (1991). The ant and the peacock: Altruism and sexual selection from Darwin to today. New York: Cambridge University Press. Dawkins, R. (1976/1989). The selfish gene (rev. ed). New York: Oxford University Press. Dawkins, R. (1986). The blind watchmaker. New York: Oxford University Press. Dell Computers. Retrieved September 20, 2002, from the World Wide Web: http://www.dell.com Dennett, D. C. (1995). Darwin’s dangerous idea: Evolution and the meanings of life. New York: Touchstone. DeVaney, S. A., Chien, Y. (2002). Children’s education as the most important savings goal. Journal of Family and Consumer Sciences, 94, 64-70. de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Evolutionary psychology: The wheat and the chaff. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11,187-191. DiClemente, D. F., & Hantula, D. A. (2000, April). John Broadus Watson, I-O psychologist. The Industrial and Organizational Psychologist. http://www.siop.org/tip/backissues/TipApril00/7Diclemente.htm Discount Store News. (1996, May 20). What’s hot in books, 31. Economist. (1993, June 5). Her passion embraced a new adventure, 70. Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 42 Economist. (2002, August 31). United States: Come on in; The great food debate, 35. Ellis, B. J. (1992). The evolution of sexual attraction: Evaluative mechanisms in women. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 267288). London: Oxford University Press. Euromonitor International. World marketing and data statistics (for Central Michigan University). Retrieved October 9, 2002, from World Wide Web: http://0-www.euromonitor.com.catalog.lib.cmich.edu/womdas/ Feigenbaum, A. (2002, Oct.). Wheeling and redealing. Bloomberg Wealth Manager, 43-46. Fine, J. (2000, August 21). Online bridal arena heats up. Advertising Age, 71, 36, 46. Fisher, H. (1999). The first sex. New York: Random House. Fitzgerald, K. (1993, February 8). Toyland’s elusive goal – win over both sexes. Advertising Age, 64, S2, S18. Frank, R. H. (1999). Luxury fever: Why money fails to satisfy in an era of excess. New York: The Free Press. Garfield, B. (1999, July 5). So cute, but someone please save the baby. Advertising Age, 70, 29. Gaulin, S J., & McBurney, D. H. (2001). Psychology: An evolutionary approach. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Geary, D. C. (1995). Reflections of evolution and culture in children’s cognition: implications for development and instruction. American Psychologist, 50, 24-37. Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 43 Geary, D. C. (1998). Male/female. Washington, D.C. American Psychological Association Press. General Motors Corporation. Retrieved September 05, 2002, from the World Wide Web: www.gmc.com Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., & The ABC Research Group (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart. New York: Oxford University Press. Gladieux, L. E., & Hauptman, A. M. (1995). The college aid quandary: Access, quality, and the federal role. Washington, DC/New York: The Brookings Institution/The College Board. Goodrum, C., & Dalrymple, H. (1990). Advertising in America. New York: Harry N. Abrams, Inc. Gorn, G. J. (1982). The effects of music in advertising on choice behavior: A classical conditioning approach. Journal of Marketing, 46, 94-101. Gould, S. J., & Minowa, Y. (1994). Are They Saying the same Thing? An Exploratory Study of Japanese and American Automobile Advertising. In B. G. Englis (Ed.), Global and multinational advertising (pp. 193-204). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Greenlees, I. A., & McGrew, W. C. (1994). Sex and age differences in preferences and tactics of mate attraction: Analysis of published advertisements. Ethology and Sociobiology, 15, 59-72. Grimm, M. (2002). Insurance gets hip. American Demographics, 24, 48-49. Gruenwedel, E. (2000, October 30). Tricks of the trade. Adweek, 41, IQ48-IQ54. Hamilton, J., Knox, B., Hill, D., & Parr, H. (2000). Reduced fat products – Consumer perceptions and preferences. British Food Journal, 102, 494-506. Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 44 Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The general evolution of social behavior. I and II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1-52. Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F.J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 165-196. Hire, P. (1986). Playing for record Sales. Marketing, 11, 59-62. Högberg, U., & Bronström, G. (1985). The demography of maternal mortality— Seven Swedish parishes in the 19th century. International Journal of Gynecology and Obstetrics, 23, 489-407. Howell, D. (2000, Oct. 2). Snack food resurgence puts Kabash on health trend. Retailing Today, 48-50. Jones, E. R. (1979). Those Were The Good Old Days. New York: Simon and Schuster Publishing. Kaplan, S. (1992). Environmental preference in a knowledge-seeking, knowledgeusing organism. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, and J.Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 581-598). New York: Oxford University Press. Kavanagh, M. (1999, May 27). Porn will continue to dominate web revenue. Marketing Week, 22, 43. Kloss, I. (2001). Advertising Worldwide. Heidelberg, Germany: Springer-Verlag. Kramer, L. (1997, January 6). Nation’s Restaurant News, 1-79. Kurlansky, M. (2002). Salt: A world history. New York: Walker and Company. Lee, M., & Johnson, C. (1999). Principles of Advertising A Global Perspective. Binghamton, NY: The Hartworth Press. Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 45 Leifer, R., McDermott, C. M., Colarelli-O’Connor, G., Peters, L. S., Rice, M., P. & Veryzer, R. W. (2000). Radical innovation. Boston, MA: Harvard Business School Press. Leiss, W., Kline, S., & Jhally, S. (1986). Social communication in advertising: Persons, products, and images of well-being. New York, NY: Methuen Publications. LIMRA (2000). Trends in U. S. household life insurance ownership: The changing mix in coverage. Hartford, CT: Author. Lindblom, C. E., & Cohen, D. K. (1979). Usable knowledge. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Loth, D. (1961). The erotic in literature. New York: Julian Messner. Magazines.org. Retrieved October 31, 2002, from the World Wide Web: http://www.magazines.org. Mandese, J. (1990, March). The marketing successes of 1989 – Toys: Playing favorites. Marketing & Media Decisions, 25, 75-79. Marketing News (1986, April 11). Asian Americans Targeted by Insurance Firm. Marketing News, 24. Marketing News. (1986, April 11). Asian Americans targeted by insurance firm, 24. McDaniel, M. A., Whetzel, D. L., Schmidt, F. L., & Mauer, S. D. (1994). The validity of employment interviews: A comprehensive review and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 599-616. Medawar, P. B. (1981). The uniqueness of the individual (2nd ed.). New York: Dover. Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 46 Messaris, P. (1997). Visual persuasion: The role of images in advertising. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Michigan Education Savings Plan. Retrieved October 22, 2002 form the World Wide Web: www.misaves,com. Miller, G. F. (2000). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. New York: Doubleday. Morgan, R. (2002, August 9). The ultimate PC professor? Chronicle of Higher Education, A6. Myers, G. (1996, January). She works without a net. American Demographics, 18, 18. Orians, G. H., & Heerwagen, J. H. (1992). Evolved responses to landscapes. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 555-580). New York: Oxford University Press. Pawlowski, B., Dunbar, R. I. M., & Lipowicz, A. (2000). Tall men have more reproductive success. Nature, 403, 156. Pawlowski, B., & Koziel, S. (2002). The impact of traits offered in personal advertisements on response rates. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 139-149. Penton-Voak, I. S., and others. (1999). Menstrual cycle alters face preference. Nature, 399, 741-742. Perusse, D. (1993). Cultural and reproductive success in industrial societies: Testing the relationship at proximate and ultimate levels. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 267-322. Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 47 Pinker, S. (2002). The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. New York: Viking. Pinker, S., & Bloom, P. (1992). Natural language and natural selection. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 451-493). London: Oxford University Press. Pocock, G., & Richards, C. (1999). Human physiology: The basis of medicine. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press Inc. Rajala, A. K., & Hantula, D. A. (2000). Towards a behavioral ecology of consumption: Delay-reduction effects on foraging in a simulated Internet mall. Managerial and Decision Economics, 21, 145-158. Reda, S. (1994). Big stores and little kids. Stores, 76, 22-24. Rijkens, R. (1992). European Advertising Strategies. London: Cassell. Rozin, P. (1976). Psychological and cultural determinants of food choice. In T. Silverstone (Eds.), Appetite and food intake (pp. 286-312). Berlin: Dahlem Konferezen. Saad, G., & Gill, R. (2000). Applications of evolutionary psychology in marketing. Psychology & Marketing, 17, 1005-10035. Scotti, E. (1998, March 16). Born to be mild, or Wild? Brandweek, 39, 22-23. Sherry, J. F., Jr. (1991). Postmodern alternatives: The interpretative turn in consumer research. In T. S. Robertson and H. H. Kassarjian (Eds.), Handbook of consumer behavior (pp. 548-591). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Shermach, K. (1994, October 10). Reassure parents the product’s good for baby. Marketing New, 28, 1, 6. Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 48 Smith, M., Kish, B., & Crawford, C. (1987). Inheritance of wealth as human kin investment. Ethology & Sociobiology, 8, 171-182. Steinauer, J. (2002). Castles in the sky. Incentive, 176 (5), 22-23. Successful Meetings. (2001, May 17). Away from the madding crowd, 10-17 Taylor, A. J., & Linforth, R. S. T. (1998). Understanding flavor release: The key to better food flavor? Nutrition and Food Science, 4, 202-206. Thornhill, R. (1998). Darwinian aesthetics. In C. Crawford and D. L. Krebs (Eds). Handbook of evolutionary psychology: Ideas, issues, and applications (pp. 543572). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990). On the universality of human natures and the uniqueness of the individual: The role of genetics and adaptation. Journal of Personality, 58, 17-67. Trieman, D. J. (1977). Occupational prestige in comparative perspective. New York: Academic Press. Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of man 1871-1971 (pp. 136-179). Chicago: Aldine. United States Department of Labor, Bureau of Labor Statistics. (1997). Percent of Full-Time Employees Participating in Selected Employee Benefit Programs, Medium And large Private Establishments 1997 – Table 1. Retrieved October 2, 2002, from http://www.bls.gov/news.release/ebs3.toc.htm Vail, K. (2002). Higher costs put higher ed out of reach. American School Board Journal, 189, 20-21. Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 49 Veblen, T. (1899/1953). The theory of the leisure class. New York. Mentor. Verburg, P. (2002, March 18). Party time! Canadian Business, 27. Wahid, A. (1999, September-October). The Grameen Bank and women in Bangladesh, Challenge, 94-101. Weiss, M. (2001). Online America. American Demographics, 23, 53-60. Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection: A critique of some current evolutionary thought. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Williams, G. C. (1996). Plan and purpose in nature. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson. Wilson, M. (1950/1964). Nyakyusa kinship. In A. R. Radcliffe-Brown and D. Forde (Eds.), African systems of kinship and marriage (pp. 111-139). London: Oxford University Press. Wilson, N., Quigley, R., & Mansoor, O. (1999). Food ads on TV: A health hazard for children? Australian & New Zealand Journal of Public Health, 23, 647-650. Winterhalder, B., & Smith, E. A. (1981). Hunter-gatherer foraging strategies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Withgott, J. (2002). Cool cats lose out in the mane event. Science, 297, 12551256. Yunus, M. (1999, November). The Grameen Bank. Scientific American, 114119. Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection—A selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53, 205-214. Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives Zangwill, W. I. (1993, March 8). When customer research is a lousy idea. Wall Street Journal, A10. Zangwill, W. I. (1992). Lightning Strategies for Innovation. New York: Lexington Books Zvoch, K. (1999). Family type and investment in education: A comparison of genetic and stepparent families. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 453-464. 50 Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives 51 Table 1 Psychological Mechanisms ____________________________________________________________________ Psychological Mechanism Functions Author(s) __________________________________________________________________________________ Altruism towards kin Increase inclusive fitness Hamilton (1964) Prestige seeking and imitating high-prestige individuals Resource acquisition and efficiently identifying useful skills, values, and beliefs Help ensure sufficient resources in uncertain environments Barkow (1989), Henrich and Gil-White, 2001 Preference for foods rich in fats and sugar Increase caloric intake Rozin (1976) Enjoy the taste of salt Maintain sufficient sodium chloride in the body Alcock, (1993) Palmer and Palmer (2002) Fear of snakes Landscape preferences for savanna-like environments Avoid poison Motivate individuals to select habitats that provide resources and offer protection Marks (1987) Kaplan (1992) Orians and Heerwagen (1992) Female mate preference for Economic resources Provisioning for children Buss (1989) Male mate preferences for youth, attractiveness, and waist-to-hip ratio Select mates of high fertility Buss (1989) Singh (1993) Superior female spatial-location Memory Increase success at foraging/ Gathering Silverman and Eals (1992) Male sexual jealousy Increase paternity certainty Symons (1979) Natural language Communication/manipulation Pinker and Bloom (1992) Detection of cheaters Prevent being exploited in social contracts Cosmides (1989) Male desire for sexual variety Motivate access to more sexual partners Symons (1979) Preference for risk to avoid loss Adapted from Buss (1995) Kahneman and Tversky, (1981); Moore (1996) Intuitive Evolutionary Perspectives Author Note We would like to thank Jeff Connor and Derek Gager for their invaluable bibliographic assistance in the preparation or this article. Address correspondence to Stephen M. Colarelli, Department of Psychology, Central Michigan University, Mt. Pleasant, MI 48859; electronic mail can be sent to: colar1sm@cmich.edu. 52