First and Second language acquisition Language Acquisition is the process through which a human being gets to know the grammar/vocabulary of a specific language. First language acquisition • Why are we interested in it? Some obvious and yet interesting facts about child language acquisition: • Every aspect of language is extremely complex; yet very young children (before the age of 5) already know most of the intricate system we call the grammar of a language. • Children do not learn a language by storing all the words and all the sentences in some giant mental dictionary. The list of words is finite, but the sentences are infinite in number. • Children learn to construct/understand sentences, most of which they have never produced/heard before. • Children must therefore construct the ‘rules’ that permit them to use language creatively. No one teaches them these rules. Their parents are no more aware of the phonological, morphological, syntactic, and semantic rules than are the children. Theories of Child Language Acquisition 1. Learning by Imitation Some people think that children acquire a language merely by imitation. Is this a plausible hypothesis? • • This is not a plausible hypothesis because: Children do not hear many of the utterances they produce (e.g. Cat stand up table); Even when they try to imitate what they hear, they are unable to produce sentences that cannot be generated by their grammar (e.g. ADULT: He’s going out. CHILD: He go out.); • Children who are unable to speak for neurological or physiological reasons learn the language spoken to them and when they overcome their speech impairment, they immediately use the language for speaking. 2. Learning by Reinforcement Another view of language acquisition suggests that children learn to produce grammatical sentences because they are positively reinforced when they say something right and negatively reinforced when they say something wrong. Is this a plausible hypothesis? • No, because: Brown and his colleagues report from their studies that reinforcement seldom occurs, and when it does it is usually incorrect pronunciation or incorrect reporting of facts that is corrected, rather than syntax. Even if it did occur often, it wouldn’t explain how or what children learn from such adult responses. In fact, attempts to correct a child’s language seem to be doomed to failure! 3. Learning by Analogy • It has been suggested that children learn how to put words together to form phrases and sentences by analogy, by hearing a sentence and using it as a sample to form other sentences. Is this a plausible hypothesis? • Lila Gleitman (1994) nicely argues against this hypothesis: -Suppose the child has heard the sentence “I painted a red barn” By analogy s/he can say: “I painted a blue barn.” So far, so good! In addition, s/he might also hear “I painted a barn red.” So, it looks as if you can take the last two words and switch them around. The child hears the sentence: “I saw a red barn.” By analogy, s/he may construct the sentence “I saw a barn red!” The analogy doesn’t work!! None of the above theories can account for: -the non-random mistakes children make, -the speed with which the basic rules of grammar are acquired, -the ability to learn language without any formal instruction, -and the regularity of the acquisition process across languages and environmental circumstances. 4. The Innateness Hypothesis Noam Chomsky: “What accounts for the ease, rapidity, and uniformity of language acquisition in the face of impoverished data?” • The answer is Universal Grammar or UG Poverty-of-the-stimulus: • The linguistic input a child is exposed to simply isn’t explicit or rich enough for a child to deduce the rules of the language based on experience alone. • Consider, for example, the following sentences: (The question is how the child comes to acquire the contrasts below:) 1. I wonder who [the men expected to see them] 1’. The men expected to see them. 2. John ate the apples vs. 2’. John ate. 3. John is too stubborn to talk to Bill. 3’. John is too stubborn to talk to. • How are the pronouns below interpreted? (They are very complex!) 4. John believes he is intelligent. he = John or someone else 4’. John believes him to be intelligent. him = NOT John Note: Children are not explicitly taught/trained to understand the differences. • Noam Chomsky suggested the following solution for the above problem in the mid/late 50’s: The knowledge is innate (or built in). This innate knowledge is also known as Universal Grammar (UG). This approach to Linguistics now has many proponents, known as generative linguists. • UG consists of some universal principles and a set of grammatical options (known as parameters). The child’s task is to set the parameters via exposure to data. • UG Data Grammar of a particular language The Stages of Child Language Acquisition • • The earliest period: cooing the noises produced by infants in all language communities sound the same. infants will increase their sucking rate when stimuli (visual or auditory) presented to them are varied, but will decrease the sucking rate when the same stimuli are presented over and over again. Infants respond to phonetic contrasts as well, so a • • change from [pa] [pa] [pa] [pa] to [ba] results in increased rate of sucking. infants respond to phonetic contrasts that are not phonemic in the language spoken in the baby’s home. So, for instance, Japanese infants can distinguish between [r] and [l], while their parents can’t. Or the alveolar ‘t’ and the retroflexed ‘t’ which exist in Hindi, sound the same to English-speaking parents but they sound different to their infants! babies do not respond to sound signals that never signal phonemic contrasts in any human language. While [i] pronounced by a male is physically different from the same sound pronounced by a female, the baby, just like an adult, ignores the difference. No change in sucking rate! They seem to be born with the ability to recognize linguistic sounds! • After 6 months: babbling From about 6 months on, babies begin to lose their ability to discriminate between sounds that are not phonemic in their language. Japanese infants no longer hear the difference between [r] and [l]. Around the same time, infants start to babble. • Some time after one year: children begin to use the same string of sounds repeatedly to mean the same thing. They are producing their first words! Before the age of two, children learn a lot of words. According to some estimates, children learn a new word every two hours! Around two (18-20 months), they enter a new stage, the two-word stage. There are no inflections for number, person, tense, etc. at this stage. • Between two and two and a half years of age, a child’s expressions become considerably more complex The morphemes and grammatical structures are generally acquired by children in a set order (see textbook). Interestingly, this order does not correlate with frequency of parental use! The most frequent of the morphemes in parents’ speech is the articles, which is learned rather late. Overgeneralization is common in child language acquisition (e.g. foots, eated, etc.) Critical Period It has been suggested that there is a critical period for language acquisition (usually around puberty). During this period, language learning proceeds easily, rapidly, and without external intervention. • There have been a number of cases of children raised in environments of extreme social isolation. These children never fully developed language. A fairly recent case was Genie, who was found in 1970 confined to a small room under conditions of physical restraint and had received only minimal human contact from the age of 18 months to 14 years. Later she did acquire language to some extent but her language lacked auxiliary verbs, the third person singular –s, the past tense marker, and most pronouns. She did not invert subjects to make questions. Such cases support the critical age hypothesis. Receptive Competence vs. Productive Competence • While most language acquisition research is based on a child’s productive competence, it should be noted that a child may in fact have the right grammar (receptive competence) and yet not be able to produce the right utterance due to physiological immaturity, etc. In other word’s we judge by the children’s production. An often cited example is the ‘FIS’ phenomenon: A child pronounced fish as fis, but objected to an adult imitating the fis pronunciation. ‘This is your fis?’ the adult asked. ‘No,’ said the child: ‘my fis’. When the adult eventually said ‘Your fish?” the child concurred: ‘Yes, my fis’! • - Not only that, but we often judge by our perception of their production! Take the word ‘pig’. It is often said that children do not distinguish voiced from voiceless initial stops, pronouncing ‘pig’ and ‘big’ alike. However, the fact is that even adults anticipate the voicing of the vowel in ‘pig’ and thus pronounce something like ‘pbig’. The crucial point is that the voicing is delayed enough for us to perceive it as voiceless, as opposed to ‘big’. Laboratory analyses show that some children have the same delaying of voicing but it is simply not long enough for adults to perceive it! WUG Test In 1958, J. Berko conducted a study that has now become a classic in our understanding of child language acquisition. She showed her child subjects a drawing of a nonsense animal and gave the animal a nonsense name. She would then elicit the plural form for the animal’s name. She showed that the children used the regular English plural marker to make the plural. Moreover, while the plural for wug would be wug[z], the plural for bik would be bik[s]. Please see videos at : http://www.ling.umd.edu/labs/acquisition/videos/index.html Second Language Acquisition Our focus in this part is on a second language acquired/learned by an adult. Important concepts: -As opposed to first language acquisition, second language acquisition is very difficult, very slow and (arguably) nonuniform. Why? What is essentially different? Does a L2 learner have access to UG? Three different approaches: 1. No access. 2. Direct access. 3. Indirect access. • Interlanguage: An interlanguage in that form of the target language that a learner has internalized. Some researchers view second-language learners as developing a series of interlanguages in their progression towards mastery of the target grammar. The interlanguage contains: rules of the first language, overgeneralizations, rules of the target grammar and some rules of neither lanuage. • Fossilization: For various reasons, the second language-learning process slows down or stops at some point, leading to an interlanguage with features that differ from that of the target language. This is called fossilization. Methods in Second language teaching (see textbook page 193) Input and output (see textbook page 196) Communicative competence (see textbook page 197) • Next week : read textbook pages 175-201 ; Study for QUIZ 2 : Chapters : 9, 10, 11, 15, 16, 17