Self esteem in foster care Fostering Self-Esteem: Exploring Adult Recollections on the Influence of Foster Parents Nikki Luke Research assistant; University of Sussex, England Sarah M. Coyne Assistant professor; Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah, USA Correspondence concerning this article should be directed to: Nikki Luke, Department of Psychology, University of Sussex, Falmer, Brighton, East Sussex, BN1 9QH, England, Email: N.Luke@sussex.ac.uk; Phone: 01273 877698 1 Self esteem in foster care 2 Abstract Foster parents are in a unique position to improve the self-esteem of children in their care, which may be lower than that of their non-fostered peers. According to Harter’s (1986) dual-influence model, both general support/attachment and domain-specific support contribute to self-esteem. The current study used this model to explore the ways in which foster parents had influenced the self-esteem of a sample of five adults with differing foster care experiences. Retrospective interviews were used to gather memories of high self-esteem from time spent in foster care. A thematic analysis of the interview transcripts supported Harter’s (1986) model, and provided a number of examples of ways in which foster parents could boost children’s self-esteem. The model was further extended to show the importance of ‘normality’ and inclusion for this sample of fostered adults. Further research on this topic is recommended with a view to widening the scope of foster carer training beyond attachment theory. Keywords: Self-esteem, foster carers, support, attachment theory Self esteem in foster care 3 Feeling Good in Foster Care: Young Adults’ Recollections on the Sources of Self Esteem Self-esteem has been defined as the evaluative component of the self-concept (Blascovich & Tomaka, 1991), which originates in the internalisation of social interactions (Vygotsky, 1978). Theories of self-esteem commonly emphasise its social nature: “…a sense of inferiority is developmental or learned, rather than organic or innate” (Hamacheck, 1978, p. 255). This learning begins early: self-evaluation based on adult reactions emerges at around 18 months of age (Stipek et al., 1992). The impact of adults may be at its greatest in these early years, when parents are the major source of knowledge (Burns, 1979). Research has examined how parenting behaviour may be linked to children’s selfesteem. Studies have suggested that children’s self-esteem is positively linked to maternal affection and mother-child rapport (Coopersmith, 1967), and negatively linked to parental indifference (Liu, 2003) and a lack of family support (Cooper et al., 1983). If parents’ behaviour has the power to influence children’s self-esteem, it may be expected that children who have encountered adverse parenting experiences are at risk for low self-esteem. This presents a particular challenge for foster parents welcoming children into their care. The most up-to-date figures show that there were 42,300 children in foster care in England on 31st March 2007 (DfES, 2007). Furthermore, children are staying in care for increasingly long periods (DfES, 2006). Many children enter care having experienced neglect, abuse, and conflict (Mallon, 1992), which damages their sense of self-worth and leaves them feeling unlovable (DeRobertis, 2003; Kazdin et al., 1985; Schofield & Beek, 2005a). Removal from their families due to abandonment or forcible separation may also damage foster children’s self-esteem (Ackerman & Dozier, 2005). When foster placements combine the loss of parents with the loss of peer relationships and community, self-esteem is further threatened (Lyman & Bird, 1996). Self esteem in foster care 4 Regrettably, there is some evidence that foster children continue to suffer from low self-esteem while in care (Ackerman & Dozier, 2005), and that they display lower levels of self-esteem than non-fostered youths (Gil & Bogart, 1982; Hicks & Nixon, 1989; Kaufman & Cicchetti, 1989). Other studies have found no difference between the self-esteem of foster children and that of non-fostered control groups (Flynn et al., 2004; Lyman & Bird, 1996) or foster parents’ own children (Denuwelaere & Bracke, 2007). However, differences in the age and experience of the samples and the use of various methods make it hard to compare these studies directly. If foster parents work on the assumption that children entering their care are at least ‘at risk’ of low self-esteem, it is important that they act to tackle this situation. Studies with fostered and maltreated samples have shown that low self-esteem is linked to anxiety, aggression, and teenage motherhood (Herrenkohl et al., 1998; Legault et al., 2006). In addition, studies in which care status is not reported have shown that low self-esteem predicts problem eating (McGee & Williams, 2000), depression and delinquency (Rosenberg et al., 1989), adolescent psychiatric disorders (Guillon et al., 2003), and adult mental and physical health problems and criminality (Trzesniewski et al., 2007). Foster parents can have a positive and lasting effect on children’s self-esteem, perhaps because the child perceives the support they offer as both important and novel (Denuwelaere & Bracke, 2007). Studies by Ackerman and Dozier (2005) and Schofield and Beek (2005b) have shown improvements in children’s self-esteem when foster carers provided acceptance, a secure base, and sensitive parenting. If foster parents hope to improve the self-esteem of children in their care, it is important that they are aware of the best practical methods for achieving this. However, the specific procedures recommended depend largely on which theory is used to explain the development of self-esteem. Attachment theory Self esteem in foster care 5 Attachment theory (Bowlby, 1964, 1973) is currently “the key theoretical perspective informing child-care decision making” (Kelly, 2000, p. 20). According to attachment theory, children who are uncared for, or who are removed from their mother-figures, are liable to feel anxious and guilty and to crave love. An unwanted child will develop internal working models of their parents as unwanting and themselves as unwantable. Rutter (1981) suggested that these negative effects may be worse when the child experiences both separation from the attachment figure and a strange environment, a combination of factors common to children entering foster care. From the perspective of attachment theory, foster carers are advised to boost foster children’s self-esteem by establishing an attachment bond, and by offering general support and encouragement in order to assist the change to a positive working model of the self. However, an alternative theory of self-esteem exists which is not currently covered by foster care training. Domain theory Harter (1982, 1983) claimed that high self-esteem is a result of competence or success in specific domains which the individual feels are important. These domains are drawn from: scholastic competence, athletic competence, social acceptance, physical appearance, and behavioural conduct. General self-worth has been shown to be largely dependent on discrepancies between performance in these areas and their importance to the child (Harter, 1986). The positive effect of domain-specific assistance on children’s self-esteem has been shown in a variety of studies, for example in Rhodes et al.’s (1999) work on mentors for fostered youths. From this point of view, the role of foster carers is to determine the child’s most valued domains, and to provide support and encouragement in these specific areas, reflecting Self esteem in foster care 6 the ‘targeted’ approach advocated by Gilligan (1999). However, it is not necessarily the case that self-esteem must be explained exclusively by reference to attachment or domains. Dual-influence model Harter (1986) proposed a model in which self-worth is equally influenced by both the cognitive-analytic aspect of salient domain performance (Harter, 1982, 1983), and the more general social support/positive regard advocated by Bowlby’s (1964, 1973) attachment theory. As yet this dual-influence model has not been explicitly applied to the study of foster children. Aims The present study aimed to explore the ways in which a small sample of adults felt their self-esteem had been influenced by foster parents, using Harter’s (1986) dual-influence model of self-esteem as a theoretical basis. If domain-specific support was found to have influenced self-esteem as well as general support and attachment, this could be used to prompt further research on what constitutes ‘good’ foster parenting. A retrospective qualitative interview was used to elicit participants’ feelings on the links between their experiences in foster care and their self-esteem. Retrospective interviews can produce narratives which illuminate the ways in which experiences in foster care continue to have meaning for individuals, even into adulthood (Schofield, 2002). They have been used to explore identity issues for fostered and adopted adults (Triseliotis, 1984), and the effects of psychological maltreatment (DeRobertis, 2003). The current study, however, was the first of its kind to use a retrospective interview to explore the influence of foster parents on selfesteem. The study explored two related research questions: Research Question 1: What kind of experiences will adults raised in foster care recall as examples of times when they felt high self-esteem? Self esteem in foster care 7 Research Question 2: Can foster parents influence the self-esteem of children in their care by providing support in specific areas as well as by offering general support and attachment? Method Participants The sample consisted of five adults with experience of living in foster care. Participants varied in the number of foster placements they had experienced, from two to “around 25”. Nine potential participants were recruited from a number of sources, including a local support group, managers at two local councils, and a local newspaper appeal for volunteers. Responses were received from five of the nine potential participants, giving a response rate of 56%. The final sample consisted of the four volunteers who had responded to newspaper appeals, plus one young person recruited via a Leaving Care team. All participants were white and British. The sample included four males and one female, aged between 18 and 46 years. While the sample size was limited, similar small-scale approaches have been used previously in qualitative research with looked-after children (e.g. Leeson, 2007). Materials Participants’ experiences of self-esteem while living in foster care were assessed by means of a semi-structured interview. The interview schedule was based on one used with non-fostered samples by Jackson (1984) and Thorne and Michaelieu (1996). The schedule covered three main areas: foster placement details (age at entry into/exit from foster care, number of placements, details of longest placement), self-esteem over time (feelings of selfworth at entry into care and now, any sense of change), and memories of self-esteem (recollections of times in foster care when participants felt good about themselves, ways in which foster carers acted or could have acted to make the child feel good). The semi- Self esteem in foster care 8 structured nature of the schedule allowed for further questioning and probing of participants’ responses. Interviews were recorded using a digital voice recorder. Procedure Potential participants were sent a letter describing the research and were asked to sign a consent form if they wished to take part. The researcher then contacted participants to arrange an interview. Interviews took place in private: three in participants’ homes, one in a local family centre, and another in a lab room at the University. An interview schedule was used to guide the interview, but any interesting points were followed up with further questioning. Interview length was determined by participants’ responses, as in previous studies by Thorne and Michaelieu (1996) and Ward et al. (2005). The shortest interview took 16 minutes, while the longest ran to 66 minutes. Analytic Strategy After each interview, recordings were transcribed by the researcher and subjected to a thematic analysis. Each transcript was read through several times to determine areas of similarity and difference, resulting in a set of preliminary ‘themes’ or categories. Sections of text were then coded according to these categories. Where difficulties arose in deciding between categories, the full list of categories was re-examined and a number of categories were combined or redefined. Transcripts were then re-coded, with any further ambiguities leading to another examination of the categories. This process continued until the researcher had produced a final list of categories into which sections of text could be easily coded. A second coder chose a random section from each participant’s transcript to code independently, covering approximately 10 per cent of the total material coded. Inter-coder reliability was acceptable throughout (κ = 0.879). Results Self esteem in foster care 9 Participants were allocated codenames: ‘PA’ (Participant A), ‘PD’, ‘PF’, ‘PH’, and ‘PI’. A thematic analysis of the interview transcripts revealed a number of themes on which participants’ experiences and thoughts could be compared. Related themes were grouped together under four headings: Self-esteem when Entering Care, General Support/Attachment, Domain-Specific Support, and Normality and Inclusion. Examples of each theme are given throughout. Self-Esteem when Entering Care Interviewees were asked how they felt about themselves when entering foster care. One participant had entered his long-term placement as a baby so could not answer. The remaining four all described negative feelings: “I felt unwanted, I felt useless…” (PA) “Hated myself.” (PD) For two of the participants, a poor sense of self-worth at this time was linked to self-blame: “And that [mother’s violence] obviously makes you feel like crap, she’s supposed to be your mam and supposed to love you… for a few years I thought it was all my fault and it was all what I’d done, so I felt useless…” (PA) “I felt like it was my fault that I lived in foster care, and I felt bad because I thought it was my fault that my sister lived there as well.” (PH) To investigate the ways in which foster carers could boost children’s self-esteem, participants were asked to recall times whilst in foster care when they had felt good about themselves. In line with Harter’s (1986) model, their responses showed that both General support/attachment and Domain-specific support were important. General Support/Attachment Self esteem in foster care 10 Love and support were central themes in all of the participants’ recollections. Both PA and PI had enjoyed loving, supportive long-term placements. For PA, this had helped him through some difficult times: “And over the past 7 years I’ve had a lot of things that have happened, quite a lot of bad things like my granddad died… But with the support I’ve got with my foster parents it helped me a lot to see things through…” (PA) The same participant went on to articulate some of the ways in which foster parents had shown this love: “There was a lot of physical affection, which was ideal for an 11 year old kid, it was good to have a hug, and good for them to say ‘I love you, we’re proud of you.’…It was good to be told that you were loved, cause obviously, being in foster care, at times it’s quite lonely…It was good to feel the love in different ways.” (PA) As well as affection and verbal expressions of love, foster parents had helped PA feel the positive benefits of support and attachment by listening to his needs and acting on them. Regrettably, there were more references to a lack of general support and attachment in participants’ accounts than to their presence. PA’s successful second placement was contrasted with his first: “Sometimes, as a child, you just want to sit and have a hug…and it was impossible… there was no emotional support there, there was no-one to sit and listen…” (PA) The expression of emotional support as expressed in a willingness and availability to talk through problems had also been missing for PH: “[W]hen I fell out with my dad they could have talked to me about it instead of just not acknowledging it.” PF described how severe problems of insecurity and low self-worth had plagued him throughout his childhood and into adult life. He felt that these problems were directly Self esteem in foster care 11 attributable to the lack of stable support and attachment in his childhood, both with his birth family and in subsequent foster placements: “It’s a basic human need to feel loved, wanted, accepted, warmth… And if those are missing there are going to be problems.” (PF) PD had experienced the largest number of placements, and had built an attachment with only one carer, although this too had broken down after he left the family. Asked what the ‘ideal carer’ could have offered, PD’s response was clear: “I reckon you need love and support, that’d be the main thing, but there’s a million ways that you can show it… I think that’s probably about the only thing you actually need. It’s the only thing that a real family can possibly give you...” (PD) Domain-Specific Support As well as more general support and attachment, several participants’ self-esteem memories showed that the way they had felt was influenced by the presence or absence of foster carers’ support in four of Harter’s (1982, 1983) specific domains: scholastic competence, athletic competence, social acceptance, and behavioural conduct (there was no spontaneous mention of physical appearance). PA had received a great deal of support in his academic career: “I wouldn’t have been able to achieve the grades I wanted to get without their constant nagging and constant pushing and pressure.” (PA) Importantly for PA, his foster father offered him targeted support in mathematics, an area in which he had previously struggled: “[H]is input and help on my homework was phenomenal… and his patience is unfounded, it’s brilliant. So their support means a lot to me, cos my mam and dad weren’t there for me educationally.” (PA) Self esteem in foster care 12 Participants for whom athletic competence was important also valued support in this area. For PI, this had come not from his foster parents, but from a teacher at school who recognised his potential as a runner and encouraged him to train during school lunch breaks. For PA, however, this was one area in which his foster parents could not support him. He described how he felt when they explained that they would be unable to support him in his blossoming football career due to financial constraints: “I took it personally that they didn’t love me and really want to support me, when in reality it was just that they couldn’t… I suppose I felt unwanted and neglected, to a certain extent…” (PA) Several participants expressed the importance of good behavioural conduct, shaped by the desire to avoid becoming someone who got into trouble and blamed it on their background. PI felt that his foster family had given him a strong sense of morality: “And the way that my brother and my mum have brought me up, it’s testament to them the way I’ve turned out today… Most of my mates have either gone into drugs or they’ve got criminal convictions... I never touched drugs. No criminal convictions…” (PI) For PA, being supported in good behavioural conduct was important because it helped remove him from the violence he had witnessed in his birth family: “I think well, am I a violent person, am I going to turn out to be my mam or turn out to be my dad… It’s worrying. But I know that with the support that I’ve got, of my foster parents, then I’m not going to be in the same situation as they are...” (PA) Supporting children in their attempts to be socially accepted was not a skill attributed to foster parents by this sample, although this was clearly an important area: Self esteem in foster care 13 “I think I find it hard to make friends, I don’t find it easy. Like, people at college have got friends from school and stuff and I haven’t cause I moved to different schools a lot.” (PH) For PH, it was the friends he made at the age of 10 who stopped him from hating himself and started making him feel good about himself. As well as Harter’s (1982, 1983) four domains, participants’ accounts also covered two other specific areas in which foster carers’ support was important for their sense of selfesteem: driving and acting lessons. PA’s foster father, a keen collector of classic cars, had sold his collection in order to buy PA a new car while he was learning to drive, which made him feel “honoured”. In contrast, PH had received no support from her foster carers when learning to drive: “They kept just saying that I should have passed first time… They made me feel like I was the only one who’d ever failed.” (PH) PA’s foster parents, and some of PD’s, had also supported their love of drama, taking them to lessons and helping them to learn their lines. For both participants drama was important as it offered them a sense of release from what was often a painful reality. PD, however, had not enjoyed this support in all of his placements. Some foster parents had refused to accommodate PD’s drama classes as they were held on the same night that the parents played bingo. Others told PD he was not allowed to pick Drama as a GCSE subject, which led to the breakdown of the placement. Subsequently, PD did not feel that his foster carers’ support of his drama lessons was particularly important: “I’d sooner just do it on my own and then I don’t get disappointed and they aren’t disappointed in me…” (PD) Normality and Inclusion Self esteem in foster care 14 While Harter’s (1986) dual-influence model covered many of the experiences recalled by adults in this sample, other memories highlighted the additional importance of a sense of normality and inclusion for foster children’s self-esteem. Participants described feeling negative when experiences highlighted their sense of ‘difference’ from other children. For PH, being different was particularly noticeable at school and at college where she had had to explain what it meant to be in foster care. One way in which foster parents could reduce this sense of difference and make children feel good about themselves was by making them feel part of a family unit: “[It] was the happiest period of my life, because we felt part of a family for the first time.” (PF) The two participants whose placements had lasted the longest (PA and PI) had foster carers who had welcomed them as part of the family, instilling in them a sense of belonging: “I knew in me as I was getting older that I wasn’t my mum’s proper child, but… the way my mum looks at me, I am her son.” (PI) The practical ways in which foster parents had made children feel part of the family differed. PA’s carers had ensured that each family member had a well-defined role with their own responsibilities. For both PA and PF, going on family outings and holidays had also made them feel included, with PF also noting enrolment into local Cub Scouts and Sunday School and calling his foster parents Mum and Dad as contributing to a sense of family. Receiving gifts from foster parents on special occasions had also been important for PH and PF. To ensure a complete sense of inclusion in the family, participants felt it was important that they also experienced the harsher side of family life. Both PH and PF claimed that equality with other family members in terms of the discipline received could make them feel loved. PH described what her ‘ideal’ foster family would do: Self esteem in foster care 15 “[W]hen you’ve done stuff good praise you, and when you’ve done stuff bad shout at you… Cause then you feel like they care about you, cause they don’t want you to get hurt and stuff, don’t want anything bad to happen to you.” (PH) Unfortunately, for PD and PH, feeling part of a family was something that had been lacking for most of their time in foster care. PH’s general feeling about living in foster care was one of exclusion: “Some of them don’t make it like it’s your home… so you’ve got to ask to get a drink or ask to have a shower… when everyone else just does what they want. And if they’ve got their own children there, you feel really different to them.” (PH) Summary In line with Harter’s (1986) model, participants’ narratives suggested that both general support/attachment and domain-specific support had influenced their self-esteem during their time in foster care. Figure 1 shows how the model could be extended to account for the additional importance of normality and inclusion for foster children. It should be noted that each of the factors in this model may have either a positive or negative influence on the child’s self-esteem, dependent on the child’s particular experience. --------------------------------------------Insert figure 1 about here-----------------------------------Discussion All but one of the participants in this study entered care feeling unworthy and inferior. Improving the self-esteem of children in foster care is an important task for foster parents because of the links between poor self-worth and negative outcomes. As the review of the literature showed, low self-esteem places children at risk for a range of physical, mental, emotional, and social problems. Although participants were not asked directly if they had experienced any of these problems, two of the interviewees mentioned spontaneously that they had attempted suicide at times when their self-esteem had been at its lowest. Self esteem in foster care 16 The foster parents described by this sample were in a unique position to boost children’s self-esteem. ‘Ideal’ foster carers struck a balance between responding to children’s neglected emotional needs and promoting age-appropriate social and academic skills (Schofield & Beek, 2005a). Regrettably, some foster parents seemed only to reinforce individuals’ negative self-concepts in their failure to meet support needs. The thematic analysis of the interview transcripts supported Harter’s (1986) dualinfluence model, as participants related self-esteem memories reflecting both general support/attachment and domain-specific support and encouragement. In addition, the interview analysis suggested that Harter’s model might be further extended when examining foster children’s self-esteem to incorporate the importance of being made to feel normal and included (see Figure 1 for a graphical representation of this view). The memories of adults in this sample provided a number of practical examples of ways in which foster parents can help to boost children’s self-esteem. In terms of general support and attachment, foster parents can offer physical and verbal expressions of their love and affection, be available when children need to talk, and show the child that they are a wanted member of the family and unconditionally accepted for who they are. For example, PA’s foster parents continued to offer him love and support despite his early violent behaviour. If foster parents offer such consistent messages of love and support, the child will begin to internalise a more positive self-concept which may help reshape their working model of the self as a person who is worthy of the love offered to them. Domain-specific support must be tailored to the individual child as particular areas may be more important for some children than for others. Foster parents need to talk to children to find out where they would most value help. Practical examples of specific support given here included instrumental help with mathematics homework, drama groups, and football training, as well as encouragement in learning to drive. In addition, teaching children Self esteem in foster care 17 a strong moral code encouraged a view of the self as a ‘good’ person. If foster parents offer domain-specific support in salient areas this can help the child’s actual performance approach their ideal standard. The reduction in the actual/ideal discrepancy can help the child to achieve a more positive cognitive self-appraisal and subsequently improve general selfworth. Finally, to foster a sense of normality and inclusion, foster parents can assign family members particular responsibilities, include children in family outings and holidays, give gifts to mark special occasions, treat all children in the household equally (including the use of discipline), and encourage involvement in community activities. By so doing, they can help to reduce any sense of ‘difference’ and make the child feel like a valued member of the family. This enhances felt security which can also contribute to improved self-esteem (Schofield & Beek, 2005a). In reviewing Harter’s (1982, 1983) domains, it is noteworthy that none of the participants in this sample mentioned receiving foster parents’ support in the area of social acceptance, although this was clearly something important to them. Encouraging foster children to build and maintain friendships could help to boost self-esteem as it reduces the sense of ‘difference’ from their peers and provides a valuable source of social support. Similarly, self-esteem memories relating to physical appearance did not feature in any of the participants’ accounts. This is particularly interesting given Harter’s (1999) finding that this was the domain most strongly correlated with a general sense of self-worth. It is possible that the adults in this sample had not received any support or encouragement regarding their physical appearance from foster parents; alternatively, instances of feeling good about one’s appearance may not be particularly memorable, or participants may have avoided relating them for fear of appearing immodest. Given the strength of its association with self-worth, Self esteem in foster care 18 however, it seems sensible to suggest that foster children may benefit from foster parents’ encouragement and praise regarding personal appearance. It has been debated whether all children need both general and domain-specific support at all times in order to enjoy high self-esteem (Cassidy, 1990). Accordingly, the accounts of the participants in this study suggested that attachment and general support may initially be the most important factors for children entering foster care. Once an attachment bond is established, further support and encouragement in salient areas may provide a further boost to self-esteem; however, without that bond, the importance of domain-specific support is discounted. This reflects the finding that support from important sources is more strongly correlated with self-worth than that from unimportant sources (Harter, 1999). Because PD had no attachment bond with most of his foster parents, they were not important to him and he did not value their support of his drama lessons. Instead, PD’s memories of high selfesteem related to his own efforts and the support of his friends. Overall, the experiences related by this group of adults suggest we can be optimistic about the potential for foster parents to improve children’s self-esteem. Although the support and encouragement necessary for fostering a positive sense of self-worth were not always present, individuals fortunate enough to have received such positive parenting continued to recognise its impact even in adulthood. Limitations The use of a small sample of five adults restricts the nature of this study to one of exploration. Although small samples allow for a deeper investigation of the data, they also limit the extent to which findings can be generalised. It is important to recognise that the experiences presented here relate to a particular group of individuals and care should be taken not to assume that this is a ‘true’ version of reality for all those experiencing foster care. Self esteem in foster care 19 The recruitment methods used in this study meant that the final sample was open to potential bias. The newspaper appeal may have attracted those who felt that they something important to say about their time in foster care, while Leaving Care teams could inevitably only suggest those who were still in contact with Social Services. In addition, by asking participants to recall instances of high self-esteem, there was a risk that they may only recall specific events (such as passing their driving test), as these would stand out more in the memory than the family background surrounding them. As it was, however, this did not occur. Finally, caution should be taken in assuming a direct cause-effect relationship between parenting behaviour and self-esteem in a retrospective study such as this. It may be the case that children with pre-existing low levels of self-esteem repel their parents and foster parents, making attachment difficult or impossible (Cassidy, 1990). Conclusion Interviews with the adults in this study suggested that foster carers can help to raise children’s self-esteem by providing general support and the opportunity to form stable attachment bonds, as well as domain-specific support and a sense of normality and inclusion in the family. Further research is needed to assess whether the importance of these factors also holds true for children currently living in foster families, for whom the impact of foster parents’ behaviour is more immediate. In the meantime, the findings from the present study may be used as the provisional basis for a suggestion that foster carer training would benefit from a move away from its current emphasis on attachment theory to a broader view of positive parenting practices. Acknowledgements I am indebted to the five individuals who took part for their willingness to share their memories with me, and to all those who helped in the recruitment of participants. Also, thanks to Janet Luke for her work as second coder. Special thanks go to Dr. V.J. Willan, Self esteem in foster care 20 Professor Ian Sinclair, and Cumbria County Council’s Research Governance Board for their guidance. Self esteem in foster care 21 References Ackerman, J.P., & Dozier, M. (2005). The influence of foster parent investment on children’s representations of self and attachment figures. Applied Developmental Psychology, 26, 507-520. Blascovich, J., & Tomaka, J. (1991). Measures of self-esteem. In J.P. Robinson, P.R. Shaver, & L.S. Wrightsman (Eds.) Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Attitudes: Volume 1 in Measures of Social Psychological Attitudes Series. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Bowlby, J. (1964). Child Care and the Growth of Love (2nd ed.). London: Pelican. Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and Loss: Volume 2. Separation: Anxiety and Anger. London: Penguin. Burns, R.B. (1979). The Self-Concept: Theory, Measurement, Development and Behaviour. London: Longman. Cassidy, J. (1990). Theoretical and methodological considerations in the study of attachment and the self in young children. In M.T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E.M. Cummings (Eds.) Attachment in the Preschool Years: Theory, Research, and Intervention. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Cooper, J.E., Holman, J., & Braithwaite, V.A. (1983). Self-esteem and family cohesion: the child’s perspective and adjustment. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 45, 153-159. Coopersmith, S. (1967). The Antecedents of Self-Esteem. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman and Company. DeRobertis, E. M. (2003). The impact of Long-Term Psychological Maltreatment by One’s Maternal Figure: A Study of the Victim’s Perspective. Journal of Emotional Abuse, 4, 27-51. Self esteem in foster care 22 Denuwelaere, M., & Bracke, P. (2007). Support and conflict in the foster family and children’s well-being: A comparison between foster and birth children. Family Relations, 56, 67-79. DfES (2006). Care Matters: Transforming the Lives of Children and Young People in Care (Government Green Paper). Norwich: TSO. DfES (2007). National statistics: first release: children looked after in England (including adoption and care leavers) year ending 31 March 2007. Internet document: http://www.dfes.gov.uk/rsgateway/DB/SFR/s000741/SFR27-2007rev.pdf , accessed 16/11/2007. Flynn, R.J., Ghazal, H., Legault, L., Vandermeulen, G., & Petrick, S. (2004). Use of population measures and norms to identify resilient outcomes in young people in care: an exploratory study. Child and Family Social Work, 9, 65-79. Gil, E., & Bogart, K. (1982). An exploratory study of self-esteem and quality of care of 100 children in foster care. Children and Youth Services Review, 4, 351-363. Gilligan, R. (1999). Enhancing the resilience of children and young people in public care by mentoring their talents and interests. Child and Family Social Work, 4, 187-196. Guillon, M. S., Crocq, M., & Bailey, P. E. (2003). The relationship between self-esteem and psychiatric disorders in adolescents. European Psychiatry, 18, 59-62. Hamacheck, D.E. (1978). Encounters with the Self (2nd edn.). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Harter, S. (1982). The perceived competence scale for children. Child Development, 53, 8797. Harter, S. (1983). Developmental perspectives on the self-system. In E.M. Hetherington Self esteem in foster care 23 (Volume Ed.): P.H. Mussen (Series Ed.) Handbook of Child Psychology (4th edn.), Volume IV: Socialization, Personality, and Social Development. New York: Wiley and Sons. Harter, S. (1986). Processes underlying the construction, maintenance, and enhancement of the self-concept in children. In J. Suls & A.G. Greenwald (Eds.) Psychological Perspectives on the Self, Vol. 3. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Harter, S. (1999). The Construction of the Self: A Developmental Perspective. New York: The Guilford Press. Herrenkohl, E. C., Herrenkohl, R. C., Egolf, B. P., & Russo, M. J. (1998). The relationship between early maltreatment and teenage parenthood. Journal of Adolescence, 21, 291303. Hicks, C., & Nixon, S. (1989). The use of a modified repertory grid technique for assessing the self-concept of children in Local Authority foster care. British Journal of Social Work, 19, 203-216. Jackson, M.R. (1984). Self-Esteem and Meaning: A Life Historical Investigation. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. Kaufman, J., & Cicchetti, D. (1989). Effects of maltreatment on school-age children’s socioemotional development: Assessments in a day-camp setting. Developmental Psychology, 25, 516-524. Kazdin, A. E., Moser, J., Colbus, D., & Bell, R. (1985). Depressive symptoms among physically abused and psychiatrically disturbed children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 298-307. Kelly, G. (2000). The survival of long-term foster care. In G. Kelly & R. Gilligan (Eds.) Issues in Foster Care: Policy, Practice and Research. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. Self esteem in foster care 24 Leeson, C. (2007). My life in care: Experiences of non-participation in decision-making processes. Child & Family Social Work, 12, 268-277. Legault, L., Anawati, M., & Flynn, R. (2006). Factors favoring psychological resilience among fostered young people. Children and Youth Services Review, 28, 1024-1038. Liu, Y. L. (2003). The mediators between parenting and adolescent depressive symptoms: dysfunctional attitudes and self-worth. International Journal of Psychology, 38, 91100. Lyman, S.B., & Bird, G.W. (1996). A closer look at self-image in male foster care adolescents. Social Work, 41, 85-96. Mallon, G. (1992). Junior life skills: an innovation for latency-age children in out-of-home care. Child Welfare Journal, 71, 585-591. McGee, R., & Williams, S. (2000). Does low self-esteem predict health compromising behaviors among adolescents? Journal of Adolescence, 23, 569-582. Rhodes, J.E., Haight, W.L., & Briggs, E.C. (1999). The influence of mentoring on the peer relationships of foster youth in relative and nonrelative care. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 9, 185-201. Rosenberg, M., Schooler, C., & Schoenbach, C. (1989). Self-esteem and adolescent problems: Modeling reciprocal effects. American Sociological Review, 54, 10041018. Rutter, M. (1981). Maternal Deprivation Reassessed (2nd ed.). London: Penguin. Schofield, G. (2002). The significance of a secure base: A psychosocial model of long-term foster care. Child and Family Social Work, 7, 259-272. Schofield, G., & Beek, M. (2005a). Providing a secure base: parenting children in long-term foster family care. Attachment & Human Development, 7, 3-25. Self esteem in foster care 25 Schofield, G., & Beek, M. (2005b). Risk and resilience in long-term foster care. British Journal of Social Work, 35, 1283-1301. Stipek, D., Recchia, S., & McClintic, S. (1992). Self-evaluation in young children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57 (1, Serial No. 226). Thorne, A., & Michaelieu, Q. (1996). Situating adolescent gender and self-esteem with personal memories. Child Development, 67, 1374-1390. Triseliotis, J. (1984). Identity and security in adoption and long-term fostering. Early Child Development and Care, 15, 149-170. Trzesniewski, K. H., Donnellan, M. B., Moffit, T. E., Robins, R. W., Poulton, R., & Caspi, A. (2006). Low self-esteem during adolescence predicts poor health, criminal behavior, and limited economic prospects during adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 42, 381-390. Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Ward, H., Skuse, T., and Munro, E.R. (2005). ‘The best of times, the worst of times’: Young people’s views of care and accommodation. Adoption and Fostering, 29, 8-17. Self esteem in foster care 26 Figure 1: Foster parents’ contributions to children’s self-esteem: an extension of Harter’s (1986) model GENERAL SUPPORT & ATTACHMENT DOMAIN-SPECIFIC SUPPORT NORMALITY & INCLUSION SELF-ESTEEM