b Massachusetts Chiefs of Police Association PRESS RELEASE FOR IMMEDIATE RELEASE Chief Richard A. Marchese, Ret. May 4, 2004 Executive Director 508 842-2935 RACIAL PROFILING DATA COLLECTION Police officers in Massachusetts do a remarkable job at protecting our citizens, addressing crime, and enforcing the law in an even-handed manner. Putting their lives on the line daily, police officers deserve our praise and thanks. Anyone who thinks that the police in this state have a practice of stopping motorists on the highway based on their race or skin color is sadly misinformed. The elimination of all forms of race-based enforcement is a commitment shared by the police chiefs and virtually all police officers across this state. Unfortunately, the single greatest threat to progress in “color blind policing” is faulty data collection and analysis that erroneously seeks to label certain departments as likely to be engaging in racial profiling. The time, energy, resources and funds expended over the past three years on data collection and analysis diverted attention from and hindered efforts at addressing the underlying causes for real or perceived biased policing. The data on a statewide basis compiled by the Northeastern University’s Institute on Race and Justice (IRJ) confirms that, overall, Massachusetts police departments did a remarkable job at enforcing the traffic laws in a fair and unbiased manner. The data reveals a statewide “racial disparity” of only 2.8%. This is truly a credit to the evenhanded enforcement practices of the overwhelming majority of our dedicated police officers. By way of comparison, the data collection projects in other states where racial profiling was “confirmed,” had disparities of 40-80%! (In some states, e.g., Connecticut, a disparity level below 5% resulted in a statewide declaration of “no profiling.”) This is not to say that any level of biased policing is acceptable – it is not. 1 Whenever a person believes they have been victimized on the basis of their race, skin color or nationality, we have a problem. Community policing depends on creating trust so that we can all address community concerns. However, all the data available tells us that the problem is not widespread. For example, the state installed a toll-free hot line three years ago and publicized it with radio ads, billboards and the like, encouraging citizens to call if they believed they had been the victim of racial profiling. As was the experience in other states, the phone hardly ever rang. This corresponds to the experience virtually all police chiefs have reported. Few, if any, complaints of profiling have been made by minority drivers to most municipal police departments in this state. With this said, we have prepared a comprehensive “Action Plan” to further address real or perceived biased policing in Massachusetts. Since the overall disparity percentage is so low, it is our hope that the Secretary of Public Safety, the Attorney General and the legislature will agree that no departments should be compelled to engage in additional data collection. Rather, the present level should continue, with frequent (monthly?) user-friendly analysis being provided to Chiefs to help monitor progress. As more information is available about what type of data collection is beneficial, the usefulness of this tool could be reconsidered. The Police Executive Research Forum (PERF) report issued in April 2004 entitled “By the Numbers: A Guide for Analyzing Race Data from Vehicle Stops,” funded by the US Department of Justice Office of Community Oriented Policing Services (COPS), concludes by stating: “ We strongly recommend, however, that agencies focus not merely on measuring racially biased policing but on responding to it.” 2 IS DATA COLLECTION WORHT IT? After spending over three years and possibly upwards of a million dollars on data collection and analysis, what do we have to show for it? This Association issued an “Action Plan to Address Racial Profiling” in the year 2000. It recommended that rather than wasting years of time and scarce tax dollars, we acknowledge that there is a problem. The report detailed several ways to address the issue. Enhancement of training, supervision, recruitment and discipline were suggested. Changes in policies and procedures were also included. We predicted, correctly as it turns out, that focusing everyone’s attention on data collection would mean ignoring those steps which could actually produce the desired result. If the state insists on more data collection, rather than the other enhancements, it will defer and even setback the effort at eliminating the perception and reality of racial profiling. GOVERNMENT STUDY FAULTS ALL DATA COLLECTION A report released in April 2004 by the Police Executive Research Forum (PERF) and funded by the U.S. Department of Justice Office of Community Oriented Police Services (COPS) entitled “By the Numbers: A Guide for Analyzing Race Data from Vehicle Stops” helps document the weaknesses in the Massachusetts project as well as those across the country. While it might be possible to design a more accurate data collection and analysis effort in the future, the numerous errors built into the Massachusetts Racial Profiling Data Collection Project to date have doomed it to failure. Flawed data, erroneous assumptions, and untested speculation cannot produce reliable conclusions. The Secretary of Public Safety has a unique opportunity to advance race relations and effective law enforcement, depending upon how he responds to the racial profiling data collection report. By bringing together a diverse group of police professionals, community groups and researchers over the past eight months, the Secretary has started a process that could help lead the way to real progress. It will take great courage on the Secretary’ s part to stand up to those who insist that the police cannot be trusted and that data collection will confirm their accusations of biased policing. It is unrealistic to expect police officers to welcome more data collection after their experience with the past three year’s problems. 3 EXAMPLES OF DATA COLLECTION ERRORS IN MASSACHUSETTS 1. Most police officers received little guidance and no training from the state on how to complete the new citation forms. No test phase was attempted to work out the “bugs”. No effort was made to learn from mistakes. 2. Despite a requirement that all police departments receive monthly reports on the data related to their department – presumably so they could make corrections and identify problems early - this never happened. Only once during the past three years was any data sent, and it was in a form that was unintelligible to most non-research professionals. 3. Officers were instructed to “guess” at a driver’s race. Brazilians, for example, were classified as Hispanic by some officers (even though they do not speak Spanish), and as white by others – as well as by the Census? The US Census allows everyone to “self declare” and often people list themselves as a certain % of more than one race or nationality. The state refused to address this and would not put the information on drivers’ licenses, forcing an unreliable guessing or “unknown” reply by many officers. 4. Despite massive confusion over when to check off the “search” box, no corrective training or instructions were issued by the state. It has been widely acknowledged by the researchers and state officials that with the low number of searches conducted, the errors have irrevocably skewed the results. 5. The lack of any funding from the state meant that the money for data analysis had to be diverted from federal grants by the Executive Office of Public Safety. For the small amount involved (reportedly in the vicinity of $100,000), the IRJ has done a remarkable amount of work. Since funds were scarce, several “shortcuts” were attempted, often with disastrous results. For example, rather than conducting on-site traffic studies as the rest of the country has done, the IRJ team resorted to modifying the “Dominos Pizza” delivery computer program to come up with an “estimated driving population.” They estimated wrong in every case that we tested. After putting observers on the roadways where most tickets were issued, the percentage of non-white drivers actually on the road was nearly identical to the percentage of nonwhite drivers cited. This varied markedly from what the researchers used as an estimate and upon which they based a major portion of their report. (To show the absurdity of the results, the map the researchers produced for drivers likely to frequent Cambridge includes those from Attleborough – near the Rhode Island border!) 6. The only time the researchers made any on-road observations was in connection with trying to determine an estimated driving population for the State Police. They picked out a sample roadway in each barracks region and sent some college students in a car to make observations of the race or nationality of drivers. As an acknowledgement of the difficulty of identifying non-white drivers before they are pulled over, the researchers only made observations during the daytime (and with three people in each car!) 4 Getting Data Collection Right The Police Executive Research Forum (PERF) issued the first ten chapters of a report in April 2004 entitled “By the Numbers: A Guide for Analyzing Race Data from Vehicle Stops.” Funded by the U.S. Department of Justice Office of Community Oriented Policing Services (COPS), the report is intended as a guide for law enforcement agencies, municipal officials, citizens and community groups on how to analyze, interpret and understand vehicle stop data being collected on drivers’ race. (The second volume, containing chapters 11-13, will be issued in August 2004.) Unfortunately, the information contained in this report was not available in 2000 when Massachusetts enacted its Racial and Gender Profiling Data Collection law. In nearly every area, the Massachusetts effort falls short of the report’s recommendations on how to properly collect and analyze data. This is not from a lack of effort or professionalism on the part of the dedicated researchers at Northeastern University’s Institute on Race and Justice (IRJ). As the IRJ’s “preliminary Tabulations” report issued in January 2004 stated: From the outset it is important to note that aggregate data, such as the data presented in this preliminary report can indicate patterns of disparate traffic citation activity in a department but cannot identify the motives involved in individual traffic stop, citation or other enforcement decisions. Therefore, this preliminary report should not be read as an indication of racial profiling by any Massachusetts law enforcement agency. Social science cannot provide reliable explanations for what individual officers are thinking when they decided to stop or cite a particular motorist. Social science can, however, help to identify whether certain groups are disproportionately targeted for enforcement practices. (emphasis added) Research on racial profiling in traffic stops is a relatively new area of inquiry. Although numerous studies have begun to address questions of differential treatment in traffic stops, no absolute consensus exists about the best way to determine disparities. Racial disparities in citations can result from a number of factors that social scientists are just beginning to understand. Bias on the part of an individual officer is one of several possible explanations for disparities in citations. For example, certain department enforcement strategies or 5 allocation of patrol resources. while perhaps race neutral on their face may, result in the disparate treatment of particular racial groups. In some communities, police commanders may assign a larger number of officers to a particular neighborhood because that neighborhood has more crime and thus an increased need for police services. It may then be the case that police assigned to this high crime area engage in traffic enforcement as part of their normal patrol activities and since there are more police working in this neighborhood, individuals who live, work or drive through this neighborhood are more likely to be stopped and cited than individuals who live in other neighborhoods. If the neighborhoods where police assign additional patrols are neighborhoods where people of color are more likely to live, then the deployment decision may result in racial disparities in traffic citations. (emphasis added) The following excerpts from the PERF report are helpful in understanding that the Massachusetts traffic stop data cannot identify instances of racial profiling: It is not difficult to measure whether there is disparity between racial/ethnic groups in stops made by police; the difficulty comes in identifying the causes for any disparity. For instance, a jurisdiction might compare the demographic profile of people stopped by police to the demographic profile of residents as measured by the census. The results might show “disparity”; that is, the results might show that some groups are stopped disproportionate to their representation in the residential population. The jurisdiction, cannot, however, identify the causes of that disparity using this measure. Only after controlling for driving quantity, driving quality, and driving location, can a researcher who finds that minorities are disproportionately represented among drivers stopped by police conclude with reasonable confidence that the disparity reflects police bias in decision making. (emphasis added) Although jurisdictions nationwide have invested considerable resources to collect race data from vehicle stops, most jurisdictions do not know how to analyze the collected data properly. They are either ill-equipped to do the analysis, or they are misinformed about what should be done. An overwhelming majority of the data analyses reviewed by PERF staff for this project were based on substandard methods. Most agencies are using models for their analyses that fall far short of minimal social science standards. In jurisdictions across the country, reports prepared by agencies or external groups (for example, some civil 6 rights groups) draw conclusions wholly unsupported by the data. Other reports indicate that despite all the efforts and resources that were dedicated to the data collection, no conclusions can be drawn. These failures can largely be explained by the complexity of the task of measuring whether policing in a jurisdiction is racially biased. A tremendous number of factors other than bias can legitimately influence police decisions to stop drivers, and these “alternative hypotheses” must be ruled out before the “bias hypothesis” can be tested. A lack of understanding about which benchmarking methods will yield the most valid interpretations of the data is hindering agencies’ efforts to reach valid, responsible conclusions. A key aspect of analyzing vehicle stop data is to determine whether the driver’s race/ethnicity has an impact on police stopping decisions. In order to assess whether there is an impact, however, we must exclude or “control for” factors other than race/ethnicity that might legitimately explain police stopping decisions. In developing “benchmarks,” the researcher is attempting to construct a comparison group that represents the drivers at risk of being stopped by police— absent bias. This group is compared to the group of drivers actually stopped to help determine whether racial bias may have been a factor in police officers’ decision-making process. The variation in quality across benchmarks is directly related to how closely each benchmark represents the group of people who should be at risk of being stopped by police if no bias exists. The strongest benchmarks take into consideration variations in driving quality, driving quantity, and driving location. We emphasize that an agency should, if feasible, select a plan for analyzing the data at the same time that the decision makers decide what stops to target and what information to collect on stops.1 We recommend that decision makers select all traffic stops or all vehicle stops, and not a subset of these categories as defined by their outcomes (for example, citations, arrests). Some jurisdictions (indeed, some entire states) are collecting data only on subsets of stops, such as traffic stops that result in a citation. In Chapter 3 we explain why this practice produces substandard data for analysis. In Chapter 3 we also encourage agencies to involve residents and agency personnel from all levels in planning data collection and analysis. We start by explaining how the data that have been collected from officers can be checked for quality, an important first step in any type of social science research and not unique to the analysis of police-citizen contact data. Although there is no cost-effective way to ensure that the data are 100 percent accurate, 1 For information on what stops to target for data collection and what information to obtain for each stop, see PERF’s first report on racial profiling entitled Racially Biased Policing: A Principled Response (Fridell et al. 2001, Chap. 8). 7 the methods described in the chapter can help the researcher check for and enhance the quality of their data. A range of methods can be used to ascertain whether officers are submitting forms to the agency for each and every stop targeted for data collection. Additionally, there are methods for assessing the level and source of missing data, errors, and intentional misstatements of facts. When selecting reference periods we recommend that, if economically and politically feasible, agencies collect one year of data before analyzing it. Agencies are advised to delay the start of the reference period for several months after data collection begins. In the first few months, officers can become accustomed to the data collection process, and their data should be reviewed to identify particular problems (such as large amounts of missing data on certain variables or missing forms). Once the problems appear to be resolved, the reference period should begin. For many reasons, it is appropriate for agencies to analyze subsets of their police-citizen contact data. In Chapter 4 we describe why a researcher might choose not to analyze all of the data submitted during the reference period but only a portion, and how and why a researcher might conduct separate, multiple analyses using subsets of the data. For example, the researcher might choose to analyze for his or her report only proactive stops (stops in which police have discretion regarding whom to stop); then the researcher might choose to conduct separate analyses of these data within geographic sub-areas of the jurisdiction. We discuss subsets based on (1) whether stops are proactive or reactive, (2) whether the officer could discern the driver’s race/ethnicity, (3) whether the driver appears in the database once or multiple times, (4) geographic locations of stops (to allow for analyses within sub-areas of the jurisdiction), and (5) whether the stops are for traffic violations or for the purpose of investigating crime. The final section of Chapter 4 explains the need for comparability of the stop data and benchmarking data or what we call “matching the numerator and the denominator.” The “numerator” refers to the data collected on stops by the police, and the “denominator” refers to the data collected to produce the comparison group, or benchmark. To “match the numerator and the denominator” the researcher adjusts the stop data to correspond to any limiting parameters of the benchmark or vice versa. For instance, in the observation benchmarking method, researchers collect data from the field regarding the race/ethnicity of drivers. Placed at various locations, the observers count the drivers in different race/ethnicity categories. This process produces a racial/ethnic profile of drivers observed at these locations that can be compared to the people who are stopped by police. Since the “denominator” (observation data) pertains only to certain areas, the relevant analysis will only include in the “numerator” the police stops in that area. Using this method, the researcher will compare the demographics of the people who are observed driving through 8 Intersection A, for example, to the demographics of the people stopped by police in and around Intersection A. (This type of analysis will be conducted separately for each intersection.) The numerator and denominator must be matched with regard to other parameters as well. For example, if observation data were collected from January through May 2002, the analysis should involve only police stops that occurred during roughly that same time period. If the researchers collected observation data only during daylight hours because of visibility issues, then the analysis should include in the numerator only those stops that occurred during daylight hours. Chapters 5 through 10 target some of the mistakes often made when comparing stop data to commonly used benchmarks. For example, many law enforcement agencies and outside analysts will compare the percentage of stops that involve African Americans or other minorities to the racial make-up of the residents of a particular area as measured by census data. More often than not, the mass media, civic groups, and citizens draw conclusions from this comparison regarding the existence or lack of racially biased policing in the jurisdiction; these conclusions are wholly unsupportable using this method of analysis. Frequently, no mention is made of non-race-related explanations for the disparity between the census population and the population of stopped drivers, explanations that relate to driving quantity, driving quality, and driving location. These are all factors that legitimately affect stopping behavior by police. Despite the weaknesses of using census data as a diagnostic tool, some jurisdictions (limited by resources or time) may have no option other than to use this method. This will be particularly true of researchers charged with analyzing data for an entire state. The obligation of the researcher in this position is to ensure that the results are conveyed in a responsible fashion. In fact, this obligation falls to all stakeholders, including concerned citizens, civil rights groups, and the media. No one interpreting results based on census benchmarking—even adjusted census benchmarking—can claim they have proved the existence or lack of racially biased policing. This caveat is not unique to adjusted census benchmarking, and the inability to identify a causal connection between driver race/ethnicity and police decisions does not mean that data collection is without value. Even if the results from data collection do not provide definitive conclusions, they can serve as a basis for constructive discussions between police and citizens regarding ways to reduce racial bias and/or perceptions of racial bias. The chapter also explains how social scientists have addressed these questions in the context of their research. A key point of controversy is whether to use as a benchmark all drivers at the selected site or only traffic law-violating drivers at the site. We recommend that the observation benchmark be based on law-violating drivers, not all drivers, because this model encompasses the fact 9 that drivers who drive poorly are at greater risk of being stopped by police. (We present the alternative viewpoint in an appendix.) Chapter 13, the final chapter in By the Numbers, will discuss how law enforcement agencies can use the results from data collection to achieve reform. Even results based on weak benchmarking methods can stimulate productive discussions between police and residents about the issues of racially biased policing and the perceptions of its practice. The chapter suggests how these discussions can be structured to produce action plans for reform. We strongly recommend, however, that agencies focus not merely on measuring racially biased policing but on responding to it. Varied responses to racially biased policing are set forth in PERF’s first DOJ COPS-funded report, Racially Biased Policing: A Principled Response, available on the PERF Web site. They can be grouped in the following areas: supervision/accountability, policy, recruitment/hiring, training/education, and outreach to diverse communities. MCOPA’s Recommendations The cost of data collection and analysis in Massachusetts has not proven to be worth the effort. At a time when police budgets are being cut, training is being reduced, and layoffs are being implemented, it is irresponsible to divert hundreds of thousands, if not another million or so dollars, to study a problem rather than working on the solution. If there is money left over after this state funds training, policy development, supervision enhancements, community policing models, and improve recruitment, hiring and discipline, data collection might be considered; but next time, try and do it right! 10 ACTION PLAN TO COMBAT RACIAL PROFILING The first and most important step in addressing the issue of racial profiling in traffic stops is to acknowledge that a problem exists. The use of racial or ethnic stereotypes has no place in law enforcement. The goal of this Action Plan is to condemn and eliminate such use. The Board of Directors of this Association joined with the Colonel of the State Police and a group of municipal chiefs during June 1999 in issuing a “Resolution” to condemn the practice of racial profiling or stereotyping. As a follow-up, the Association’s Traffic and Highway Safety Committee conducted a study of the problem and made recommendations. Based on the recommendations of that group, the Board of Directors at its meeting, on June 8, 2000, unanimously voted to adopt an Action Plan. The following represents an updating of said Plan, incorporating recommendations from national experts, and groups such as NOBLE (National Association of Black Law Enforcement Executives), the Police Executive Research Forum (PERF) and even the ACLU! GOAL IDENTIFICATION The police throughout the state should commit themselves to raising the level of public trust and confidence in the law enforcement community. The use of racial profiling or stereotyping is morally and legally wrong and has no place in modern policing efforts. It corrodes the presumption of innocence to which all our citizens are entitled. It also alienates law-abiding citizens and undermines the effectiveness of community policing efforts. Given the proper support, the police departments in Massachusetts are capable of identifying the problem and eliminating it. A statewide effort, coordinated by the Secretary of Public Safety, is recommended. We are convinced that through improvements in policy, training, supervision and discipline, we can achieve these noble goals. 11 POLICIES & PROCEDURES The “Policies and Procedures” utilized by police agencies should reflect the philosophy that racial profiling and stereotyping is prohibited. The Municipal Police Institute, Inc. (MPI) is the private, non-profit research and training affiliate of the Massachusetts Chiefs of Police Association. Its sample manual has served as the basis for the “Policies & Procedures” currently in use by the majority of police departments in this state. We have reviewed, updated and, where appropriate, prepared new samples addressing the issue of racial profiling in connection with traffic stops. He will pay particular attention to discipline for violations. TRAINING We have asked our representatives on the Massachusetts Municipal Police Training Committee (MPTC) to work with the Executive Office of Public Safety to identify funding to ensure that all future police training has a component to deal with the Issue of racial profiling. We intend this to include. Basic training for recruits; In-service training for veteran officers; Supervisory training for all superior officers from Sergeant to Chief; and Dispatcher and Communications Officer training. We recommend that the Training Council produce a video concerning racial profiling and provide copies to all departments in the state. The video should be suitable in length for use at roll-call and should be updated periodically as appropriate. (Making training available on a “distance learning” basis, as the Governor’s Crime Commission recently recommended, would be the most appropriate way to assure all officers are trained. Local budgets for training overtime and travel require that internet-based options be encouraged.) The efforts of the State Police and individual police departments and regional training groups over the past few years should be reviewed and incorporated into all MPTC programs. PUBLIC AWARENESS Focusing solely on officer training and discipline is not enough. A statewide public relations campaign is needed. Citizens should be sensitized to the need to identify unlawful behavior, and not simply the 12 race of an unknown person in their neighborhood, before calling to report a suspicious person. A request form the Governor, Attorney General and Secretary of Public Safety would be appropriate. The media responded well in a similar situation involving hate crimes several years ago. Chiefs should encourage local cable TV providers to get involved as well. Providing speakers to local, civic, school and professional groups will also help spread the word. Some effort should also be made to inform drivers of what to expect and do. DISCIPLINE Police departments should work toward adopting a “zero tolerance” policy for intentional racial profiling offenses. Once proper policies and procedures are in place, and officers receive adequate training in what is expected, discipline for racial profiling should be severe. This will involve working with unions to secure their understanding and cooperation. An effort must also be undertaken to educate arbitrators, the Civil Service Commission and judges of the seriousness of these offenses. Summary of Massachusetts Chiefs of Police Association’s Action Plan Recommendations 1. Acknowledge that, while not widespread, there is a problem of racial profiling by some Massachusetts police officers. The perception of the problem by some citizens means that it must be addressed. 2. The use of racial profiling and stereotyping is condemned. 3. The police throughout this state are committed to raising the level of citizens’ trust and confidence in the law enforcement community. 4. Given the proper support, the police departments in Massachusetts are capable of identifying the problem and eliminating it. Racebased enforcement is neither taught nor tolerated in this state. 13 5. A statewide effort, coordinated by the Secretary of Public Safety, aimed at addressing everyone’s legitimate concerns is recommended. 6. Sample “Policies and Procedures” such as those developed by the Municipal Police Institute for use by police departments across the state should be reviewed and updated regularly. These should address the proper policy for traffic stops, appropriate discipline, and a uniform procedure whenever a motor vehicle is subjected to a consent search. 7. The Municipal Police Training Committee – with the necessary funding to do the job right - should review all existing procedures and curriculum and offer training in racial profiling issues at all levels including: basic training for recruits; in-service training for veteran officers; supervisory training for all superior officers, from Sergeants to Chiefs; and Dispatcher and Communication Officer training. 8. The MPTC should produce a roll-call training video on racial profiling and make such training available on a “distance learning” basis to all officers in the state.. 9. The Governor, Attorney General and Secretary of Public Safety should encourage the media to conduct a statewide public education effort, similar to the one done several years ago concerning hate crimes. Chiefs and the State Police should support this effort and enhance it with the use of local cable TV and meetings with civic, school and professional groups. 10. The Registry should include in new driver training programs information about what motorists should expect and do during a traffic stop. 14 11. Once proper policies and procedures are in place and training has been accomplished, departments should adopt a “zero tolerance” policy for cases of intentional racial profiling. An effort to involve unions and to educate arbitrators, the Civil Service Commission and judges will be needed to assure that such discipline is upheld on appeal. 12. Regional training seminars on Internal Affairs and racial profiling should be conducted throughout the State. 13. Cooperative training efforts with municipal and State Police officers and command staff should be undertaken. 14. The use and expansion of the existing data collection and analysis capabilities of the Registry of Motor Vehicles appears to be far too expensive for the potential return. Unless and until there is a national consensus on the best way to collect data on motor vehicle stops, this state’s limited resources should be spent addressing rather than studying the problem. 15 RACIAL PROFILING STATISTICS Of the 351 cities and towns in Massachusetts, only 340 reportedly issued traffic citations during the 27 months (4/1/01 – 6/30/04) studied. A total of approximately 1,025,978 citations were issued. CENSUS COMPARISONS According to the 2000 U.S. Census, the state’s population, 18 years of age or older, is comprised of 93.3% whites and 6.7% non-white. During the 27 months of the study, on average, a total of 3,018 citations were issued per community. Of these, 89.5% were issued to white drivers, while 10.5% went to non-whites. When the percentage of non-whites in the state’s population (6.7%) is compared with the percentage of citations given to non-white drivers (10.5%), this produces what the researchers refer to as the “Non-White Disparity” of 3.8% (10.5% - 6.7%). The researchers broke down the Non-White Disparity by police departments in various population groups, the same size categories as those used by the FBI in compiling its Uniform Crime Reports (UCR’s). The average Non-White Disparities by population group, based on the 2000 U.S. Census, were: POPULATION NON-WHITE DISPARITY 100,000+ 3.2% 50,000 – 99,999 3.8% 25,000 – 49,999 6.6% 10,000 – 24,999 4.3% under 10,000 2.7% 16 SEX COMPARISONS In Massachusetts, 52% of the residents 18 years and older are female and 48% are male. Of the 1,054,913 citations issued statewide for which the sex of the driver was determined, 28.7% were issued to females and 71.3% to males. This produces what the researchers call a “Sex Disparity” of 23.3%. This is derived by subtracting the percentage of females cited from their percentage in the census population. The same results are reached by subtracting the male percentage of the population from the percentage of citations issued to males. (71.3% - 48.0% = 23.3%) When the researchers broke down the same information by UCR population group, the following resulted: POPULATION AVERAGE SEX DISPARITY 100,000+ 23.0% 50,000 – 99,999 22.6% 25,000 – 49,999 23.1% 10,000 – 24,999 24.3% under 10,000 23.0% Clearly, there is very little difference among various sized communities in the percentage breakdown between male and females that are cited. While the researchers’ tables do not show a “Racial Disparity” for each of the five race (or “nationality”) categories, the statewide average figures break down as follows: RACE/NATIONALITY STATEWIDE RACIAL DISPARITY White 3.8% Black -2.7% Hispanic -2.7% Asian 0.2% Native American 0.2% 17 This means that, on average, across Massachusetts, the percentage of white drivers cited is 3.8% less than the percentage 18 and older whites comprise in the state’s population. Blacks and Hispanics are each cited 2.7% more than their percentage of the state’s 18 and older population. Asians and Native Americans are cited 0.2% less than their percentage of the 18 and older state population. When reviewing the “racial disparity” numbers for the various UCR population groups, several trends appear. First, the percentage of white drivers cited increases steadily as the community’s population size increases, from 61.4% for the largest cities to 93.3% for the smallest towns. (Note: 93.3% is the exact statewide white population.) It is often in the smallest towns, especially in the more rural areas of the state, where the Census population and the driving population are most alike. The exact opposite is seen when each of the four “non-white” categories are studied. The largest cities cite the greatest percentages of all non-white categories, and the figures decrease as the size of the population group decreases. Blacks receive 17.3% of the citations issued in cities over 100,000 population, yet only 2.7% of those in the smallest towns. Similarly, the figures for Hispanics decrease steadily from 15.1% for the biggest cities to 3.0% for the smallest towns. Asians receive 6.1% of the largest cities’ citations, yet only 0.9% of the smallest towns. And, finally, Native Americans receive 0.3% of the citations written by cities of over 100,000 population, yet only 0.1% from towns with less than 10,000 residents. Again, in every category, there is a steady, and often uniform drop as the population category decreases. The researchers compared the breakdown of cited drivers, by both sex and either white or not-white, to the census population. On average, across the state, of those cited, 26.3% were white females; 2.4% were non-white females; 63.2% were white males; and 8.1% were non-white males. 18 SEX DISPARITY While the tables do not show one, a “Sex Disparity”, derived by comparing the percentage breakdown of persons receiving citations with their percentage breakdown in the census population, the results are as follows: 22.4% for white females; 1.0% for non-white females; -18.6% for white males; and -4.7% for non-white males. This means that on average, across the state, females are cited less often than their proportion of the population, with white females being cited 22.4% less than their proportionate share and non-white females being cited 1% less than their proportionate share of the population. Conversely, white males are cited 18.6% more than their share of the population and non-white males are cited 4.7% more often than the percentage of the population they comprise. When the “Sex Disparity” percentages are looked at for each of the five UCR population groupings, several trends appear. The disparity figures increase from 20.0% to 26.7% for white females from the largest to the smallest communities. They conversely decrease from 10.3% in the biggest cities for non-white females, to 1.4% in the smallest towns. As for males, the disparity increases steadily from 41.4% in the largest cities to 66.4% in the smallest communities, as do figures for non-white males, from 28.3% to 5.5%. ESTIMATED DRIVING POPULATION In the next series of charts, the researchers compare the percentage of persons cited with what they estimated to be the driving population of each community. When estimating the state’s driving population, one might expect that the overall breakdowns should be identical to the census population. With one exception, this was the case. For some reason, the percentage breakdown by race was 19 reportedly 92.3% white in the state’s driving population in Table B2a and 93.3% in Table A1a. This results in a “Non-White Disparity” of 2.8% for the state’s “Estimated Driving Population” yet 3.8% for the state’s Census Population. Presumably the researchers determined that not all persons 18 or over are drivers. When comparing the average percentage breakdowns of citations by the race or nationality of what the researchers estimate to be the state’s driving population, the race disparity breaks down as follows: 2.8% fewer whites were cited than their percentage of estimated drivers; 2.3% more of both the Black and Hispanic drivers were cited; and 0.3% fewer of the Asians and 0.2% fewer of the Native Americans were cited. SEARCH DISPARITIES It appears that there were over 900,000 citations for which the “search” box on the citation was checked either “yes” or “no”. The exact number is difficult to determine from the Preliminary Tabulations. For example, Table C1 shows a total of 924,411 citations; Table C2 shows 930,805; and Table C3 lists 907,028 total citations issued. Since only about 40 searches on average for each department were reportedly conducted, the researchers noted that the numbers were so low that any meaningful analysis on a department level were statistically impossible. NON-WHITE SEARCH DISPARITIES Statewide, 2.0% of the white drivers that were cited were also searched, while 2.8% of the non-whites cited were searched. This results in what the researchers label as a “Non-White Disparity” of 0.8%. There is almost no change in this figure when looking at departments in various sized groups. The disparity figure ranges from 0.7% to 0.9% for the five size categories. 20 MALE SEARCH DISPARITIES Of the forty-one searches conducted by departments on average over the 27 month period of the study, 7 involved female drivers and 34 were conducted in vehicles driven by male drivers. This means that 2.4% of the male drivers that were given citations also had some search of their vehicle conducted, while 1.3% of the cited females were also searched. The researchers compare these percentages and list a “Male Disparity” of 1.2%. When looking at the disparity by population groups, the only notable variances occur among the largest cities and the smallest towns. The cities of over 100,000 report a “Male Disparity” of 0.6% while towns with less than 10,000 residents have a “Male Disparity” of 2.3%. Of the 40 persons that were searched on average per department over the 27 months of the study, 2.0% of the white drivers that were cited were also searched. This figure is 3.1% for Blacks, 3.0% for Hispanics, 1.3% for Asians, 0.9% for Middle Eastern and 3.2% for Native Americans. (The 2000 U.S. Census classifies Middle-Easterners as white. Therefore, the researchers did not list this as a category in earlier tables. However, because it was a category that officers could list on the citation form, it appears here for the first time.) The bulk of the disparity for Blacks and Hispanics appears to result from communities of over 50,000 residents. On average, 2.3% of the white male drivers that were cited were searched. The number was 3.2% for non-whites, 1.3% for white females and 1.7% for non-white females. The small numbers of searches made it difficult for the researchers to conduct any meaningful analysis, especially when looking at a breakdown by population group. However, for the largest cities, where the largest number of searchers were conducted, the percentages are virtually identical for white (0.5%) and non-white (0.7%) females and white males (0.7%). The percentage of cited non-white males that were searched was 1.6%. This is approximately half of the statewide average. 21 The researchers looked at those 847,715 cases where drivers were cited and searched, but not arrested. On average, departments searched 23 persons during the 27 month study period without arresting the driver. White drivers were searched and given a citation, but not arrested, 1.3% of the time. For non-white drivers, the figure was 1.9%. This produced what the researchers call a “Non-White Disparity” of 0.6%. A review of these figures by the population group of the department shows very little variances among all but the largest cities. Departments from communities of less than 100,000 residents searched citied whites without arresting them from 1.1% to 1.4% and non-whites 1.7% to 2.1% of the time. For the state’s five largest cities, the figures are 0.5% for whites and 0.8% for non-whites, with a non-white disparity of only 0.4%. Table A1a: Traffic Citations for White and Non-White Drivers Compared to the Census Population — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Citations Census Benchmark (Population 18 and Older) % % Non% Total Agency Number Non% White Non- White White Population White White Disparity AVERAGES 3,018 89.5 10.5 14,273 93.3 6.7 3.8 TOTAL 1,025,978 Table A1b: Traffic Citations for White and Non-White Drivers Compared to the Census Population — STATEWIDE AVERAGES BY POPULATION GROUPS Citations Census Benchmark (Population 18 and Older) % % Non% Total Agency Number Non% White Non- White White Population White White Disparity 100,000+ AVERAGE 40,943 61.4 38.6 175,458 64.6 35.4 3.2 50,000-99,999 AVERAGE 9,922 76.4 23.6 54,370 80.7 19.8 3.8 25,000-49,999 AVERAGE 5,218 82.0 18.0 26,113 88.6 11.5 6.6 10,000-24,999 AVERAGE 2,173 90.5 9.5 11,770 94.8 5.2 4.3 Under 10,000 AVERAGE 1,028 93.3 6.7 3,301 96.0 4.0 2.7 22 Table A2a: Traffic Citations for Male and Female Drivers Compared to the Census Population — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Citations Census Benchmark Sex (Population 18 and Older) % % Total % Agency Citations % Female Disparity Female Male Population Male AVERAGES 3,112 28.7 71.3 14,284 52.0 48.0 23.3 TOTAL 1,054,913 Table A2b: Traffic Citations for Male and Female Drivers Compared to the Census Population — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Grouped by Population Size Citations Census Benchmark (Population 18 and Older) % % Total % Sex Agency Citations % Female Female Male Population Male Disparity 100,000+ AVERAGE 43,514 29.6 70.4 175,458 52.6 47.4 23.0 50,000-99,999 AVERAGE 10,219 30.7 69.3 54,259 53.3 46.7 22.6 25,000-49,999 AVERAGE 5,320 30.1 69.9 25,972 53.1 46.9 23.1 10,000-24,999 AVERAGE 2,211 28.3 71.7 11,767 52.6 47.4 24.3 Under 10,000 AVERAGE 1,041 28.2 71.8 3,300 51.2 48.8 23.0 23 Table A3a: Traffic Citations for White, Black, Hispanic, Asian and Native American Drivers Compared to the Census Polulation — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Citations Agency Number AVERAGE 3,018 TOTAL 1,025,977 RACIAL DISPARITY % White % Black % Hispanic % Asian 89.5 4.1 4.9 1.5 3.8 -2.7 -2.7 0.2 Census Benchmark (Population 18 and Older) % Native % % % % % Native American White Black Hispanic Asian American 0.1 93.3 1.4 2.2 1.6 0.3 0.2 Table A3b: Traffic Citations for White, Black, Hispanic, Asian and Native American Drivers Commpared to the Census Population — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Grouped by Population Size Citations Agency 100,000+ AVERAGE 50,000-99,999 AVERAGE 25,000-49,999 AVERAGE 10,000-24,999 AVERAGE Under 10,000 AVERAGE Census Benchmark (Population 18 and Older) % Native % % % % % Native American White Black Hispanic Asian American Number % White % Black % Hispanic % Asian 40,943 61.4 17.3 15.1 6.1 0.3 64.6 11.4 12.7 8.1 0.2 9,922 76.4 8.2 12.0 3.3 0.1 80.2 3.9 7.9 4.8 0.2 5,120 82.0 6.5 8.9 2.4 0.1 88.9 2.2 4.3 2.9 0.1 2,160 90.6 3.8 4.2 1.3 0.1 94.8 1.0 1.5 1.5 0.2 1,058 93.3 2.7 3.0 0.9 0.1 96.0 0.9 1.1 0.8 0.4 24 Table A4a: Traffic Citations for White Females, Non-White Females, White Males and Non-White Males Compared to the Census Population — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Citations Census Benchmark (Population 18 and Older) Agency AVERAGE TOTAL SEX DISPARITY Number 3,013 1,024,515 % White % Non-White % White Male Female Female % Non-White Male Total Population 14,246 26.3 2.4 63.2 8.1 22.4 1.0 -18.6 -4.7 % % % Non- % NonWhite White White White Female Female Male Male 48.7 3.3 44.6 3.4 Table A4b: Traffic Citations for White Females, Non-White Females, White Males, and Non-White Males Compared to the Census Population — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Grouped by Population Size Citations Census Benchmark (Population 18 and Older) Agency 100,000+ AVERAGE 50,000-99,999 AVERAGE 25,000-49,999 AVERAGE 10,000-24,999 AVERAGE Under 10,000 AVERAGE Number % White % Non-White % White Male Female Female % Non-White Male Total Population % % % Non- % NonWhite White White White Female Female Male Male 40,829 20.0 10.3 41.4 28.3 175,458 33.8 18.7 30.7 16.7 9,905 25.0 5.7 51.4 17.9 54,259 43.0 10.3 37.1 9.6 5,213 25.8 4.2 56.1 13.8 25,972 47.3 5.9 41.3 5.6 2,171 26.2 2.1 64.3 7.4 11,767 50.0 2.6 44.7 2.6 1,030 26.7 1.4 66.4 5.5 3,301 49.3 1.9 46.6 2.2 25 Table B2a: Traffic Citations for White and Non-White Drivers Compared to the Driving Population Estimate — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Citations Driving Population Estimate Non-White Agency Number % White % Non-White % White % Non-White Disparity AVERAGE 3,018 89.5 10.5 92.3 7.7 2.8 TOTAL 1,025,977 Table B2b: Traffic Citations for White and Non-White Drivers Compared to the Driving Population Estimate — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Grouped byPopulation Size Citations Driving Population Estimate Non-White Agency Number % White % Non-White % White % Non-White Disparity 100,000+ AVERAGE 40,943 61.4 38.6 71.5 28.5 10.1 50,000-99,999 AVERAGE 9,922 76.4 23.6 80.7 19.3 4.3 25,000-49,000 AVERAGE 5,218 82.0 18.0 86.7 13.3 4.7 10,000-24,999 AVERAGE 2,173 90.5 9.5 92.6 7.4 2.1 Under 10,000 AVERAGE 1,028 93.3 6.7 95.6 4.4 2.4 26 Table B3a: Traffic Citations for White, Black, Hispanic, Asian and Native American Drivers Compared to the Driving Population Estimate — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Citations Agency AVERAGE TOTAL RACE DISPARITY Number % White % Black % Hispanic % Asian 3,018 1,025,977 89.5 4.1 4.9 1.5 2.8 -2.3 -2.3 0.3 Driving Population Estimate % Native % % % % % Native American White Black Hispanic Asian American 0.1 92.3 1.7 2.6 1.8 0.3 0.2 Table B3b: Traffic Citations for White, Black, Hispanic, Asian and Native American Drivers Compared to the Driving Population A182Estimate — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Grouped by Population Size Citations Driving Population Estimate % Native % % % % % Native Agency Number % White % Black % Hispanic % Asian American White Black Hispanic Asian American 100,000+ AVERAGE 40,943 61.4 17.3 15.1 6.1 0.3 71.5 8.7 10.6 6.5 0.2 50,000-99,999 AVERAGE 9,922 76.4 8.2 12.0 3.3 0.1 80.7 4.5 7.4 4.6 0.2 25,000-49,000 AVERAGE 5,218 82.0 6.5 9.0 2.4 0.1 86.7 3.0 5.0 3.3 0.1 10,000-24,999 AVERAGE 2,173 90.5 3.9 4.2 1.3 0.1 92.6 1.7 2.3 1.9 0.2 Under 10,000 AVERAGE 1,031 93.1 2.8 3.1 1.0 0.1 95.8 0.9 1.3 0.9 0.2 27 Table C1a: Traffic Citations for White and Non-White Drivers Which Result in a Search for All Citations — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Agency Citations Total White Searches Non-White Searches Non-White # Non# Total # White White # Searched % Searched # Searched % Searched Searched Disparity AVERAGE 2,719 2,087 581 40 2.0 30 2.8 10 0.8 TOTAL 924,411 Table C1b: Traffic Citations for White and Non-White Drivers Which Result in a Search for All Citations — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Grouped by Population Size Agency Citations Total White Searches Non-White Searches Non-White # Non# Total # White White # Searched % Searched # Searched % Searched Searched Disparity 100,000+ AVERAGE 34,978 19,077 15,901 286 0.6 107 1.4 178 0.7 50,000-99,999 AVERAGE 8,747 6,359 2,389 141 1.8 89 2.7 52 0.9 25,000-49,999 AVERAGE 5,148 3,876 909 58 1.5 44 2.2 14 0.7 10,000-24,999 AVERAGE 1,892 1,707 185 38 2.3 33 3.2 5 0.9 Under 10,000 AVERAGE 916 841 75 17 2.1 15 2.9 2 0.8 28 Table C2a: Traffic Citations for Male and Female Drivers Which Result in a Search for All Citations —STATEWIDE AVERAGES Agency Citations Total Female Searches Male Searches Male Total # Female # Male # Searched % Searched # Searched % Searched # Searched Disparity AVERAGE 2,738 830 1,908 41 1.3 7 2.4 34 1.2 TOTAL 930,805 Table C2b: Traffic Citations for Male and Female Drivers Which Result in a Search for All Citations — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Grouped by Population Size Agency Citations Total Female Searches Male Searches Male Total # Female # Male # Searched % Searched # Searched % Searched # Searched Disparity 100,000+ AVERAGE 37,129 11,327 25,802 329 0.5 63 1.2 266 0.6 50,000-99,999 AVERAGE 8,964 2,845 6,119 148 1.2 23 2.3 124 1.1 25,000-49,999 AVERAGE 4,878 1,505 3,373 60 1.0 10 1.8 50 0.9 10,000-49,999 AVERAGE 1,922 561 1,360 38 1.5 7 2.7 32 1.2 Under 10,000 AVERAGE 926 270 656 17 1.4 3 2.5 14 2.3 29 Table C3a: Traffic Citations for White, Black, Hispanic, Asian, Middle Eastern and Native American Drivers Which Result in a Search for All Citations — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Agency Citations Searches % White % Black % Hispanic % Asian % Middle Eastern % Native Am. Total Total Searched Searched Searched Searched Searched Searched AVERAGE 2,668 40 2.0 3.1 3.0 1.3 0.9 3.2 TOTAL 907,028 Table C3b: Traffic Citations for White, Black, Hispanic, Asian, Middle Eastern and Native American Drivers Which Result in a Search for All Citations — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Grouped by Population Size Agency Citations Searches % White % Black % Hispanic % Asian % Middle Eastern % Native Am. Total Total Searched Searched Searched Searched Searched Searched 100,000+ AVERAGE 34,980 286 0.6 1.4 1.5 0.6 0.4 4.4 50,000-99,999 AVERAGE 8,748 141 1.8 3.4 2.8 0.7 0.9 1.2 25,000-49,999 AVERAGE 4,785 58 1.5 1.8 2.4 1.7 0.6 1.1 10,000-24,999 AVERAGE 1,892 38 2.3 3.4 3.6 0.9 1.4 1.7 Under 10,000 AVERAGE 914 17 2.1 3.3 2.9 1.6 0.6 7.2 30 Table C4a: Citations for White Male, Non-White Male, White Female and Non-White Female Drivers Which Result in a Search for All Citations — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Agency Citations Searches Total Total STATE AVERAGE 2,665 40 TOTAL 903,482 % White Male Searched 2.3 % Non-White Male Disparity Searched 3.2 0.9 % White Female Searched 1.3 Table C4b: Citations for White Male, Non-White Male, White Female and Non-White Female Drivers Which Result in a Search for All Citations — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Grouped by Population Size % White % Non-White % Non-White Agency Citations Searches Male Male % White Female Female Total Total Searched Searched Searched Searched 100,000+ AVERAGE 34,899 284 0.7 1.6 0.5 0.7 50,000-99,999 AVERAGE 8,736 141 2.1 3.2 1.2 1.2 25,000-49,999 AVERAGE 4,789 58 1.7 2.4 1.0 1.4 10,000-24,999 AVERAGE 1,897 38 2.6 3.6 1.5 1.7 Under 10,000 AVERAGE 916 17 2.4 3.3 1.4 1.9 31 % NonWhite Female Disparity Searched 1.7 0.4 Table C5a: Traffic Citations for White and Non-White Drivers Which Result in a Search for Only Citations without Arrests — STATEWIDE AVERAGES Agency Citations Total White Searches Non-White Searches Non-White # Non% Total # White White # Searched # Searched % Searched # Searched Searched Disparity STATE AVERAGE 2,523 1,986 544 23 17 1.3 6 1.9 0.6 TOTAL 857,715 Table C5b: Traffic Citations for White and Non-White Drivers Which Result in a Search for Only Citations without Arrests STATEWIDE AVERAGES Grouped by Population Size Agency Citations Total White Searches Non-White Searches Non-White # Non% Total # White White # Searched # Searched % Searched # Searched Searched Disparity 100,000+ AVERAGE 34,408 19,007 15,401 200 81 0.5 119 0.8 0.4 50,000-99,999 AVERAGE 8,221 6,058 2,163 76 50 1.1 26 1.7 0.6 25,000-49,999 AVERAGE 4,547 3,707 840 35 28 1.1 8 1.4 0.3 10,000-24,999 AVERAGE 1,740 1,572 168 20 18 1.4 3 1.9 0.5 Under 10,000 AVERAGE 856 793 67 10 8 1.4 1 2.1 0.8 32