Developing Ratio and Proportion Schemes: A Story of a Fifth Grader Author(s): Jane-Jane Lo and Tad Watanabe Source: Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, Vol. 28, No. 2 (Mar., 1997), pp. 216236 Published by: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/749762 . Accessed: 15/02/2011 20:38 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at . http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=nctm. . Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. http://www.jstor.org Journalfor Researchin MathematicsEducation 1997, Vol. 28, No. 2, 216-236 Developing Ratio and Proportion Schemes: A Story of a Fifth Grader Jane-JaneLo, Cornell University Tad Watanabe,TowsonState University There is a growing theoretical consensus that the concepts of ratio and proportiondo not developin isolation.Rather,theyarepartof the individual'smultiplicativeconceptualfield, which includesotherconceptssuch as multiplication,division,andrationalnumbers.The currentstudy attemptedto clarify the beginning of this developmentalprocess. One fifth-grade student, Bruce,was encouragedto schematizehis trial-and-error-based method,which was effective in solving so-called missing-valuetasks.This studydescribesseveraladvancementsBrucemade during the teaching experiment and analyzes the challenges Bruce faced in attemptingto schematizehis method.Finally, the mathematicalknowledge Bruceneededto furtherdevelop his ratioandproportionconcepts is identified.The findings provideadditionalsupportfor the view thatthe developmentof ratioandproportionconceptsis embeddedwithinthe development of the multiplicativeconceptualfield. Accordingto PiagetandInhelder(1975), proportional reasoningis a second-order relationshipthatinvolves an equivalentrelationshipbetweentwo ratios.Forexample, the statement"Fourcandybarscost five dollars"describesa ratiobetweenan amountof money andthe amountof candythatcan be boughtwith thatamountof money. Yet, proportionalreasoningis needed to figure out how an increasefrom 5 dollarsto 15 dollarsaffects the numberof candy barsone can get. Ratioandproportionareimportantconceptsin currentmathematicscurricula.Very oftenmultiplicationanddivisiontasksin lowergradesarepresentedin unit-rateform, which is a specialformof ratioandproportion.Forexample,"Sallypaid $ 1.25 for each muffin [unitprice].How muchdid she pay for a dozen muffins?"In the middle grades,wordproblemsinvolvingequivalentfractionsandfractioncomparisons can also be thoughtof as ratioandproportionsituations.Forexample,considerthe task "GroupA has 4 pizzas and6 girls. GroupB has 6 pizzas and8 boys. Who gets morepizza,theboys or the girls?"(adaptedfromLamon,1993a).To solve thisproblem, some studentsmay drawpicturesto figure out thatin GroupA, each member gets 2/3of a pizza, whereasin GroupB, each membergets 3/4of a pizza. They can then comparethese two fractionswith the pictures.Otherstudentsmay use ratio andproportionreasoning:"IfI add2 pizzasto GroupA, I wouldneed to add3 more The researchin this paperwas supportedby the ArizonaState UniversityWest Research Grant.The opinions expresseddo not necessarilyreflect the views of the funding agency. An earlyversionof thisarticlewas presentedatthe annualmeetingof theAmericanEducation ResearchAssociation in 1994. This articlewas writtenwhile the first authorwas a Visiting Fellow of the Departmentof Educationat CornellUniversity.The first authorwishes to thank Dr. JereConfreyandDr. David Hendersonfor the opportunityto participatein the activities of the CornellMathematicsEducationResearchGroup. Jane-JaneLo and Tad Watanabe 217 people.So GroupA is likehaving6 pizzasand9 members.So, eachmemberin Group B gets more pizza."The ability to recognize structuralsimilarityand the sense of covariationandmultiplicativecomparisonsillustratedin such a reasoningprocess areat the core of algebraandmoreadvancedmathematics(Confrey& Smith,1995). Because of the importanceof this topic in school mathematics,children'sconeducationresearch, havelongbeena focusof mathematics ceptsof ratioandproportion and much has been learnedaboutstudents'errorsand difficulties in solving ratio andproportiontasks(Hart,1984;Karplus,Pulos,& Stage, 1983) as well as different taskvariablesthataffectstudents'choicesof strategiesandperformance(Harel,Behr, Post, & Lesh, 1991; Kaput& West, 1994). But what arethe roots of these difficulties?Whatarithmeticknowledgemay be useful in developingthe conceptsof ratioandproportion?Vergnaud(1988, p. 141) used the term "multiplicativeconceptualfield" to refer to "all situationsthat can be analyzedas simpleor multipleproportionproblems."Mathematicalconceptsthat are tied to those situationsinclude, as Vergnaudpointedout, multiplication,diviandlinearfunctions.He suggestedthatstudentsdevelop sion,fraction,ratio,proportion, these concepts not in isolation but in concertwith each otherover long periodsof time throughexperiencewith a largenumberof situations.Therefore,researchstudies on children'sratioandproportionconceptsalso need to considerthe otherconcepts thatare a partof children'sdeveloping multiplicativeconceptualfield. In this article,we reportthe findingsfrom such an attempt.The analysisis based on datagatheredfromfifteen 70-minuteteachingsessions with one fifth-gradestudent, Bruce, over a period of 6 months.The purposeof this teachingexperiment was to documentthe schematizingprocessof his self-generatedstrategiesandhow the developmentof these strategiesmightinfluenceor be influencedby otherconstructsof the multiplicativeconceptualfield. Specifically, we askedthe following researchquestions: 1. How did Bruce's informalstrategieswith ratio and proportiontasks develop as he gained knowledge of otherconcepts in his multiplicativeconceptualfield? 2. Whatchallengeswould Bruceface when he was encouragedto schematizehis informalstrategies? 3. Whatmathematicalknowledgewas usefulfor Brucein his developmentof ratio and proportionconcepts? We wereawarethata longitudinalteachingexperimentwas neededto answerthese thatcould questions,andwe hopedthiscase studyof Brucewouldprovideinformation be used in a muchlargerresearchprogram.The currentanalyses,althoughsubjectto haveprovidedus witha paradigmatic furtherrefinementandrefutation, case,havechaland have raised more for our own notions of proportion, questions futureresearch. lenged BRUCE AND HIS INFORMALSTRATEGIES Brucewas 10 yearsand 10 monthsold at thebeginningof theteachingexperiment. He wasjudged to be a top mathematicsstudentby his teacherandwas qualifiedfor an enrichmentprogramofferedeveryThursdaymorningby the schooldistrict.Even 218 DevelopingRatio and ProportionSchemes thoughhe was good at school mathematics,he did not particularlylike it. Whenhe had sparetime, he liked to ride his bicycle, play basketball,and read science ficas he describedit,includedlisteningto theteacher's tion.His wayof doingmathematics, in and class homework. Mathematicsfor him meant either doing explanation which he solved computationalproblems, by applying the proceduredemonstratedby the teacher,or wordproblems,which he solved by choosing the correct operationhe had learnedin class. The mathematicsinstructionin Bruce's classroom was the typical teacherexplanation/student-practice approach.A textbookthat containedmore than 100 mini-unitswas followedfaithfullyby theteacher.Therewas alwaysone correctanswer for every mathematicsproblemand a right way to reachthatanswer.The foci of the fifth-grademathematicstextbookincludedmultidigitmultiplicationand divito decimals,anda littlegraphing.Although initialintroduction sion,fractionoperations, and measurementat the end of the textbook,the therewere a few unitsof geometry teacherdid not have time to cover them. On the basis of his classroomexperience,Bruce did not see the need to provide explanationorjustificationsat the beginningof the teachingexperiment.He rarely madeanycommentsvoluntarily.He frequentlydescribedhis methodas a luckyguess andwas quickto say he couldnot see alternativesolutionsor methods.Nevertheless, he was cooperativeand triedto do his best to answerthe interviewer'squestions. With more experience,Bruce improvedhis abilityto explain his solutionmethod andbeganto see the explanationas an integralpartof his problem-solvingactivity. Bruce was chosen to participatein the teachingexperimentbecause of an interesting strategyhe used to solve ratioandproportiontasks duringa previousstudy (Lo& Watanabe,1993a,1993b)whenhe wasin thefourthgrade.His strategy,although not widely documentedin the existing literature,was also used by four other fourthgradersand one second graderwe interviewed.One sampleis as follows: Task:Yesterday,I bought28 candieswith 12 quarters.Today,if I go to the same store with 15 quarters,how many candies can I buy? [Note: Bruce had 12 quartersand 28 Unifix cubes availablefor his use.] Bruce's SolutionStrategy(2 June 1992) Brucefirstdivided28 "candies"(Unifixcubes)intosevengroupsof 4. Whenasked why, he answered, "Because 4 could be divided into 28." Then he startedto divide the quartersinto groupsof two, countedthe numberof groups,and found thattherewere six groupsratherthanseven groups.Then he went on to find other ways to groupthe quarters.Whennone of those groupingswas satisfactory,he then regroupedthe candies. Afterseveraltries,he madefourgroupsof sevencandies.Thenhe startedto divide 12 quartersinto groupsof 4, ratherthantryingto formfourgroups.Whenit did not seem to work,Brucefinally formedfour groupsof 3 quartersand claimedthatthe 15 as 12 and3 more, relationshipwas "3 quartersfor 7 candies."He reconceptualized thus adding7 to 28 to get 35 candies. Jane-JaneLo and Tad Watanabe 219 Significanceof thisstrategy.Initially,we were surprisedto find thattherewas no recordof sucha strategyin the existingliterature.We conjecturedtwo possiblereasons forthe lackof literatureon thisparticular typeof solutionmethod:(a) Mostexistolder studies worked with students who tended to begin computingrightaway, ing with or withoutanyreasonsbehindthosecomputations;(b) The availabilityof physical objectsmighthave encouragedthe manipulationof objects.In the remainderof this article,we referto Bruce'sstrategyof findinga useful"xquartersfory candies" relationship,thenusing this relationshipas a countableunitto find the answeras a method.We use the word"unit"becauseBrucewas able to conratio-unit/build-up siderthe ratio"3 quartersfor 7 candies"as if it were a unitin a whole-numbersense, such as "3 quartersfor 7 candies, 6 quartersfor 14 candies, 9 quartersfor 21 candies,..." (Lamon,1993a, 1993b).This strategyhas two advantages.First,it avoids any fractionalor decimalcomputationswith which Bruce could not deal comfortably at thattime. Second,it has the potentialto become a powerfulmethodfor solving all the missing-valuetasks withinthe contextsthatmake sense to Bruce. Bruce's strategyseemedto featurea conceptmost researchersconFurthermore, sideran importantelementof proportionconcepts,homogeneity.His gesturesindicated thathe had the intentionto "even out" the quartersand candies. Therewas an implicit notion thata relationshipexisted between the numberof quartersand the numberof candiesin the given condition,andthisrelationshipneededto be preserved between certainsubsets of the quartersand certainsubsets of the candies. Thus,3 morequartersrequired7 morecandies.Not all studentswe hadinterviewed previouslyhad this sense of homogeneity.Forthose students,therewas no conflict in thinkingthatone groupof 2 quartersgot 6 candiesandanothergroupof 2 quarters got 4 candies when given the initial conditionof "4 quartersfor 10 candies."' However, at thattime we did not know whetherBruce would be able to schematize this strategy. Bruce'sratio-unit/build-up methodwas nota schemebecauseit was notyet a deliberateandreflective activity.Accordingto von Glasersfeld(1989), Piaget's notion of scheme consists of threeparts:(a) the child's recognitionof an experientialsituation as one thathas been experiencedbefore; (b) the specific activity the child has come to associate with the situation;and (c) the resultthatthe child has come to expect of the activity in the given situation.To schematizethis method,Bruce neededto (a) articulatemathematicallythe goal of his trial-and-error-based actions; (b) give mathematicalmeaningto theseactions,thusmakingthe wholeprocessmore systematic;(c) internalizehis physicalactions so thathe could execute them mentally withoutthe sensory-motoractions;and (d) generalizehis actions acrosssimilar ratio and proportionsituations. 1Therealso were studentswho questionedthe validity of buying two candiesfor half of the price on the basis of theirown daily experience.They arguedthatthe shop might chargea higherpricefor buying a few; for example, "Onefor 35 cents and threefor a dollar."With these studentswe triedto negotiate thatthis particularstore would indeed sell half the numberof the candiesfor half the price. 220 DevelopingRatio and ProportionSchemes RESEARCHMETHODOLOGY In this study we operatedfrom a constructivistview of knowledge as the epistemologicalbasis for examiningchildren'smathematicalthinking.We believe that knowledgeoriginatesin a learner'sactivityperformedon mentalconstructsthatare directlyrelatedto the action and experienceof thatlearner(Wheatley, 1991) and that learningoccurs when an individualadaptshis or her schemes to cope with a problematicsituation(Steffe, 1990). Therefore,as researchers,we had two highly relatedtasks,(a) to identifyanddevelopmathematical tasksthatarelikelyto be problematic to individual students and have the potential to advance their current mathematicalknowledge,and(b) to listento childrencarefullyso thatwe canunderstandtheirmathematicsandfind ways to discuss it with them (Confrey, 1991). In the process,we were preparedto examineandrevise ourown mathematicalunderstandingas we triedto understandthe meaningindividualstudentsgave to different ratio andproportiontasks. Duringeach teachingsession, selectedmathematicaltaskswere presentedto the students.Tools such as paper,colored pens, rulers,and a calculatorwith fraction operationswere availablefor theiruse. The studentswere encouragedto verbalize and reflect on theirthinkingprocesses. All sessions were videotaped.Preliminary analyseswereconductedaftereach sessionto developnew tasksfor subsequentsessions. At the sametime we also revisedourtentativemodelof these students'developmentof ratioandproportionschemesin light of othermathematicsconceptsthat arose during the sessions (e.g., unit construction and coordination,arithmetic operations,measurement,and rationalnumberconcepts). We emphasizethatthe word teaching here means providingpotentiallearning for studentsto constructmathematical meaningoveran extensiveperiod opportunities of time. The objectives of the teachingexperimentwere to (a) identify constructive mechanisms that children use in establishing ratio and proportionknowledge, (b) studythe dynamicsof children'sconstructionsof these concepts,and (c) identifyinstructionalactivitiesconsistentwith the children'sconstructiveprocesses (Hunting,1983). The dataanalyseswere heavily influencedby ourown understandingof the various concepts in the multiplicativeconceptualfield and by our knowledge of the researchliteratureon children'sknowledge of these concepts. Neitherof these is static. Some recentstudieshave influencedour analyses.Among the most significant areVergnaud's(1988) workon multiplicativestructures,Schwartz's(1988) work on intensive quantity, Confrey and Smith's (1995) Splitting Conjecture, Steffe's (1988) workon multiplicationanddivision, and Streefland's(1984, 1985, 1991) work on fraction,ratio,and proportion. RESULTS Throughoutthe teaching experiment, we used many different mathematical tasks to help Bruce develop his ratio and proportionschemes. As much as possible we triedto let Bruce's actionsandexplanationsguide the choices of new tasks. Jane-Jane Lo and Tad Watanabe 221 In the following sections, we firstpresentan overview of the teachingexperiment, including sample activities and brief descriptions of significant events. The overview is followed by detailedanalyses of these events. Overviewof the TeachingExperiment We startedthe firstsession(8 December1992)withthecandy-buyingtaskbecause Brucehad success with it duringthe previousinvestigation.To help Brucefocus on thenumberrelationship, we usedtaskswithdifferentratios,progressingfromeasy ones = = 2:6 8:? to ?:21) hardones (e.g.,9:12= 21:?= ?:40).Duringthissession,Bruce (e.g., usedmultiplication andmissing-multiplicand approachesto solve taskswithratiosthat canbe simplifiedto 1:2and1:3.Otherwise,he eitherusedtheratio-unit/build-up method describedabove or he guessedthe numberof candiesperquarter(e.g., I and 1/2per quarter,I and 1/3per quarter)to solve more difficult tasks. We conjecturedthat Brucewas tryingto avoidthe divisionoperationandfractions.We also conjectured thatremovingthe physicalobjectsfor manipulationmighthelpBrucefocus moreon the numberrelationshipwhentherewere difficultratios. To test these conjectures,we continuedto use the candy-buyingtaskswith difficult ratiosbut did not providephysicalcounters(14 December 1992). In addition, we also introducedcomparative-ratio tasksin a pizza-sharingcontext(e.g., "Group A has 2 pizzasand 3 girls. GroupB has 3 pizzas and4 boys") in Session 2. Without the physical objectsfor manipulation,Bruce startedto drawcircles to help him. Duringthis session we observedthatBruce would assign values of mixed fractions to circles symbolizingquartersor candiesbut would physicallycut lines for circles symbolizingpizzas.This observationmadeus wonderaboutthe role of context in Bruce's solutionstrategies.Therefore,Sessions 3 (10 February1993) and4 (17 February1993) hadidenticalmissing-valuetasks,butone used a candy-buying situationandthe otheruseda pizza-sharingsituation.TheresultconfirmedthatBruce conceptualizedthese situationsdifferently.During these two sessions, we also chose the numbersin the taskcarefullyto furtherstudyBruce'sconceptsof fractions. For example,the task "12 studentsshare16 pizzas equally,so how muchpizza can 3 studentsget?"was givento see if Brucewouldnoticethefractionrelationship between "12 students"and "3 students"because he had used the multiple relationship between "3 students"and "12 students"to help him solve a similar problem before. But Bruce paid little attentionto the relationshipand solved the task with his usual trial-and-errorapproach. The next five sessions were moreexploratory.Based on ouranalysesof the first four sessions, we wonderedif the multiple/fractionrelationshipmight be formed more easily with quantities in continuous contexts than in discrete contexts. Therefore,we introducedtwo tasks,Magic LiquidandFish Feeding,which hadthe following basic formats: Magic-liquidtask:"A house is 9 feet tall and has a window which is 6 feet abovetheground.Thishousebecame18 feet tallafterapplyinga certainamount of magic liquid.How tall would the window be above the groundafterapplying the magic liquid?" 222 Developing Ratio and ProportionSchemes Fish-feedingtask (adaptedfromPiaget, Grize, Szeminska,& Bang, 1977): "Fish A is 9 centimeterslong and Fish B is 4 centimeterslong. If Fish A needs 9 pieces of food every day, how many pieces of food does Fish B need every day?" Notice thatin the magic-liquidtask,the heightof the window is a fractionalpart of the house, andboth arecontinuousquantities.The idea of "magicliquids,"liquidsthatcanenlargeor shrinkeitherhorizontallyor vertically,was presentedthrough a book Anno's Math GameIII (Anno, 1991) in Session 5 (February24, 1993). The fish-feedingtask was presentedwith a pictureof two lines to symbolize the lengths of the two fish. These pictureshelped to establisha linearrelationshipfor this particulartask. In this task the length of the fish is a continuousquantity,but the numberof food pieces is a discretequantity.These two tasks, along with the candy-buyingtask (discretevs. discrete),were used repeatedlywith variousnumber sizes and ratiosthroughoutthe rest of the teachingexperiment.2 Two majorteachinggoals for the last six sessions graduallyemergedat the end of the ninthsession. The firstgoal was to help Brucebuild a deeperunderstanding of numberstructuressuch as multiplesanddivisors.This knowledgewas essential for Bruce to develop a strategyfor identifyingthe ratiounitin his ratio-unit/buildup methodsystematically.The secondgoal was to help him constructandintegrate his knowledge of division, which includedinterpretingthe meaningof a division operation,recognizingtheneedfor a divisionoperation,developingmeaningsbehind his division algorithm, and integratinghis understandingof division with his existingknowledgeaboutnumbersandoperations.This kindof understandingwas crucialwhenBrucetriedto solve taskswith largernumbers.We also decidedto pay special attentionto those of Bruce's mentalimages that seemed to have an effect on his choice of strategies.The following area few examplesof the taskswe used to achieve these goals. Coveringtask (adaptedfrom Reynolds, 1993): "Ihave lots of small 2-by-2 squares.How many of these small squaresdo I need to have in orderto cover a big rectanglethatis 30 by 40 with no gaps and overlappings?" Cuttingtask:"A wood blockis 36 centimeterslong, 48 centimeterswide, and 60 centimeterstall. I wantto cut this block into cubes of exactly the same size withnothingleft over.Whatis the biggestsize of cubeI can cut, andhow many cubes will I get?" task:"Ifa rectangular pizza(2 inchesby 10 inches)is largeenough Pizza-sharing to serve 4 people, how many people can be served with a pizza 5 inches by 8 inches?" In the subsequentsections,we addressthe threeresearchquestionsthroughexamples of Bruce's experiencewith differentmathematicaltasks. 2Werecognize the artificialfeaturesof many of the tasks we used and believe that classroomadaptationof these activities would requiremuch carefulthought. Jane-JaneLoandTadWatanabe 223 TheDevelopmentof Bruce's Unit-Ratio/Build-UpMethod To encourageBruceto focus on the mathematicalmeaningof his actions,we first removedthe physicalobjectsfromthe candy-buyingtask.Brucerespondedby drawing circles to representquartersandcandies(see Figure1 for an example).The fact thatBrucecould not move these pictorialcirclesfreelyhadindeedcreatedthe need for him to reflect on the purposeof his actions, thatis, to find the equivalentrelationshipbetweenthe numberof quartersandthe numberof candies.It also created the need to develop a more systematictrial-and-error approach-"one quarterfor two candies,""twoquartersfor threecandies, "onequarterfor one and a half candies," and so on. He was more aware of the requirementof coordinatinghis actions with the numberof quartersand the numberof candies. Figure 1. Bruce's drawingfor the candy-buyingtask Episode 1 The pictures were an important part of Bruce's thinking process. On 17 February1993 (the fourth session), we asked Bruce to solve the following task without drawingcircles: Yesterday,I bought8 candieswith 12 quarters.Today,if I go to the same store with 9 quarters,how many candies can I buy? Withoutpictures,Brucelost the meaningof this situation.He concludedthatthe answerwould be 5 because 8 is 4 less than 12 and 5 is 4 less than9. This "additive error,"which was documentedby Hart(1984) in her studywith olderstudents,had 224 Developing Ratio and ProportionSchemes not appearedin Bruce's reasoningwhen he was allowed to use physicalobjectsor to drawpictures.Brucelaterrealizedhis errorwhenhe triedto use the sameapproach to answerthe question,"Howmanycandiescan six quartersbuy?"Brucethenwrote down numerals1 to 12 in one column and 1 to 8 in anothercolumn and used the numeralsas circles to solve the task (Figure2). Bruce's initial action of finding a rightway to groupquarterssuggestedto us the need to find a factor(divisor)of the total number of quarters.To coordinate with the grouping of candies, Bruce needed to find an appropriatecommonfactor3of the total numberof quartersand the total numberof candies. Once the ratiounit was established,Bruce could use the coordinatedbuild-upapproachto find the solution.He could furthercurtailthis processby the abbreviatedbuild-upapproach,which requiredan understandingof the division operation. Figure 2. Bruce's solution when asked not to drawcircles Thus,we providedactivitiesthatwouldencouragehimto makesucha construction. It took fourmore sessionsbeforewe saw any evidencethatBrucewas awareof the idea of "commonfactor"and able to provideverbalexplanationsof his method. Episode 2 On 7 April 1993 (theeighthsession),Brucewas askedto solve thefollowingtask: becauseBrucewas not thinkingaboutreducingthe ratio 3Wedeliberatelyused the word"appropriate" to the smallestintegerratio,butjust a reducedratiothatwouldhelphim achievehis goal, "avoidingcomputationsinvolving fractionsor decimals."For example,on 2 June 1993, Brucewas askedto solve the following task: ABC Toy Storesells 400 toy carsfor 640 dollars.(1) How muchdoes it cost to buy 160 toy cars?(2) How many toy cars can a personbuy with 128 dollars? Brucefoundthe commonfactorof 10 between400 and640. Thenhe usedthe following table to answerboth questions. 120 160 40 80 256 128 192 64 Jane-Jane Lo and Tad Watanabe 225 A house was 24 feet tall andhad a window thatwas 12 feet above the ground. Thishousebecame18 feet tallaftera certainamountof magicliquidwas applied. Howtallwouldthewindowbe abovethegroundafterthemagicliquidwasapplied? Brucesaid"9feet"ratherquickly,yet his explanationrevealeda rathercomplicated thoughtprocess.He explained,"BecauseI divided6 into 24, came up with 4, 6 into 18, came up with 3, so I divided somethinginto 12, and made it into fourth,and I took the third."The interviewer asked Bruce to explain again. Bruce said, "I divided 6 into 24, came up with 4, 6 into 18 came up with 3. I took the 3 for ... the 3 for 24 is18, 3 for 12 is 9. Nine is one half of 18. Twelve is one half of 24." Still, the interviewerwas not sure wherethe six came from. Brucethen wrotedown the followinglist andelaborated:"Thehousewentdownone fourth,so thewindowwent down one fourth." 4 3 8 6 12 9 16 12 20 15 24 18 Bruce's solution of this task clearly indicatedhis intentionto find the multiplicative relationshipbetweenthe new heightof thehouseandthe old heightof thehouse, which he knew neededto be preservedbetween the new height of the window and the old height of the window. He identified6 as a common factorbetween 24 and 18. The number 24 was reconceptualizedas 4 sixes, and the number 18 was reconceptualizedas 3 sixes. Then it appearedthat there was another level of reconceptualization,similarto what Lamon (1993a) describedas "norming":the 4 sixes were reconceptualizedas "one,"but the 3 sixes were reconceptualizedas "threefourths."Bruce's last statements,"Nineis one half of 18. Twelve is one half of 24," did not appearto be partof his originalthinking,butratheranotherway to explain his solution.Implicitly,Bruce seemed to be saying, "See, all these equivalent relationshipsprove thatmy answeris correct." In summary,Bruceidentifiedthe "went-down-one-fourth" relationshipbetween the old height of the house and the new height of the house, which he knew needed to be preservedbetween the old height of the window and the new height of the window. More significantly,once he had constructedthis relationship,his methodwould work whetherthe magic liquid enlargedor shortenedthe house. Episode 3 Duringthe next session (21 April 1993), Bruce developed a new interpretation of this numericalre-presentation.He was asked to solve the following task: Fish A is 18 centimeterslong andFish B is 12 centimeterslong. If Fish B needs 60 piecesof foodeveryday,how manypiecesof foodwill FishA needeveryday? Priorto this session, Bruce had solved similartasks eitherby his ratio-unit/buildup method or by finding how many pieces of food a 1-cm-longfish would need. Both approacheswere still basedon trial-and-error. Forexample,he would try 11/2 pieces of food for 1 cm, 11/3pieces of food for 1 cm, and so forth,until he found somethingthatworked. DevelopingRatio and ProportionSchemes 226 To solve this particular task, Bruce first did two computations, 18 x 4 = 72 and 18 x 3 = 54, on paper.We inferredthatBruce wantedto know if a "niceratio"existed between 18 and 60. He found out throughthese two computationsthat60 was not a multipleof 18. (Brucedid not realizethat60 pieces of food was whatFish B, but not whatFish A, needed.Nevertheless,the rest of this thinkHe thenstaredat the result ing processwas still valid withinhis own interpretation.) andkept silentfor about50 seconds.It appearedthathe was tryingto find an alternative approach.He then wrote down the following: 3 6 9 12 15 18 21 24 27 30 x2 60 He laterexplainedthathe madethis list by firstidentifying3 as a numberthatcould be divided into 18 and 60 evenly, andhe knew if he wrote down all the multiples of 3, the list wouldincludeboth 18 and60, andthatlist was whathe wantedto have. Afterlookingat the resultfor 10 seconds,he pointedsix timesfrom3 to 18. Then he stoppedand thought,while tappinga few times on the 18. He then wrotedown the following row of numbersunderneaththe numbersabove: 2 4 6 8 10 12 12 14 16 18 x2 40 Bruce then announced,"Fortypieces of food." His explanation indicatedthathe wantedto findanotherlist of multiplesthatincluded 12 in the same position in the sequence as 18 in the originalsequence,thatis, the sixthposition.Thenwhatevercorrespondedto 60 in the secondsequencewouldbe the answer.He was able to come up with this list by findingthe multiplicativerelationshipbetween3 and 18, anddecidedthe samerelationshipneededto be preserved between the numberhe was seeking and 12. This is a very complicatedsolution, andit demonstratesa genuineattemptto coordinatethreeknownquantitiesandone unknownquantityto preserveone relationship.At this time,Brucewas alreadyable to take the table he created from the build-up process as a mental object and could build the whole table (multiplesof 2 and 3) directlyfrom partof it (the relative position of 18, 60, and 12). Episode 4 EventhoughBruceintendedto find a commonfactor,he hadnot developeda systematicway to do it, especially when the numberswere large. He also continued to experiencedifficultyin his attemptto curtailthe build-upprocess throughmultiplicationanddivisionwhen the numberswere large.Forexample,on 5 May 1993 (the eleventh session), Bruce was given the following task: A carof thefuturecantravel8 milesin 3 minutes.How farwill it travelin 5 hours? Bruce picked up the pencil and wrote down "60 + 3 = 20. 20 x 5 = 100."Then he claimedthe answerwas 100 miles. The intervieweraskedBruceto explainthe meaningof his computation.Bruce was not able to do so. The interviewerasked him to reconsiderhis computation.Bruce thoughta while and wrote down "20 x 8 = 160"butwas not sureif he got it right.Whenhe was askedto explainthis again, Bruce changedto the build-upapproachand made the following table: Jane-JaneLo and Tad Watanabe 8 16 24 32 227 40 48 56 64 72 80 12 6 15 18 21 24 27 3 9 30 He thenadded10 eightiestogetherandgot 800. Brucecommentedthatit was a long way andappearedto be a littlerestless.The interviewerencouragedBruce.She said thateven thoughthis methodtook a long time, it was still a good methodbecause it was meaningfulto him. Also, Bruce was more confidentaboutthe result.After the interviewer'scomment,Brucesaid, "Oh,I know how, I know I hadto do these" (pointingto his originalcomputations).The interviewerencouragedBruce to follow through.Bruce said, "Twentytimes 5 is right,thenI had to time 8." The interviewer then asked, "If you computedthis first (20 x 8), then what did you need to do next? Whatdoes 160 mean?"Bruce then realizedthat 160 was the distancefor 1 hour;thus he needed to multiply 160 by 5 to get the distancefor 5 hours.Bruce continuedto requiresimilarprobingsto applythe abbreviated build-upapproachduring the next threesessions.We believe thathis difficultycame largelyfromhis lack of experiencewith givingmeaningto his multiplicationanddivisioncomputationsfor example, formingan intensive quantitylike "xpieces of food per centimeter." Bruce also continuedto avoid using fractionsor decimals in any computations otherthanrepeatedaddition.Nevertheless,his uneasinesswith fractionsanddecimal numbersseemed to decreaseas he "discovered"the fractionfunctionkeys on the calculatorandfiguredout how to use those keys fromreadingthe manual.(The role of the calculatorin Bruce's developmentof variousconcepts like multiplication, division,fractions,anddecimalnumberswas one areathatwe wished we had had time to explore more.) Episode 5 On 2 June 1993 (the last session), Bruce was given the following task: FishA is 108 centimeterslong andFishB is 48 centimeterslong.If FishA needs 45 piecesof foodeveryday,how manypiecesof foodwill FishB needeveryday? As usual, Bruce went for the calculatorimmediately.He entered108 + 48 = 2.25, then 108 + 45 = 2.4. Again,he did not like the results,becauseneitherof thesecomputationsgave him whole-numberquotients.The interviewerencouragedBruceto thinkaboutwhateach computationmeant.Brucethoughta while, entered45 + 2.25 into the calculator,and got an answer,20. He was quite sureabouthis answer.He explained,"BecauseFish B is 2.25 pieces of Fish A. So he could eat 2.25 pieces of food of Fish A." (Note:We believedthatBrucehadtherightidea,butthathe had just mislabeledthese fish). This was the first time Bruce accepteda nonintegeras a divisor,andhe was now muchmorearticulate aboutthemeaningof his multiplication and division operationseven when fractionsand decimals were involved. Challengesto Bruce's Attemptto SchematizeHis Ratio-Unit/Build-UpMethod From the five examples above, we could identify the following challenges Brucehadwhen developinghis ratio-unit/build-up method.The firstchallengewas 228 DevelopingRatio and ProportionSchemes to distancehimselffromthe actionof doing,to reflecton the meaningof his actions, and then finally to construct a mental object of these activities that he could manipulate.The processtook place gradually.Brucefirstdrewpicturesof quarters and candies to representthe physical objects (Figure 1). These pictureswere static, primarilyfor the purposeof signifyinghis actions(Figure2). At the sametime, he also developed a numericaltable to keep trackof the build-upprocess afterhe identifiedtheratiounit.The needfor a tablearosewhenlargenumberswereinvolved in the tasks. Finally, the table in Episode 2 became an object.Bruce knew how to createthe entirelist (multiplesof 4 vs. multiplesof 3) from one element of it (24 vs. 18). By creatingan appropriatetable, Bruce was able to identify the ratiounit (whichwas less importantfor him now) andthe answerhe was seekingwithinone framework.In the followingweek, Brucestartedto curtailthe tableprocessby using multiplicationto replacea seriesof repeatedadditions(Episode3). His originalratiounit/build-upmethodhad takenan entirelydifferentform,which came very close to being called a scheme. Unfortunately,we did not have enoughtime with him to see its furtherdevelopment. The secondchallengewas to distinguishbetweenpartitiveandquotitivedivision andto determinewhichdivisionwas usefulfora givensituation.Whenanalyzedcarefully,Bruce'sinitialstrategyrequireda shiftbetweenquotitiveandpartitivedivision. In his initialstrategy,he firsttriedto put all quartersinto groupsof certainamounts of his choice (quotitivedivision),andthenhe neededto groupcandiesaccordingly so thatthenumberof candygroupswouldbe the sameas thenumberof quartergroups (partitivedivision).The switchfromquotitivedivisionto partitivedivisionwas difficult for him. Whenhe successfullyput 12 quartersinto fourgroupsof 3 (quotitive division),thenextthinghe wantedto do was to put28 candiesintogroupsof 3 (quotitive division)ratherthantryingto put28 candiesintofourgroups(partitivedivision). Withoutthe shifting,he couldstillget whathe wantedby trialanderror.We suspected thatwas why his pictureswere so importantto him. Later,Brucedid makethe shift successfullywhen creatinghis table (Episode3). Identifying3 as a commonfactor of 18 and60 involveda quotitivedivision.Then,when he countedhow manysteps between 3 and 18, he was essentiallytryingto decidehow many groupshe needed to makewith 12, which was a partitivedivision. When the numbersinvolved in a task were large, Bruceknew thatit would take a long time to make his table. Thus, he createdthe thirdchallenge for himself: to curtailthe build-upprocess.Normally,he attemptedto achieve this goal by either making"jumps"withinthe table,as in Episode 3, or using multiplicationanddivision, as in Episode4. Brucehadless troublewiththe firstapproachbecausethe table was meaningfulto him. When using multiplicationand division with large numbers, Bruce experiencedboth technical andconceptualdifficulties. The technicaldifficultycame fromthe fact thatin school he hadnot learnedhow to divide with a multidigitdivisor,how to write a quotientin fractionalformat,or how to compute with fractions. At the beginning of the teaching experiment, Bruce had only learned how to divide a multiple-digitnumber by a one-digit number.He statedthathe couldnot solve a problemsuch as "1000dividedby 105" (3 March 1993) because he had not learnedthatin school. As for fractions,Bruce Jane-Jane Lo and Tad Watanabe 229 a fractionwitha circularorrectangular hadlearnedthenamingof a fraction,representing shape,and comparingunit fractionslike 1/2and 1/5.However,we soon realizedthat these technical difficulties were not the main obstacle in curtailinghis build-up process.Withsome help,Brucewas ableto solve computationaltaskslike 21/3x 30 by calculatingthe whole-numberpartandthefractionalpartseparately.By theend of thefourthteachingsession(17 February1993),Brucehadlearnedin classhow to representa quotientin fractionformat(e.g., converting126 + 8 = 15 R 6 to 153/4). Bruce'sconceptualdifficultywithcurtailingthebuild-upprocesswas muchgreater and more complicated.Ourdatashowed thathis confusion frequentlycame from a limitedunderstandingof divisionandmultiplicationoperations.Forexample,the following episode, which occurredon 28 April 1993 (1 week priorto Episode 4) sheds some light on his limited understandingof multiplicationoperation. Episode 6 A helicopterflies 16 miles fromthe airportto a downtownhotelin 10 minutes. At this rate, how far could the helicopterfly in 2 hours?" Bruce solved this problemby makinga long table like the following: 10 16 20 32 30 48 ... ... 110 176 120 192 He used a calculator,on which he repeatedlyentered"+ 16"each time, to help him writedownthebottomrow.TheintervieweraskedBruceif he couldthinkof a quicker way to do it, thinkingthathe might use multiplicationand division. Surprisingly, Bruceanswered,"NotanythingI knowof' ratherquickly.Theinterviewerthenasked Bruce to close his eyes, and they had the following dialogue: Interviewer: A helicopteris flying16 milesper10 minutes.Canyoutellmehowfarit canfly for50 minutes? I canmultiply16times5. Bruce: Interviewer: Why? Bruce: Interviewer: Bruce: Interviewer: Because5 times10is 50. Closeyoureyes.Howabout2 hours? Multiplyby 120. Yes, 2 hoursare120minutes.Whatdo youreallywantto multiply? Brucestillwantedto multiplyby 120, even thoughtheinterviewerkeptemphasizing that 16 miles is for 10 minutes.So, the interviewergave Brucea new piece of paper and asked him to write down exactly what he wantedto do to solve this question. Quickly,Brucewrotedown "120x 16 = 1920."Whenconfrontedwith the answer he got previously, Bruce said, "Oh,I need to take away the zero" and proceeded to cross the 0 out of 1920. However,when askedwhy he wantedto cross the 0 out, Brucesaidfranklythathe hadno idea.The intervieweraskedBruceto close his eyes again, and posed anotherquestion,"Sixteenmiles 10 minutes, 100 minutes...?" 230 Developing Ratio and ProportionSchemes Bruce: One-hundred andsixty.Because100isjustadding0 to 10,so youadd0 to 16. Interviewer: So howabout110minutes? Bruce: Interviewer: Add 16 to 160. Whatmultiplication Okay,orI cando what?If I wantto use one multiplication. can I use? Brucewas confused.He said it's not possible to do just one multiplication.Then the interviewerasked Bruce to open his eyes, and she wrote down "16 x 10 + 16 = 16 x ?" and asked Bruce to fill the blank.Bruce solved this task by first calculatingthe left side andgot 176. He thenused the missing-multiplicand approachand 11 he his Then that confirmed with a calculator. the interwork; guess guessed might viewer gave Bruce a similartask, "16 x 10 +16 x 3 = 16 x ?"Again, Bruce solved this taskwith the same approach,butthis time, he hadto guess threetimes, first 16, then 14, then 13. Bruce still did not see any patternsin these two questions.So the interviewerasked Bruce to close his eyes again. Interviewer: Canyouthinkaboutstringsof beads?Everystringhas 16beads.If I have 10 strings,thenadd3 strings. Bruce: Interviewer: Say thatagain? If I have 10 stringsof beads. Each stringhas 16 beads on it. Now I add 3 morestrings.Theyall have16 on eachof them.So howmanybeadsdo I havealtogether? Bruce thoughta while. Suddenly,he smiled, openedhis eyes, and said, "It's 13 x 16. " The interviewerthen directedBruce's attentionto the originalquestion,and Bruce figuredout he could multiply 16 by 12 to get 192. as repeated Thisandseveralpreviousinstancessuggestedto us thatthe"multiplication addition"model made sense to Bruceonly when the multiplicandwas less than 10. Moreover,althoughBrucecouldcarryouttwo-digitby two-digitmutliplicationwith the learnedprocedureeasily, he was not surehow andwhy the procedureworked. Bruce's understandingof division operations and the division algorithmwere similarlyproceduralwhen the divisor was largerthan 10. Thus, he had to relearn each type of multiplicationand division as the number increased. Like Steffe (1988), we do not think "multiplicationas repeated addition"was a primitive model for Bruce.The mentaldemandin buildingan iterableunitlargerthan 10 was of multiplication Brucehada gapin his understanding great.Withoutthisconstruction, and division. Mathematical Knowledge Needed for Bruce to Develop His Ratio and Proportion Schemes The observationsdiscussedin the previoussectionsled us to concludethatthe folknowledgewas usefulfor Bruce'sdevelopmentof his ratioand lowingmathematical proportionschemes:(a) the structureof numbers,suchas divisorsandmultiples;(b) anddivisionthroughmultidigitnumbers;(c) famila conceptualbasisof multiplication of with a variety multiplicationanddivision situations,includingbothquotiiarity tive and partitivedivision; (d) meaningfulmultiplicationand division algorithms; Jane-Jane Lo and Tad Watanabe 231 and (e) integrationof the above with the developmentof rational-number concepts. Anotherimportant observationconcernedtheinfluenceof mentalimageson Bruce's strategies (Lo & Watanabe, 1993a). For example, Bruce changed his strategy from the ratio-unit/build-upapproachto the "how manyper one" approachwhen we changedthe settingof the tasksfromcandiesto pizzas,even thoughall the numbers stayed the same. This suggests thatmany of the tasks encouragedthe use of drawings,which tendedto be geometricallyrich (Streefland,personalcommunication). We were surprisedto see Bruce's difficultywith some geometricallyrich tasks.Forexample,we used the following questionto understandBruce's concepts of multiplicativestructure(adaptedfrom Reynolds, 1993): I have lots of small2-by-2 squares.How manyof these smallsquaresdo I need to cover a big rectanglethatis 30 by 40 with no gaps and overlappings? Immediately, Bruce added 30 and 40, got 70, divided 70 by 2, and got 35, which he claimedwas the answer.To help Brucereevaluatethe situation,the intervieweraskedhim to drawa picturefor an easiertask,"Coveran 8-by-8 squarecompletely with 2-by-2 squares"(see Figure3a). The activityof drawinghelpedBruce form a more elaboratedimage of the situation,even thoughit was still not correct. After being remindedthatthe big squareneeded to be completely coveredby the small squares,Bruceredrewanotherpicture(Figure3b). In this picture,Brucefirst drew four squaresacrossfrom the left to the right,addedthreemore down, added threemore across from the right to the left, then addedthreemore up to complete the outline.Finally,he drewa "cross"in the middleto cover the "hole"in the middle. Whenaskedhow manysquareshe needed,Brucecounted"one,two, three,four" across fromthe rightto the left, then "five, six, seven, eight"fromthe rightagain. Whenhe realizedthathe hadfoursquaresin eachrows,he recountedthe whole picture as "four,eight, twelve, sixteen." (A) (B) Figure 3. Bruce's initial drawingsfor covering tasks 232 DevelopingRatio and ProportionSchemes However,this experiencestill did not help Brucesolve the originaltask.He now claimedtheanswershouldbe "140,"whichhe gotby adding30, 30, 40, and40 together. He was not ableto makefurtheradjustmentwhenthe interviewerpointedout to him thateachsmallsquarewas already2 inchesby 2 inches.Theinterviewerconjectured thatthe confusionmightbe due to the factthatsquareshave equallengthandwidth. So, she changedthe questionto "...2-by-3 rectangles,to cover 6-by-9 rectangle." Brucethoughtaboutthenew question.He carefullydrewtheoutlineof therectangle by drawing"oneunit"at a time.He thencarefullydrewthe outlineof the 2-by-3rectangle.He startedto dividethe sides into twos andthrees.He thenproceededto subdividethe whole rectangleinto 9 smallrectangles(Figure4). The interviewerasked, "Istherea way you candecidehow manyyou will get on thisside withoutdrawing?" Brucesaid"yes"immediatelyandexplained,"Divide2 into 6, you will get 3." The interviewerrepeatedthe same questionon the otherside. Bruceansweredcorrectly. Thenthe interviewersaid,"Onceyou decidedthatyou got 3 on this side and3 on the otherside,will yoube ableto knowhow manyrectanglesyouwill need?"Brucereplied, "Yeah.Multiply3 by 3."It was clearto us thatBrucehaddevelopeda mentalimage of this type of taskanda procedureto solve it with confidence.We believe thatit is very importantfor studentsto work on this type of geometricallyrich task to build meaningfulandflexible mentalimagesthatcan facilitatethe developmentof multiplicationanddivisionconceptsat a muchdeeperlevel. Figure 4. Bruce's drawingfor the covering task:Use 2-by-3 rectanglesto cover 6-by-9 rectangle DISCUSSION Throughoutthe teachingexperiment,Bruce had developedmethodsotherthan method.Thosemethodsweresimilarto whatVergnaud(1988) theratio-unit/build-up identified as scalar and functional methods. However, because task variables were notthefocus of ourstudy,we can only offerthefollowinglimitedobservations for consideration. Jane-JaneLo and Tad Watanabe 233 Compatible with the previous findings of Kaput and West (1994), Bruce's choice of methodappearedto be influencedby threemajorfactors:(a) the size of the numbers,(b) the type of ratio, and (c) the situationpresentedin each task. For example,when askedto solve the task, "If2 quarterscan buy 6 candies,how many candies can 8 quartersbuy," Bruce determinedthe unit price, threecandies for 1 quarter,thenmultipliedthe unitpriceby 8 to solve this task (functionalreasoning). When he was asked to solve anothertask, "If 4 quarterscan buy 10 candies, how many candies can 12 quartersbuy,"Bruce figuredout the multiplicativerelationship between4 quartersand 12 quarters,thenmultiplied10 candiesby 3 (scalarreasoning). Also, he used a scalarrelationshipto solve the magic-liquidtask (Episode 2) andused functionalreasoningto solve the fish-feedingtask (Episode3). When the ratiosinvolved could not be reducedto either 1:Kor K:1 (with K being a positive integer)or the numbersinvolvedbecamelarge,Brucepreferredto use theratiounit/build-upmethod.Bruceseemedto havethe mostconfidencethatthisparticular methodwould give him the correctanswereventually.Generallyspeaking,Bruce became more and more flexible and effective in choosing an efficient methodfor a particulartask. of multiplication,diviIn this study,we identifiedBruce's limitedunderstanding sion, and fractionand decimal concepts as the root of his difficulties in developing ratioandproportionconcepts.Do these findingsimply thatinstructionon ratio andproportionshouldwait untila moreintegratedunderstandingof multiplication, division, and fractionand decimalconcepts is achieved?Ouransweris "no."Our analyses demonstratedthatratio and proportiontasks were accessible to younger students(see also Schorn,1989;Van den Brink& Streefland,1979),andthese tasks have the potentialto encouragestudentsto examine theirknowledge of multiplicationanddivisionandto recognizethe need for havingnonintegernumbers.Being exposed to the routinemultiplicationanddivisiontasksalone will not help students build a deeperunderstandingof these concepts. When asked to solve a varietyof ratioandproportiontasks,studentswill developtheirratioandproportionconcepts with othertopics in theirmultiplicativeconceptualfields, just as Bruce did. The presentstudy has suggested several possible roots of students'difficulties with ratio and proportiontasks. These findings provide additionalsupportfor the view thatthe developmentof ratioandproportionconceptsis embeddedwithinthe developmentof the multiplicativeconceptualfields. The observationswe reported here suggestthatthe interrelationships andinterdependencyof the topics withinthe fields are multiplicativeconceptual complex.Bruce'sconceptsof ratioandproportion were influencedby his understandingof such topics as multiplicationanddivision operations.However,his experienceswith ratioandproportiontasksalso provided contextswithinwhichhe was ableto developmorecomplexunderstanding of those operations.Additionalinvestigations,longitudinalin nature,areneededto answer many questionsraisedor left unansweredby this preliminaryresearch. Consideringthe growingliteratureon elementaryschoolteachers'difficultieswith divisionandrational-number concepts(Ball, 1990;Graeber,Tirosh,& Glover,1989; Simon, 1993),it shouldnotbe surprisingthatthesedifficultiesexistfor middleschool or high school students,thusinterferingwith theirlearningof ratioandproportion 234 Developing Ratio and Proportion Schemes concepts.Futureresearchstudieson ratioandproportion,or even algebra,will be enhancedby the study of students'constructionsof those topics usually included in the elementaryschool curriculum.It is clearthatwe need morelongitudinalstudies to examine the developmentof the multiplicativeconceptualfield as a whole. ouranalysisquestionsthe claim thatfor a wordproblemthatcan be Furthermore, modeledby two quantities,the appropriate operationto performis invariantoverthe numbersinvolved in the problem(Greer,1994). Ourdataindicatethateven within whole numbers,numbersthatareless than10feel differentfor Brucefromthosethat arelargerthan10 whenhe uses multiplicationanddivision.A similarpointhasbeen raisedby CobbandWheatley(1988) throughtheirstudyon children'sconstructions of 10 as a unit.Greer(1994) pointedout the importanceof payingspecial attention to the transitionfromintegersto fractionsto decimalswhenextendingthe meanings of multiplicationanddivision. Ourstudy showed thatthe processof extendingthe anddivisionoperationsfromsingle-digitnumbersto mulmeaningsof multiplication and to then rationalnumbers,shouldalso be takenseriously. tidigitnumbers, Our analysis has pointed out the importanceof providingstudentswith a variety of mathematicallyrich activitiesas the foundationfor mathematicscurriculum andinstructionin any topic area.Most important,studentsneed to have moreexperiencewith tasksinvolving geometryandmeasurement,becausebothproviderich contextsfor developingconceptsof numbersandoperationsat all gradelevels. But we also wantto be clear aboutourgoal of using a varietyof mathematicaltasksto provide opportunitiesfor students to construct their mathematics.We do not believe thataddinga set of routineratio and proportiontasks to the mathematics curriculum,classified by eithermathematicalor semanticstructure,is the way to introduceratio and proportionconcepts in early gradelevels. Rather,we believe that the instructionshould startwith situationsthatare meaningfulto students. In this study,we did not have the time to examineBruce's constructionof fraction concepts carefully.Nevertheless, our limited data suggest that Bruce had an intuitiveknowledgeof fractionsthatcouldbe used to dealwith a wide rangeof ratio andproportiontaskswhen he neededto use fractions.If the goal for teachingratio andproportionis morethanintroducingthe cross-multiplicationalgorithm,we do not see any reasonto wait to introduceratiosandproportionsuntil afterinstruction on fractions,as is typically done. Throughoutthe teachingexperiment,we used manyothertasks. Because of the scope of this article,we reportedonly those occasions where we felt Bruce made significantchanges. But the questionremains,Whatkinds of mathematicalexperiences would help childrendevelop more sophisticatedunderstandingof proportion?Ourstudypointsto the potentialrichnessof geometryandmeasurement(e.g., magic-liquidtask)andthe importanceof encouragingstudentsto verbalizethe meanings of theiractions. Additionalstudies may shed new light on this issue. REFERENCES Anno, M. (1991). Anno's mathgames III. New York:Philomel Books. Ball, D. (1990). Prospectiveelementaryand secondaryteachers'understandingof division.Journalfor Research in MathematicsEducation,21, 132-144. Jane-Jane Lo and Tad Watanabe 235 Cobb,P., & Wheatley,G. (1988). Children'sinitialunderstandingsof ten. Focus on LearningProblems in Mathematics,10, 1-28. Confrey, J. (1991). Learning to listen: A student's understanding of powers of ten. In E. von Glasersfeld,(Ed.), Radical constructivismin mathematicseducation(pp. 111-138). Dordrecht,The Netherlands:Kluwer. Confrey,J., & Smith,E. (1995). Splitting,covariation,and theirrole in the developmentof exponential functions.Journalfor Research in MathematicsEducation,26, 66-86. Graeber,A., Tirosh,D., & Glover,R. (1989). Preserviceteachers'misconceptionsin solvingverbalproblems in multiplicationand division. Journalfor Research in MathematicsEducation,20, 95-102. Greer,B. (1994). Extendingthe meaningof multiplicationanddivision.In G. Harel& J. Confrey(Eds.), Thedevelopmentof multiplicativereasoningin the learningof mathematics(pp.61-85). Albany,NY: State Universityof New York Press. Harel,G., Behr, M., Post, T., & Lesh, R. (1991). Variablesaffectingproportionality.In F. Furinghetti (Ed.), Proceedings of the FifteenthAnnualMeetingof the InternationalGroupfor the Psychology of MathematicsEducation,Vol. 2 (pp. 125-132). Assisi, Italy:International Groupforthe Psychology of MathematicsEducation. Hart,K. M. (1984). Ratio: Children'sstrategies and errors. Windsor,England:NFER-Nelson. Hunting,R. P. (1983). Emergingmethodologiesfor understandinginternalprocesses governingchildren's mathematicalbehaviors.AustralianJournalof Education,27, 45-67. Karplus,R., Pulos, S., & Stage, E. (1983). Proportionalreasoning of early adolescents. In R. Lesh & M. Landau(Eds.),Acquisitionof mathematicsconceptsandprocesses (pp. 45-90). Orlando,FL: Academic Press. Kaput,J., & West, M. (1994). Missing value proportionalreasoningproblems:Factorsaffectinginformal reasoningpatterns.In G. Harel& J. Confrey(Eds.), Thedevelopmentof multiplicativereasoning in the learning of mathematics(pp. 61-85). Albany, NY: StateUniversityof New York Press. In T. P. Carpenter, E. Fennema,& T. A. Romberg(Eds.),Rational Lamon,S. (1993a).Ratioandproportion. numbers:An integrationof research (pp. 131-156). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Lamon,S. (1993b).Ratioandproportion: Connectingcontentandchildren'sthinking.Journalfor Research in MathematicsEducation,24, 41-61. tasks. Lo, J., & Watanabe,T. (1993a,April).Children'simage-basedschemesforsolvingproportion-type Paperpresentedat theAnnualMeetingof theAmericanEducationalResearchAssociation,Atlanta,GA. Lo, J., & Watanabe,T. (1993b).Conceptualbasesof youngchildren'ssolutionstrategiesof missingvalue proportionaltasks.Proceedingof the SeventeenthAnnualMeetingof the InternationalGroupfor the Psychology of MathematicsEducation(Vol. 3, pp. 162-169). Tsukuba,Japan. Piaget,J., & Inhelder,B. (1975). Theoriginof the idea of chance in children.New York:W. W. Norton. Piaget,J., Grize,J., Szeminska,A., & Bang,V. (1977).Epistemologyandpsychologyoffunctions.Boston: D. Reidel. Reynolds,A. (1993). Imaginingin children'smathematicalactivity.(Doctoraldissertation,FloridaState University, 1993). DissertationAbstractsInternational,54A, 1274. Schorn,A. C. (1989). Proportionalreasoningbyyoung children.Unpublishedmaster'sthesis, Cornell University,Ithaca,NY. Schwartz,J. (1988). Intensivequantityand referenttransformingarithmeticoperations.In J. Hiebert & M. Behr (Eds.),Numberconceptsand operationsin the middlegrades (pp. 41-52). Reston, VA: NationalCouncil of Teachersof Mathematics. Simon, M. (1993). Prospectiveelementaryteachers'knowledge of division. Journalfor Research in MathematicsEducation,24, 233-254. Steffe,L. P. (1988). Children'sconstructionof numbersequencesandmultiplyingschemes.In J. Hiebert & M. Behr(Eds.),Numberconceptsand operationsin the middlegrades (pp. 119-140). Reston,VA: National Council of Teachersof Mathematicsand Erlbaum. Steffe, L. P. (1990). Overviewof the actiongroupAl: Earlychildhoodyears.In L. P. Steffe & T. Wood (Eds.), Transformingchildren's mathematicseducation: Internationalperspectives (pp. 3-15). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. L. (1984).Searchfortherootsof ratio:Somethoughtson thelong termlearningprocess(Towards Streefland, ... a theory).PartI: Reflections on a teachingexperiment.EducationalStudiesin Mathematics,15, 327-348. 236 Developing Ratio and Proportion Schemes L. (1985).Searchfortherootsof ratio:Somethoughtson thelongtermlearningprocess(Towards. Streefland, ... a theory). Part II: The outline of the long term learning process. Educational Studies in Mathematics,16, 75-94. Streefland,L. (1991). Fractions in realistic mathematicseducation. Dordrecht,The Netherlands: Kluwer. Van den Brink,J., & Streefland,L. (1979). Young children(6-8)-ratio and proportion.Educational Studiesin Mathematics,10, 403-420. Vergnaud,G. (1988). Multiplicativestructures.In J. Hiebert& M. Behr (Eds.), Numberconcepts and operations in the middle grades (pp. 141-161). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. von Glasersfeld,E. (1989). Cognition,constructionof knowledge,andteaching.Synthese,80, 121-140. Wheatley, G. (1991). Constructivist perspectives on mathematics and science learning. Science Education,75, 9-21. Authors Jane-Jane Lo, Visiting Scholar,Departmentof Mathematics,CornellUniversity,Ithaca,New York, 14850-7901; e-mail:janejanel@aol.com Tad Watanabe, AssistantProfessor,Departmentof Mathematics,Towson StateUniversity,Towson, Maryland,21252-7097; e-mail: tad@midget.towson.edu