P a g e | 376 Chapter 52: POST-OCCUPANCY EVALUATION OF THE PREDATOR HOUSE AT THE BIRMINGHAM ZOO Stephen Bitgood, . J. T. Roper, Jr., Donald Patterson, & Arlene Benefield Jacksonville State University Visitor Behavior, 1986, 1(2) 4-5 and Technical Report No. 86-40. Jacksonville, AL: Psychology Institute] INTRODUCTION The Predator House at the Birmingham Zoo was opened in March, 1985 (see a diagram of the building at the end of the paper). The building was designed to house the Zoo’s large cats as well as smaller predators such as the genet, jaguarondi, river otter, and arctic fox. The building was constructed so that the visitor walks down a winding corridor usually observing only one exhibit at a time; thus, there are minimal visual distractions and the visitor is able to attend to one exhibit at a time. In addition to the signs describing each species on exhibit, there are nine interpretive signs dealing with the topic of predation distributed throughout the building. The exhibits consist of museum-like displays with glass barriers between visitors and animals and a replication of the natural surroundings of the species. For example, the Cougar Exhibit is set in a scene resembling the Grand Canyon. Air conditioning is provided for visitor comfort. Post-Occupancy Evaluation (POE), similar to summative evaluation, was initiated in the fall of 1985 and continued into Spring of 1986. Data collected in the Summer of 1985 and reported in previous manuscripts (Bitgood, Benefield, Patterson, Lewis, & Landers, 1985; Bitgood, England, Lewis, Benefield, Patterson, & Landers, 1985) was also used as part of the overall analysis. The project was funded by a grant from the Jacksonville State University Faculty Research Committee. METHOD Several procedures were employed to collect data for the POE: (1) direct observation of visitor behavior at the Birmingham Zoo; (2) direct observation of visitor behavior at similar exhibits at other zoos; (3) surveys of visitors’ reactions to the Predator House; and (4) other data collected in previous studies (general visitor survey of the Zoo and a study of visitor orientation and circulation). One of the direct observation procedures entailed a recorder tracking a randomly selected visitor through the Predator House. A stop watch was used to measure the amount of time that a visitor stopped and was visually oriented to the exhibit. Several measures were taken from the P a g e | 377 recorded data including attracting power (percent of visitors stopping) and holding power (average viewing time) of the exhibits and signs. In addition to visitor tracking, observers were stationed at four exhibits to record data on the above variables. Again, visitors were chosen at random as they approached the particular exhibit. Throughout the study, an equal number of adults, children, males, and females were selected for observation. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION In a general survey of visitors conducted in May, 1985, 33.3 percent stated that the predators were their favorite exhibit and 29.6 percent reported that they would like to see more predator exhibits. Since these percentages were much higher than responses referring to any other animals, one might assume that the Predator House was extremely successful in pleasing the visitors. The total average time in the Predator House was 13 min and 47 sec. Results from the measurement of direct observation of visitors are summarized in Table 1. In general, the larger species generated greater holding power (viewing time) than the smaller species. Thus, larger cats (tiger, leopard, cougar) were viewed longer than small mammals (genet, jaguarondi, arctic fox). This is consistent with our previous findings. However, this difference is not necessarily inherent in the size of the animal. The smaller species are more difficult to see and are better able to hide than are the larger animals, and this probably accounts, at least somewhat, for the difference in holding power between the larger and smaller species. For example, the three species with the shortest view time (cacomistle, genet, and jaguarondi) were the best hiders. They quite often were difficult or impossible to see. Contrary to the results of Falk, Koran, Dierking, & Dreblow (1985), the attracting and holding power of exhibits did not decrease as visitors proceeded through the Predator House. Holding power varied with the size of the animal and the amount of animal activity. For example, the tiger, the largest animal in the Predator House, was one of the longest-viewed animals, and it was the second-to-last exhibit viewed. Viewing decrements due to fatigue, satiation, or exit gradients were not evident. Also of interest is the comparatively lower attracting power for three exhibits: Eagle, Otter, and Genet. The Eagle and Otter exhibits were on opposite sides of the corridor and thus may have produced competition for the viewers’ attention. Once the visitor made a choice, he/she was less likely to turn around and view the exhibit on the opposite side of the corridor. Table 2 shows the comparison between Birmingham Zoo exhibits and similar exhibits at other zoos. The attracting and holding power data are generally similar when activity level is P a g e | 378 controlled. Sign reading, however, is lower in the Birmingham Zoo exhibits than in other zoos. The long text and the poor lighting probably account for the lower sign reading in Birmingham. P a g e | 379 P a g e | 380 An Exhibit –by-Exhibit Analysis (See Table 2) Spotted Leopard: This is the first exhibit in the building. Table 2 suggests that the Birmingham Zoo exhibit compares favorably with other zoos in terms of percentage of stopping and viewing time. The average viewing time of 42.1 sec was the highest compared with similar exhibits in three other zoos. However, the Birmingham Leopard exhibit received the lowest label reading percentage 11.3 sec compared to 15-, 20-, and 27.5 sec for the other zoos. Cougar: The percentage of visitors who stopped was high and comparable to other zoos in the sample. The holding power was second lowest of the five zoos, but the low (4.1%) of animal activity during viewing events must be considered. Again, the lowest percentage of label reading occurred at the Birmingham exhibit compared with the other four zoos. River Otter: The comparison zoos for this exhibit were San Diego and the Sonora-Desert Museum. Both of these comparison zoos included more expensive and elaborate exhibit design in their Otter exhibit. This design difference may explain the 25% difference in attracting power (percent who stop). As with the other exhibits described above, less label reading occurred at the Birmingham Zoo exhibits. Tiger: The Birmingham tiger exhibit generated the highest percentage of visitors who stop (95.7% compared with 90 and 91% for other zoos. However, the viewing time (holding power) was lower (36.3 sec compared with 63.4 and 53.4 sec. Activity level differences are likely to explain the viewing time variations. Only 8.5% of visitors read the exhibit label at the Birmingham exhibit compared with 14 and 18.8% at other zoos. Jaguarundi: Cincinnati Zoo also had a new exhibition house in which Jaguarundi’s were exhibited. While attracting power was comparable (90.9 vs 95%), the viewing time was lower in Birmingham (17.3 sec) compared with 32.1 sec) in Cincinnati. Within Zoo Comparison of Exhibits Two animals in the Predator House were also exhibited in other areas of the Birmingham Zoo and thus provide another way to assess the Predator House. The Arctic fox had higher attracting power (87.9% stopping) in the Predator House and 76.9% stopping in the Small Mammal area. Viewing time in the Predator House was 20.7 sec and only 11.4 sec in the Small Mammal area. The Jaguarundi also had a higher attracting power in the Predator House (90.9% stopping) compared with 70.8% in the Small Mammal area. Viewing time in the was 17.3 sec and in the Small Mammal area, 9.2 sec. P a g e | 381 SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION Overall, the Predator House at the Birmingham Zoo must be rated high in terms of visitor appeal. Its exhibits have been shown to have excellent attracting and holding power when compared with other zoos or with exhibits of similar species within the Zoo. Visitors report a very favorable experience in the Predator House. The primary shortcoming of the Predator House is the effectiveness of interpretive labels. The lack of label reading has a profound impact on the educational objectives of the exhibition. Visitors are likely to learn very little about predators if they do not read the interpretive material. The text passages could be shortened or the font size increased as one way to increase reading. Another approach would be to use prompting techniques to stimulate increased reading. A study using a hand-out titled, “Predator Pursuit” demonstrated the viability of this approach. Ten questions on the hand-out directed visitors to information within the label text. With the handout, visitors increased their reading of exhibit labels. REFERENCES Bitgood, S., Benefield, A., Patterson, D., Lewis, D., & Landers, A. (1985). Zoo visitors: Can we make them behave? 1985 AAZPA Proceedings. Bitgood, S., England, K., Lewis, D., Benefield, A., & Patterson, D. (1985). Care and management of Homo Sapiens at zoos and museums. Southeastern Association of Behavior Analysis. Charleston, SC. Falk, J., Koran, J., Dierking, L., & Dreblow, (1985). Predicting visitor behavior. Curator, 28(4), 249-257. P a g e | 382