DIFFERENTIAL EFFECTS OF SUBSIDIZED GUARDIANSHIP ON PLACEMENT STABILITY FOR CHILDREN IN KINSHIP CARE by Ge Hong Dissertation submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate School of the University of Maryland Baltimore County in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy 2006 ABSTRACT Title of Dissertation: Differential Effects of Subsidized Guardianship on Placement Stability for Children in Kinship Care Ge Hong, Doctor of Philosophy, 2006 Dissertation directed by: Dr. Marvin Mandell, Professor, Department of Public Policy, UMBC Dr. Dave Marcotte, Associate Professor, Department of Public Policy, UMBC Dr. Donna Harrington, Professor, School of Social Work, University of Maryland, Baltimore Dr. Claudia Lawrence-Webb Dr. Willie Tompkins, Washington D.C. Child and Family Services Agency For over three decades, placement stability of children in foster care has been a serious concern for policy makers as well as the general public. Frequent placement disruption has been found to have negative impacts on children’s psychological development and educational progress. Yet, a substantial proportion of children in foster care have experienced three or more placements while they are in foster care. The objective of this dissertation is to examine the effects of guardianship subsidy availability on placement stability for a cohort of children in the Maryland’s Guardianship Assistance Demonstration Project (GAP) who were placed with relative caregivers before May 2000 in Baltimore City. The evaluation of GAP was a true experiment in which study participants were randomly assigned to either the experimental group or the control group. Members of the experimental group were eligible for a monthly subsidy for guardianship participation. Members of the control group, on the other hand, were ineligible for the subsidy. This study examines the differential effects of the eligibility of the subsidy on placement stability between four distinctive groups of children who were placed in kinship care at the beginning of the project and in the absence of the subsidy: 1. Those who would remain in kinship care in the absence of the subsidy; 2. Those who would move to restricted foster care in the absence of the subsidy; 3. Those who would exit out-of-home care through guardianship in the absence of the subsidy; and 4. Those who would exit out-of-home care through reunification. Subgroups are defined using the propensity score matching method and number of placement changes is examined using a Poisson regression model. The study did not find any subsidy effects on placement stability regardless of children’s permanency outcome in the absence of the subsidy. Given that earlier evaluation found the subsidies to accelerate the pace of children leaving out-of-home care, the implementation of a guardianship subsidy program should be implemented as a universal policy. Additionally, this dissertation demonstrates that propensity score matching has the potential to improve sample balance. The estimates of program effects obtained with the propensity score methods, however, is sensitive to the selection of predictors. Table of Contents Chapter 1. Introduction ................................................................................................... 1 1.1 The Foster Care Drift: Length of Stay and Placement Stability ......................... 2 1.2 Out-of-home Placements with Relatives ............................................................ 3 1.3 Permanency and Placement Stability for Children in Formal Kinship Care ...... 7 1.4 Legal Guardianship ........................................................................................... 11 1.5 Maryland Guardianship Assistance Project ...................................................... 12 Chapter 2. Literature Review ........................................................................................ 17 2.1 Relative Foster Care Vs. Non-relative Foster Care ................................................ 17 2.1.1 Child and Caregiver Characteristics ................................................................ 17 2.1.2 Services Received by Caregivers and Children in Relative Foster Care ......... 21 2.1.3 Child Outcomes ............................................................................................... 22 2.2 Placement Stability for Children in Out-of-Home Care ......................................... 26 2.2.1 Placement Disruption Rates ............................................................................. 26 2.2.2 Predictors of Placement Disruption ................................................................. 28 2.2.3 Consequences of Placement Instability ........................................................... 31 2.3 Outcomes for Children in Legal Guardianship ....................................................... 32 2.4 IV-E Waiver Demonstrations on Subsidized Guardianship ................................... 33 2.5 Propensity Score Matching ..................................................................................... 35 Chapter 3. Methodology ................................................................................................. 38 3.1 Research Question .................................................................................................. 38 3.2 Sample and Data ..................................................................................................... 41 3.3 Measurement of Placement Stability ...................................................................... 43 3.4 Statistical Analysis .................................................................................................. 43 3.4.1 Propensity Score Matching .............................................................................. 44 3.4.2 Poisson Regression .......................................................................................... 48 3.4.3 Sensitivity Analysis ......................................................................................... 50 Chapter 4. Results ........................................................................................................... 52 4.1 Sample Characteristics ............................................................................................ 52 4.2 Bivariate Analysis ................................................................................................... 54 I 4.3 Subgroup Analysis: Children who Would Exit and Children who Would Stay ..... 56 4.3.1 Predicting the Probabilities of Exiting Foster Care in the Absence of the Subsidy...................................................................................................................... 57 4.3.2 Sample Balance ................................................................................................ 59 4.3.3 Placement Stability .......................................................................................... 64 4.4 Subgroup Analysis: Guardianship, Reunification, Restricted Foster Care, or Kinship Care ................................................................................................................. 67 4.4.1 Predicting Child Outcomes using a Multinomial Logit Model ....................... 68 4.4.2 Propensity Score Matching .............................................................................. 73 4.4.3 Placement Stability .......................................................................................... 80 4.4.4 Estimates of the Effects of the Eligibility of the Guardianship Subsidy Based on Multinomial Logit Model 1 ................................................................................. 86 4.5 Sensitivity Analysis – African-American Children Only ....................................... 87 Chapter 5. Discussions and Policy Implications .......................................................... 90 References ........................................................................................................................ 94 Appendix: Additional Statistical Tables ..................................................................... 103 List of Figures Figure 1- 1 Number of Children in Foster Care (1980 – 2002) .......................................... 1 Figure 1- 2 Types of Kinship Care in Maryland ................................................................. 7 List of Tables Table 3. 1 Predictors of child permanency and placement outcomes ............................... 47 Table 4. 1 Sample Characteristics ..................................................................................... 52 Table 4. 2 Number of placement changes ......................................................................... 55 Table 4. 3 Permanency Outcomes .................................................................................... 56 Table 4. 4 Coefficients obtained from the logit models predicting the probabilities of exiting from foster care for the members of the control group ....................... 57 Table 4. 5 Sample Balance without Propensity Score Matching ...................................... 59 Table 4. 6 Sample Balance after Propensity Score Matching based on Logit Model 1 ... 61 Table 4. 7 Sample Balance after Propensity Score Matching Based on Logit Model 2 .. 63 II Table 4. 8 Exposure Adjusted Mean Number of Placement Change by Permanency Status ......................................................................................................................... 65 Table 4. 9 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would exit from outof-home placements in the absence of the subsidy ....................................... 66 Table 4. 10 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would not exit from out-of-home placement in the absence of the subsidy ................... 67 Table 4. 11 Coefficients from the multinomial logit models on permanency outcomes 69 Table 4. 12 Sample balance without propensity score matching ...................................... 75 Table 4. 13 Sample Balance after Propensity Score Matching (Based on Multinomial Logit Model 2) ................................................................................................ 77 Table 4. 14 Exposure-adjusted mean change in placements per year by predicted outcomes and eligibility for the guardianship subsidy ................................... 81 Table 4. 15 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would exit through guardianship in the absence of the subsidy ............................... 82 Table 4. 16 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placements for children who would exit through reunification in the absence of the subsidy ...................................... 83 Table 4. 17 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placements for children who would move to Restricted Foster Care in the absence of the subsidy .................................... 84 Table 4. 18 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placements for children who would remain in kinship care in the absence of the subsidy ................................................ 85 Tables in Appendix Table A- 1 Sample Balance after Propensity Score Matching (Not considering participating in caregiver interview) .......................................................... 104 Table A- 2 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would exit through guardianship in the absence of the subsidy (Not considering participation in caregiver interview) ........................................................ 106 Table A- 3 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would exit through reunification in the absence of the subsidy (Not considering participation in caregiver interview) ........................................................ 106 III Table A- 4 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would move to restricted foster care in the absence of the subsidy (Not considering participation in caregiver interview) ........................................................ 107 Table A- 5 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would remain in kinship care in the absence of the subsidy (Not considering participating in caregiver interview) ........................................................ 108 Table A- 6 Coefficients obtained from a multinomial logit model on permanency status (African-American children only) ........................................................... 108 Table A- 7 Sample Balance After Propensity Score Matching (African-American children only, not considering participation in caregiver interviews) ..................... 110 Table A- 8 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placements for children who would exit through guardianship in the absence of the subsidy – African-American children only ............................................................................................. 112 Table A- 9 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would exit through reunification in the absence of the subsidy (Not considering participation in caregiver interview) - African-American children only . 112 Table A- 10 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placements for children who would move to restricted foster care in the absence of the subsidy – African-American children only ............................................................................................. 113 Table A- 11 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would remain in kinship care in the absence of the subsidy (Not considering participation in caregiver interview) - African-American children only .. 114 Table A- 12 Sample Balance after Propensity Score Matching (African-American children only, considering participation in caregiver interviews) ............. 115 Table A- 13 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would exit through guardianship in the absence of the subsidy (Considering participation in caregiver interview) - African-American children only . 117 Table A- 14 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would exit through reunification in the absence of the subsidy (Considering participation in caregiver interview) - African-American children only . 118 Table A- 15 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would move to RFC in the absence of the subsidy (Considering participation in caregiver interview) - African-American children only .......................... 119 IV Table A- 16 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would remain in kinship care in the absence of the subsidy (Considering participation in caregiver interview) - African-American children only . 120 V Chapter 1. Introduction More than four decades have passed since Maas and Engler (1959) drew the nation’s attention to the ordeals of children in foster care. Although child welfare in the U.S. underwent great changes during these four decades, many believe the foster care system has yet to walk out of the quagmire. These beliefs are not unfounded. Figure 1 illustrates the trend of foster care population since 1980. The number of children in out-of-home care increased from 302,000 in 1980 to 581,000 in 1999. The size of the foster care population has been on a slight downward trend since the turn of the 21st century. Nevertheless, some 532,000 children were in foster care as of September 2002. Figure 1- 1 Number of Children in Foster Care (1980 – 2002) 700 600 500 400 in thousands 300 200 100 0 80 83 86 89 92 95 98 01 Source: 1980-1997 Ladner (2000); 1998 – 2002 from Adoption and Foster Care Analysis and Reporting System (AFCARS). Available at http://www.acf.dhhs.gov While the number of children placed in out of home care has almost doubled from its 1980 level, the number of non-relative foster parents declined over the 1980s, dropping from 134,000 in 1984 to 125,000 in 1989 (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services [USDHHS], 1994). The shortage was most felt in urban areas and for foster parents who were minorities and who were willing to accept teenagers. This was 1 when relative foster homes became the place where a considerable proportion of children in out-of-home care lived. This chapter will discuss the cultural and policy contexts of relative foster care, and the motivation for this study. 1.1 The Foster Care Drift: Length of Stay and Placement Stability The concerns for children’s experience in foster care center on “foster care drift” – a phrase used to describe children’s experience of staying in foster care for lengthy periods of time without permanency plans, while drifting from one placement to another. Although permanency planning has become a standard practice, permanency remains an elusive goal for many children in foster care. The dramatic growth of the foster care population in a large part is due to the fact that a large proportion of children stay in care for prolonged periods of time (Ladner, 2000). When children cannot exit from care in a timely manner, their risks for placement disruptions increase (Olsen, 1982; Proch & Taber, 1985). A substantial proportion of children in foster care today have experienced three or more placements while they are in foster care (Chamberlain & Whaley, 2001; Newton, Litrownik & Landsverk, 2000; Smith, Stormshak, USDHHS, 2003a). This has serious consequences for children’s healthy development. Research suggests that children who do not receive continuous and responsive care from seven months of age through toddlerhood are more likely to develop difficulties in forming attachments to their primary caregivers and healthy relationships in the future than are children who receive such care (McRoy, 1994). In fact, placement disruption may be more detrimental to the child’s development than out-ofhome care itself. Frequent changes in placement for children in foster care have been found to result in increased behavioral problems (Newton et al., 2000). Additionally, 2 multiple placements were shown to disrupt educational progress and lead to increased criminality, while having been placed in out-of-home care alone was not (Eckenrode, Rowe, Laird & Braithwaite, 1995; Widom, 1991) In response to the concerns about the experience of children in foster care, the congress passed the Adoption Assistance and Child Welfare Act (P.L. 96-272) in 1980 in the hope of limiting the time it takes for children to be reunified with their parents and to accelerate adoption efforts when children cannot be returned home. It also encouraged states to place children in family-like settings. P.L. 96-272, however, fell short of achieving its goal as the number of children in foster care continued to increase. The Adoption and Safe Families Act (P.L. 105-89), hereafter referred to as ASFA, of 1997 placed a new emphasis on adoption, as it further shortened the timeframe within which reunification efforts must be made to 12 months. Although ASFA exempted kinship placements from the timeframe of reasonable efforts for reunification, and recognized relative placements could constitute “planned permanent living arrangement” (USDHHS, 2000), states appeared to have increased their efforts to encourage relative caregivers to pursue adoption and legal guardianship in the wake of ASFA (Chipungu, 2004). 1.2 Out-of-home Placements with Relatives The rise of kinship care in the child welfare system is closely related to the overrepresentation of African-American children in foster care. The reason for this overrepresentation is manifold. Factors associated with foster care placement such as chronic poverty, homelessness, joblessness, and teen pregnancy disproportionately affect the African-American population (Hampton, 1991; Hofferth & Owens, 2001; Leashore, McMurray & Bailey, 1991). This is reflected by the fact that the majority of African- 3 American children in foster care are placed in out-of-home care because of neglect, which is closely related to poverty (Smith, 2004; Williams, 1991). The situation is exacerbated by the drug epidemic, which has led to a drastic increase in the number of children needing out-of-home placement services (Danzy & Jackson, 1997). For example, it was estimated that between one third and two thirds of children known to the child welfare agencies had families beset with substance abuse problems (Courtney, 1999). Some local programs reported as high as 81% of children in out-of-home placements with drug addicted mothers (Bonecutter & Gleeson, 1997). Furthermore, differential treatment by medical professionals and the juvenile justice system as well as the child welfare system contribute to the rapid increase of African-American children in out-ofhome care (Hill, 2003). For example, some research findings suggested that given the same situation, African-American children were consistently more likely than white children to be placed in out-of-home care while white children were more likely to receive in-home services. Additionally, doctors were more likely to diagnose physical injury as accidents for affluent families, and abuse for poor families, who were predominantly African Americans (Hill, 2003). No matter what the dominant cause for the over-representation of AfricanAmerican children in out-of-home care is, it is an undeniable fact that the influx of African-American children has greatly changed the landscape of the child welfare system. However, the current system is not equipped to meet the unique needs of AfricanAmerican children. The current child welfare system was developed based on white middle-class values such as individualism and the work ethic (Chipungu, 1991). Even in the 20th century, African-American children were denied child welfare services in the 4 modern child welfare state for a prolonged period of time (Chipungu, 1991). It should not come as a surprise that the child welfare system does not take into consideration the unique culture and beliefs of African-American families and the needs of AfricanAmerican children. Many researchers have been calling for an Africentric perspective in planning and working towards permanency for African-American children (Bonecutter & Gleeson, 1997; Chipungu, 1991). This perspective would take advantage of the AfricanAmerican value that individuals within extended families view the extended family as one collective entity. Members of the extended family share resources and care for each other’s children (Chipungu, 1991). Kinship care is a natural outcome of the collective identity of African-American families and the need for out-of-home care for African-American children. Its roots can be traced back to slavery, when the extended families would take care of a child when the parents were sold to other slave owners (Danzy & Jackson, 1997). Today, extended families continue to be an important resource for child rearing when African-American parents are not able to care for their children, especially after the passage of P.L. 96-272, which encouraged state child welfare agencies to place children in the least restrictive placement, which, in many cases, would be with kin. In the U. S., approximately 2.13 million children were living with relatives without the presence of a parent in 1998 (USDHHS, 2000). Depending on the degree of the involvement of the child welfare system, a child may be placed in informal kinship care or formal kinship care. Children who live with relatives without the involvement of child welfare agencies are said to be in informal kinship care. The vast majority of children living with relatives fall into this category. It was estimated that only 200,000 5 children in 1997 were formally placed in relative care by child welfare agencies (USDHHS, 2000). Children in this type of care are said to be in formal kinship care. In the past, although the custodies of children in formal kinship care belonged to the state, many states did not offer foster care board payments to kinship caregivers. Instead, relative caregivers might receive an AFDC child-only grant. However, in most cases, the AFDC payment was considerably less than the foster care board payment. Further, AFDC paid less for each additional child in care while foster care had a flat monthly rate for each child. The discrepancy in payment rates triggered a class-action lawsuit in Illinois (Miller vs. Youakim, 1979), for which the Supreme Court ruled that the states must pay relative caregivers at the same rate as traditional foster parents if the relative caregivers meet the IV-E requirements.1 Currently a continuum of kinship care policies exists among the 50 states and the District of Columbia (USDHHS, 2000). All states offer kin the option of being licensed and receiving the full foster care board payment.2 Many states waive certain licensing requirements for kin. Relative caregivers who are not licensed may receive payments at a reduced level, often at the TANF rates. Figure 2 illustrates the system of relative care in Maryland. There are two types of relative caregivers in formal kinship care. The first group, referred hereafter as kinship caregivers, includes relative foster parents who are not licensed. They receive TANF child-only grants, which averaged $188 per month per child at the time of this study. The second group, referred to as restricted foster care (RFC) caregivers, are relative foster parents who are licensed to care for specific children. They receive the foster care board 1 Title IV-E of the Social Security Act provides federal matching funds for foster care payments and adoption subsidies. 2 California, Oregon, and New Jersey only allow IV-E eligible relative caregivers to be fully licensed. 6 payment, which averaged approximately $600 per month among the participants of this study. Figure 1- 2 Types of Kinship Care in Maryland Informal Kinship Care No child welfare involvement Unlicensed Formal Kinship Care Kinship Care Receiving TANF payments Formal Kinship Care With child welfare involvement Restricted Foster Care Receiving Foster Care payments 1.3 Permanency and Placement Stability for Children in Formal Kinship Care There is evidence indicating that placements with relative caregivers are associated with longer stays in care due to lower adoption and reunification rates, the two major channels of children exiting from foster care (Gleeson, 1999a). African-American children placed in formal kinship care have the lowest rates of adoption and reunification compared with other groups of children in out-of-home placements (Barth, Courtney, Berrick, & Alert, 1994), The unique cultural heritage of African-American families plays an important role in African-American relative caregivers’ reluctance to pursue adoption. AfricanAmerican families’ sense of collective identity determines that relative care is viewed by caregivers more as a family responsibility than a legal arrangement. It also determines that the caregivers’ relationships with the birth parents will play an important role 7 throughout the permanency planning process. Many relative caregivers had been taking care of the children prior to the involvement of the child welfare agencies. They tend to feel that adoption is unnecessary because the child is already family (Barth et al., 1994; Gleeson, O’Donnell, & Bonecutter, 1997; Testa, Shook, Cohen, & Woods, 1996). Some studies revealed that kinship caregivers, especially grandmothers, were not willing to adopt children in their care because they did not wish to terminate the rights of the biological parents (Meyer & Link, 1990; Testa et al., 1996; Thornton, 1991). Some relative caregivers viewed adoption as a punishment to the birth parents (Geen, 2003). Many hoped that the birth parents will eventually regain custody of the children (Geen, 2003; Gleeson et al., 1997; Testa et al., 1996). Some caregivers felt that they had a tacit agreement with the biological parents that they would care for the children until majority age and the parents would, in some way, remain involved in the children’s lives. Williams (1991) described a case where a mother placed her child with a friend voluntarily, believing that her child would be taken good care of in their custody. While the mother and the kinship caregivers viewed the arrangement as in the child’s best interest, the child welfare agency considered it abandonment. The kinship caregivers were reluctant to pursue adoption in fear of undermining their relationship with the biological mother. Financial difficulties were also cited in research as one reason why kinship caregivers did not pursue adoption, although subsidies were available for special needs adoptions (Barth et al., 1994; Testa et al., 1996). This may be due to the fact that a sizable proportion of caregivers were never informed of the existence of subsidized adoption (Geen, 2003; Testa et al., 2001). The fact that special needs adoptions, which qualify for 8 the adoption subsidies, outnumbered regular foster care adoptions between 1983 and 1987 after the passage of P.L. 96-272 suggests that financial assistance attracted at least some caregivers to pursue adoption (Schwartz & Fishman, 1999). More recent research in Illinois indicated that a considerable proportion of kin were in fact willing to adopt (Gleeson et al., 1997). The authors attributed this change to the pressure on achieving permanency created by litigations in the state, which motivated caseworkers to discuss adoption more fully with the caregivers. Unfortunately, this has not been the case in other states. Caseworkers, seeking middle-class two-parent adoptive families, often deem relative caregivers, predominantly elderly African-American females with limited income, unsuitable to be adoptive parents (McRoy, 2004). Apart from lower adoptions rates, slower reunification with birth parents is a contributing factor to the longer stay in care for children placed with relatives. The financial difference in foster care board payments and the TANF payments, with the former considerably higher than the latter, has been identified as an obstacle to reunification for children cared for by relatives (Takas, 1992). When a child is returned home, the total subsidies that the extended family receives often fall from the higher foster care board payment to the lower AFDC/TANF rates. This is evidenced by research findings in California, which showed that reunification rates were lower for IV-E eligible children than children who were not IV-E eligible (Berrick & Needell, 1999). In California, licensed relative caregivers of the former group received the foster care board payment while those of the latter group were not allowed to become foster parents and could only receive the AFDC payments if they were eligible. As such, for the former group, if the child in relative care was returned home, the extended family as a whole 9 would see their total subsidies drop from the foster care rate to the AFDC rate if the parents qualified for AFDC, and nothing if they did not. The latter group, already receiving AFDC, would not face this disincentive if the parents qualified for AFDC. Even if their parents did not qualify for AFDC, the reduction in assistance would be shallower compared with that for IV-E eligible children. Caseworkers indicated that IV-E eligible families might avoid reunification to maximize their combined benefits. Financial disincentive is not the only factor in delaying reunification. Ironically, the advantages of relative foster care, such as stability and safety, may have stalled permanency. Research indicated that birth parents were often less motivated to work towards reunification than their counterparts whose children were placed with non-kin placements (Geen, 2003). Birth parents felt less propelled to meet case plan requirements, partly because relative care does not carry as much a stigma as having their children raised by foster care parents. Caseworkers as well as the parents may deem the placements to be safe and stable, and therefore, lack the urge and commitment to make efforts towards reunification (Courtney, 1999). The close relationship between placement stability and length of stay in care cannot be stressed enough. Length of stay in care is the single most important predictor of multiple placements (Proch & Taber, 1985). The number of placements that a child experiences increases as s/he spends more time in foster care. Multiple placements, in turn, reduce a child’s probability of being adopted or returned home (Usher, Randolph & Gogan, 1999). Given the long length of stay in care for children in formal kinship care, placement stability appears to be of paramount importance for the well being of these children. Relative placements are found to be more stable than non-relative foster care 10 placements (Courtney & Needell, 1997; Leslie et al., 2000; Scannapieco, 1999; Usher et al., 1999; Webster, Barth & Needell, 2000). It is nonetheless not uncommon for children placed with relatives to experience more than one placement during their stay in care (Terling-Watt, 2001). The data indicated that as high as nearly 30% of children in relative care experienced more than one placement during the first six months in care. Relative caregivers, although emotionally committed to the children in their care, receive little training and meager assistance, and may lack the resources to take care of children for extended periods of time. 1.4 Legal Guardianship There is contention about whether reunification and adoption are safer and less detrimental to the development of children than growing up in kinship care. Research has repeatedly pointed out the danger of sending a child home when the same risk for which the child was removed from home still exists (Schwartz & Fishman, 1999). Further, some children in out-of-home care, especially older children, may be emotionally attached to their biological parents despite neglect and abuse (Christian & Ekman, 2000). However, it is undeniable that long-term kinship care lacks the judicial sanction necessary for a child’s protection and healthy development. In particular, legally sanctioned permanency options such as adoption provides clarity as to who has the authority to make decisions related to the child’s life while long-term kinship care usually does not (Williams, 1991). Combined with the expectation of continued care, the psychological effects may be significant for the child (Williams, 1991) For children who cannot be returned home or be adopted safely and in a timely manner, an alternative permanency option is legal guardianship, which ASFA defines as 11 “a judicially created relationship between child and caregiver which is intended to be permanent and self-sustaining as evidenced by the transfer to the caretaker of the following parental rights with respect to the child: protection, education, care and control of the person, custody of the person, and decision-making” (Christian & Ekman, 2000). The establishment of a guardianship agreement must be sanctioned by the court, as well as its dissolvent. Because the legal guardianship arrangement between the child and the relative caregiver allows children to live in a relatively stable environment and yet stay in the family, it provides a “middle-ground” between the instability associated with foster care and the traumatization associated with having to be separated from their families permanently (Leashore, 1984). For relative caregivers, this may be an attractive option because it does not require the termination of parental rights, and yet grants the caregivers considerable decision-making power. Despite the desirable nature of legal guardianship, only 3% of all foster care cases in the country have the permanency goal of guardianship (USDHHS, 2003b). This is, in part, due to the fact that payments to legal guardians are not reimbursable under Title IVE of the Social Security Act. Many caseworkers think that caregivers will not be able to provide adequate care for the children in their care with only the TANF payments (Gleeson, 1999b). 1.5 Maryland Guardianship Assistance Project In order to test the idea of providing caregivers who assume guardianship with a subsidy, the federal government granted seven states with Title IV-E waivers to conduct assisted guardianship demonstration projects. The objectives of these projects were to determine 12 whether the subsidy accelerates children’s exit from foster care, and to examine its impact on key child well-being indicators such as placement stability. Maryland is one of the seven states that received IV-E waivers to implement a subsidized guardianship program. The Maryland’s Guardianship Assistance Demonstration Project (GAP), which started in 1998 and was completed in June 2003, provided a $300 monthly subsidy to relative caregivers who assumed legal guardianship. In order to be eligible for the GAP project, participants had to meet the following criteria: the child must be legally committed to the state; the child must have been with the present caregiver for at least six months; adoption or reunification is not a viable permanency alternative; the caregiver is able to provide a stable, healthy, and safe home for the child; the child is adjusting well to the caregiver's home; the caregiver must have a means of financial support other than the guardianship subsidy; and the child and caregiver must reside in Maryland (University of Maryland School of Social Work [UMSSW], 2003) The GAP project was formally evaluated by the University of Maryland School of Social Work. The evaluation was a randomized social experiment and adopted an intentto-treat (ITT) approach. The results indicated that children who were initially placed in kinship care and who were eligible for the guardianship subsidy exited from foster care at a significantly faster pace than their peers who were not eligible for the subsidy. Among the children who exited, those who were eligible for the guardianship subsidy were more 13 likely to exit via guardianship. The guardianship subsidy was not found to significantly affect children in restricted foster care in terms of exit rates or exit types. The effects of the eligibility of the guardianship subsidy on placement stability while program participants were in foster care were inconclusive. The literature review section will discuss the results of this evaluation in further detail. Although the GAP evaluation was methodologically sound, two factors tend to limit the usefulness of the evaluation results. The first factor concerns the eligibility of program participants. To participate in the GAP project, reunification and adoption must have been ruled out as viable permanency goals. This requirement was intended to preclude the possibility of legal guardianship substituting for either reunification or adoption as a permanency outcome for children. However, whether reunification and adoption can be successfully ruled out as permanency outcomes is questionable. In fact, among GAP participants who exited from care during the life of the program, approximately 17% exited via reunification, even though at the time of study recruitment caseworkers had ruled out reunification as a permanency option. This indicates that the estimated program impact may not represent the true program effects on the target population of GAP – those who would not eventually be adopted or be reunified with their natural parents. As such, the estimated program effects may not be relevant when GAP is implemented at a time when program eligibility can be determined more effectively. The second factor that introduces uncertainty to the external validity of the evaluation is the distinction between kinship care and restricted foster care. As discussed previously, Maryland has a two-tired payment system for relative caregivers. Unlicensed 14 formal kinship caregivers receive the TANF payments, averaging $188 per child per month among program participants. RFC caregivers, on the other hand, received approximately $600 per child per month. The existence of the dual payment system to relative caregivers means that a typical RFC caregiver, who received a $600 monthly subsidy for being a foster parent, stands to lose $300 per month if s/he assumed guardianship, which paid only $300 per month. It was hypothesized, however, that some of the RFC caregivers might opt to take up guardianship because the guardianship subsidy continues until the child is 18 years of age while the foster care board payment may be temporary (UMSSW, 2003). In addition, there is no stigma associated with guardianship as that of foster care. It was nonetheless expected that the subsidy would have a greater impact on children in kinship care rather than those in RFC. The discrepancy in subsidy rates also entails that for a child who was initially placed in kinship care, several possible permanency outcomes existed: he/she may have exited from foster care, have moved to RFC, or remained in kinship care and continued to receive subsidy at the TANF rate. Children who would exit from care, move to RFC, or remain in kinship in the absence of the guardianship subsidies may have very different characteristics. As such, the effects of the availability of the subsidy may be very different. The GAP estimate of program impact may not be generalizable to a kinship care population that has a different composition than the GAP participants. The GAP evaluation, with an experimental design and an ITT approach to analyses, ensured that causal effects can be convincingly inferred. However, the settings of this experiment limit the generalizability of the estimates of the program impact. In particular, what are the true effects of the availability of the subsidy on children who 15 could not exit via adoption or reunification? What will be the effects of the GAP program if the composition of the children in relative foster care changes in the future, with more children entering restricted foster care or exiting from care in the absence of the subsidy? How will this change affect program effects? This dissertation is an attempt to answer these questions. More specifically, this study will examine the differential effects of the eligibility of the guardianship subsidy on placement stability between four distinctive groups of children who were placed in kinship care at the beginning of the project: in the absence of the subsidy, those who would remain in kinship care, those who would move to RFC, those who would exit relative foster care via guardianship, and those who would exit from care via reunification. By differentiating the program effects for these four groups of children, one is able to generalize the estimated program effects to kinship care population with different characteristics than the current cohort of children. Innovative methods of propensity score matching proposed by Frangakis and Rubin (2002) were used to define the four groups. 16 Chapter 2. Literature Review 2.1 Relative Foster Care Vs. Non-relative Foster Care Empirical studies on relative foster care started to appear only in the last two decades. Most of these studies focused on comparison between formal relative foster care and nonrelative foster care (Beeman, Kim & Bullerdick, 2000; Berrick, Barth & Needell, 1994; Brooks & Barth, 1998; Grogan-Kaylor, 2000; Scannapieco, 1999). Many were studies on child and caregiver characteristics (Beeman et al., 2000; Grogan-Kaylor, 2000). Other research focused on services provided for the children and their caregivers (Scannapieco, Hegar & McAlpine, 1997), and child outcomes including permanency, length of stay, and long-term outcomes (Buehler, Orme & Post, 2000; Link, 1996; Wells & Guo, 1999; Courtney & Needle, 1997) 2.1.1 Child and Caregiver Characteristics The majority of studies on relative foster child characteristics are in comparison to nonrelative foster child characteristics. Most studies find that children in relative foster care are predominantly African-American (Beeman, Kim & Bullerdick, 2000; Berrick, Barth & Needell, 1994; Grogan-Kaylor, 2000; Scannapieco, 1999). Race and ethnicity appear to be a factor in determining the type of placement when a child enters out-of-home care. Being African American significantly increases a child’s probability of being placed with relatives (Grogan-Kaylor, 2000). Very young children appear not to be the focus of relative care. Infants and toddlers under the age of two are less likely to be placed with kin than non-relative foster 17 parents (Beeman et al., 2000; Grogan-Kaylor, 2000). Placement with relatives has not been found to be related to the gender of the child (Scannapieco, 1999). In addition to race and age, the presence of any child problems appear to affect the caseworker’s decision to place a child with a relative, possibly reflecting caseworkers’ lack of confidence in kin’s capability to cope with difficult situations. Placement with relative caregivers as opposed to non-relative foster caregivers is consistently found to be associated with the absence of observed physical, developmental, and behavioral problems (Beeman et al., 2000; Berrick, Barth & Needell, 1994; Brooks & Barth, 1998; Grogan-Kaylor, 2000; Scannapieco, 1999). Overall, children placed in relative foster care experience lower mortality rates than children cared for by nonrelative caregivers (Barth & Blackwell, 1998). Furthermore, children cared for by relatives have lower death rates from “congenital abnormalities, SIDS, and ill-defined conditions” than children in the general population, suggesting that caseworkers may tend to place healthier children with relatives. Additionally, compared with children in traditional foster care, children placed in relative care tend to have no prior placements (Beeman et al., 2000; Brooks & Barth, 1998; Grogan-Kaylor, 2000). In Maryland, children with mental health problems and disruptive behavior are more likely to be placed in non-relative foster care than with kin (Chipungu, Everett, & Verduik, 1998). Similarly, a child is more likely to be placed with non-relative foster parents if he/she is in special education. Somewhat conflicting results were reported concerning the health status of children in relative foster care. Some studies revealed that they had similar health problems to children in non-relative foster care, though the rates of health problems for 18 both groups were higher than those of children in the U.S. in general (Dubowitz et al., 1992). Others found children in relative foster care to have better health than their peers in regular foster care (Berrick et al., 1994; Grogan-Kaylor, 2000). A review of early empirical works concluded that the lack of consensus was due to the different sources of assessment. Relative foster caregivers tended to rate the health of children in their care as good whereas medical evaluations tended to reveal more health problems (Scannapieco, 1999). Children placed with relatives differ from children placed with non-relatives with regard to family background and reason for removal. Studies showed that compared with children in traditional foster care, children placed with kin were less likely to be removed from families receiving AFDC/TANF than families not receiving AFDC/TANF, although they were more likely to be removed from single-parent families rather than those with both parents (Grogan-Kaylor, 2000). A significantly higher fraction of children in relative foster care than their peers in traditional foster care had been removed from home because of neglect or maltreatment (Grogan-Kaylor, 2000; Scannapieco, 1999). Children who had been removed from parents because of parental substance abuse were more likely to be placed in relative foster care than children who were placed because of other parent-related issues such as parents’ illness, disability, incarceration, death, inability to cope, or abandonment (Beeman et al., 2000). Findings regarding relative caregiver characteristics have been fairly consistent. Compared with non-relative foster caregivers, relative caregivers appeared to be more disadvantaged socio-economically as a group. Similar to children in relative care, relative caregivers were predominantly African-American (Berrick et al., 1994; Gebel, 1996; 19 Link, 1996; Scannapieco, 1999). The majority of them were grandmothers of the children in their care (Berrick, 1997; McLean & Thomas, 1996; Testa et al., 1996). It is therefore, not surprising that they were on average at a more advanced age than non-relative foster parents, with an over-representation of people approaching retirement age (Berrick et al., 1994; Gebel, 1996; Link, 1996; Scannapieco, 1999). For example, one study reported that 29% of the relative caregivers were over 55 years of age while that age group only accounted for 19% of the non-relative caregivers (Berrick et al., 1994). With regard to economic status, relative caregivers, on average, tended to be less educated and with more meager income than non-relative caregivers (Berrick et al., 1994; Gebel, 1996; Scannapieco, 1999). One study indicated that over half of the relative caregivers did not have high school diplomas (Gebel, 1996). The proportion of non-high school graduates among non-relative caregivers was 19.5%. A significantly higher fraction of relative caregivers than non-relative caregivers were single parents (Berrick et al., 1994; Scannapieco, 1999). For many of them, foster care payments accounted for a large proportion of their income (Pecora, Le Prohn & Nasuti, 1999). They were less likely than non-relative foster caregivers to own homes, and more likely to be concerned with drugs in their families and neighborhoods (Berrick, 1997). In spite of their compromised economic positions, relative caregivers are committed to caring for the children in their care (McLean & Thomas, 1996). They are more willing to help maintain children’s relationships with birth parents, and assist with the social and emotional development of the children in their care (Pecora, Le Prohn, & Nasuti, 1999). However, relative caregivers were found to be unwilling to adopt children in their care (Meyer & Link, 1990; Thornton, 1990). 20 2.1.2 Services Received by Caregivers and Children in Relative Foster Care Although children and caregivers in relative foster care appear to be a group that has considerable resource needs, there is well-documented evidence that they receive both less contact with caseworkers and fewer types of services when compared with nonrelative foster caregivers (Barth et al., 1994; Berrick et al., 1994; Scannapieco et al., 1997). One early study found that as high as 29% of the relative caregivers had no contact with their caseworkers at all during the past year (Dubowitz & Feigelman, 1993). Although both relative and non-relative caregivers appear to be in need of services, relative caregivers are less likely to receive respite care, support group service, and general and specialized trainings (Barth et al., 1994). Children in non-relative foster care received more mental health, as well as transportation services, than those in relative foster care (Scannapieco et al., 1997). The only type of service which was found to be offered to children in relative care more frequently than children in non-relative care was substance-abuse treatment (Scannapieco et al., 1997). This may reflect the great proportion of drug-exposed children in relative foster care. Surprisingly, despite the inadequacy of services provided to relative caregivers, they are consistently more likely to have positive views of their social workers and the services they receive (Berrick et al., 1994; Brooks & Barth, 1998). The vast majority of studies on relative out-of-home care used child welfare agencies’ administrative records (Barth et al., 1994; Beeman et al., 2000; Grogan-Kaylor, 2000; McLean & Thomas, 1996; Scannapieco et al., 1997). Some studies obtained information through caregiver surveys (Berrick et al., 1994; Dubowitz & Feigelman, 1993; Gebel, 1996). A number of studies relied on descriptive or bivariate analysis techniques (Berrick et al., 1994; Dubowitz & Feigelman, 1993; Gebel, 1996; McLean & 21 Thomas, 1996; Scannapieco et al., 1997). While these techniques are valid when simply comparing the background characteristics between relative and non-relative care, it may be misleading in analyzing other outcomes. For example, the fact that children in relative care receive less mental health services may be due to the lower proportion of children with mental health problems among those placed with relatives. It may also reflect the fact that African Americans are less likely to utilize mental health services than their Caucasian counterparts (Cooper-Patrick et al., 1999; Mcmiller & Weisz, 1996). 2.1.3 Child Outcomes Child outcomes have been a frequent subject of foster care research. Studies in this area focus on length of stay, permanency issues, re-entry rates, and long-term outcomes. Regarding length of stay, a longer stay in care for foster children in general has been found to be associated with older ages of children, financial hardship of parents, problems that affect the mother-child relationship, and maternal mental illness (Maluccio, Fein, & Davis, 1994). African-American boys are at particularly high risk of staying in care for prolonged periods of time (Kemp & Bodony, 2000; Vogel, 1999). Studies, mostly based on administrative data, indicated that children in relative foster care remained in care longer than their peers in non-relative foster care (Link, 1996; Scannapieco, 1999; Vogel, 1999). Longer length of stay in care is the result of slower reunification and lower rates of adoption. Early studies reported that children in relative foster care returned home at a slower rate (Berrick, Needell, & Barth, 1999; Scannapieco, 1999). This may be due to the fact that birth parents considered placement with relatives less undesirable than placements with nonkin, and therefore, lacked the motivation to work hard towards 22 reunification (Berrick et al., 1999). More recent studies found that reunification rates did not differ by a big margin between relative and non-relative foster care (Wells & Guo, 1999). When there was a difference, it was exclusively a function of a difference in rates over the first few months that children were in care (Barth et al., 1994; Wells & Guo, 1999). Children placed with relatives were significantly less likely to be reunified with birth parents than their peers placed with non-relative foster parents during the first few months in care. However, the first few months appeared to be crucial, because those who exited via reunification during the first few months accounted for a large proportion of the children who would be returned home eventually (Barth et al., 1994). As such, the majority of children in relative out-of-home care may have missed the best opportunities of reunification. Overall, the effects of race/ethnicity, poverty, and family structure on family reunification are similar for children in relative and non-relative care. Being African-American; having health, emotional, and behavioral problems; coming from families that are AFDC/TANF eligible; and having only one parent present reduces the likelihood of returning home (Barth et al., 1994; Courtney & Needle, 1997; Landsverk et al., 1996; Wells & Guo, 1999). Additionally, for children placed with relatives, being removed from care because of abandonment negatively affects their chance of returning home (Landsverk et al., 1996). With regards to adoption, age at entry is the strongest predictor of being adopted (Barth et al., 1994; Finch, Fanshel & Grundy, 1986; Olsen, 1982). The probability of being adopted decreases as the age of a child increases. Other factors include race, length of stay in care, poverty, and placement with relatives (Barth et al., 1994; Finch et al., 1986; Olsen, 1982). Having stayed in foster care for a long time and being AFDC/TANF 23 eligible reduces the chance of being adopted. Initial placement with relatives was found to halve the odds of later adoption, even after controlling for child and family characteristics (Courtney & Needle, 1997). Caucasians, even when placed with relatives, are more likely to be adopted than African Americans (Barth et al., 1994). AfricanAmerican children placed with relatives are the least likely to be adopted. The effects of race, however, do not appear to occur prior to termination of parental rights. On the contrary, African-American children were found to be freed for adoption at a faster pace than Caucasian children (Barth, Courtney, & Berry, 1994; Olsen, 1982). The difference appears to be the time of waiting to be placed in adoptive placements and adoption legalization, which was significantly shorter for Caucasian children than for AfricanAmerican children (Barth et al., 1994; Olsen, 1982). Although children in relative foster care stay in care longer, research revealed that once they exited from foster care, they reentered care at a slower rate. A study using event history analysis reported that a child whose last placement was kinship care reentered at a rate 226% slower compared to a child in non-relative foster homes and 232% slower than a child in a group home (Wells & Guo, 1999). In the long term, people who had experience in both relative and non-relative foster care fare worse than the average young adults in the U.S. Young adults who spent at least six months in foster care have more problems with alcohol use than young adults in general. They are less satisfied with marriage and more likely to live with a spouse before getting married (Buehler et al., 2000). However, former relative foster care children and non-relative foster care children are similar when they grow up. There is no difference between the two groups in terms of education, employment, income, housing, 24 social support, and experience of life stress (Scannapieco, 1999). Former members of relative foster care are more self-sufficient and in somewhat better health, while former members of non-relative foster care reported less heroin usage and less trading sex for drugs (Scannapieco, 1999; Zuravin, Benedict, & Stallings, 1999). While viewing the results concerning child outcomes, it is noteworthy that the results are potentially subject to selection bias. Because random assignment of placement into relative or non-relative foster care is generally impossible, the majority of the comparative studies of relative and non-relative foster care use some type of regression technique on cross-sectional data. It is not clear whether differences in outcomes are due to the differences in type of care or some unobserved heterogeneity. The same is true for studies that compare foster children with children in general. A second note of caution concerns the external validity of the empirical studies on children in relative foster care reviewed above, due to the limited geographic areas from which the study samples were drawn. National samples were rare (Buehler et al., 2000). Among the 12 studies reviewed by Scannapieco (1999), six used samples from either Baltimore City or Baltimore County. A large number of more recent studies focused on children from various areas of California (Berrick et al., 1999; Courtney & Needell, 1997; Grogan-Kaylor, 2000; Leslie et al., 2000). Given this limitation, results concerning children in relative foster care may not be nationally representative. However, studies that concentrate on children in Baltimore City will be particularly relevant to this dissertation, because all children in the study sample of this dissertation came from Baltimore City. 25 2.2 Placement Stability for Children in Out-of-Home Care Many efforts have been made to find out the rates and determinants of placement disruption (Barber, Delfabbro, & Cooper, 2001; James, Landsverk, & Slymen, 2004; Olsen, 1982; Smith et al., 2001; Taber & Proch, 1987; Webster et al., 2000; Wulczyn, Kogan, & Harden, 2003). This attention is duly deserved, because placement instability has been repeatedly found to be detrimental to children’s emotional development and lead to increased behavioral problems (Kurtz et al., 1993, McRoy, 1994; Newton et al., 2000). Some recent studies examined the dynamics of children’s movements in out-ofhome care (James et al., 2004; Wulczyn et al., 2003). However, there is insufficient amount of knowledge on how to prevent placements from disrupting. 2.2.1 Placement Disruption Rates Various sources have reported on the placement disruption rates of children in foster care. In a study as early as 1959, it was estimated that one in four children in foster care had experienced four or more placements (Mass & Engler, 1959). Similarly, a study in 1961 revealed that 28% of all children in foster care changed placements three or more times while they were in foster care (Proch & Taber, 1985). The disruption rate appeared to have declined to 17% by 1977 (Proch & Taber, 1985). However, one study in 1986 found that the percentage for children having three or more placements after two years in care was an astounding 56% (Newton et al., 2000). Other studies in the ‘80s and early ‘90s reported percentages of children having three and more placements ranging from 22 to 48% (Newton et al., 2000; Smith et al., 2001). According to the USDHHS’ child welfare outcome annual report of 2000, only 4.6% of children who had been in foster care for less than 12 months had three or more placements in Maryland, considerably lower than the 26 50-state median of 15.7% (USDHHS, 2003a). However, the rate increased to 18.1% for Maryland children who had been in care between 12 to 24 months. The percentage of Maryland children who had three or more placements was as high as 50.8% for children who had been in care for over 48 months. Placements with relatives are generally deemed to be more stable than those with traditional foster parents (Courtney & Needell, 1997; Leslie et al., 2000; Scannapieco, 1999; Usher et al., 1999; Webster et al., 2000). However, one qualitative study found that it was not uncommon for relative foster care placements to fall apart. Terling-Watt (2001) reported that in a sample of children who were placed in relative foster care in Houston, Texas between 1993 and 1996, 29.2% of children who had been in care less than six months experienced placement disruption. The highest disruption rates were among children who had been in care between 15 to 36 months. Almost 50% of this group of children had multiple placements. This estimate, however, may not apply to all children in relative care, because the study population included only those whose placements were meant to be permanent, and only subsequent abuse or neglect, reentry to foster care, or disappearance of child were defined as placement disruption. The estimates of placement disruption rates vary widely among studies conducted in different times. One possible explanation of this variation is the impact of drugs on the physical and mental health of children. For example, high rates of placement disruption were reported in 1986, which coincided with the year when crack cocaine was introduced and popularized in African-American communities. The inconsistency may also be due to different definitions of placement disruption. Some studies excluded placement changes in the first few days of placement, arguing that most of the placements were emergency 27 placements that were meant to be temporary. Some studies only counted unplanned placement changes while others recorded all changes as placement disruptions. As a result, comparison of number of placements should be viewed with caution. Furthermore, statistics prior to 1960 may not be comparable to statistics after the 1960s. Historically, African-American children were not admitted to the modern welfare system due to racial segregation. Instead, a parallel system of child welfare existed for children of color. In the early 1960s, a large number of African-American children began to enter the children welfare system in the wake of the Flemming Rule (Lawrence-Webb, 1997)3. The foster care population prior to the Flemming Rule, which had a much lower percentage of African-American children, is therefore not comparable with that after the Flemming Rule. 2.2.2 Predictors of Placement Disruption Much of the foster care placement literature focuses on finding the predictors of placement disruptions. The only demographic characteristic that has been found to be associated with placement stability thus far was age of the child at the time of placement (Barber, Delfabbro, & Cooper, 2001; James, et al., 2004; Olsen, 1982; Smith et al., 2001; Taber & Proch, 1987; Webster et al., 2000; Wulczyn et al., 2003). Most studies found that children of older age were at greater risk of multiple placements. The effects of gender and race are less conclusive than age (Olsen, 1982; Proch & Taber, 1985; Smith et 3 Prior to the 1960s, it was a common practice of some Southern and Mid-Western states to deny AFDC benefits to African-American families on the grounds of “unsuitability” for a variety of reasons including if the child was born out-of-wedlock, or a unmarried male adult was living in the household. In the early 1960s, in response to this discriminatory practice, the U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare issued the “Flemming Rule”, which limited the states’ use of the “suitability” rule to deny AFDC benefits. However, as an unexpected consequence of the “Fleming Rule”, many cases that would be denied on the ground of “unsuitability” were then labeled “child neglect”, which result in a dramatic increase of AfricanAmerican children entering foster care. See Lawrence-Webb (1997) for a full discussion of the impact of Flemming Rule on African-American children and families. 28 al., 2001; Webster et al., 2000). There is evidence showing weak impact of race, with Caucasian children averaging higher numbers of placements than ethnic minorities (Olsen, 1982; Webster et al., 2000). It is uncertain, however, how much of this effect is due to the greater proportion of African-American children placed with relative caregivers. Somewhat conflicting results were found with respect to the effects of gender. While one study found male children to be more prone to placement disruption than their female peers (Webster et al., 2000), another study found teenage girls to be at greater risk of multiple placements than children from other gender/age groups (Smith et al., 2001). Compared with race and gender, the presence of behavioral and emotional problems of the child was more consistently found to be predictive of frequent placement changes (Barber et al., 2001; Newton et al., 2000; Proch & Taber, 1985). Specifically, violent or aggressive behavior, withdrawing behavior, and inability to integrate in the foster family or cope with authority were the main child behavioral problems leading to placement disruption. Children’s maltreatment history and experience with out-of-home care have also been found to be closely related to placement stability. The strongest predictor of multiple placements is length of stay in care (Proch &Taber, 1985). The longer a child’s stay in foster care, the more likely it is that s/he will experience multiple placements. However, number of placements is not merely a function of time spent in foster care. Most placement changes occur during the first six months in care (Wulczyn et al., 2003). Unstable placements in the first year of care are predictive of multiple placements in the future (Barber et al., 2001). It is, therefore, more likely that the same factors preventing children from exiting from foster care also affect their chance for stable placements. 29 Reason for removal from home and type of placements may be such factors. Victims of neglect are less likely to experience unstable placements than children removed from care for other reasons (Barber et al., 2001; James et al., 2004; Webster et al., 2000). Sexual abuse appears to be most likely to lead to multiple placements, possibly due to its more detrimental impact on a child’s emotional health (James, 2004). Children placed in relative foster care experience fewer placement changes than those placed with non-relative foster caregivers (Courtney & Needle, 1997; Leslie et al., 2000; Scannapieco, 1999; Testa, 2001; Webster et al., 2000). Closer relationship with the child leads to more stable placements. However, placements with aunts are less stable compared with those with grandmothers, although aunts are equally close to children as grandmothers (Testa, 2001). Placements with grandparents are the least likely to break. However, the advantage of relative placement appears to have waned after approximately three years into the placement, after which the difference in placement stability between relative and non-relative placement is negligible (Testa, 2001). Children placed with relative caregivers face unique risks for placement disruption. Terling-Watt (2001) found that the most frequently cited reason for relative foster care placement disruption was the continued influence of the child’s parents. Relative caregivers frequently failed to abide by rules prohibiting unsupervised visitation by parents, exposing children to risks of further abuse. Moreover, relative caregivers often lacked the resources and expertise to care for children with special needs. The old age and relatively poor health of relative caregivers were also barriers to placement stability. 30 2.2.3 Consequences of Placement Instability Although frequent change of primary caregiver has been found to affect a child’s ability to develop attachment to his/her caregivers, which is considered a developmental milestone (McRoy, 1994), early research did not find overwhelming evidence that multiple placements were related to child wellbeing and social effectiveness (Proch & Taber, 1985). More recent research suggests that frequent changes in placement for children in foster care results in increased behavioral problems such as poor selfadjustment, low self-concept, and internalizing and externalizing behaviors, including aggressiveness, withdrawal, and inabilities to integrate with the environment (Newton et al., 2000; Kurtz et al., 1993). Even the perception of impermanency is significantly associated with behavioral problems (Harden, 2004). Additionally, placement stability is an important mediating factor for educational progress. The mobility related to foster care placements were found to account for much of the poor academic performance of children in out-of-home placements (Eckenrode et al., 1995). Multiple placements are also predictive of increased juvenile delinquency. For neglected and maltreated children, frequent placement disruption leads to increased probability of being arrested, while having been placed in out-of-home care itself does not (Widom, 1991). Those who spent over ten years in the first placement have lower rates of criminality than those who did not. However, the differences disappear when factors such as behavioral problems are controlled. One prevalent issue concerning the placement stability research is that the majority of these studies were retrospective and, as such, lack the baseline information necessary to establish the direction of causality between children’s emotional and behavioral problems and placement instability (Webster et al., 2000; Barber et al., 2001; 31 Proch & Taber, 1985; Kurtz et al., 1993). One study that controlled baseline behavioral problems found that behavioral problems were both the cause and consequence of frequent placement disruptions (Newton et al., 2000). After controlling for baseline behavioral scores measured on the Child Behavior Checklist, no demographic characteristic remained significant. Furthermore, children who initially had no behavioral problems were at the greatest risk of being harmed by multiple placements. However, this study did not focus on an incoming cohort of children, which inevitably biased the results towards those who stayed in care for long periods of time. 2.3 Outcomes for Children in Legal Guardianship There exists a scant amount of research examining the outcomes for children in the care of legal guardians or whose permanency goal is legal guardianship. One such study found guardianship placements to be generally stable (Iglehart, 1994). The majority (81%) of the guardianship adolescents in the study were in their first and only placement, and half of them had been with their current guardian for five or more years. However, most of the adolescents were with non-relative guardians and the study was limited to those who were 16 years or older. Another study based on a group of abused or neglected children under legal guardianship revealed that the rate for permanency failure in adjudicated abuse/neglect guardianship was 36%, which was two to three times higher than that in most adoption studies (Henry, 1999). The disruption rate of non-adjudicated abused/neglected children, on the other hand, was only 9%, which was similar to adoption failure rate of 7% to 14% reported in the literature. 32 2.4 IV-E Waiver Demonstrations on Subsidized Guardianship In order to better understand subsidized guardianship as a permanency option and its effect on child outcomes, the federal government granted seven states Title IV-E waivers to conduct demonstration projects that have assisted guardianship components for children in relative foster care4. Four states - Illinois, Maryland, New Mexico and Montana used experimental designs with random assignment. Delaware, North Carolina and Oregon based their evaluations on quasi-experimental designs or descriptive analyses (USDHHS, 2005). As of September 2005, quantitative results regarding permanency and placement stability were available for the states of Illinois, New Mexico, Oregon, and Maryland. Illinois, which implemented a large scale true experiment on assisted guardianship, reported that the availability of the guardianship subsidy increased combined permanency rate by 6 percentage points while the rate of subsequent reports of abuse or neglect remained unchanged. However, the final results indicated a possible trade-off between overall legal permanency rate and the rate of adoption. The experimental and control groups, however, did not differ in terms of placement stability (Testa, 2002; USDHHS, no date). The New Mexico assisted guardianship demonstration consisted of two components: children under state custody and children under tribal custody. In the state custody component, non-Native American children under state custody were randomly assigned to either the experiment group, which was eligible for a guardianship subsidy, or the control group, which was not eligible for the subsidy. Outcomes of the two groups 4 These seven states are Delaware, Illinois, Maryland, Montana, New Mexico, North Carolina and Oregan. Two more states – Minnesota and Wisconsin – have received waiver approval and were scheduled to implement demonstrations involving assisted guardianship in late 2005 to early 2006 (USDHHS, 2005). 33 were compared. In the tribal custody component, conversely, all Native American children under tribal custody were eligible for assisted guardianship. These children were compared with Native American children under state custody, who were not offered the subsidy. The state reported that children in the experimental group of the state custody component spent less time in out-of-home placements compared with their peers in the control group (TriWest, 2004). The state report, however, did not indicate whether the difference was statistically significant. No statistics on placement stability were reported. The evaluation of Oregon's assisted guardianship project compared children for whom assisted guardianship was established with children from a comparison county where subsidized guardianship was not offered. The evaluation revealed that the eligibility of a guardianship subsidy did not have any statistically significant effect on placement stability (Lehman, Liang, O’Dell, & Duryea, 2003). Oregon did not report statistics regarding permanency outcomes. The evaluation of Maryland’s GAP project was based on an experimental design (Mandell, 2001; UMSSW, 2003). The evaluation compared outcome measures for children who were eligible for the subsidy and those who were not eligible for the subsidy. Program participants were grouped into two cohorts: cohort one, which was randomly assigned to the experimental or control group between October 1998 and February 1999; and cohort two, which was assigned on May 1, 2000. Results indicated that the guardianship subsidy expedited children’s exit from care. An event history analysis on the time until exit revealed that the kinship care experimental group, which was eligible for the subsidy, had a hazard ratio that was 43 to 123% higher than that of 34 the control group, which was not eligible for the subsidy. Further, bivariate analysis revealed that among those who exited from care, kinship children eligible for subsidy were more likely to exit via legal guardianship than children who were not eligible for the subsidy. However, the subsidy did not have any significant effects on the speed of achieving permanency or exit types for children in the RFC (restricted foster care) group. Regarding placement stability, Poisson regression and negative binomial regression models were estimated to examine the effects of the availability of the subsidy (UMSSW, 2003). The results were inconclusive. For some unknown reason, the availability of the subsidy increased placement instability experienced by the group of children who were initially placed in restricted foster care. For the rest of the study sample, the effects of the availability of the subsidy were not statistically significant regarding placement stability. 2.5 Propensity Score Matching The probability of receiving a particular treatment conditional on observed covariates is commonly known as the propensity score (Rosenbaum & Rubin, 1983). Since its formal introduction in 1983, propensity score matching (PSM) has been a commonly used strategy in observational studies to overcome selection bias. Its advantage is to solve the dimensionality problem in matching by reducing the often numerous background characteristics into one single variable (Rubin, 1997). Dehejia and Wahba (1999) assessed the ability of PSM to produce unbiased estimates of program effects by examining the impact of the National Supported Work Demonstration using the PSM method. The results were then compared with those generated by Lalonde (1986) in his ground-breaking study evaluating various nonexperimental methods. The authors concluded that the PSM method could produce estimates much closer to the experimental 35 benchmark when the range of propensity scores of those receiving intervention overlapped with that of those who did not receive the intervention. However, the estimates from PSM were sensitive to the selection of covariates. A similar study using data from the National Evaluation of Welfare-to-Work Strategies found that selecting control cases through PSM improved sample balance and reduced bias (Bloom, Michalopoulos, Hill, & Lei, 2002). The PSM strategy has recently been used in a number of social experiments to generate treatment-on-treated (TOT) estimates. One interesting example is a study conducted by Barnard, Frangakis, Hill, and Rubin (2001), who estimated the effects of using a school voucher – not the effects of a voucher offer, which generally requires straightforward intent to treat (ITT) estimates – by stratifying the study sample into compliers, always takers, never takers, and defiers. Propensity scores for being in each group were calculated through probit models. Control group members were thus matched to the experimental group members with the closest propensity score. The impact of the voucher was derived by comparing standardized test scores of the compliers who were offered a voucher and the compliers who were not offered the voucher. A similar study examined the effects of taking advantage of a housing voucher on participants’ wellbeing for people living in high-poverty areas (Katz, Kling, & Liebman, 2001). The PSM strategy can also be applied when multiple treatments are involved. Gibson (2001), for example, examined the impact of the New Hope demonstration, a randomized anti-poverty program using the PSM method. The benefits offered by the program included child care subsidies, wage subsidies, health insurance, and temporary community service jobs. The author divided the experimental group into five subgroups 36 according to the services that they took advantage of, and matched control group members to the experimental group members with the closest propensity scores. Thus, she was able to show the differential effects of different program components. Using the PSM strategy to generate TOT estimates in a randomized experiment is different from standard PSM in that usually only the members of the experimental group will be used in the estimation of the model that predicts the likelihood to participate. A similar application of this strategy is the use of PSM in sub-group analysis where only the members of the control groups are used in the initial model estimation. One example is a study that examines the differential effect of high-quality center-based child care on children who would, in the absence of the center-based child care provided, end up having no non-maternal care, some non-maternal care but no center-based care, and some center-based care (Hill, Waldfogel, & Brooks-Gunn, 2002). Another example is a study that aimed to find out whether the impacts of two anti-poverty programs – the New Hope demonstration and the Minnesota Family Investment Program – differed by the participants’ risk for unemployment (Yoshikawa, Magnuson, Bos, & Hsueh, 2003). In both studies, the defining variables for sub-groups were post-randomization variables that were correlated with whether or not the program participant received intervention. Furthermore, the defining variables were only observed for the members of the control groups. Authors of both studies concluded that the PSM strategy was successful in achieving sample balance, and the authors of the former study reported that results after PSM were considerably different than if the sample was stratified using observed types of child care. 37 Chapter 3. Methodology 3.1 Research Question For children who have been formally placed with relative caregivers, there are many possible outcomes. Some may be returned to parents. Some may be adopted by their caregivers. Some may leave the child welfare system by moving to guardianship care, and yet some may remain. Even those who remain in out-of-home placements may experience different types of care. Some caregivers may eventually be licensed and become restricted foster caregivers. Some may remain in kinship care and their caregivers continue to receive subsidies at the TANF level. This dissertation focuses on the children who were placed with relative caregivers at the time of the random assignment of GAP. Although reunification and adoption had, in principle, been ruled out for program participants as a condition for eligibility in the study, predicting permanency outcomes appeared to be difficult. As a result, a substantial number of children actually exited foster care through reunification (UMSSW, 2003). As such, the majority of children achieved one of the four types of outcomes: 1) exiting through reunification, 2) exiting through guardianship, 3) remaining in out-of-home care but moving to restricted foster care, and 4) remaining in kinship care. Many studies have shown that permanency outcomes are associated with various background characteristics of children, as the previous chapter has indicated. Similarly, the effects of the eligibility for the guardianship subsidy on placement stability may vary among these children. Although the GAP evaluation indicated that the overall effects of guardianship subsidy on placement stability were negligible, no efforts thus far have been 38 devoted to examining the distribution of the effects. It is not inconceivable that eligibility for the subsidy may benefit some groups of children by enhancing their placement stability while hurting other children by undermining it. As such, this dissertation examined the following research question: What are the differential effects of the availability of the guardianship subsidy on placement stability among four distinctive groups of children who were in kinship care placements at the beginning of the project: 1. Those who would remain in kinship care in the absence of the subsidy; 2. Those who would move to restricted foster care in the absence of the subsidy; 3. Those who would exit relative foster care via guardianship in the absence of the subsidy; and 4. Those who would be reunified with their parents in the absence of the subsidy. There is reason to believe that the availability of a guardianship subsidy may reduce the number of placements a child experiences in foster care. It is conceivable that the prospect of receiving a guardianship subsidy will encourage relative caregivers to stay with the children in their care rather than withdraw from providing care in case of any problems. In addition, the knowledge that his/her placement with the relative caregiver is more likely to become permanent may help the child develop attachment to the caregiver and vice versa. Regarding the differential effects of the subsidy among the four sub-groups of children, it is expected that the subsidy will not have a great impact on those children who would exit from care even if a guardianship subsidy were not available. The effects on those who would exit from care via guardianship in the absence of the subsidy are 39 expected to be especially small, if any. Regarding those who would be reunified with their natural families, the availability of the subsidy may result in guardianship, substituting reunification as a permanency outcome for children. However, there is no reason to believe that the subsidy may affect placement stability. Although the subsidy offer may not have much impact on children who would exit from care in the absence of the subsidy, it may benefit children who would otherwise remain in kinship care. It has been reported that the availability of the subsidy increased the overall exit rates from foster care (Mandell, 2001; UMSSW, 2003). It is reasonable to assume that the eligibility of the subsidy induced a fraction of the group members which would remain in kinship care in the absence of the subsidy to exit from care and take up guardianship. This suggests that the subsidy offer’s effects on placement stability may be the greatest among those who would remain in care in the absence of the subsidy. Compared with the other three sub-groups, the magnitude of the effects of the subsidy offer for those who would otherwise move to restricted foster care is more difficult to predict. Whether the child is eligible for guardianship subsidy may not have much bearing on the caregiver’s motivation to continue providing care, because RFC provides a subsidy twice as high as the guardianship subsidy. However, subsidized guardianship as an additional permanency option may still have psychological effects on the caregiver as well as the child. As indicated by the research question above, subgroup status is defined by the permanency outcome of the child in the absence of the subsidy. Permanency outcomes, however, were not realized until after randomization. Further, post-randomization permanency outcomes are expected to be correlated with the eligibility for the subsidy. 40 As such, post-randomization and observed permanency outcomes are not a good approximation of the defining variable of the subgroups. This issue will be discussed in more detail when the methodology of the statistical analysis is presented. 3.2 Sample and Data Two cohorts of children in Baltimore City and one group of children from other jurisdictions in Maryland participated in GAP. They were randomly assigned to either the experimental group or the control group. Members of the experimental group were eligible to receive a $300 monthly subsidy until the child reaches maturity at 18 years of age if the caregiver assumed guardianship5. Members of the control group, on the other hand, were not eligible for this subsidy. Random assignment of the first cohort of 772 children, which consisted of 40% of Baltimore City’s eligible relative foster care population, took place between October 1998 and February 1999. However, a process study revealed that the offers of guardianship subsidy were never made to many RFC caregivers even if they were eligible, perhaps because their caseworkers believed that they would not be interested in participating (UMSSW, 2003). The second cohort of children, which included the remaining 986 eligible children in relative foster care in Baltimore City, was randomly assigned to the control or experimental group on May 1, 2000. The offer of subsidy was implemented more successfully with this cohort of children due to more aggressive training of the caseworkers (UMSSW, 2003). The group of 107 children from other jurisdictions in Maryland was also randomly assigned. However, with such a small sample size, the subsidy effects would have to be quite substantial to be detected for children coming from other jurisdictions of Maryland. 5 In the event that the child is in an approved education setting, the age of maturity is 21. 41 This dissertation focuses on the second cohort of children from Baltimore City, with whom the subsidy was implemented as originally designed. Among these children, 511 were in kinship care placements at the time of random assignment. Further examination indicated that 74 children exited from foster care before random assignment, and never reentered the system. These 74 cases were excluded from any analysis. Further, 13 cases exited from foster care after random assignment and reentered the system at some point during the study. Placement dynamics were expected to be different for these cases. These cases were also excluded from the study. As a result, the effective sample included 424 children who were randomly assigned to the experimental or the control group on May 1, 2000, and were in kinship care placements at the time of random assignment. Data on case history and some child characteristics were downloaded monthly from the state administrative database Foster Care and Adoption Child Tracking System (FACTS). During the period between December 2000 and April 2003, 20 monthly downloads were provided by the Maryland Department of Human Resources.6 In addition to the administrative data, information regarding caregiver characteristics was available for some caregivers. All caregivers were invited to participate in a self-administered and computerized interview, which asked for extensive information about caregivers, including caregiver demographics, economic and health status, spirituality, and social support, as well as some information about each child in 6 Downloads were not available in 9 of the 29 months between December 2000 and April 2003: March, April, June, and October of 2001; April, October, and November of 2002; and February and March of 2003. Because none of the relevant variables involve exact time, it is not expected that the missing downloads will have a significant impact on the results. Further, even if some information regarding case history was made unavailable by the missing downloads, the effects of such missing information should have affected the control and experimental groups equally. 42 their care. However, only 159 caregivers of 258 children from cohort two agreed to and completed the interview by May 2003. Among the 258 children whose caregivers have been interviewed, 146 were initially placed in kinship care. 3.3 Measurement of Placement Stability Placement stability is defined as the change in number of placements between December 2000 and April 2003. Ideally a measure of placement stability should exclude moves from a more restrictive setting to a less restrictive setting (Smith et al., 2001). Additionally, one may also argue that placement changes should exclude stays at emergency shelters, which serve as temporary placements before a more permanent placement can be arranged (James et al., 2004). However, because distinguishing “good” placement movements versus “bad” placement movements requires more information than is currently available, this study only examines the aggregate count of placement changes during the follow-up period. 3.4 Statistical Analysis This study examined the differential effects of the availability of a guardianship subsidy among four groups of children. Group status was defined as the permanency outcome of the child in the absence of the subsidy, a variable that is only observed after randomization. The statistical analysis, therefore, adopted a two-stage procedure. In the first stage, the propensities to achieve the four different types of permanency outcome for the control group were analyzed with a multinomial logit model. Members of the experimental group were subsequently matched with members of the control group based on the propensity scores. In the second stage, separate Poisson regression models were estimated for each group of children to gauge the effects of the availability of the subsidy. 43 3.4.1 Propensity Score Matching The research question of this dissertation distinguished between the effects of eligibility for the subsidy among four groups of children. The first step in the data analysis, therefore, was to identify the children in each of the four groups. Group identification was complicated in this dissertation study as the defining variable of groups was a postrandomization variable that was only observed for the control group. Specifically, let Z denote the variable representing whether the child was in the experimental group and, as such, was eligible for the subsidy: 0 Z 1 If the child was not eligible for the guardianship subsidy If the child was eligible for the guardianship subsidy Let G denote the variable that represents the permanency outcome of a child: g If the child exited from foster care via guardianship r If the child exited from foster care via reunification G k If the child remained in kinship care f If the child moved to RFC The defining variable of subgroup status, namely permanency outcomes of the child in the absence of the subsidy, can be written as [ G Z 0 ]. This however, was only observed for the members of the control group. What we observed for the members of the experimental group, on the other hand, was [ G Z 1 ]. For these children, [ G Z 0 ] was a potential outcome which was not observed. Further, as the previous section points out, stratifying the sample using the observed permanency does not produce unbiased estimates of the subsidy effects within strata because permanency outcome G was likely to be affected by the eligibility of the subsidy, represented by Z. Children from the 44 control group and children from the experimental group who had achieved the same outcome may not be equivalent. This issue was addressed by the propensity score matching method described by Frangakis and Rubin (2002), who showed that treatment effects derived by comparing cases of different treatment status within principal strata are causal effects. Principal stratification with respect to a post-treatment variable, such as permanency outcomes, refers to the process of classifying cases according to the probabilities of potentially obtaining the values of the post-treatment variable, predicted by pre-treatment covariates. In this dissertation, a multinomial logit model was estimated with data related to the control group, for whom permanency outcomes in the absence of the subsidy were observed. Using the estimated parameters, propensity scores, namely predicted probabilities, were computed for members of both the control and experimental groups. For each individual, four propensity scores, representing the probabilities of achieving each of the four permanency outcomes, are computed. The variable for group definition ([ G Z 0 ]) was observed for members of the control group. Experimental cases, for whom [ G Z 0 ] was not observed, were matched to control cases with the closest propensity score. For example, suppose child A from the control group exited from care via guardianship during the study period. Four propensity scores, namely Pr(GA g x A ) , Pr(G A r x A ) , Pr(G A k x A ) , and Pr(G A f x A ) would be computed from the multinomial logit model. Four propensity scores would also be computed for each member of the experimental group, one for each permanency outcome. In reality, suppose it was observed that the child A exited from care via guardianship. To find the experimental group member that should be matched to child A, one would 45 compare Pr(GA g x A ) with Pr(G g x ) for each member of the experimental group. Assume that child B is the member of the experimental group that has the closest Pr(G g x ) to child A. Child B, then, would be matched to Child A. Both child A and child B are said to belong to the group of children who would exit through guardianship in the absence of the subsidy ([ G g Z 0 ]), regardless of child B’s actual permanency outcome. Matching was conducted with replacement. As such, it is possible that one experimental case was matched to more than one control group case in the same subgroup. It is also possible for one experimental case to be matched to control cases belonging to different permanency outcome groups. Because some experimental cases may be matched to more than one control case, matching with replacement results in a smaller sample size. However, it produces more balanced treatment and comparison groups (Bloom, Michalopoulos, Hill, & Lei, 2002). Table 3.1 presents the variables that were considered as for being included in the predictive models. The main source of data is FACTS, the state administrative database, which contains variables regarding demographic characteristics, special needs of the child, and case history. The caregiver interview provided much needed information regarding caregiver characteristics, which were considered in the sensitivity analysis discussed in the end of this chapter. 46 Table 3. 1 Predictors of child permanency and placement outcomes Variable Type Variables Gender and race Age at the time of entry into out-of-home care Child characteristics Whether the child has a sibling group in out-of-home care Presence of special needs Length of stay in care prior to random assignment Case history Reason for entering into out-of-home placement Number of placements before the current episode Child Characteristics Child race, gender and age at the time of entry into out-of-home care were included as predictors in the models predicting permanency status. A substantial number of children in the sample have sibling groups in out-of-home care. An indicator of whether the child has siblings was included in the model. Whether the child was in special education was also considered a predictor. However, because the proportion of child in special education was small, it was not able to be included in the predictive models of permanency and placement outcomes. A small proportion of children (less than 10%) have any special needs. These special needs include a variety of physical, mental, and behavioral problems that would warrant special attention in placement.7 Because the types of special needs vary widely, and accounting for each type of these special needs would dramatically reduce the cell sizes, a dummy variable representing the presence of any special needs, therefore, was considered in the predictive models. Case History Length of stay in care prior to random assignment was computed for all children. Also included in the model was the number of placements during the previous episode of out7 The percentages of children with different types of special needs are presented in Table 4.1 in Chapter 4. 47 of-home care. Regarding the child’s entry reason, whether the child was placed in out-ofhome care because of abandonment was included in the model. Abandonment was the second most common reason of entry, accounting for over 10% of children. Almost 80% of all children in the sample were placed in out-of-care solely or partially due to parental neglect. A small fraction of children were removed from homes because of abuse and a number of other reasons.8 Dummy variables representing whether the child was removed from home because of parental neglect and abandonment were considered as predictored in the models. Other entry reasons were not included due to the lack of variations. Another variable not included in the model was primary caregiver at removal. Although children removed from parents and other caregivers might differ in a way that affects the permanency outcomes, information on primary caregiver at removal was missing in a substantial number of the cases (19%, n=80). Including this variable as a predictor would reduce the effective sample size significantly. 3.4.2 Poisson Regression The outcome variable of this study, namely changes in the number of placements during the follow-up period, is a count variable. As common to variables representing patterns of events occurring independently over a period of time, it was assumed that placement changes follow a Poisson probability distribution. Specifically, Exp(-µ)µy Pr(y|µ) = y! 8 See Table 4.1 in Chapter 4 for a tabulation of entry reasons by the eligibility of the subsidy. 48 where y was a Poisson random variable representing the number of changes a child experiences during a certain time period and µ was the mean number of changes in placements. Poisson regressions, where the conditional mean of a Poisson random variable is explained by individual characteristics, was employed to estimate the effects of being in the experimental group on placement stability. Specifically, the structural model estimated was the following: i E( yi xi ) exp( Zi xi β) , where yi ~ Poisson ( i ) . Variable Z represents whether the child is eligible for the guardianship subsidy, and x is a vector of covariates that have been identified in the literature as predictive of placement stability, including child race, gender, age at entry, length of stay in care, child special needs, and entry reason. Due to the fact that some children exited from foster care at different stages of the study, the time of exposure to the risk of placement disruption varied among cases. The model, therefore, was also adjusted for the amount of exposure of the cases. Regressions models are estimated separately for each of the four groups: children who would exit from care via guardianship, children who would exit care via reunification, children who would move to RFC, and children who would remain in kinship care in the absence of the guardianship subsidy. One critical assumption of the Poisson probability distribution is that the variance of the Poisson random variable is equal to its expected value. When this assumption is not satisfied, Poisson regression is inefficient, and the standard errors of the coefficient estimates are smaller than what they should be (Long, 1997). If this is the case, negative binomial regression, which allows for overdispersion, should be used. The resulting model will be the following: 49 ~i E( yi x i ) exp( Z i x i β i ) exp( Z i x i β) i , where δi represents unobserved heterogeneity and is assumed to have an expected value of 1 and follow a Gamma distribution with variance 1/vi. It is assumed that vi is constant across individuals and vi 1 where 0 . The equal-dispersion assumption was tested using a log-likelihood ratio test, where the null hypothesis is 0 . When the null hypothesis was not rejected, the negative binomial model reduced to a Poisson regression model. Otherwise the negative binomial regression model was used. 3.4.3 Sensitivity Analysis Sensitivity Analysis 1: Caregiver Characteristics As discussed previously, the number of children in the sample whose caregivers completed the caregiver interview accounted for approximately 25% of all children in the sample. Although caregiver characteristics were expected to be predictive of permanency outcomes, we faced a trade off in that including variables retrieved from the caregiver interviews would reduce the sample size dramatically. To determine whether the omission of caregiver characteristics has any impact on the matching results, a multinomial logit model was estimated with child characteristics and a dummy variable representing whether or not the caregiver of the child participated in the caregiver interview. While the individual impact of caregiver characteristics, such as caregiver demographics, household income, and caregiver health status, could not be determined, the dummy variable served as a proxy for the set of characteristics that distinguish interview participants from non-participants. 50 Sensitivity Analysis 2: Excluding non-African-American Children Over 95% of the sample of this study was African-American. A sensitivity analysis was conducted with the American-American sub-sample. Non-African-American children were excluded from both the propensity score matching process and the second-stage Poisson regression or negative binomial models. Results are discussed in the next chapter. 51 Chapter 4. Results 4.1 Sample Characteristics A total of 424 children who were initially placed in kinship care and did not reenter outof-home care during the study period were included in the analysis. Among these children, 223 were assigned to the control group, and 201 were assigned to the experimental group. Table 4.1 presents the characteristics of these children. Both the control and experimental groups were approximately evenly split between males and females. African-American children accounted for the overwhelming majority of the experimental and control groups, while children from other ethnic groups accounted for less than 5% of either group. There are slightly more African-American children in the experimental group than the control group. At the time that a typical child entered kinship care, s/he was approximately 6 years old. Children in the experimental group entered kinship care at a slightly older age than those in the control group, but not significantly so. Table 4. 1 Sample Characteristics Gender Female Male Race African American Other Education Regular School Special Education Not in School Entry reason Neglect Abandonment Abuse Control Group Experimental Group 49.3% 50.7% 51.2% 48.8% χ2= 0.16, p=0.69 95.5% 4.5% 98.5% 1.5% χ2= 3.15, p=0.07 70.9% 2.7% 26.5% 64.8% 3.9% 31.3% χ2= 1.68, p=0.43 70.7% 11.6% 5.6% 72.5% 11.8% 5.6% 52 Control Group Other More than one of the above Special needs None Sibling groups Other special needs Both sibling group and other special needs Caregiver at removal Mother only Father only Both mother and father Mother/father and other adult Relatives Non-relatives Age at entry Length of stay in care (in months) prior to random assignment N 1.5% 10.6% Experimental Group 0.6% 9.6% χ2= 0.95, p=0.92 35.5% 52.7% 7.5% 4.3% 45.2% 45.8% 7.1% 1.8% χ2= 4.75, p=0.19 79.6% 1.1% 2.8% 4.0% 76.2% 1.2% 1.8% 3.6% 6.3% 6.3% 5.9 43.4 11.9% 5.4% 6.4 41.6 223 201 χ2= 3.74, p=0.59 t=-1.23, p=0.22 t = 0.66, p=0.51 African American, Non-Hispanic Other entry reasons include parental incapacity, mental injury due to abuse or neglect, child’s disruptive behavior, and child’s special needs. Other special needs include child physical, mental and behavioral problems. Sample mean Children in the sample were relatively free of problems. Only approximately 10% of the children have any identified special needs involving physical, mental, and behavioral problems. About half of the children had sibling groups. Less than 4% of children were in special education. For sample members five years and older, 4.67% were in special education. This was considerably lower than the proportion of children in special education in Maryland during the same time period. During the 2000/2001 school year, as high as 8.55% of the children from 6 to 21 years of age in Maryland were in special education (U.S. Department of Education, 2002). However, 10.75% of the sample members 5 years and older were not in school at the beginning of the study. All of these children were under 15 years old. It is unclear why these children were not in school. No 53 statistically significant differences were detected between the experimental and control groups regarding child special needs or education status. The members of the experimental and the control groups were also similar in terms of family background and maltreatment history. Most children were removed from single-parent families. Only about 3% of the control group members and 2% of the experimental group members were removed from families with both parents present. The most common reason for out-of-home placement was parental neglect. In over 70% of the cases in the sample, parental neglect was identified as the only reason why the child was removed from home. The second most commonly identified reason for removal was abandonment, followed by abuse. At the time of random assignment, the members of the experimental group and the control group had spent an average of 41.64 and 43.40 months (or about three and a half years) in out-of-home care, respectively. No significant difference was found between members of the experimental and control groups in terms of family background or reasons for entry into out-of-home care. 4.2 Bivariate Analysis While the background characteristics of the members of the experimental group and those of the control group were similar at the baseline, their experiences in out-ofhome care started to diverge after the guardianship subsidies were made available to the experimental group members. Table 4.2 presents the number of placement changes during the study period between December 2000 and April 2003. The majority of both the control and experimental groups experienced no placement disruption during this period. However, a significantly higher percentage of the members of the experimental 54 group (73%) had no placement changes compared to the members of the control group (57%). Table 4. 2 Number of placement changes Control Group Experimental Group None 128 146 57% 73% One 64 33 28% 16% Two 20 11 9% 5% Three or more 9 8 4% 4% N = 424 χ2 = 12.54 P=0.006 The difference in the number of placement changes, however, was not simply a function of placement stability when the sample members were in out-of-placement care. It also reflected the higher percentage of children who exited from out-of-home care before the end of the study. As previous evaluation results indicate, children in the experimental group exited from care faster than children in the control group (Mandell, 2001; UMSSW, 2003). As children exited from care, they were no longer exposed to the risks of placement disruption. As such, everything else equal, it was expected that the group of children who exited from care faster would experience fewer placement changes. Table 4.3 shows the number of children who exited from kinship care between December 2000 and April 2003 and the permanency status of these children as of April 2003. Approximately 65% of the members of the experimental group exited from out-ofhome care during this period while only 48 % of the members of the control group exited from care. Most of the increase in the number of exits appeared to be attributable to guardianship. A significantly higher percentage of the experimental group members (40%) exited via guardianship than those in the control group (27%). On the other hand, a 55 higher age of the children in the control group (27%) than children in the experimental group (15%) remained in kinship care. Additionally, a lower percentage of children in the experimental group (7%) than those in the control group (13%) were returned to their birth parents, indicating that guardianship may have substituted for reunification as a permanency outcome for a small proportion of children. Table 4. 3 Permanency Outcomes Control Group 61 27% 28 13% 10 4% 7 3% 56 25% 61 27% Exited to guardianship Exited to reunification Exited to adoption Exited for other reasons9 Moved to RFC Remained in KC N = 424 χ2 = 31.57 Experimental Group 107 40% 14 7% 5 3% 5 2% 41 20% 30 15% P<0.001 4.3 Subgroup Analysis: Children who Would Exit and Children who Would Stay The objective of providing a guardianship subsidy is to expedite the pace of children exiting from out-of-home care. As such, a policy relevant question is whether the subsidy has a bigger impact on children who would remain in kinship care than children who would exit out-of-home care in the absence of the subsidy. To address this question, based on the probabilities of exiting, children in kinship care placements at the beginning of the project were divided into two groups: 1) those who would exit from foster care in the absence of the subsidy; and 2) those who would not exit from foster care in the 9 Other exit reasons include independent living, running away, agency service not appropriate, and other. 56 absence of the subsidy. The impact of the availability of the guardianship subsidy was examined separately for each group. 4.3.1 Predicting the Probabilities of Exiting Foster Care in the Absence of the Subsidy Logit models were estimated to determine the impact of demographic characteristics and case history on the probabilities of exiting for children in the control group. Table 4.4 presents the results obtained from the logit models. In addition to demographic and case history variables, Logit model 2 included whether the caregiver participated in the caregiver interview as a predictor while Logit model 1 did not. Among the 223 children in the control group who were in kinship care placements at the time of random assignment, 44 children were not included in the logit models due to missing data, resulting in a sample size of 179. Because more than one child might come from the same sibling group and be cared for by the same caregiver, the independence assumption of the maximum likelihood procedure may be violated. The clustering effects were adjusted for by using the Huber/White robust estimator of variance to compute the standard errors of the coefficient estimates. Table 4. 4 Coefficients obtained from the logit models predicting the probabilities of exiting from foster care for the members of the control group Logit Model 1 Logit Model 2 Male -0.05 -0.05 (0.33) (0.33) African American -0.96 -0.91 (0.88) (0.92) Age at entry (in months) -0.00 -0.01 (0.00) (0.00) Length of stay in care prior to random -0.02*** -0.02*** assignment (in months) (0.01) (0.01) Entered foster care due to neglect -0.21 -0.04 (0.55) (0.56) Entered foster care due to abandonment 0.85* 0.91** 57 Logit Model 1 (0.47) Number of placements in the previous 0.11* episode (0.06) Having sibling groups -0.30 (0.34) Having any special needs -0.30 (0.51) Participated in caregiver interview ---Constant N= Model Significance p<= Pseudo R2 = Log Likelihood = 2.18** (1.06) 179 0.01 0.10 -111.53 Logit Model 2 (0.46) 0.12** (0.06) -0.25 (0.34) -0.27 (0.50) -0.62 (0.40) 2.13* (1.10) 179 0.01 0.11 -110.25 Significant at .1 level.; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level The reference group is non-African-American children As table 4.4 indicates, three variables were predictive of the probability of exiting from out-of-home care. Having spent more time in out-of-home placement significantly reduces the likelihood of exiting foster care. This is consistent with the exiting body of literature, which has provided voluminous evidence on the negative correlation between length of stay in care and the chances of achieving permanency (Barth et al. 1994; Courtney & Needle, 1997; Finch et al., 1986; Olsen, 1982). Children who were abandoned by their parents had a higher probability of exiting from care, probably because relative caregivers were more motivated to pursue permanency options other than reunification when the parents of the children were not involved. A greater number of placements in the previous episode led to a higher probability of exiting care. This result is somewhat unexpected because the number of placements is likely to be related to physical, behavioral, and emotional problems, which normally reduces the likelihood of achieving permanency. 58 The results indicate that both logit model 1 and logit model 2 are statistically significant. A log-likelihood ratio test showed that adding the variable representing whether the caregiver completed the caregiver interview did not significantly add to the precision of the model (p=0.11). 4.3.2 Sample Balance Table 4.5 presents the baseline characteristics of children in kinship care by observed outcomes and whether the child was in the control or the experimental group. Only one child in each sibling group was randomly chosen for this analysis, regardless of whether the child was eligible for the guardianship subsidy. As table 4.5 indicates, the control and experimental groups were similar in most background characteristics. The only statistical difference concerns the race of the children who did not exit during the life of the GAP study. In the control group, about 5% of the children who remained in out-of-home care were not of African-American decent. None of the non-African-American children in the experimental group remained in foster care. Table 4. 5 Sample Balance without Propensity Score Matching Exited Foster Care Did Not Exit Foster Care Control Experimental Control Experimental Group Group Group Group Gender Female 48.7% 44.4% 56.6% 56.0% Male 51.3% 55.6% 43.4% 44.0% χ2 0.28 0.00 Race African American 93.4% 97.5% 94.7%* 100.0%* Other 6.6% 2.5% 5.3%* 0.0%* 2 χ 1.47 2.72 Education Regular School 63.5% 55.6% 72.1% 78.7% Special Education 0.0% 4.2% 4.4% 6.4% 59 Not in School χ2 Entry reason Neglect Abandonment Abuse Other More than one of the above 2 χ Sibling Groups Yes No χ2 Special needs Yes No 2 χ Caregiver at removal At least one parent present No parent present χ2 Age at entry t Length of stay in care (in months) prior to random assignment t Number of placement changes in the previous episode t N Exited Foster Care Control Experimental Group Group 36.5% 40.3% 3.11 67.7% 10.8% 4.6% 1.5% 15.4% 67.6% 13.5% 8.1% 1.4% 9.5% Did Not Exit Foster Care Control Experimental Group Group 23.5% 14.9% 1.41 70.0% 15.7% 7.1% 1.4% 5.7% 1.87 76.2% 9.5% 4.8% 0.0% 9.5% 2.26 45.9% 54.1% 47.0% 53.0% 0.01 58.2% 41.8% 43.2% 56.8% 2.40 13.1% 86.9% 7.6% 92.4% 1.06 14.9% 85.1% 13.6% 86.4% 0.04 90.2% 80.9% 83.6% 82.1% 9.8% 16.4% 36.26 19.1% 2.21 5.82 -0.06 37.78 49.0 18.0% 0.04 6.60 -0.65 47.9 0.58 -0.40 1.73 0.40 0.19 0.59 76 -0.92 81 76 -0.99 50 5.79 6.11 * Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level African American, Non-Hispanic Other entry reasons include parental incapacity, mental injury due to abuse or neglect, child’s disruptive behavior, and child’s special needs. Other special needs include child physical, mental and behavioral problems. Sample mean Tables 4.6 and 4.7 present sample characteristics by predicted outcome and whether the child was eligible for the guardianship subsidy. The matched sample for 60 Table 4.6 was based on the propensity scores computed from logit model 1 in Table 4.4, where the variable representing participation in the caregiver interview was not included, whereas the matched sample for Table 4.7 was based on the propensity scored computed from logit model 2, where the variable representing participation in caregiver interview was included as a predictor. As Table 4.6 shows, the propensity matching procedure produced a more balanced sample than the unmatched sample. No statistically significant difference with regard to background characteristics was detected between the control and experimental groups, regardless of whether the child would exit out-of-home care in the absence of the subsidy. Table 4. 6 Sample Balance after Propensity Score Matching based on Logit Model 1 Would Exit Would Not Exit Control Experimental Control Experimental Group Group Group Group Gender Female 47.5% 47.2% 51.6% 42.0% Male 52.5% 52.8% 48.4% 58.0% χ2 0.00 1.03 Race African American 91.5% 97.2% 96.9% 100.0% Other 8.5% 2.8% 3.1% 0.0% χ2 1.23 1.59 Education Regular School 64.3% 55.6% 70.5% 60.0% Special Education 0.0% 2.8% 4.9% 6.0% Not in School 35.7% 41.7% 24.6% 34.0% χ2 2.03 1.36 Entry reason Neglect 64.4% 61.1% 68.8% 72.0% Abandonment 11.9% 16.7% 17.2% 10.0% Abuse 5.1% 8.3% 6.3% 8.0% Other 1.7% 0.0% 1.6% 0.0% More than one of the 17.0% 13.9% 6.3% 10.0% above χ2 1.53 2.48 Sibling Groups 61 Yes No χ2 Special needs Yes No χ2 Caregiver at removal At least one parent present No parent present χ2 Age at entry t Length of stay in care (in months) prior to random assignment t Number of placement changes in the previous episode t Number of placement changes in GAP t N Would Exit Control Experimental Group Group 47.5% 52.8% 52.5% 47.2% 0.00 Would Not Exit Control Experimental Group Group 60.9% 50.0% 39.1% 50.0% 1.36 13.6% 86.4% 8.3% 91.7% 0.60 14.1% 85.9% 12.0% 88.0% 0.10 89.3% 85.7% 86.5% 86.8% 10.7% 13.5% 37.89 14.3% 0.26 5.47 -0.22 38.05 53.94 13.2% 0.00 6.04 -0.04 54.22 0.46 -0.03 0.17 0.31 -0.04 0.30 0.46 1.53 0.47 1.02** 0.08 0.38** 5.28 6.02 -0.06 59 36 2.04 64 50 * Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level African American, Non-Hispanic Other entry reasons include parental incapacity, mental injury due to abuse or neglect, child’s disruptive behavior, and child’s special needs. Other special needs include child physical, mental and behavioral problems. Sample mean Matching based on the propensity scores produced by logit model 2 was less successful than that of logit model 1. As Table 4.7 indicates, the experimental group members and the control group members were different in two background characteristics. Among those children who would exit out-of-home care in the absence of the subsidy, the control group had a significantly higher average number of placement changes in the previous episode than those in the experimental group. Additionally, 62 among children who would not exit out-of-home care in the absence of the subsidy, a higher percentage of children in the control group belonged to sibling groups than that of children in the experimental group. This result shows that the propensity score matching method is highly sensitive to unobserved heterogeneity. Models with different sets of predictors may produce vastly different matching results. Table 4. 7 Sample Balance after Propensity Score Matching Based on Logit Model 2 Would Exit Would Not Exit Control Experimental Control Experimental Group Group Group Group Gender Female 47.5% 59.5% 51.6% 44.0% Male 52.5% 40.5% 48.4% 56.0% 2 χ 1.43 0.64 Race African American 91.5% 97.6% 96.9% 100.0% Other 8.5% 2.4% 3.1% 0.0% χ2 1.63 1.59 Education Regular School 64.3% 52.4% 70.5% 64.0% Special Education 0.0% 4.8% 4.9% 6.0% Not in School 35.7% 42.7% 24.6% 30.0% χ2 3.56 0.53 Entry reason Neglect 64.4% 69.1% 68.8% 74.0% Abandonment 11.9% 16.7% 17.2% 8.0% Abuse 5.1% 2.4%% 6.3% 6.0% Other 1.7% 0.0%% 1.6% 0.0% More than one of the 17.0% 11.9% 6.3% 12.0% above χ2 2.07 3.75 Sibling Groups Yes 47.5% 54.8% 60.9%** 36.0%** No 52.5% 45.2% 39.1%** 64.0%** 2 χ 0.52 6.98 Special needs Yes 13.6% 7.1% 14.1% 16.0% No 86.4% 92.9% 85.9% 84.0% χ2 1.04 0.08 Caregiver at removal 63 At least one parent present No parent present Would Exit Control Experimental Group Group 89.3% 81.6% 10.7% 37.89 18.4% 1.13 5.66 -0.47 40.04 0.46** 59 χ2 Age at entry t Length of stay in care (in months) prior to random assignment t Number of placement changes in the previous episode t N 5.28 Would Not Exit Control Experimental Group Group 86.5% 85.7% 13.5% 53.94 14.3% 0.01 6.18 -0.21 48.4 -0.42 0.12** 0.31 0.92 0.32 2.01 42 64 -0.05 50 6.02 * Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level African American, Non-Hispanic Other entry reasons include parental incapacity, mental injury due to abuse or neglect, child’s disruptive behavior, and child’s special needs. Other special needs include child physical, mental and behavioral problems. Sample mean 4.3.3 Placement Stability Compared with logit model one, logit model two, which included the variable representing participation status in the caregiver interviews, did not significantly improve model fit. Additionally, logit model two produced a less balanced sample. As such, matched sample produced by logit model one was used in estimating the impact of the subsidy on placement stability. Number of placement changes between December 2000 (the time of the first FACTS download) and April 2003 (the time of the last available FACTS download) was computed for each child in the sample. Because some children exited from out-of-home care faster than others, those who exited earlier were exposed to lower risks of placement 64 disruption. Exposure-adjusted mean number in placement change, therefore, was computed separately for the experimental group and the control group on a yearly basis 10. Table 4.8 presents the exposure adjusted mean number of placement changes by permanency status. For the group of children who would exit out-of-home care in the absence of the subsidy, members of the control group experienced slightly higher rates of placement change than members of the experimental group. However this difference was not statistically significant. Similar results were found for the group of children who would remain in out-of-home care in the absence of the subsidy. Table 4. 8 Exposure Adjusted Mean Number of Placement Change by Permanency Status Control Group Experimental Group t-statistic Exit out-of-home care 0.70 0.51 0.45 Remain in out-of-home care 0.47 0.34 0.62 * Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. Table 4.9 presents the estimates from Poisson and negative binomial (NB) models on the number of placement changes for children who would exit out-of-home care in the absence of the subsidy. The first two columns of the tables present results from two Poisson models while the last two columns present results of Models 3 and 4 are negative binomial models. A log-likelihood ratio test indicates that the set of covariates in Poisson model two significantly increases model precision over model one (p=0.001). Further, Poisson model two satisfies the equi-dispersion assumption of the Poisson distribution. Poisson model 2, therefore, is the most appropriate model. The availability of the 10 To adjust for exposure, number of change in placement was divided by the number of days that the child was under observation after random assignment. The result was then scaled up to a yearly basis. Because exposure was calculated by subtracting the last day of available information – the exit date if the child exited from care during the study period, and the date of the last download if the child did not exit – exposure would be negative if the child exited after random assignment (May 2000) and the first FACTS download (December 2000). These children (n=42) were excluded from the analyses. 65 guardianship subsidy was not found to have any impact on placement stability for this group of children. Table 4. 9 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would exit from out-of-home placements in the absence of the subsidy Poisson Poisson NB Model NB Model Model 1 Model 2 1 2 Experimental group 0.03 -0.21 -0.02 -0.27 (0.31) (0.36) (0.42) (0.40) Male ----0.29 ----0.27 (0.34) (0.37) Age at entry (in months) ---0.00 ---0.00 (0.00) (0.00) Sibling groups ---0.44 ---0.45 (0.39) (0.41) Neglected ---0.40 ---0.47 (0.66) (0.72) Abandoned ---0.66 ---0.68 (0.48) (0.51) Number of placements in ---0.38** ---0.34* previous episode (0.16) (0.19) Length of stay in care prior ---0.02*** ---0.02*** to random assignment (0.01) (0.01) African-American ----0.69 ----0.74 (0.62) (0.66) Special needs ----0.03 ---0.00 (0.67) (0.70) Constant -6.49**** -7.77**** -6.47**** -7.80**** (0.19) (1.01) (0.25) (1.06) Log likelihood -74.68 -60.82 -68.64 -60.70 N 63 63 63 63 Standard errors are in parentheses. Models one and two are Poisson models. Models three and four are negative binomial models. Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level * Table 4.10 presents the coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial models on exposure adjusted number of placement changes for children who would not exit from out-of-home placements in the absence of the subsidy. For both Poisson models one and two, the equi-dispersion assumption was not satisfied. A negative binomial model, therefore, was appropriate. Further, as a log likelihood ratio test indicated, the set of covariates in NB model 2 did not add to model precision (p=0.28). As such, NB 66 model one was the most appropriate model. As the results of NB model one revealed, the availability of subsidy did not have any statistically significant effects on placement stability for this group of children. Table 4. 10 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would not exit from out-of-home placement in the absence of the subsidy Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Experimental group -0.19 -0.25 -0.17 -0.15 (0.27) (0.28) (0.36) (0.36) Male ---0.22 ---0.23 (0.23) (0.32) Age at entry (in months) ---0.01*** ---0.01** (0.00) (0.00) Sibling groups ---0.48* ---0.59 (0.26) (0.37) Neglected ---0.47 ---0.60 (0.45) (0.65) Abandoned ---0.30 ---0.46 (0.66) (0.47) Number of placement in ----0.27 ----0.21 previous episode (0.19) (0.25) Length of stay in care prior ---0.00 ---0.00 to random assignment (0.00) (0.01) African-American ---0.14 ----0.32 (1.05) (1.28) Special needs ---0.01 ---0.24 (0.33) (0.48) Constant -6.70**** -8.71**** -6.70**** -9.24**** (0.12) (1.28) (0.18) (1.68) Log likelihood -139.42 -128.76 -119.66 -114.23 N 96 96 96 96 Standard errors are in parentheses. Models one and two are Poisson models. Models three and four are negative binomial models. * Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level 4.4 Subgroup Analysis: Guardianship, Reunification, Restricted Foster Care, or Kinship Care The previous section analyzed separately the impact of the availability of the subsidy on placement stability for children who would exit out-of-home care and those who would stay in the absence of the subsidy. The sample was divided into two sub-groups: those 67 who exit care in the absence of the subsidy and those who would remain. However, intra-group heterogeneity might still exit. Specifically, among children who exited outof-home care, those who were returned home might have a different profile from those who exited via guardianship. Among children who remained in care, those who remained in kinship care for which the caregiver continued to receive subsidy at the TANF rate might be different from children whose caregivers eventually became licensed and received the higher foster care board payment. The following section, therefore, discusses the subsidy effects on the following four groups: 1. Those who would remain in kinship care in the absence of the subsidy; 2. Those who would move to restricted foster care in the absence of the subsidy; 3. Those who would exit relative foster care via guardianship in the absence of the subsidy; and 4. Those who would be reunified with their parents in the absence of the subsidy. 4.4.1 Predicting Child Outcomes using a Multinomial Logit Model A multinomial logit model was estimated using data available for the control group members in this sample who were placed in unlicensed formal kinship care at the time of random assignment. The outcome variable was a categorical variable representing the permanency outcomes of these children. Four types of permanency outcomes were examined: exiting via guardianship, exiting via reunification, moving to RFC, and remaining in kinship care. Control group members who exited from out-of-home care through adoption or other reasons, accounting for approximately 7% of the control group, were not included in the model. 68 Table 4.11 presents the coefficients obtained from two multinomial logit models with children who exited via guardianship as the reference group. Model one included such explanatory variables as child gender, age at entry, length of stay, abandoned by birth parents, number of placements in previous episode, and whether the child belonged to a sibling group. Child race was excluded because over 95% of the children in the sample were African-American. The lack of variation does not allow the effects of race to be estimated. Further, preliminary testing indicated that the lack of variation regarding whether the child was placed in out-of-home care due to neglect makes it impossible to compute standard errors of the estimates of the parameter. This variable, therefore, was not included in the model. The presence of any special needs was excluded from the models for the same reason. Model 2 added a variable indicating whether the caregiver completed a caregiver interview. Coefficients were estimated using the statistical software STATA versions 8 and 9. The Huber/White robust estimator of variance was used to compute the standard errors of the coefficient estimates to adjust for the clustering of children cared for by the same caregiver. Table 4. 11 Coefficients from the multinomial logit models on permanency outcomes Multinomial Multinomial Logit Logit Model 1 Model 2 Reunification Male -0.42 -0.61 Age at entry (in months) 0.01* 0.01* Length of stay in care prior to -0.02 -0.01 random assignment (in months) Abandoned by parents 0.34 0.63 Number of placements in the -0.05 0.34 previous episode Having sibling groups 0.48 0.82 Participated in caregiver interview ----43.11*** Constant -1.07 -1.58 Moved to RFC 69 Male Age at entry (in months) Length of time in care prior to random assignment Abandoned by parents Number of placements in the previous episode Having sibling groups Participated in caregiver interview Constant Remained in Kinship Care Male Age at entry (in months) Length of time in care prior to random assignment Abandoned by parents Number of placements in the previous episode Having sibling groups Participated in caregiver interview Constant N= Model Significance p<= Pseudo R2 = Log Likelihood = * -0.25 0.01* 0.02** -0.25 0.01* 0.02** -0.44 -0.13 -0.44 -0.11* 0.09 ----1.44* 0.10 0.04 -1.49* 0.14 0.01* 0.02* 0.16 0.01 0.02 -1.42** -0.25 -1.45** -0.25 0.59 ----1.52* 164 0.02 0.08 -202.85 0.50* 0.71 -1.73** 164 0.001 0.12 -193.77 The reference group is “exited to guardianship” Significant at .01 level.; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level As shown in Table 4.11, among the child demographic characteristics, gender did not have any effect on permanency outcomes. Age at entry, on the other hand, had a marginal effect on permanency outcome. Children who exited from out-of-home care via guardianship tended to be younger when they entered into care than children who were returned home, moved to RFC, or remained in kinship care. For example, everything else equal, one month older in the age at entry decreased the odds of exiting through guardianship rather than remaining in kinship care by approximately 1 percentage point 11. The effects of age at entry on exiting through guardianship versus reunification or moving to RFC were similar. One month older in age at entry decreased the odds of 11 e0.01=1.01. Unit change in odds in the following paragraphs were computed in similar ways. 70 exiting through guardianship versus reunification and moving to RFC by 1 percentage point. Among the case history variables, length of stay in care prior to random assignment was the strongest predictor of permanency outcomes. Longer length of stay was associated with decreased chance of achieving permanency. In model one, when a child stayed in care for one more month prior to random assignment, his/her odds of exiting through guardianship versus moving to RFC and remaining in kinship care reduced by approximately 1 percentage point and 2 percentage points, respectively. Furthermore, the odds of reunification versus moving to RFC and remaining in kinship care reduced by 2 percentage points and 3 percentage points, respectively, with every one month increase in the length of stay in care.12 With the exception of the odds of achieving guardianship versus remaining in kinship care, the same results were found in model 2 after controlling for whether the caregiver completed the caregiver interview, although the magnitude of coefficients were slightly smaller. Having entered out-of-home care because of abandonment was found to be predictive of exiting through guardianship or reunification versus remaining in kinship care. Children who entered out-of-home care because of abandonment were almost five times less likely than other children to be reunified versus remaining in kinship care according to model one and seven times less likely according to model two. Additionally, abandoned children’s odds of exiting through guardianship versus remaining in kinship care were more than four times as likely as other children in both models one and two. 12 The effects of any variable on the logit of any two outcomes can be calculated by comparing the coefficients in Table 4.4. The standard errors can be calculated through the variance-covariance matrix. See Long (1997) for more details. The variance-covariance matrices of the two models are included in the appendix. 71 This may be explained by the possibility that the caregiver is more willing to pursue guardianship when they believe that the child’s chance of returning home is slim, as with children who were abandoned by their parents. There was some evidence showing that the number of placements in the previous episode was related to reunification. When participation in caregiver interview was controlled, one more placement during the previous episode led to an increase in the odds of reunification versus moving to RFC or remaining in kinship care by 79 percentage points and 56 percentage points, respectively. Having a sibling group was not found to be predictive of permanency outcomes in either multinomial logit model one or model two. Participation in the caregiver interview was predictive of reunification versus any other permanency outcome. Children in the care of the caregivers who completed the surveys were substantially less likely to be returned home. This may be due to the fact that caregiver interview was conducted well after the inception of the project, and the research team generally did not interview caregivers who did not care for the participating children anymore. A proportion of the children may have already returned home when the caregiver interviews started. Additionally, caregivers who expected that the child would return home might be less willing to participate in the interviews then caregivers who expected that the child would stay in their care for the foreseeable future. Overall, model two with the variable representing participation in the caregiver interview significantly improved model fit. A log likelihood ratio test indicated that the null-hypothesis that the two models were equivalent can be rejected (χ2 = 18.16, p<0.001). As such, multinomial logit model two is the more appropriate model predicting child outcomes. 72 4.4.2 Propensity Score Matching Children in the experimental group were matched to the children in the control group based on predicted probabilities obtained through the multinomial logit models discussed in the previous section. Because the variable representing caregiver participation in the caregiver interviews significantly improved model fit, this section will present the results based on multinomial logit model two. Results based on model one are briefly discussed in section 4.5. To address the interdependency issue of children from the same sibling group, one member of each sibling group in the control group was randomly selected. Only selected control group members were entered into the propensity score matching process. This led to significant attrition of the sample, with approximately 32 percent of the control group members (n=71) dropping out of the sample. Another 33 children in the control group were not included in propensity score matching due to missing data. Further, a total of 14 children in the control group exited from care for reasons other than guardianship or reunification13. Among these 14 children, two were siblings or had missing data and therefore were already excluded. The remaining 12 children were further excluded from the analyses. As such, a total of 107 control group members remained in the sample. Among these children, 33 exited through guardianship, 14 exited through reunification, 27 moved to RFC, and 33 remained in kinship care at the end of the study. For the experimental group, propensity scores were not able to be computed for a total of 43 children due to missing data. The pool of experimental children eligible for 13 These reasons included adoption, independence, ran away, agency custody not appropriate, and other reasons. 73 matching contained 158 children, of which 78 children were matched to 107 children in the control group based on their propensity scores. Among these 78 children, 11 children were matched to control cases in two subgroups, and 2 were matched to control cases in three subgroups. Tables 4.12 and 4.13 compare the balance of matched and unmatched samples. Table 4.12 presents the comparison of the control group and the experimental group by actual permanency status. For both the control and experimental groups, only one member of a sibling group was randomly selected and included in the table. Table 4.13 presents the comparison of background characteristics between the control group members and the matched experimental group. 74 Table 4. 12 Sample balance without propensity score matching Exited through Exited through Guardianship Reunification Control Experimental Control Experimental Group Group Group Group Gender Female 44.4% 47.8% 44.4% 62.5% Male 55.6% 52.2% 55.6% 37.5% 2 χ 0.11 0.07 Race African American 91.1% 98.5% 100.0% 87.5% Other 8.9% 1.5% 0.0% 12.5% * 2 3.31 χ 2.34 Education Regular School 62.2% 56.7% 71.4% 80.0% Special Education 0.0% 5.0% 0.0% 0.0% Not in School 37.8% 38.3% 28.6% 20.0% χ2 1.97 0.14 Entry reason Neglect 68.4% 65.6% 60.0% 66.7% Abandonment 15.8% 14.8% 0.0% 0.0% Abuse 7.9% 8.2% 6.7% 16.7% Other 0.0% 1.6% 0.0% 0.0% More than one of the 7.9% 9.8% 33.3% 16.7% above χ2 0.77 0.90 Sibling Group Yes 48.6% 46.4% 57.1% 60.0% No 51.4% 53.6% 42.9% 40.0% χ2 0.04 0.01 Special needs Moved to Restricted Remained in Kinship Foster Care Care Control Experimental Control Experimental Group Group Group Group 59.4% 40.6% 48.2% 51.9% 0.74 51.2% 48.8% 47.8% 52.2% 0.07 96.8% 3.2% 100.0% 0.0% 0.88 95.4% 4.7% 100.0% 0.0% 1.10 65.5% 0.0% 34.5% 88.0% 12.0% 0.0% 12.99*** 72.2% 8.3% 19.4% 63.6% 0.0% 36.4% 3.49 58.6% 27.6% 6.9% 3.5% 3.5% 59.1% 18.2% 9.1% 0.0% 13.6% 79.0% 5.3% 7.9% 0.0% 7.9% 95.2% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 4.8% 2.96 50.0% 50.0% 31.8% 68.2% 1.67 3.38 63.9% 36.1% 59.1% 40.9% 0.13 75 Yes No χ2 Caregiver at removal At least one parent Relatives or friends 2 χ Age at entry t Length of stay in care (in months) prior to random assignment t Number of placement changes in the previous episode t N Exited through Guardianship Control Experimental Group Group 11.4% 7.14% 88.6% 92.9% 0.43 94.3% 5.7% 37.1 81.0% 19.0% 3.18* 6.3 -1.45 36.8 1.3 0.07 2.0 45 -0.37 67 5.1 Exited through Reunification Control Experimental Group Group 7.1% 40.0% 92.9% 60.0% 2.99* 93.3% 6.7% 31.9 83.3% 16.7% 0.50 7.3 -0.36 41.8 0.7 -1.08 0.4 18 0.71 8 6.7 Moved to Restricted Remained in Kinship Foster Care Care Control Experimental Control Experimental Group Group Group Group 7.1% 13.6% 19.4% 13.6% 92.9% 86.4% 80.6% 86.4% 0.57 0.32 84.0% 16.0% 45.7 73.9% 26.1% 0.74 7.6 -1.51 40.9 0.5 0.64 0.6 32 0.31 27 6.0 77.1% 22.9% 48.5 100.0% 0.0% 4.59** 5.8 -0.01 50.6 0.5 -0.32 0.6 43 -0.55 23 5.8 African American, Non-Hispanic. Other entry reasons include parental incapacity, mental injury due to abuse or neglect, child’s disruptive behavior, and child’s special needs. Other special needs include child physical, mental, and behavioral problems. Sample mean. * Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level. 76 Table 4. 13 Sample Balance after Propensity Score Matching (Based on Multinomial Logit Model 2) Exit through Guardianship Exit through Move to Restricted Remain in Kinship Reunification Foster Care Care Control Experimental Control Experimental Control Experimental Control Experimental Group Group Group Group Group Group Group Group Gender Female 39.4% 55.6% 42.9% 45.5% 51.9% 68.2% 45.5% 38.7% Male 60.6% 44.4% 30.8% 27.3% 48.2% 31.8% 54.6% 61.3% 2 χ 1.56 0.02 1.34 0.30 Race African American 87.9% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 92.6% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% Other 12.1% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 7.4% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 2 χ 3.51* N/A 1.70 N/A Education Regular School 64.5% 51.9% 69.2% 72.7% 61.5% 63.6% 71.0% 71.0% Special Education 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 4.6% 9.7% 0.0% Not in School 35.5% 48.2% 30.8% 27.3% 38.5% 31.8% 19.4% 29.0% χ2 0.95 0.04 1.34 3.60 Entry reason Neglect 63.6% 85.2% 57.1% 45.5% 55.6% 63.6% 78.8% 80.7% Abandonment 18.2% 11.1% 0.0% 27.3% 29.6% 22.7% 6.1% 12.9% Abuse 9.1% 0.0% 7.1% 18.2% 7.4% 4.6% 6.1% 3.2% Other 0.0% 3.7% 0.0% 0.0% 3.7% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% More than one of the 9.1% 0.0% 35.7% 9.1% 3.7% 9.1% 9.1% 3.2% above χ2 7.57 6.42* 1.90 1.96 Sibling Groups Yes 51.5% 44.4% 57.1% 45.5% 51.6% 63.6% 66.7% 51.6% No 48.5% 55.6% 42.9% 54.6% 48.2% 36.4% 33.3% 48.4% χ2 0.30 0.34 0.69 1.50 Special needs 77 Exit through Guardianship Yes No χ2 Caregiver at removal At least one parent Relatives or friends 2 χ Age at entry t Length of stay in care (in months) prior to random assignment t Number of placement changes in the previous episode t N Control Experimental Group Group 12.1% 3.7% 87.9% 96.3% 1.38 93.6% 6.5% 38.3 84.0% 16.0% 1.32 4.8 0.05 38.7 1.4 -0.06 0.4 33 0.90 27 4.8 Exit through Reunification Control Experimental Group Group 7.1% 0.0% 92.9% 100.0% 0.82 93.3% 6.7% 33.7 60.0% 40.0% 4.17** 8.0 -1.12 39.7 0.8 -0.79 0.6 14 0.29 11 6.1 Move to Restricted Remain in Kinship Foster Care Care Control Experimental Control Experimental Group Group Group Group 7.41% 4.6% 18.2% 6.5% 92.6% 96.5% 81.8% 93.6% 0.17 2.01 86.4% 13.6% 50.3 79.0% 21.1% 0.40 7.2 -1.07 48.9 0.4 0.17 0.3 27 0.45 22 5.9 82.1% 17.9% 51.9 84.6% 15.4% 0.06 6.0 -0.25 50.5 0.2 0.21 0.3 33 -0.25 31 5.8 African American, Non-Hispanic Other entry reasons include parental incapacity, mental injury due to abuse or neglect, child’s disruptive behavior, and child’s special needs. Other special needs include child physical, mental, and behavioral problems. Sample mean. * Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. 78 As Table 4.12 indicates, experimental group members who exited through guardianship were slightly more likely to be African American than their peers in the control group. Additionally, children in the control and experimental groups also differed with regard to their caregivers at the time of removal. Members of the experimental group who exited to gurdianship were less likely to be removed from their parents when compared with children in the control group. Members of the experimental group who remained in kinship care, conversely, were more likely to be removed from relatives and friends than their peers in the control groups. Table 4.12 also shows that the experimental group members who moved to RFC were significantly more likely to be in special education and less likely to be out of school than members of the control group in the same permanency category. This is a significant difference which may potentially bias the results of comparative analyses if comparison is made between the experimental and the control groups based on observed outcome for this group of children. Propensity score marginally improved sample balance. As Table 4.13 shows, the difference between the experimental and control groups with regard to education status was eliminated through propensity score matching. However, the racial difference remained for those who exited through guardianship remained. Additionally, the control and experimental groups who exited through guardianship and remained in kinship care no longer differed with respect to caregiver at removal. However, such difference was found with children who would exit through reunification, with the control group members more likely to be removed from the parents than similar experimental group members. Furthermore, when identifying children in the experimental group who would be reunified with their parents in the absence of the guardianship subsidy, the model 79 failed to match children with respect to entry reasons. Children in the control group were more likely to have entered out-of-home care because of neglect, and less likely to have entered because of abandonment or abuse. This difference was marginally significant. As such, it may be necessary to control for entry reasons when placement stability was examined for this group of children. The drawback of the propensity score matching procedure is that it has resulted in considerable sample attrition. For example, the guardianship subgroup dropped from 112 without matching to 60 with matching. This is because the matching procedure with replacement entails that one child in the experimental group may be matched to more than one control group member. Furthermore, propensity scores have to be computed based on background characteristics, some of which may not be available for all children. 4.4.3 Placement Stability Table 4.14 presents the mean yearly change in placement for the control group members randomly selected from their sibling groups and the matched experimental group members14. Children who would be reunified with their biological parents on average had the highest rate of change in placement. Those who would exit through guardianship or remain in kinship care, on the other hand, had relatively low numbers of change in placement. For none of the four groups was there any statistically significant difference between the control and the experimental group. 14 As discussed previously, because exposure was calculated by subtracting the last day of available information – the exit date if the child exited from care during the study period, and the date of the last download if the child did not exit – exposure would be negative if the child exited after random assignment (May 2000) and the first FACTS download (December 2000). These children (n=36) were excluded from the analyses. 80 Table 4. 14 Exposure-adjusted mean change in placements per year by predicted outcomes and eligibility for the guardianship subsidy Control Group Experimental Group t-statistic Exit via guardianship 0.22 0.30 -0.50 Exit via reunification 1.72 0.60 1.06 Move to RFC 0.70 0.38 0.91 Remain in kinship care 0.24 0.29 -.038 Exposure-adjusted numbers of change in placement were subsequently examined in the framework of Poisson models or, in the case that the equidispersion assumption of the Poisson distribution was not satisfied, negative binomial models. Separate models with different sets of explanatory variables were estimated. The first model was estimated with eligibility for the subsidy as the only independent variable. A set of covariates were included sequentially in the second model. These covariates were the following: child race, age at entry, entry reasons, length of stay in care, number of placements in the previous episode, and whether had a sibling group, and the presence of any special needs. Because the propensity score matching procedure used all of these variables as predictors, adding the covariate should not significantly change the direction or the size of the coefficient for the main dependent variable: the eligibility for the subsidy. It may, however, reduce the variance of the estimate if the covariates improve the precision of the model significantly. Table 4.15 presents the coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial (NB) models on exposure adjusted number of placement changes for children who would exit through guardianship in the absence of the subsidy. The first two columns present results from the two Poission models and the last two columns present the results from the NB models. None of the explanatory variables in Poisson model two were statistically significant. As expected, a log likelihood ratio test indicate that the set of 81 covariates did not contribute significantly to the model (χ2 = 6.88, p = 0.44). Further, a comparison of Poisson model one and NB model one indicated that the equidispersion assumption was not satisfied, and that NB model one was more appropriate. NB model one was also found to be more appropriate than NB model two (χ2 = 3.71, p = 0.81). In no model was eligibility for the subsidy or any of the covariates found to have any significant effect on placement stability. This is consistent with our hypothesis that the subsidy would have little effect on those children who exit via guardianship even in the absence of the subsidy. Table 4. 15 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would exit through guardianship in the absence of the subsidy Poisson Poisson NB Model NB Model Model 1 Model 2 1 2 Experimental Group 0.10 -0.07 0.14 -0.06 (0.45) (0.54) (0.55) (0.56) Male ---0.05 ---0.05 (0.56) (0.58) Age at entry (in months) ---0.01 ---0.01 (0.01) (0.01) Sibling Groups ---0.35 ---0.34 (0.59) (0.61) Neglect ---1.06 ---1.07 (1.35) (1.37) Abandonment ----0.09 ----0.03 (1.03) (1.07) Number of placement in ----0.32 ----0.30 previous episode (0.39) (0.41) Length of stay in care prior ----0.00 ----0.00 to random assignment (0.01) (0.01) Participated in caregiver ---0.05 ---0.63 interview (0.56) (0.65) Constant -6.98**** -8.35**** -7.03**** -8.33**** (0.33) (1.52) (0.40) (1.55) Log likelihood -35.25 -31.08 -33.67 -31.07 N 43 43 43 43 Standard * errors are in parentheses. Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. 82 Table 4.16 presents the coefficients obtained from the Poisson and negative binomial regression models on number of placement changes for children who would exit through reunification in the absence of the subsidy. Results from the Poisson regression models and the negative binomial regression models were almost identical. The only covariate that appeared to have a significant effect was sibling group. Those having sibling groups had an average number of placement changes more than six times higher than those without a sibling group. This difference was marginally significant (p=0.08). It may also reflect the difficulties faced by the child welfare agency in its efforts to place sibling groups together. Although sibling group affected a child’s chance of having a stable placement, the set of covariates as a whole did not. After the specification tests, it was determined that Poisson model one, with only the variable representing the eligibility of the guardianship subsidy as the independent variable, was the most appropriate model. Being eligible was not found to be related to the number of placement changes that children may have, if they would be reunified with their parents even in the absence of the subsidy. Table 4. 16 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placements for children who would exit through reunification in the absence of the subsidy Poisson Poisson NB Model NB Model Model 1 Model 2 1 2 Experimental Group -0.51 0.54 -0.51 0.54 (0.47) (1.23) (0.47) (1.23) Male ---0.45 ---0.45 (0.85) (0.85) Age at entry (in months) ----0.00 ----0.00 (0.01) (0.01) Sibling Groups ---2.03* ---2.03* (1.17) (1.17) Neglect ----0.80 ----0.80 (1.42) (1.42) Abandonment ----0.80 ----0.80 83 Poisson Model 1 Number of placement in previous episode Length of stay in care prior to random assignment Participating in caregiver interview Constant Log likelihood N NB Model 1 ---- Poisson Model 2 (1.06) 0.12 (0.27) -0.01 (0.03) ---- ---- NB Model 2 (1.06) 0.12 (0.27) -0.01 (0.03) ---- -5.98**** (0.35) -22.67 19 -6.51**** (1.40) -19.74 19 -5.98**** (0.35) -22.67 19 -6.51**** (1.40) -31.51 19 ------- ------- Standard errors are in parentheses. “Participation in caregiver interview” dropped out of the model because none of the caregivers of children in this group participated in caregiver interview. * Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. Table 4.17 presents the results obtained through Poisson and negative binomial regressions for children who would move to RFC in the absence of the guardianship subsidy. Comparison of the Poisson models with the NB models indicated that the equidispersion assumption was not satisfied in Poisson, and NB models should be used instead. Further, the set of covariates in NB model two did not contribute to model precision. As such, NB model one with the eligibility of the subsidy as the sole explanatory variable (model three) was the most appropriate model. As NB model one indicates, being eligible for the guardianship subsidy did not affect placement stability for children who would moved to RFC in the absence of the subsidy. None of the covariates had any effect on placement stability for this group of children. Table 4. 17 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placements for children who would move to Restricted Foster Care in the absence of the subsidy Poisson Poisson NB Model NB Model Model 1 Model 2 1 2 Experimental Group -0.46 -0.83** -0.46 -0.56 (0.34) (0.37) (0.50) (0.49) Male ---0.08 ----0.11 (0.29) (0.44) Age at entry (in months) ---0.01*** ---0.01* 84 Poisson Model 1 Sibling Groups ---- Neglect ---- Abandonment ---- Number of placement in previous episode Length of stay in care prior to random assignment Participating in caregiver interview Constant ---- Log likelihood N -------6.26**** (0.15) -79.22 42 Poisson Model 2 (0.00) 0.51* (0.32) 0.30 (0.51) -0.27 (0.43) -0.38 (0.24) -0.00 (0.01) 0.09 (0.36) -7.51**** (0.99) -67.35 42 NB Model 1 -------------------6.26**** (0.26) -62.86 42 NB Model 2 (0.01) 0.20 (0.47) 0.22 (0.75) -0.02 (0.61) -0.31 (0.32) -0.00 (0.01) 0.30 (0.52) -7.30**** (1.35) -59.13 42 * Standard errors are in parentheses. Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. Table 4.18 presents the results of various models for children who would remain in kinship care in the absence of the subsidy. Using the specification tests discussed previously, model three, the NB model one with the eligibility as the sole independent variable, was found to be the most appropriate model. No statistically significant effect of the eligibility for the subsidy was found, nor was any covariate found to have any effect on placement stability. Table 4. 18 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placements for children who would remain in kinship care in the absence of the subsidy Poisson Poisson NB Model NB Model Model 1 Model 2 1 2 Experimental Group 0.32** 0.32** 0.31 0.35 (0.39) (0.43) (0.49) (0.52) Male ---0.47 ---0.44 (0.46) (0.56) Age at entry (in months) ----0.00 ----0.00 (0.01) (0.01) 85 Sibling Groups Poisson Model 1 ---- Neglect ---- Abandonment ---- Number of placement in previous episode Length of stay in care prior to random assignment Participating in caregiver interview Constant ---- Log likelihood N -------7.32** (0.24) -52.87 52 Poisson Model 2 0.40 (0.44) -0.69 (0.66) -0.58 (0.82) -0.44 (0.48) -0.01 (0.01) 0.42 (0.46) -6.72**** (1.12) -49.54 52 NB Model 1 -------------------7.32**** (0.29) -50.16 52 NB Model 2 0.42 (0.54) -0.61 (0.82) -0.60 (0.96) -0.41 (0.51) -0.01 (0.01) 0.44 (0.56) -6.85*** (1.34) -47.98 52 * Standard errors are in parentheses. Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. 4.4.4 Estimates of the Effects of the Eligibility of the Guardianship Subsidy Based on Multinomial Logit Model 1 Estimates of the effects of guardianship subsidy on placement stability were also obtained with a experimental group selected based on the multinomial logit model where participation in caregiver interview was not considered (multinomial logit model one). Results were similar to those based on multinomial logit model two with one exception. For the subgroup of children who would move to restricted foster care in the absence of the subsidy, members of the experimental group experienced placement disruption 75% less frequently than their peers in the control group15. However, because multinomial logit model one was considered less accurate than multinomial logit model two, the results based on the former model may be less reliable than those discussed in the previous section. 15 Coefficient=-1.38; Standard error=0.54. Percent increase is computed as follows: e -1..38 *100%=25%. See the appendix for table of coefficients and standard errors. 86 4.5 Sensitivity Analysis – African-American Children Only Because the overwhelming majority of the sample was African-American children, the effect of race was not able to be assessed and controlled for due to the lack of variation. However, existing literature has repeatedly pointed out that AfricanAmerican children are less likely than Caucasian children to achieve permanency in a timely manner (Barth et al., 1994; Courtney & Needell, 1997; Landsverk et al., 1996). The sensitivity analysis attempted to address the effects of race by excluding nonAfrican-American children from the analyses. Only African-American children were included in the propensity score matching process and the estimation of the effects of the subsidy. Propensity scores were computed separately based on multinomial logit models without and with the variable representing whether the caregiver participated in the caregiver interview (multinomial logit model one and multinomial logit model two respectively) . A log likelihood ratio test indicated that the multinomial logit model with the caregiver interview participation was superior in predicting permanency status. Moreover, the experimental group members selected based on the results of multinomial logit two were more similar to those selected based on multinomial logit model one. Tables of coefficients obtained from the multinomial logit models and sample balance are included in the Appendix. Poisson models and negative binomial models were estimated to examine the effects of the eligibility of the subsidy on placement stability. Results are presented in the appendix. When propensity score matching were based on the multinomial logit model with the variable representing caregiver participation in the interview as a predictor, the results of the analysis with African-American children did not differ from that with the overall sample. The availability for the subsidy did not have any significant 87 effects on placement stability for any of the four subgroups. When members of the experimental group were matched to the control group based on the multinomial logit model without the caregiver interview information, however, one major difference emerged. The results indicated that among children who would exit through guardianship in the absence of the subsidy, those who were eligible for the guardianship subsidy experienced placement disruption 316% as frequently as those who were not eligible for the subsidy (p=0.04)16. This result was somewhat unexpected. Although one would not anticipate the availability of the subsidy to increase placement stability for this group of children, it was not expected to hurt them. One explanation is that the availability of the subsidy may have selectively expedited some caregiver’s decision to assume guardianship. These caregivers were more likely than not to have been in stable relationship with the children in their care. As a result, more children in the experimental group who had been in stable placements may have exited before the first FACTS download were retrieved in December 2000 and, therefore, were excluded from the analyses. As the data indicated, 36% (n=8) of the experimental group members exited before December 2000, compared to 26% (n=7) of the control group members. However, the difference in early exiting appears to be marginal, and not likely to explain all the differences in terms of placement changes between members of the control and experimental groups given the size of the coefficient. Overall, as previously discussed, the multinomial logit model which included caregiver interview participation as a predictor explained more variation in permanency 16 Coefficient=1.15; Standard error=0.56. Percentage increase is computed as follows: e 1.15 *100%=316%. See the appendix for table of coefficients and standard errors. 88 status than the model without caregiver interview information, and, therefore, rendered more balanced samples of the control and experimental groups. The relationship between the availability of the subsidy and placement stability estimated based on the latter model, as such, may very well be spurious. The inconsistency in the results based on the two slightly different models, however, indicate that the propensity score matching method may be very sensitive to model specification and the existence of unobserved heterogeneity. 89 Chapter 5. Discussions and Policy Implications This dissertation examined whether the availability of a guardianship subsidy has differential effects on different groups of children initially placed with unlicensed kinship caregivers. The sample was divided in two groups: children who would exit out-of-home care and children who would remain in care in the absence of the subsidy. Further, a four-group analysis was conducted, which examined the differential effects of the availability of the subsidy on children who would: 1. remain in kinship care in the absence of the subsidy; 2. move to restricted foster care in the absence of the subsidy; 3. exit out-of-home care via guardianship in the absence of the subsidy; and 4. be reunified with their parents in the absence of the subsidy. Propensity score matching methods were used to identify the subgroups, and Poisson and negative binomial models were estimated to determine the differential effects of the availability of the guardianship subsidy on placement stability for the four subgroups of children. Subgroups identified through the propensity score matching procedure marginally improved sample balance when compared with subgroups identified based on observed permanency status at the end of the GAP project. The matched sample in the two group analysis eliminated differences between the experimental and control groups in race. In the four-group analysis, propensity score matching eliminated differences in the percentage of children in special education, and reduced the differences between the experimental and control groups with respect to the caregiver at removal. These two factors were likely to be correlated with the probability of achieving permanency within a 90 short timeframe. However, racial differences between the control and experimental groups remained, and propensity scores failed to match the experimental group members and the control groups with respect to entry reason. Sensitivity analyses were conducted with different sets of predictors and with the African Americans only sub-samples. When analyses of placement stability were based on the predictive models that produced the more balanced samples, no subsidy effects on placement stability was found for any subgroup. However, alternative predictive models produced considerably different samples and results. More specifically, when the variable presenting whether the caregiver participated in the caregiver interview was omitted from the multinomial logit models, members of the experimental group who would move to restricted foster care were found to have experienced placement disruptions 75% less frequently than their peers in the control group. Further, for AfricanAmerican children who would exit through guardianship in the absence of the subsidy, members of the experimental group experienced placement disruption 316% as frequently as their peers in the control group. This indicates that although propensity score matching has the potential to improve sample balance and reduce bias, estimates obtained through propensity score matching is sensitive to the selection of covariates in the predictive model, and the existence of unobserved heterogeneity. This is consistent with previous studies using the propensity score methods, which found that the accuracy of propensity score match varies with regard to the observables selected (Dehejia & Wahba, 1999). This dissertation revealed that, while the availability of the subsidy did not enhance placement stability for children in kinship care, it did not harm any subgroup of 91 children, regardless of their potential permanency outcomes in the absence of the subsidy. Additionally, previous evaluations indicated that the subsidy accelerated children’s pace leaving out-of-home care (Mandell, 2001; UMSSW, 2003). As such, one may conclude that both the kinship care population and the society as a whole will benefit from a universal subsidized guardianship for children in kinship care. The current study did not find any subsidy effects on placement stability. However, as discussed previously, propensity score matching is susceptible to sample selection bias if insufficient amount of information exists to build the prediction model. As demonstrated by this study, propensity score matching may be especially fragile given a small sample size. To ascertain the findings of this study, it is desirable that the evaluations of similar subsidized guardianship programs in other states include analyses examining the subsidy effects on placements for similar subgroups, especially in places where the out-of-care population is over-represented by African-American children, and where the percentage of kinship caregivers who become licensed foster caregivers is historically high. As one considers the results of this study, several limitations must be noted. First, an ideal study would follow an incoming cohort of children from entry into foster care to exit. Due to the design of the GAP project, however, this study included a cohort of children who had been in foster care for varied periods of time. As such, children who spent long periods of time in foster care were over-represented in the sample. Second, the children in this study were randomly assigned to the experimental and control groups in May 2000. However, no information regarding their placement changes was available until December 2000. Their placement experience during these seven months is unknown. 92 Finally, regarding the measure of placement stability, the data did not distinguish desirable placement changes from undesirable placement changes. More nuanced data is needed to assess the extent to which the guardianship subsidy affected children’s abilities to stay in stable and family-like environments during their stay in out-of-home placements. 93 References Barber, J. G., Delfabbro, P. H. & Cooper, L.L. (2001). The predictors of unsuccessful transition to foster care. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 42(6), 785-791. Barnard, J., Frangakis, C., Hill, J. & Rubin, D. B. (2001). School choice in NY City: A Bayesian Analysis of an imperfect randomized experiment. In C. Gatsonis, R. E. Kass, B. Carlin, A. Carriquiry, A. Gelman et al. (Eds.) Case Studies in Bayesian Statistics Vol. V. New York: Springer-Verlag New York, Inc. Barth, R. P., Courtney, M., Berrick, J. D. & Altert, V. (1994). From child abuse to permanency planning. New York: Aldine De Bruyter. Barth, R. P., Courtney, M. & Berry, M. (1994). Timing is everything: An analysis of the time to adoption and legalization. Social Work Research, 18(3), 139-148. Barth, R. P. & Blackwell, D. L. (1998). Death rates among California’s foster care and former foster care populations. Children and Youth Services Review, 20(7), 577604. Beeman, S., Kim, H., & Bullerdick, S. (2000). Factors affecting placement of children in kinship and nonkinship foster care. Children and Youth Services Review, 22(1), 36-54. Berrick, J.D. (1997). Assessing quality of care in kinship and foster family care. Family Relations, 46(3), 273-280. Berrick, J. D. & Needell, B. (1999). Recent trends in kinship care: Public policy, payments, and outcomes for children. In P. A. Curtis, G. Dale, Jr. & J. C. Kendall (eds.), The foster care crisis: Translating research into policy and practice. Lincoln: Nebraska Press. Berrick, J. D., Barth, R. P. & Needell, B. (1994). A comparison of kinship foster homes and foster family homes: Implications for kinship foster care as family preservation. Children and Youth Services Review, 16(1/2), 33-63. Berrick, J. D., Needell, B. & Barth, R. P. (1999). Kin as a family and child welfare resource: The child welfare worker’s perspective. In R. L. Hegar & M. Scannapieco (Eds.), Kinship foster care: Policy, practice and research. New York: Oxford University Press. Bloom, H. S., Michalopoulos, C. M., Hill C. J. & Lei, Y. (2002). Can nonexperimental comparison group methods match the findings from a random assignment 94 evaluation of mandatory welfare-to-work programs? [Online]. Available: www.mdrc.org [Retrieved: 2003, November, 26]. Bonecutter, F. J. & Gleeson, J. P. (1997). Broadening our view: Lessons from kinship foster care. In G. Anderson, A. S. Ryan & B. R. Leashore (Eds.), The challenge of permanency planning in a multicultural society. New York: Haworth Press. Brooks, D., & Barth, R. P. (1998). Characteristics and outcomes of drug-exposed and non drug-exposed children in kinship and non-relative foster care. Children and Youth Services, 20(6), 475-501. Buehler, C., Orme, J. G. & Post, J. (2000). The long-term correlates of family foster care. Children and Youth Services Review, 22(8), 595-625. Chipungu, S. P. (1991). A value-based policy framework. In J. E. Everett, S. P. Chipungu & B. R. Leashore (Eds.), Child Welfare: An Afrocentric Perspective. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press. Chipungu, S. P. (2004). The impact of child welfare policies on African American families: A decade later. In J. Everett, S. Chipungu & B. Leashore (Eds.), Child welfare revisited: An Africentric perspective. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press. Chipungu, S. P., Everett, J. E. & Verduik, M. J. (1998). Children placed in foster care with relatives: A multi-state study. Washington D. C.: Department of Health and Human Services. Christian, S & Ekman [Ekerman in text], L. (2000). A place to call home: Adoption and guardianship for children in foster care. [Online]. Available: http://www.ncsl.org/programs/pubs/bkfstrs.htm [Retrieved: 2003, August 5] Cooper-Patrick, L, Gallo, J. J., Powe, N. R., Steinwachs, D. M., Eaton, W. W. & Ford, D. E. (1999). Mental health service utilization by African Americans and whites: The Baltimore epidemiologic Catchment area follow-up. Medical Care, 37(10), 1034-1045. Courtney, M. E. (1999). Foster care and the costs of welfare reform. In P. A. Curtis, G. Dale, Jr. & J. C. Kendall (Eds.), The foster care crisis: Translating research into policy and practice. Lincoln: Nebraska Press. Courtney, M. & Needell, B. (1997). Outcome of kinship care: Lessons from California. In R. Barth, J. D. Berrick & N. Gilbert (Eds.), Child Welfare Research Review: Vol 2. New York: Columbia University Press. 95 Danzy, J. & Jackson, S. M. (1997). Family preservation and support services: A missed opportunity for kinship care. S. Jackson & S. Brissett-Chapman (Eds.), Serving African American children. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers. Dehejia, R. H. & Wahba, S. (1999). Causal effects in nonexperimental studies: Reevaluating the evaluation of training programs. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 94(448), 1053-1062. Dubowitz, H. & Feigelman, S. (1993). A profile of kinship care. Child Welfare, 72(2), 153-170. Dubowitz, H., Feigelman, S & Zuravin, S. (1992). The physical health of children in kinship care. American Journal of Diseases of Children, 146, 603-610 Eckenrode, J., Rowe, E., Laird, M. & Braithwaite, J. (1995). Mobility as a mediator of the effects of child maltreatment on academic performance. Child Development, 66, 1130-1142. Finch, S. J., Fanshel, D. & Grundy, J. F. (1986). Factors associated with discharge from foster care. Social Work Research & Abstracts, 22(1), 10-18 Frangakis, C. & Rubin, D. B. (2002). Principal stratification in causal inference. Biometrics, 58(1), 20-29. Gebel, T. J. (1996). Kinship care and non-relative family foster care: A comparison of caregiver attributes and attitudes. Child Welfare, 75(1):5-18. Geen, R. (2003). Finding Permanent Homes for Foster Children: Issues Raised by Kinship Care. [Online]. Available: www.urban.org. [Retrieved 2004, November 5]. Gibson, C. (2001). Privileging the participant: The importance of take-up rates in social welfare evaluations. Chicago: Joint Center for Poverty Research. Gleeson, J. P., O’Donnell, J. & Bonecutter, F. J. (1997). Understanding the complexity of practice in kinship foster care. Child Welfare, 76(6), 801-826. Gleeson, J. P. (1999a). Kinship care as a child welfare services: What do we really know. In J. P. Gleeson & C. F. Hairston (Eds.), Kinship care: Improving practice through research. Washington D.C.: Child Welfare League of America. Gleeson, J. P. (1999b). Who decides? Predicting caseworkers’ adoption and guardianship discussion with kinship caregiver. In J. P. Gleeson & C. F. Hairston (Eds.), Kinship care: Improving practice through research. Washington D.C.: Child Welfare League of America. 96 Grogan-Kaylor, A. (2000). Who goes into kinship care? The relationship of child and family characteristics to placement into kinship foster care. Social Work Research, 24(3), 143-155. Hampton, R. L. (1991). Child abuse in the African American community. In J. E. Everett, S. P. Chipungu & B. R. Leashore (Eds.), Child Welfare: An Afrocentric Perspective. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press. Harden, B. J. (2004). Safety and stability for foster children: A developmental perspective. Children, Families, and Foster Care, 14(1): 31-48. Henry, J. (1999) Permanency outcomes in legal guardianship of abused/neglected children. Families in Society. 80, 561-568. Hill, R. (2003). Disproportionality of minorities in Child Welfare: Synthesis of research findings. [Online]. Available: http://www.chs-wa.org/2_cfkreportsData.htm. [Retrieved: 2004, November 5]. Hill, J. Waldfogel, J. & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2002). Differential effects of high-quality child care. Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 21, 601-627. Hofferth, S. L. & Owens, T. J. (2001). Children at the Millennium: Where have we come from, where are we going? Amsterdam: JAI. Inglehart, A. (1994). Kinship foster care: Placement, services, and outcome issues. Children and Youth Services Review, 16, 107-122. James, S., Landsverk, J. & Slymen, D. J. (2004). Placement movement in out of home care: Patterns and Predictors. Children and Youth Service Review, 26, 185-206. Katz, L, Kling, J & Liebman, J. (2000). Moving to opportunity in Boston: Early results of a randomized mobility experiment. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(2), 607654. Kemp, S. P. & Bodony, J. M. (2000). Infants who stay in foster care: Child characteristics and permanency outcomes of legally free children first placed as infants. Child and Family Social Work, 5(2), 95-106. Kurtz, P. D., Gaudin, P. Howing, P. & Wodarski, J. (1993). The consequences of physical abuse and neglect on the school age child: Mediating factors. Children and Youth Services Review, 15(2), 85-104. Ladner, A. J. (2000). Children in Out-of-Home Placements. [Online]. Available: http://www.urban.org [Retrieved: 2004, November 5]. 97 LaLonde, R. (1986). Evaluating the econometric evaluations of training programs. American Economic Review, 76, 604-620. Landsverk, J., Davis, I., Ganger, W., Newton, R. & Johnson, I. (1996). Impact of child psychosocial functioning on reunification from out-of-home placement. Children and Youth Services Review, 18(4/5), 447-462. Lawrence[Laurence in text]-Webb, C. (1997). African American children in the modern child welfare system: A legacy of the Flemming Rule. In S. Jackson & S. Brissett-Champman (Eds). Serving African American children. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers. Leashore, B.R. (1984). Demystifying legal: An unexplored option for dependent children. Journal of Family Law, 23, 391-400. Leashore, B. R., McMurray, H. L. & Bailey, B. C. (1991). Reuniting and preserving African American families. In J. E. Everett, S. P. Chipungu & B. R. Leashore (eds), Child welfare: An Africentric perspective. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press. Lehman, C., Liang, S., O’Dell, K., & Duryea, M. (2003). Evaluation of Oregon’s Title IV-E waiver demonstration project: Final report. [Online]. Available: http://www.cwp.pdx.edu/pdfs/Waiver%20Final%20Report%203-27-03.pdf [Retrieved: 2003, August, 5]. Leslie, L. K., Landsverk, J., Ezzet-Lofstrom, R., Tschann, J. M., Slymen, D.J., Garland, A. F. (2000). Children in foster care: Factors influencing outpatient mental health service use. Child Abuse and Neglect, 24, 465-476. Link, M. K. (1996). Permanency outcomes in kinship care: A study of children placed in kinship care in Erie County, NY. Child Welfare, 75(5): 509-528. Long, S. L. (1997). Regression models for categorical and limited dependent variables. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications. Maluccio, A., Fein. E., & Davis, I. (1994). Family reunification: Research findings, issues, and directions. Child Welfare, 73(5), 489-504 Mandell, M. B. (2001). The Effect of Subsidized Guardianship on Exits from Kinship Care: Results from Maryland's Guardianship Assistance Demonstration Project. The 23rd Annual Association of Public Policy Administration and Management (APPAM) Conference, November 1-3, 2001, Washington D.C. Mass, H. S., & Engler, R. E. (1959). Children in need of parents. New York: Columbia University Press 98 McLean, B. & Thomas, R. (1996). Informal and formal kinship care populations: A study in contrasts. Child Welfare, 75(5), 489-508. Mcmiller, W. P. & Weisz, J. R. (1996). Help-seeking preceding mental health clinic intake among African-American, Latino, and Caucasian youths. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 35(8), 1086-1094. McRoy, R. (1994). Attachment and racial identity issues: Implication for child placement decision making. Journal of Multicultural Social Work, 3(3), 59-74. McRoy, R. (2004). African American Adoptions. In J. Everett, S. Chipungu & B. Leashore (Eds.), Child Welfare Revisited: An Africentric Perspective. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press. Meyer, B. S. & Link, M. K. (1990). Kinship foster care: The double edge dilemma. New York: Task Force on Permanency Planning for Foster Children. Newton, R. R., Litrownik, A. J. & Landsverk, J. A. (2000). Children and youth in foster care: Disentangling the relationship between problem behaviors and number of placements. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24(10), 1363-1374. Olsen, L. J. (1982). Predicting the permanency status of children in foster care. Social Work Research and Abstracts, 18(1), 9-20. Pecora, P.J., Le Prohn, N.S. & Nasuti, J.J. (1999). Role perceptions of kinship and other foster parents in family foster care. In R. L. Hegar & M. Scannapieco (Eds.), Kinship foster care: Policy, practice and research. New York: Oxford University Press. Proch, K. & Taber, M.A. (1985). Placement disruption: A review of research. Children and Youth Services Review, 7, 309-320. Rosenbaum, P. R. & Rubin, D. (1983). The central role of the propensity score in observational studies for causal effects. Biometrika, 70(1), 41-55. Rubin, D. B. (1997). Estimating causal effects from large data sets using propensity scores. Annals of Internal Medicine, 127(8), 757-763. Scannapieco, M (1999). Kinship care in the public child welfare system. In R. L. Hegar & M. Scannapieco (Eds.), Kinship foster care: Policy, practice and research. New York: Oxford University Press. Scannapieco, M., Hegar, R. L. & McAlpine, C. (1997). Kinship care and foster care: A comparison of characteristics and outcomes. Families in Society, 78(5), 480-488. 99 Smith, D. K., Stormshak, E., Chamberlain[Camberlain in text], P. & Whaley, R.B. (2001). Placement disruption in treatment foster care. Journal of Emotional & Behavioral Disorder, 9(3), 200-212. Smith, J. (2004). Demography of African American families. In J. Everett, S. Chipungu & B. Leashore (Eds.), Child Welfare Revisited: An Africentric Perspective. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press. Schwartz, I. M. & Fishman, G. (1999). Kids raised by the government. Westport: Praeger. Taber, M. A., & Proch, K. (1987). Placement stability for adolescents in foster care: findings from a program experiment. Child Welfare, 66, 433-445. Takas, M (1992). Kinship care: Developing a safe and effective framework for protective placement of children with relatives. Zero to Three, 13(3), 12-17. Terling-Watt, T. (2001). Permanency in kinship care: An exploration of disruption rates and factors associated placement disruption. Children and Youth Services Review, 23(20), 111-126. Testa, M. F., Shook, K. L., Cohen, L. & Woods, M.G. (1996). Permanency planning options for child in formal kinship care. Child Welfare, 75(5), 451-470. Testa, M. F. (2001). Kinship care and permanency. Journal of Social Service Research, 28(1): 25-43. Testa, M. F. (2002). Subsidized guardianship: Testing an idea whose time has finally come. Social Work Research, 26(3), 145-158. Thornton, J. L. (1991). Permanency planning for children in kinship care. Child Welfare, 70, 593-601. TriWest Group (2004). New Mexico Title IV-E waiver evaluations: Evaluation Update. [Online]. Available: http://www.cyfd.org. [Retrieved: 2006, April 22]. University of Maryland, School of Social Work (2003). Maryland subsidized guardianship demonstration project evaluation final report. Unpublished. U.S. Department of Education. (2002). Twenty-Fourth Annual Report to Congress on the Implementation of the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act. [Online]. Available: http://www.ed.gov/about/reports/annual/osep/2002/index.html. [Retrieved: 2005, October 3]. U.S. Department of Health and Human services, Administration for Children and Families, Children’s Bureau. (2000). Report to the Congress on Kinship Foster 100 Care. [Online]. Available: http://www.acf.dhhs.gov. [Retrieved: 2000, December 20]. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2003a). Child welfare Outcomes 2000: Annual Report. [Online]. Available: http://www.acf.hhs.gov/programs/cb/publications/cwo.htm [Retrieved: 2004, April 10]. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2003b). AFCARS report (Preliminary FY2001 estimates as of March 2003). [Online]. Available: www.acf.dhhs.gov/programs/cb/publications/ afcars/report8.pdf [Retrieved: 2003, September 19]. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1994). The national survey of current and former foster parents. Washington D.C.: Author. U.S. Department of Health and Human services, Administration for Children and Families, Children’s Bureau (no date). Assisted guardianship/kinship permanence. [Online]. Available: http://www.acf.dhhs.gov/programs/cb/initiatives/cwwaiver/assisted.htm [Retrieved: 2003, July 23]. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2005). Synthesis of findings: Assisted guardianship child welfare waiver demonstrations. [Online]. Available: http://www.acf.hhs.gov/programs/cb/programs_fund/cwwaiver/agissue/index.ht m. [Retrieved: 2006, April 21]. Usher, C., Randolph, K. & Gogan, H. (1999). Placement patterns in foster care. Social Service Review, 73(1), 22-36. Vogel, C.A. (1999). Using administrative database to examine factors affecting length of stay in substitute care. Children and Youth Services Review, 21(8), 677-690. Webster, D., Barth, R. & Needell, B. (2000). Placement stability for children in out-ofhome care: A longitudinal analysis. Child Welfare, 79(5), 614-632. Wells, K. & Guo, S. (1999). Reunification and Reentry of Foster Children. Children and Youth Services Review, 21 (4), 273-294. Widom, C. (1991). The role of placement experiences in mediating the criminal consequences of early childhood victimization. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 61(2), 195-209. Williams, C. C. (1991). Expanding the options in the quest for permanence. In J. E. Everett, S. P. Chipungu & B. R. Leashore (Eds.), Child Welfare: An Africentric Perspective. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press. 101 Wulczyn, F., Kogan, J & Harden, B. J. (2003). Placement stability and movement trajectories. Social Service Review, 77(2), 212-237. Yoshikawa, H., Magnuson, K, Bos, J. M. & Hsueh, J. (2003). Effects of earningssupplement policies on adult economic and middle-childhood outcomes differ for the “hardest to employ”. Child Development, 74(5), 1500-1521. Zuravin, S., Benedict, M & Stallings, R. (1999). The adult functioning of former kinship and non-relative foster care children. In R. L. Hegar & M. Scannapieco (Eds.), Kinship foster care: Policy, practice and research. New York: Oxford University Press. 102 Appendix: Additional Statistical Tables 103 Table A- 1 Sample Balance after Propensity Score Matching (Not considering participating in caregiver interview) Exit through Guardianship Exit through Move to Restricted Remain in Kinship Reunification Foster Care Care Control Experimental Control Experimental Control Experimental Control Experimental Group Group Group Group Group Group Group Group Gender Female 39.4% 42.9% 50.0% 69.2 51.9% 60.9% 54.3% 48.4% Male 60.6% 57.1% 50.0% 30.8% 48.2% 39.1% 45.7% 51.6% 2 χ 0.08 1.03 0.41 0.23 Race African American 87.9% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 92.6% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% Other 12.1% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 7.4% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% χ2 3.63* N/A 1.70 N/A Education Regular School 62.5% 50.0% 71.4% 76.9% 61.5% 78.3% 72.7% 61.3% Special Education 3.1% 7.1% 0.0% 7.7% 3.9% 17.4% 9.1% 6.5% Not in School 34.4% 42.9% 28.6% 15.4% 34.6% 4.4% 18.2% 32.3% χ2 2.39 1.63 1.86 1.72 Entry reason Neglect 60.6% 64.3% 57.1% 69.2% 55.6% 82.6% 80.0% 77.4% Abandonment 18.2% 21.4% 0.0% 7.7% 29.6% 8.7% 8.6% 12.9% Abuse 9.1% 3.6% 7.1% 7.7% 7.4% 4.4% 2.9% 3.2% Other 0.0% 3.6% 0.0% 0.0% 3.7% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% More than one of 12.1% 7.1% 35.7% 15.4% 3.7% 4.4% 8.6% 6.5% the above χ2 2.38 2.31 5.11 0.41 Sibling Group Yes 48.5% 60.7% 57.1% 38.5% 55.6% 56.5% 68.6% 67.8% No 51.5% 39.3% 42.9% 61.5% 44.4% 43.5% 31.4% 32.3% 2 χ 0.52 0.94 0.00 0.01 104 Exit through Guardianship Control Experimental Group Group Special needs Yes No 2 χ Caregiver at removal At least one parent Relatives or friends 2 χ Age at entry t Length of stay in care (in months) prior to random assignment t Number of placement changes in the previous episode t N Exit through Reunification Control Experimental Group Group Move to Restricted Foster Care Control Experimental Group Group Remain in Kinship Care Control Experimental Group Group 19.2% 81.8% 7.1% 82.9% 1.62 0.0% 100.0% 15.4% 84.6% 2.33 7.4% 92.6% 4.4% 95.7% 0.21 17.1% 82.9% 9.7% 90.3% 0.78 92.6% 6.5% 92.9% 7.1% 50.9 94.1% 5.9% 0.14 5.9 0.11 50.3 82.8% 17.2% 33.7 83.4% 16.7% 0.57 7.9 -0.91 33.9 90.9% 9.1% 38.1 91.7% 8.3% 0.07 4.9 0.48 40.0 53.0 92.0% 8.0% 0.41 5.8 0.04 58.3 0.48 -0.33 0.57 0.8 -0.03 0.2 0.3 0.06 0.2 0.3 -0.74 0.3 33 -0.34 28 14 1.55 13 27 0.48 23 35 0.12 31 4.4 6.4 6.1 5.9 African American, Non-Hispanic Other entry reasons include parental incapacity, mental injury due to abuse or neglect, child’s disruptive behavior, and child’s special needs. Other special needs include child physical, mental, and behavioral problems. Sample mean. * Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. 105 Table A- 2 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would exit through guardianship in the absence of the subsidy (Not considering participation in caregiver interview) Poisson Poisson NB Model NB Model Model 1 Model 2 1 2 Experimental Group -0.12 0.01 -0.12 0.01 (0.50) (0.54) (0.50) (0.54) Male ---0.38 ---0.38 (0.58) (0.58) Age at entry (in months) ----0.00 ----0.00 (0.01) (0.01) Sibling Groups ----0.46 ----0.46 (0.56) (0.56) Neglect ---0.73 ---0.73 (0.71) (0.71) Abandonment ------- ------- Number of placement in ---previous episode Length of stay in care prior ---to random assignment Constant -6.90**** (0.33) Log likelihood -25.83 N 38 -0.41 (0.37) -0.01 (0.01) -7.09**** (0.86) -24.38 38 -------6.90**** (0.33) -25.83 38 -0.41 (0.37) -0.01 (0.01) -7.09**** (0.86) -24.38 38 Standard errors are in parentheses. due to lack of variation. Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. Dropped * Table A- 3 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would exit through reunification in the absence of the subsidy (Not considering participation in caregiver interview) Poisson Poisson NB Model NB Model Model 1 Model 2 1 2 Experimental Group -0.79 -0.46 -0.79 -0.46 (0.57) (1.04) (0.57) (1.04) Male ---0.08 ---0.08 (1.08) (1.08) Age at entry (in months) ----0.00 ----0.00 (0.01) (0.01) Sibling Groups ---1.30 ---1.30 (1.22) (1.22) Neglect ----0.93 ----0.93 (1.31) (1.31) Abandonment ----0.67 ----0.67 106 Poisson Model 1 Number of placement in ---previous episode Length of stay in care prior ---to random assignment Constant -5.98**** (0.35) Log likelihood -17.28 N 17 Poisson Model 2 (1.30) -0.05 (0.39) -0.01 (0.02) -5.37**** (2.09) -16.02 17 NB Model 1 -------5.98**** (0.35) -17.28 17 NB Model 2 (1.30) -0.05 (0.39) -0.01 (0.02) -5.37**** (2.09) -16.02 17 Standard errors are in parentheses. Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. * Table A- 4 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would move to restricted foster care in the absence of the subsidy (Not considering participation in caregiver interview) Poisson Poisson NB Model NB Model Model 1 Model 2 1 2 Experimental Group -1.18*** -1.64*** -1.20** -1.38** (0.44) (0.47) (0.54) (0.54) Male ---0.23 ---0.36 (0.31) (0.42) Age at entry (in months) ---0.02**** ---0.02*** (0.00) (0.01) Sibling Groups ---0.94*** ---0.77* (0.33) (0.43) Neglect ---0.15 ----0.02 (0.55) (0.69) Abandonment ----0.58 ----0.62 (0.49) (0.62) Number of placement in ----0.33 ----0.24 previous episode (0.30) (0.35) Length of stay in care prior ---0.01 ---0.01 to random assignment (0.01) (0.01) Constant -6.23**** -9.08**** -6.23**** -8.42**** (0.15) (1.09) (0.24) (1.29) Log likelihood -71.70 -55.55 -58.65 -52.52 N 42 42 42 42 * Standard errors are in parentheses. Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. 107 Table A- 5 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would remain in kinship care in the absence of the subsidy (Not considering participating in caregiver interview) Poisson Poisson NB Model NB Model Model 1 Model 2 1 2 Experimental Group 0.24 0.14 0.25 0.12 (0.41) (0.44) (0.52) (0.56) Male ---0.78 ---0.77 (0.42) (0.51) Age at entry (in months) ---0.00 ---0.00 (0.01) (0.01) Sibling Groups ---0.16 ---0.22 (0.45) (0.55) Neglect ---0.84 ---0.72 (1.19) (1.30) Abandonment ---0.10 ---0.03 (0.73) (0.85) Number of placement in ----0.27 ----0.27 previous episode (0.31) (0.37) Length of stay in care prior ---0.00 ---0.01 to random assignment (0.01) (0.01) Constant -7.43**** -9.11**** -7.43**** -9.06**** (0.24) (1.66) (0.30) (1.89) Log likelihood -53.25 -50.44 -49.77 -47.88 N 56 56 56 56 Standard * errors are in parentheses. Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. Table A- 6 Coefficients obtained from a multinomial logit model on permanency status (African-American children only) Multinomial Multinomial Logit Model 1 Logit Model 2 Reunification Male -0.37 -0.51 Age at entry (in months) 0.01* 0.01** Length of stay in care prior to random -0.02 -0.01 assignment (in months) Abandoned by parents 0.17 0.52 Number of placements in the previous -0.07 0.31 episode Having sibling groups 0.41 0.71 Participating in caregiver interview ----41.66**** Constant -0.79 -1.39 Moved to RFC Male -0.30 -0.30 108 Multinomial Logit Model 1 Age at entry (in months) 0.01** Length of time in care prior to random 0.02* assignment Abandoned by parents -0.72 Number of placements in the previous -0.18 episode Having sibling groups 0.16 Participation in caregiver interview ---Constant -1.26 Remained in Kinship Care Male 0.17 Age at entry (in months) 0.01** Length of time in care prior to random 0.01 assignment Abandoned by parents -1.61** Number of placements in the previous -0.28 episode Having sibling groups 0.57 Participation in caregiver interview ---Constant -1.24 157 N= 0.01 Model Significance p<= 2 0.09 Pseudo R = -193.49 Log Likelihood = * Multinomial Logit Model 2 0.01** 0.02* -0.72 -0.14* 0.17 0.13 -1.34 0.16 0.01** 0.01 -1.65** -0.28 0.47 0.74 -1.47 157 0.001 0.13 -184.47 The reference group is “exited to guardianship” Significant at .01 level.; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level 109 Table A- 7 Sample Balance After Propensity Score Matching (African-American children only, not considering participation in caregiver interviews) Exit through Guardianship Exit through Move to Restricted Remain in Kinship Reunification Foster Care Care Control Experimental Control Experimental Control Experimental Control Experimental Group Group Group Group Group Group Group Group Gender Female 33.3% 45.5% 46.2% 36.4% 61.5% 66.7% 50.0% 36.7% Male 66.7% 54.6% 53.9% 63.6% 38.5% 33.3% 50.0% 63.3% 2 χ 0.75 0.24 0.14 1.15 Education Regular School 56.0% 36.4% 75.0% 54.6% 64.0% 83.3% 68.8% 70.0% Special Education 0.0% 9.1% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 4.7% 9.4% 3.3% Not in School 44.0% 54.6% 25.0% 45.5% 36.0% 12.5% 21.9% 26.7% χ2 3.50 1.06 4.43 1.03 Entry reason Neglect 63.0% 50.0% 61.5% 45.6% 61.5% 79.2% 76.5% 86.7% Abandonment 14.8% 18.2% 0.0% 36.4% 26.9% 12.5% 8.8% 3.3% Abuse 11.1% 9.1% 7.7% 9.1% 7.7% 0.0% 5.9% 0.0% Other 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 9.1% 3.9% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% More than one of 11.1% 22.7% 30.8% 0.0% 0.0% 8.3% 8.8% 10.0% the above χ2 1.49 9.59** 6.79 2.76 Sibling Group Yes 48.2% 50.0% 53.9% 27.3% 57.7% 54.2% 67.7% 46.7% No 51.9% 50.0% 46.2% 72.7% 42.3% 45.8% 32.4% 53.3% χ2 0.02 1.73 0.06 2.88* Special needs Yes 14.8% 9.1% 7.7% 9.1% 7.7% 16.7% 17.7% 13.3% No 85.2% 90.9% 92.3% 90.9% 92.3% 83.3% 82.4% 86.7% 110 Exit through Guardianship χ2 Caregiver at removal At least one parent Relatives or friends χ2 Age at entry t Length of stay in care (in months) prior to random assignment t Number of placement changes in the previous episode t N Control Experimental Group Group 0.37 96.0% 4.0% 39.9 76.2% 23.8% 3.95** 5.1 -1.02 41.2 1.7 -0.21 4.9 27 -0.77 22 4.1 Exit through Reunification Control Experimental Group Group 0.02 92.3% 7.7% 34.9 81.8% 18.2% 0.60 7.1 -0.42 31.5 0.9 0.46 0.5 13 0.79 11 6.4 Move to Restricted Foster Care Control Experimental Group Group 0.95 95.5% 4.6% 50.2 84.2% 15.8% 1.46 6.7 -0.26 50.8 0.4 -0.06 0.2 26 0.90 24 6.3 Remain in Kinship Care Control Experimental Group Group 0.23 82.8% 17.2% 51.7 92.6% 7.4% 1.24 6.3 -0.68 47.1 0.2 0.68 0.3 34 -0.32 30 5.6 Other entry reasons include parental incapacity, mental injury due to abuse or neglect, child’s disruptive behavior, and child’s special needs. Other special needs include child physical, mental, and behavioral problems. Sample mean. * Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. 111 Table A- 8 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placements for children who would exit through guardianship in the absence of the subsidy – African-American children only Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Experimental or Control 1.15** 0.95 1.18* 1.01 (0.56) (0.61) (0.61) (0.68) Male ----0.15 ----0.21 (0.60) (0.64) Age at entry (in months) ---0.01 ---0.01 (0.01) (0.01) Sibling Groups ----0.09 ----0.09 (0.64) (0.69) Neglect ----0.05 ----0.01 (0.71) (0.78) Abandonment ----0.44 ----0.34 (0.61) (0.68) Number of placement in ----0.09 ----0.09 previous episode (0.13) (0.15) Length of stay in care prior ---0.00 ---0.00 to random assignment (0.01) (0.02) Constant -7.54**** -7.53**** -7.56**** -7.44**** (0.50) (1.17) (0.52) (1.21) Log likelihood -27.83 -26.86 -27.39 -26.71 N 34 34 34 34 Standard errors are in parentheses. Models one and two are Poisson models. Models three and four are negative binomial models. * Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. Table A- 9 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would exit through reunification in the absence of the subsidy (Not considering participation in caregiver interview) - African American children only Poisson Poisson NB Model NB Model Model 1 Model 2 1 2 Experimental Group -1.13 -1.65 -1.13 -1.65 (0.80) (2.22) (0.80) (2.22) Male ----0.24 ----0.24 (1.56) (1.56) Age at entry (in months) ---0.01 ---0.01 (0.02) (0.02) Sibling Groups ---1.61 ---1.61 (1.58) (1.58) Neglect ----3.28 ----3.28 (3.39) (3.39) Abandonment ---0.19 ---0.19 (2.30) (2.30) 112 Poisson Model 1 ---- Number of placement in previous episode Length of stay in care prior ---to random assignment Constant -6.02**** (0.38) Log likelihood -13.86 N 15 Poisson Model 2 0.25 (0.55) 0.01 (0.04) -3.96 (4.25) -11.73 15 NB Model 1 -------6.01**** (0.38) -13.86 15 NB Model 2 0.25 (0.55) 0.01 (0.04) -3.96 (4.25) -11.73 15 Standard * errors are in parentheses. Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. Table A- 10 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placements for children who would move to restricted foster care in the absence of the subsidy – African-American children only Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Experimental or Control -0.38 -0.64* -0.45 -0.73 (0.31) (0.36) (0.50) (0.53) Male ---0.16 ----0.07 (0.31) (0.49) Age at entry (in months) ---0.02**** ---0.02*** (0.00) (0.01) Sibling Groups ----0.08 ----0.11 (0.30) (0.48) Neglect ----0.46 ----0.57 (0.48) (0.75) Abandonment ---0.56 ---0.77 (0.44) (0.65) Number of placement in ---0.10 ---0.01 previous episode (0.15) (0.28) Length of stay in care prior ---0.02*** ---0.02** to random assignment (0.01) (0.01) Constant -6.24**** -8.62**** -6.24**** -8.96**** (0.15) (0.88) (0.29) (1.45) Log likelihood -85.63 -70.39 -63.41 -56.90 N 43 43 43 43 Standard errors are in parentheses. Models one and two are Poisson models. Models three and four are negative binomial models. * Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. 113 Table A- 11 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would remain in kinship care in the absence of the subsidy (Not considering participation in caregiver interview) - African-American children only Poisson Poisson NB Model NB Model Model 1 Model 2 1 2 Experimental Group 0.56 0.54 0.56 0.57 (0.36) (0.41) (0.41) (0.45) Male ---0.54 ---0.54 (0.42) (0.44) Age at entry (in months) ---0.00 ---0.00 (0.00) (0.01) Sibling Groups ---0.65 ---0.65 (0.42) (0.46) Neglect ---0.73 ---0.73 (1.13) (1.17) Abandonment ----0.02 ----0.01 (0.67) (0.71) Number of placement in ----0.05 ----0.04 previous episode (0.27) (0.29) Length of stay in care prior ----0.00 ----0.00 to random assignment (0.01) (0.01) Constant -7.35**** -8.94**** -7.35**** -8.96**** (0.24) (1.47) (0.41) (1.54) Log likelihood -55.34 -52.30 -54.16 -46.78 N 56 56 56 56 Standard * errors are in parentheses. Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. 114 Table A- 12 Sample Balance after Propensity Score Matching (African-American children only, considering participation in caregiver interviews) Exit through Guardianship Exit through Move to Restricted Remain in Kinship Reunification Foster Care Care Control Experimental Control Experimental Control Experimental Control Experimental Group Group Group Group Group Group Group Group Gender Female 33.3% 39.1% 46.2% 76.9% 61.5% 57.7% 50.0% 62.1% Male 66.7% 60.8% 53.9% 23.1% 38.5% 42.3% 50.0% 37.9% 2 χ 0.18 2.60 0.08 0.92 Education Regular School 56.0% 52.2% 75.0% 61.5% 64.0% 42.3% 68.8% 51.7% Special Education 0.0% 8.7% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 11.5% 9.4% 3.5% Not in School 44.0% 39.1% 25.0% 38.5% 36.0% 46.2% 21.9% 44.8% χ2 2.27 0.52 4.33 3.99 Entry reason Neglect 63.0% 69.6% 61.5% 38.5% 61.5% 69.2% 76.5% 82.8% Abandonment 14.8% 21.7% 0.0% 7.7% 26.9% 19.2% 8.8% 3.5% Abuse 11.1% 8.7% 7.7% 15.4% 7.7% 3.9% 5.9% 10.3% Other 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 3.9% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% More than one of 11.1% 0.0% 30.8% 38.5% 0.0% 7.7% 8.8% 3.5% the above χ2 3.04 2.13 3.78 1.90 Sibling Group Yes 48.2% 34.8% 53.9% 61.5% 57.7% 65.4% 67.7% 55.2% No 51.9% 65.2% 46.2% 38.5% 42.3% 34.6% 32.4% 44.8% χ2 0.91 0.16 0.33 1.03 Special needs Yes 14.8% 17.4% 7.7% 7.7% 7.7% 7.7% 17.7% 10.3% No 85.2% 82.6% 92.3% 92.3% 92.3% 92.3% 82.4% 89.9% 115 Exit through Guardianship χ2 Caregiver at removal At least one parent Relatives or friends χ2 Age at entry t Length of stay in care (in months) prior to random assignment t Number of placement changes in the previous episode t N Control Experimental Group Group 0.06 96.0% 4.0% 39.9 90.5% 9.5% 0.57 4.5 -0.37 41.2 1.7 -0.84 0.3 27 0.28 23 4.1 Exit through Reunification Control Experimental Group Group 0.00 92.3% 7.7% 34.9 45.5% 54.6% 6.33** 6.0 0.24 43.2 0.9 -1.14 0.5 13 0.67 13 6.4 Move to Restricted Foster Care Control Experimental Group Group 0.00 95.5% 4.6% 50.2 85.7% 14.3% 1.21 5.3 0.87 54.7 0.4 -0.53 0.3 26 0.48 26 6.3 Remain in Kinship Care Control Experimental Group Group 0.68 82.8% 17.2% 51.7 95.0% 5.0% 1.65 5.2 0.42 56.8 0.2 -0.65 0.4 34 -0.88 29 5.6 entry reasons include parental incapacity, mental injury due to abuse or neglect, child’s disruptive behavior, and child’s special needs. Other special needs include child physical, mental, and behavioral problems. Sample mean. * Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. Other 116 Table A- 13 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would exit through guardianship in the absence of the subsidy (Considering participation in caregiver interview) - African-American children only Poisson Poisson NB Model NB Model Model 1 Model 2 1 2 Experimental Group 0.95* 1.18 1.06 1.18 (0.56) (0.82) (0.76) (0.82) Male ----0.42 ----0.42 (0.66) (0.66) Age at entry (in months) ---0.00 ---0.00 (0.01) (0.01) Sibling Groups ----0.40 ----0.40 (0.75) (0.75) Neglect ----1.08 ----1.08 (0.70) (0.70) Abandonment ----0.10 ---0.10 (0.75) (0.75) Number of placement in ----0.05 ----0.05 previous episode (0.11) (0.11) Length of stay in care prior ---0.00 ----0.00 to random assignment (0.02) (0.02) Participating in caregiver ---1.73** 1.73** interview (0.86) (0.86) Constant -7.54**** -7.08**** -7.63**** -7.08 (0.50) (0.86) (0.62) (0.82) Log likelihood -39.33 -24.76 -31.34 -24.76 N 37 37 37 37 Standard * errors are in parentheses. Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. 117 Table A- 14 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would exit through reunification in the absence of the subsidy (Considering participation in caregiver interview) - African-American children only Poisson Poisson NB Model NB Model Model 1 Model 2 1 2 Experimental Group -0.54 -2.44** -0.54 -2.44** (0.49) (1.21) (0.49) (1.21) Male ----0.77 ----0.77 (0.87) (0.87) Age at entry (in months) ---0.01 ---0.01 (0.01) (0.01) Sibling Groups ---1.27 ---1.27 (1.36) (1.36) Neglect ----4.12** ----4.12** (2.06) (2.06) Abandonment ---1.66* ---1.66* (0.91) (0.91) Number of placement in ---0.32 ---0.32 previous episode (0.43) (0.43) Length of stay in care prior ---0.02 ---0.02 to random assignment (0.03) (0.03) Participating in caregiver ------------interview Constant -6.01**** -3.31* -6.01**** -3.31* (0.38) (1.83) (0.38) (1.83) Log likelihood -23.21 -18.89 -23.21 -18.89 N 20 20 20 20 Standard errors are in parentheses. Variable “whether participating in caregiver interview” dropped out of the model because none of the caregivers of children in this group participated in caregiver interviews. * Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. 118 Table A- 15 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would move to RFC in the absence of the subsidy (Considering participation in caregiver interview) - African-American children only Poisson Poisson NB Model NB Model Model 1 Model 2 1 2 Experimental Group -0.26 -0.59 -0.28 -0.56 (0.30) (0.37) (0.50) (0.57) Male ----0.23 ----0.11 (0.32) (0.49) Age at entry (in months) ---0.01*** ---0.01** (0.00) (0.01) Sibling Groups ---0.09 ---0.22 (0.31) (0.52) Neglect ----0.37 ----0.37 (0.40) (0.73) Abandonment ---0.76* ---0.85 (0.91) (0.68) Number of placement in ---0.13 ---0.02 previous episode (0.16) (0.33) Length of stay in care prior ---0.02*** ---0.02* to random assignment (0.01) (0.01) Participating in caregiver ----0.13 ----0.10 interview (0.39) (0.70) Constant -6.24**** -8.25**** -6.24**** -8.59**** (0.15) (1.83) (0.29) (1.38) Log likelihood -87.36 -75.44 -64.65 -60.47 N 43 43 43 43 Standard * errors are in parentheses. Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. 119 Table A- 16 Coefficients obtained from Poisson and negative binomial regression models on exposure adjusted numbers of placement changes for children who would remain in kinship care in the absence of the subsidy (Considering participation in caregiver interview) - African-American children only Poisson Poisson NB Model NB Model Model 1 Model 2 1 2 Experimental Group 0.37 0.95* 0.38 0.84 (0.41) (0.50) (0.54) (0.59) Male ---1.22*** ---1.16** (0.46) (0.52) Age at entry (in months) ---0.00 ---0.01 (0.01) (0.01) Sibling Groups ---0.63 ---0.49 (0.51) (0.57) Neglect ---0.24 ---0.26 (0.69) (0.79) Abandonment ---0.64 ---0.62 (0.65) (0.73) Number of placement in ----0.22 ----0.18 previous episode (0.37) (0.41) Length of stay in care prior ----0.01 ----0.01 to random assignment (0.01) (0.01) Participating in caregiver ----0.38 ----0.46 interview (0.49) (0.59) Constant -7.35**** -8.24**** -7.35**** -8.16**** (0.24) (1.09) (0.54) (1.22) Log likelihood -52.37 -46.25 -48.82 -45.14 N 52 52 52 52 Standard * errors are in parentheses. Significant at .1 level; ** Significant at .05 level; *** Significant at .01 level; **** Significant at .001 level. 120