THE EYE Our visual systems perform all kinds of amazing jobs, from finding stars in the night sky, to picking out just the right strawberry in the supermarket, to following a ball flying into a glove in a baseball game. How do our eyes and brains recognize shape, movement, depth, and colour? How do we so easily pick a friend's face out of a crowd, yet get fooled by optical illusions? 1. Our eyes allow us to see electromagnetic radiation reflected from objects Most animals and many plants are photosensitive; that is, they can detect different light intensities. Some organisms do this with single cells or with simple eyes that do not form images but do allow the organism to react to light by moving toward or away from it. In order for an eye to collect more information about the world it must be able to form an image. Our eyes, like those of many animals, detect a just narrow range of all the wavelengths of electromagnetic radiation. This range of light is called the visible spectrum. Figure 1 shows how the visible spectrum fits into the entire electromagnetic spectrum. Figure 1. The electromagnetic spectrum and the visible spectrum. 2. The eyeball is an optical device for focusing light The mammalian eyeball (Figure 2) is an organ that focuses an image onto the retina, which lines the back of the eye. Light from a scene passes through the cornea, pupil, and lens on its way to the retina. The cornea and lens focus light from objects onto photoreceptors (rods and cones), which absorb and then convert it into electrical signals that carry information to the brain through the optical nerve. Two rooms of transparent fluid feed eye tissues and maintain constant eye shape. The lens projects an inverted image onto the retina in the same way a camera lens projects an inverted image onto film; the brain adjusts this inversion so we see the world in its correct orientation. To control the images that fall upon our retinas, we can either turn our heads or turn our eyes independently of our heads by contracting the extraocular muscles. The cornea and lens bend or refract light rays as they enter the eye, in order to focus images on the retina. The eye can change the way the rays are bent and thus can focus images of objects that are close by or far away, by changing the shape of the lens. The ciliary muscle (ciliary body) does this by relaxing to pull at the lens and allowing it to flatten up so it can bend light rays less, or contracting for the opposite effect (Fig. 2B, C). blind spot A blind spot B C Figure 2. The mammalian eyeball. 3. Mistakes in the eye cause focusing problems Mistakes occur when the bending of light rays by the cornea and lens does not focus the image correctly onto the retina. An eyeball that is too long or too short for the optics of the cornea and lens or an irregularly shaped cornea can cause refractive errors. Near sightedness (bijziendheid) results either when the eyeball is too short or when the cornea is curved too much, and the focused image falls in front of the retina. Far sightedness (verziendheid) is the opposite, with the image falling behind the retina. Fortunately, most mistakes can be corrected with prescription lenses (Figure 3). Figure 3. nearsightedness farsightedness 4. The retina contains photoreceptors for detecting light The photoreceptors in the retina are of two types: rods and cones, so named because of their shapes (see Fig. 2a). These cells detect light by molecules that absorb certain wavelengths of light. These molecules are called photopigments which absorb light; in so doing they undergo a shape change. This shape change leads to a change in the electrical state of a rod or cone cell membrane. This change in the rod or cone cell membrane is passed on to nerve cells in the retina, and from there to the brain. 5. Rods function in dim light In dim light, we use our rods, which cannot work in bright light. Rods outnumber cones (120 million rods and about 6 million cones in each retina). A little bit of light can trigger a group of rods, leading to an electrical signal sent to the brain. Cones, on the other hand, must each absorb a lot of light in order to send signals. Many rods (up to 150) work together to form one signal. This way, where cones can make an image consisting of 1500 dots, rods can make an image of only 10 dots. Therefore, we cannot see fine detail using rods and cones we can. 6. Cones give us day vision Our vision in bright is completely done by cones, which give us colour vision, black and white vision, and the ability to see fine detail. Like rods, cones contain photopigments which are sensitive to one of three colours: red, green or blue. Cones are spread throughout the retina but are especially concentrated in a central area called the fovea. When we want to read or inspect fine detail, we move our heads and eyes until the image of interest falls onto the fovea. Because the fovea lacks rods, it is easier to see in dim light by looking to the side of an object instead of directly at it (Figure 4). You can test this by looking to the side of a faint star so that its image falls on rods, rather than on the fovea where it probably will not register. When you look directly at the faint star, it disappears. fovea rods cones nose ear Figure 4. Thus, cones give us day vision and rods take over in dim light and at night. Both rods and cones can operate at the same time under some conditions: in dim or dark conditions, rods are most sensitive, but cones respond to stimuli that are sufficiently bright. This is why we can see the colours of neon lights on dark nights. TABLE 1. PARTS OF THE EYE STRUCTURE FUNCTION Blind spot small area of the retina where the optic nerve leaves the eye: any image falling here will not be seen Ciliary muscles (ciliary body) involuntary muscles that change the lens shape to allow focusing images of objects at different distances Cornea transparent tissue covering the front of the eye: does not have blood vessels; does have nerves Cones photoreceptors responsive to colour and in bright conditions; used for fine detail Rods photoreceptors responsive in low light conditions; not useful for fine detail Fovea central part of the macula that provides sharpest vision; contains only cones Iris circular band of muscles that controls the size of the pupil. The pigmentation of the iris gives "color" to the eye. Blue eyes have the least amount of pigment; brown eyes have the most Lens transparent tissue that bends light passing through the eye: to focus light, the lens can change shape Optic nerve bundle of over one million axons from ganglion cells that carry visual signals from the eye to the brain Pupil hole in the centre of the eye where light passes through Choroid Thin tissue layer containing blood vessels, lying between the sclera and retina; also, because of the high melanocytes content, the choroid acts as a light-absorbing layer. Retina layer of tissue on the back portion of the eye that contains cells responsive to light (photoreceptors, cones and rods) Sclera tough, white outer covering of the eyeball; extraocular muscles attach here to move the eye n y