Rebecca Ladd April 22, 2009 Decisions That Involve Risk Among Children and Adolescents Rebecca Ladd Introduction Risky decisions are those decisions that hold a strong possibility of danger, hazard or loss to the decision maker(s) and/or others. Risky decisions are usually associated with children and, more commonly, adolescents. This research report is aimed at targeting the causes involved with decision making processes that lead to these risky behaviors. Psychological, developmental, neurological, and behavioral explanations will all be explored as possible reasons for risky decision making among children and adolescents. A particularly important area of interest in this topic involves determining the key difference(s) between the decision making mechanisms of children/adolescents versus adults. In identifying the key difference(s), we can more clearly see why children and/or adolescents choose to participate in risky behaviors. This question often arises in the creation of behavior management plans by school systems, law enforcement, youth crime prevention programs, etc. It is also important to explore the environmental and social factors that play into the previously mentioned key differences between children/adolescents versus adults. Literature Review The first article I chose to include in this report is entitled “The Development of Judgment and Decision Making during Childhood and Adolescence (Jacobs & Klaczynski, 2002).” This article focuses on the development of decision making processes from childhood, through adolescence, all the way up to through adulthood. Jacobs and Klaczynski give evidence that says that, although adults are somewhat successful at making “good” decisions, they often fall victim to judgment biases, just like children and adolescents. The article even states that the use of heuristics increases with age; the reason for this being that “judgment heuristics and other biases appear to be linked to increases in knowledge (e.g., stereotypes) and to preservation of social beliefs (e.g., religious beliefs) (Jacobs & Klaczynski, 2002).” The article also outlines two forms of developmental change: 1. improvements in reasoning competencies and 2. increases in the number of heuristics obtained and the frequency those heuristics are used (Jacobs & Klaczynski, 2002). Additionally, Jacobs and Klaczynski conclude that the development of decision making is by no means a progressive, one-way process. It is a very complex course of development with a multitude of components. Development occurs through two information processing systems, an experiential system and an analytical system (Jacobs & Klaczynski, 2002). “Whereas the experiential system relies heavily on the contents of memory for judgments and decision, analytic processing is based on the acquisition and application of abilities that are important for normative reasoning and decision making (Jacobs & Klaczynski, 2002).” The second article included in my report is entitled “Risk and Rationality in Adolescent Decision Making: Implications for Theory, Practice, and Public Policy (Reyna & Farley, 2006).” In this article, Reyna and Farley make the claim that both adults and adolescents use an optimistic bias. This bias causes the individual to feel that he/she is at less risk than others for entering a risky, potentially dangerous situation. There are even some cases in which adolescents perceive themselves as being more susceptible to risky situations than adults perceive themselves. Reyna and Farley acknowledge the previously mentioned information but they then go on to explain that adolescents may feel more vulnerable to risky situations, but they do not always recognize the consequences and long-term effects. Therefore, they are more likely to ignore their feelings of vulnerability and engage in risky situations. One of the major focuses of the article is cognitive-developmental differences between adolescents and adults. Adolescents Rebecca Ladd April 22, 2009 use “qualitative gist-based thinking, which explains increases in cognitive illusions with age (Reyna & Farley, 2006).” This type of thinking encourages “globally adaptive but locally flawed thinking processes (Reyna & Farley, 2006).” Finally, the article concludes that apparent benefits seem to overrun possible risks in the mind of adolescents, and therefore, account for risk-taking behaviors (Reyna & Farley, 2006). Lastly, I chose to include the article entitled “Risk Taking in Adolescence: New Perspectives from Brain and Behavioral Sciences (Steinberg, 2007).” This article proposes a developmental neuroscience approach to explaining adolescent risk taking. In addition to discussing the causes of these risky behaviors, this paper attempts to provide characteristics of solutions that would be successful at reducing risky behaviors among adolescents. Steinberg suggests that “heightened risk taking in adolescence is the product of the interaction between two brain networks (Steinberg, 2007).” The first is the socio-emotional network and the second is the cognitive-control network. The socio-emotional network deals with social and emotional arousal and is highly associated with puberty and hormonal changes. The cognitive-control network, however, is largely divorced from the cycles of puberty and is responsible for basic brain functioning and procedural cognitive regulation (Steinberg, 2007). The article also pays a great deal of attention to contextual situations in which adolescents make risky decisions. For example, Steinberg suggests that some risky behaviors may be partially due to the fact that adolescents are surrounded by their peers very frequently. Being in this environment makes the rewarding components of risky situations more prominent and desirable for adolescents (Steinberg, 2007). Relationship to Other Topics Risky decision making practices among adolescents and children have very clear relevance to other topics in the field of behavioral decision theory. Some of the literary pieces that I have previously referenced make specific in-text references to theories and concepts within the field of behavioral decision theory. Some of the main concepts that are specifically relevant to risky decision making among adolescents and/or children are: framing effects, de-contextualization, heuristics, and active, open-minded thinking. A large part of risky decisions made by children and/or adolescents can be attributed to the individual’s reliance upon heuristics. Judgment biases (heuristics) can be seen in the decision making skills of children as young as four years old. The frequency of using heuristics increases greatly during elementary school years, again in mid-adolescence, and once again in the onset of adulthood. However, risky decisions are more often made in childhood and adolescence; this is because although heuristics are used in adulthood, adults additionally experience an increase in social knowledge and an increase in ability to de-contextualize problems. Therefore, the key developmental issue is that children and adolescence often encounter the same amount of, for example, framing effects in decision making, but adults are more apt to overcome these heuristics due to their heightened ability to de-contextualize problems and engage in active, open-minded thinking. Much of the research about risky decision making among adolescents and children is focused on creating and changing public policy in order to decrease the frequency of risky decisions. The most prevalent recommendations of how to change policy are centered on changing the context in which adolescents are faced with potentially risky decisions. Although this is not de-contextualization, it is a form of altering context. One example of altering social context is raising the cost of cigarettes. For many adolescents, this would change the context of the situation in that it may no longer be a decision of whether or not they wish to participate in the risky behavior but more of an issue of not being able to afford the monetary cost of the behavior. The rewarding aspect of the behavior may now be less desirable due to the increased price of receiving that social reward (peer acceptance). Rebecca Ladd April 22, 2009 Works Cited Jacobs, J. E. & Klaczynski, P. A. (2002). The Development of Judgment and Decision Making During Childhood and Adolescence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 145-149. Reyna, V. F., & Farley, F. (2006). Risk and Rationality in Adolescent Decision Making; Implications for Theory, Practice, and Public Policy. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 7, No. 1 Steinberg, L. (2007) Risk taking in Adolescence: New Perspectives from Brain and Behavioral Sciences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 55-59