The Effect of Extensive Reading On Writing Style Instruction 廣泛閱讀效果對寫作風格教學之影響 張真維 Chen-wei Chang Over the past decades, there has been numerous studies reporting that extensive reading not only facilitates learners’ reading skills, language skills, but also personal growth and learning motivations. Among theses studies, Palmer, who was a pioneer to distinguish the intensive reading and extensive reading, stated that extensive reading (ER) means to read “rapidly” with “quantity” (Palmer, 1964, p.111, cited from Day & Bamford, 1998, p. 5). Richards and Platt (2003) also referred that “extensive reading means reading in quantity and in order to gain a general understanding of what is read.” “It is intended to develop good reading habits, to build up knowledge of vocabulary and structure, and to encourage a liking for reading.” (p. 168). In other words, students spend most of their time to read a lot of easy, enjoyable books without needing to answer comprehension questions, to write reports or translation. ER is much like the way people read for pleasure in their native language. For that reason, Day and Bamford (1998) suggest that it is best if the book is self-selected. Any material can be used, graded readers which present stories with controlled vocabulary and, at times, limited grammar and information flow are often used. These are books that can help readers become fluent, skilled readers. More specifically, many researches found that the benefits of extensive reading include gains in reading proficiency (Kutiper, 1983; Tudor & Hafiz, 1989; Robb & Susser, 1989; Nash & Yuan, 1992/1993), reading speed (Bell, 2001), reading habits (Nash & Yuan 1992/1993), listening proficiency (Eley & Mangubhai, 1983), speaking abilities (Hafiz & Tudor, 1990; Elley, 1991), writing proficiency (Haifiz & Tudor, 1989), positive affect (Krashen, 1993), and even spelling abilities (Krashen, 1989). These studies provide valuable insights and pedagogic implications for educators and teachers with desires to implement their instructions in the classroom. Moreover, the majority of researches have shown that extensive reading is the key to achieving higher reading proficiency (Krashen, 1993). Krashen (2004) have even suggested that free voluntary reading (FVR) is the key to students in improvement in reading skills, linguistic competence, vocabulary, spelling and writing. Researchers focus on the benefits of extensive reading program to the language learning of learners; some of them provide a specific perspective to argue that extensive reading leads to language knowledge that supports better writing abilities. For example, Elley and Mangubhai (1983) stated that extensive readers outperformed “on the tests of reading comprehension, vocabulary, oral language, grammar, listening comprehension, and writing” (p. 55). Yu and Wong (1991) indicated that ER motivates learners and leads to improvement in writing. Beniko Mason’s studies (Mason and Krashen 1997) also showed that extensive reading benefits learners in writing as well as reading. Lastly, Krashen (2004) also states that “those who report they read more write better” (p. 132). Those results summarized in the previous section have demonstrated that a large amount of reading (ER) can be means of comprehensible input to stimulate language acquisition. These results are consistent with the Input Hypothesis, which claims that “we acquire language by understanding messages, that ‘comprehensible input’ is the essential ingredient in language acquisition.” (Krashen, 1989, p. 440) In Krashen’s Input Hypothesis (1993), it assumes that learners can acquire “i+1” language (“i” represents students’ current level of competence, “i+1” means the difficulty of the language is just beyond the students’ current competence) if they understand “i+1” message. This claim, by receiving comprehensible input, states that “we acquire language by understanding input that is a little beyond our current level of acquired competence” (p. 32). On the contrary, Day and Bamford (1998) suggest that in order to develop L2 learners’ language proficiency, “i minus 1” material are better choice as they can provide L2 learners with linguistic exposure without too many “i+1” distractions. Hence, whether large amounts of comprehensible input “i+1” or “i-1” are necessary for significant language acquisition and that extensive reading with graded readers can be an enjoyable, motivating source of such input, depending on learners’ linguistic competence. In other words, in order to acquire language fluency and proficiency, learners need to encounter these unfamiliar words and linguistic forms repeatedly through reading of quantity. As a source of plentiful out-of-class input and a resource for interaction in the classroom, an extensive reading program can be a useful component of a course focusing on writing skills by accumulating the knowledge of the language and knowledge of writing system. Here, the effect of ER has provided L2 learners with more opportunities as a mean of comprehensible input to facilitate writing production (Output Hypothesis) in the exposure of the second language. Output Hypothesis, advanced by Merrill Swain, makes a claims that “output is necessary to increase fluency, that is, learners must practice producing second language [written form] if they are to learn to use their interlanguage system confidently and routinely” (Mitchell & Myles, 2004, p.174). For example, Stotsky (1983) and Krashen (1984) reviewed a number of L1 studies that appear to show the positive effect of reading on subjects' writing skills, indicating that students who are prolific readers in their pre-college years become better writers when they enter college. L2 studies by Hafiz & Tudor (1989) in the UK and Pakistan, and Robb & Susser (1989) in Japan, revealed more significant improvement in subjects' written work than in other language skills. These results shows that ER can lead to improvement in writing In addition, Mason and Krashen (1997) conducted two studies concerning the effect of extensive reading on writing. In the first study, two group from a prestigious university and two groups from a two-year college served as experimental group. The experimental groups’ cloze test gain scores were significantly greater than those of the control groups, which followed the traditional curriculum. In addition, students’ summaries written in English were rated by three native raters as good, average, and not good. Through out one academic year, the result showed that there was significant movement from average or not good to the category of good. In the second study, a much larger choice of books was available (over 3000). Two experimental groups used different languages (English vs. Japanese) to write their summaries and the control group was asked to do the extensive work on cloze exercise instead of following the traditional curriculum. The same 100 item cloze test used in the previous study was administered as pre- and post test and a comprehension test was used as post test only. The cloze test results showed that the extensive reading groups made better gains than the comparison group and that only the English response group showed the statistical significance. With respect to reading comprehension test, both extensive reading groups made significantly greater gains than the comparison group. Moreover, the writing results demonstrated that it was the Japanese response group that yielded greater gains on writing, which thus suggested that the improvement in writing did not come from more writing (output hypothesis), but from the input that extensive reading provided. This finding is consistent with Tsang’s study (1996), which found that the extensive reading group had better writing performance than the group that practiced writing. In Tsang's (1996) study, carried out in Hong Kong secondary schools, it provided further persuasive evidence of the effectiveness of extensive reading in fostering learners' language development. He found that “the reading program was significantly more effective than the writing program” (p. 225). Extensive reading programs can provide very effective platforms for promoting reading improvement and development from elementary levels upwards. Although they do require a significant investment in time, energy and resources on the part of those charged with managing the materials, the benefits in terms of language and skills development for the participating learners far outweigh the modest sacrifices required. If such programs receive institutional support and can be integrated into the curriculum so that they become agreed school policy, as suggested in Davis (1995), they will likely be more readily and widely adopted, particularly in countries where material and financial resources are adequate. Generalizing the above studies, Grabe (2003) proposed a hypothesis, called Extensive Reading Hypothesis, to depict the reading and writing relations. He states that “extensive reading leads to better vocabulary knowledge, better verbal fluency, better syntactic knowledge, better semantic memory, better metalinguistic awareness, and broader knowledge of the world” (p. 249). Based on the findings, he continues that “a major implication for reading and writing relation, …, is that connections between reading and writing may variable, but they can be interconnected more efficiently through extensive reading in combination with consistent writing practice” (p. 249). He concluded the relationship between reading (input) and writing (output) that they are both interdependent fundamental components in acquiring language proficiency. More specifically, Swain (1985) stated the input-out relationships at the level of language proficiency traits. Smith (1985) gave the theoretical explanations of how input becomes intake through the processes of interactions in our cognitive dimension. Moreover, Krashen (1989) proposed that “Output, especially writing, can have very positive cognitive consequences” (p. 456). In his recent book, The power of reading, he makes two crucial points: first is that “actual writing can help us solve problems and make us smarter” and second is that “writing style does not come from actual writing experience, but from reading”. (p. 132). In other words, it is referred that reading and writing facilitate mutually. Sufficient input in form of reading can stimulate writing productions, while output in form of writing can reinforce the logical thinking of reading. In conclusion, input hypothesis proposed by Krashen and output hypothesis proposed by Swain form a cyclic and interdependent correlation. Whereas the above research has reviewed numerous previous studies of effects of extensive reading program on language learning, a more specific perspective proposed by Krashen can be observed in the field of research on second language learners’ writing style that it is facilitated by extensive reading as means of input. Therefore, this study is to investigate that the direct effect of extensive reading program, as a mean of input, promotes learners’ writing proficiency, and as a production (output) of cognitive processes, leads to learners’ writing styles. References Bamford, J., & Day, R. R. (2004). Extensive reading activities for teaching language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Bell, T. (2001, April). Extensive reading: Speed and comprehension. The Reading Matrix, 1(1). Retrieved December 18, 200, from http://www.readingmatrix.com/ articles/bell/index.html Davis, C. (1995). Extensive reading: An expensive extravagance? ELT Journal, 49(4), 329-36. Day, R. R., & Bamford, J. (1998). Extensive reading in the second language classroom. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Day, R.R., & Bamford, J. (1998). Extensive reading: what is it? Why bother?. The Language Teacher Online, 21(5). Elley, W. B., & Mangubhai, F. (1983). The effect of reading on second language learning. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 53-67. Grabe, W. (1991). Current developments in second language reading research. TESOL Quarterly, 25(3), 375-406. Grabe, William. (2003). Reading and writing relations: second language perspectives on research and practice. In Barbara Kroll (Ed.), Exploring the dynamics of second language writing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Krashen, S. (1989). We acquire vocabulary and spelling by reading: additional evidence for the input hypothesis. The Modern Language Journal, 73(4), 440-464. Krashen, S. (1989). Language Acquisition and Language Education: Extensions and Applications. New York: Prentice Hall International. Krashen, S.(2004). The power of reading: Insights from the research (2nd ed.) Englewood, CA: Libraries Unlimited. Krashen, S., & Terrell, T. (1993). The Natural Approach. Oxford: Pergamon Alemany. Kutiper, K. (1983). Extensive reading: A means of reconciliation. English Journal, 72(7), 58-61. Mason, B., & Krashen, S. (1997). Extensive reading in English as a foreign language. System, 19(2), 31-55. Mitchell, Rodamond & Myles Florence. (1998). Second language learning theories. New York: Oxford University Press. Nash, T., & Yuan, Y. P. (1992/1993). Extensive reading for learning and enjoyment. TESOL Journal, 27-31. Nation, I. S. P. (1997, May 21). The language learning benefits of extensive reading. The Language Teacher Online. Retrived December 18, 2005, from http://www.jalt-publications.org/tlt/files/97/may/benefits.heml Renandya, W., & G. Jacobs. (2002). Extensive reading: why aren’t we all doing it? In J.C. Richards, & W. Renandya (Eds.), Methodology in Language Teaching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Richards, J. C. & Platt, John & Platt, Heidi. (Eds). (2003). Longman Dictionary of language teaching and applied linguistics. (3rd ed.). London: Longman. Robb, T. N., & Susser, B. (1989). Extensive reading vs. skills building in an EFL context. Reading in a Foreign Language, 5, 239-251. Swain, Merrill. (1985). Communicative competence: some roles of comprehensible input and comprehensible output in its development. In Susan M. Gass & Carolyn G. Madden (Eds.), Input in second language acquisition (pp. 235-253). Boston: Heinle & Heinle publishers. Smith, M. S. (1985). From input to intake: on arguementation in second language acquisition. In Susan M. Gass & Carolyn G. Madden (Eds.), Input in second language acquisition (pp. 394-403). Boston: Heinle & Heinle publishers. Tudor, I., & Haifiz, F. (1989). Extensive reading as a means of input to L2 learning. Journal of Research in Reading, 12(2), 164-78.