Academic Performance 1 Running head: ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE AND DISCIPLINE OF COLLEGE STUDENTS Academic Performance and Discipline of College Students in Relation to Prior Public or Non-Public High School Education Alex Beguiristain Loyola University New Orleans Academic Performance 2 Abstract The present study was conducted to determine if there was a relationship between college academic success and what type of high school (either public or non-public) an individual has attended. Forty-seven participants (36 women and 11 men) from introductory psychology and sociology classes were surveyed. The independent variable was the type of high school with two levels: public and non-public. The dependent variable was college academic success. Results supported the research hypothesis that there were no differences in college academic success between public and non-public high school graduates. The participants’ ratings about their study skills, approximate number of study hours spent per week on college study and desired high level degree were strong predictors of college academic success. Academic Performance 3 Academic Performance and Discipline of College Students in Relation to Prior Public or Non-Public High School Education Throughout recent decades, non-public high school officials have believed that their high schools promote higher academic achievement and discipline more effectively than public high schools. On the other hand, public high school officials believe that both public high schools and non-public schools are equally effective in helping their graduates academically succeed in college. Also, both public and non-public high schools have been shown to have different academic curriculums. Most private high schools have been shown to put more emphasis on college preparation while public high schools have been shown to put emphasis on labor force preparation (Coleman & Hoffer, 1987). In addition, private high schools have shown to offer more advanced science and math courses, such as algebra II, trigonometry, etc., than public high schools (The National Center for Education Statistics, 1997). Moreover, many psychologists have used the Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT) and the American College Test (ACT) to predict college academic success of both public and non-public high school students. Gibbins and Bickel (1991) did research to see whether or not there were any differences in academic achievement between public and private high schools. Two SAT data sets from Florida and a national SAT data set were analyzed (Gibbins & Bickel, 1991). The national data set included a 10% random sample of US high school students who took the SAT in the 1983-1984 high school year. The Florida data sets only included results from high school students who took the SAT during the 1982-1984 high school years. In addition, Gibbins and Bickel (1991) used a Student Descriptive Questionnaire (SDQ) to obtain non-academic variables, Academic Performance 4 such as gender, race, and socioeconomic status from their participants. The independent variable was the type of high school with two levels: public and private. The dependent variable was academic performance. The results showed SAT had two limitations. Firstly, only about half of all high school students take the SAT. Secondly, the SAT was originally designed to measure a high students’ aptitude rather than academic success (Gibbins and Bickel, 1991). Also, statistical results of the study did not prove that non-public high schools were more efficient than public high schools in instilling academic discipline and effective study skills in their graduates. Similarly, research has been done to see whether or not ACT composite scores from both private and public high school graduates predicted college academic success. The ACT measures a high school student’s knowledge of a variety of subjects, such as social studies, mathematics, natural sciences and English (Thompson, 1976). Thompson (1976) used ACT composite scores from a random sample of 996 of freshman and sophomores, to measure a college student’s mental capacity to do academic work. Also, Thompson (1976) tested the validity of the Student Attitudes Inventory (SAI) by combining its’ results with the participants’ ACT composite scores. The SAI was a type of questionnaire that measured an individual’s motivation, study methods, testing techniques, and the ability to do academic work without being distracted. The independent variables were the SAI scores while the dependent variable was college GPA. Statistical results showed that college success was easier to predict when the participants’ ACT composite scores were added with their SAI results. Likewise, non-intellectual factors, such as academic discipline, anxiety, study skills, and assessment tests scores, have been shown to be stronger predictors of college academic Academic Performance 5 success than the type of high school an individual has attended. McCausland and Stewart (1974) studied non-intellectual factors and randomly selected forty-seven male and fifty-nine female college freshmen students. The Study Habits Inventory (SHI), classroom attendance (ATT), the American College Test (ACT) and the participants’ first semester GPA were used to test the participants. Results showed that both a freshmen college student’s GPA and ACT composite score were the best predictors of academic success. In addition, statistical results showed that the relationship between non-academic factors, such as academic aptitude, study skills and academic discipline, and academic success was very difficult to comprehend. For this reason, we decided to conduct the present study to research whether or not there was an unknown relationship between non-intellectual factors and academic success of both private and public high school graduates. Another study conducted by Reiter (1964) tried to determine if there was a relationship between non-academic factors and academic success of college freshmen and sophomore students. Participants were 76 randomly selected male and female college students from an introductory psychology course. The independent variable was college success while the dependent variable was the college GPA. Statistical results showed that high school achievement was a superior predictor of academic success (Reiter, 1964). Also, Reiter (1964) stated that non-intellectual factors, such as anxiety, academic discipline, and SAT composite scores, were not proven to be true predictors of academic success. In the same way, Mouw and Khanna (1993) did a review study on predicting academic success and pointed out that the best predictors of academic success were high school academic performance and aptitude test performance on both the ACT and SAT tests. Also, non- Academic Performance 6 academic factors, such as social class, personality traits, part time employment, and attitudes toward an academic atmosphere were shown not to predict college success. The present study hypothesized that if both public and non-public high school graduates would have equal academic performance and discipline rates then a high school graduate’s academic performance and discipline in college were determined by non-academic factors, such as the readiness to learn, study skills, and attitudes. Method Participants Forty-seven under-graduate university students (36 women and 11 men, mean age = 19 years) volunteered to participate. All participants were recruited using convenience sampling and were given extra credit from their professors. Also, all participants were treated in compliance to the “ Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct” (American Psychological Association, 1992). Materials The participants who consented to do the consent forms were asked to provide some demographic information about themselves such as in what state they had attended high school. Also, the participants’ academic success and discipline were measured by their responses to fifteen impersonal questions dealing with their former high schools and their college life such as checking off an option that best corresponded to how they felt about their high schools’ and Loyola’s academic standards. In addition, the participants were asked to provide their Academic Performance 7 approximate GPA and ACT or SAT scores. The questionnaire that was used in the present study can be found in the appendix section of this paper. Design and Procedure The study was a single variable, quasi-experiment with a between subjects design. The independent variable was the type of high school, with two levels: public and non-public schools. The dependent variable was college academic performance which was measured by the participants’ GPA. The control used in the present study was eliminating participants who were either older than 23 years of age or attended a high school outside of US territory. When the time came to perform the experiment, we first requested various Loyola faculty to announce to their classes that a project that wished to study college success based on the high school attended needed volunteers to participate in the study. A sheet of times was distributed for the students to sign up for the study. Participants were tested in classrooms on the Loyola University campus. The study took place on weekdays between the hours of 11:00 am and 7:00 pm during Spring 2001. Before consent forms were distributed, the participants were informed that the experiment that they were about to participate in only required them to fill out a 15minute survey questionnaire. Two consent forms were distributed and participants were told that if they wanted the results of the experiment to write either their e-mail or residence address on one of the consent forms that was handed in with a written signature. After all the participants consented and signed their consent forms, we collected the consent forms and distributed survey packets. Moreover, the survey asked participants to answer some demographic information and questions about the high school they attended and Loyola University. After all consent forms Academic Performance 8 were collected, the participants were debriefed. Proof of participation sheets with our signatures were distributed to all the participants to turn in to their professors for extra credit. Results The present study supported the research hypothesis that stated that both public and private high school graduates would achieve equal college academic performance. Analysis provided descriptive statistical data for the participants’ number of A’s earned in their most recent semester (M = 2.9, SD = 2.5), GPA (M =3.2, SD = .64), and approximate number of hours devoted to their college study (M = 9.4, SD = 7.4). The means for the participants’ ratings about their high school and college academic standards, college satisfaction, approximate number of study hours devoted to their college academic study, SAT composite scores, ratings about their study skills, and desired high level degree are listed in Table 1. An independent groups t-test was performed comparing the means, for several variables none the participants’ number of A’s earned in their most recent semester, (t (40) = -.613, p = .543), GPA, (t (40) = -.263,p = .794), approximate number of study hours devoted to their college academic study, (t (38.759) = -.896, p = .376), ratings about their high school standards,(t (40) = 1.169, p = .249), college standards, (t (40) = -.106, p = .916), approximate SAT composite scores, (t (40) =1.107, p = .275), desired high level degree, (t (40) = -1.414 , p = .165), and ratings about their study skills, (t (40) = .015, p = .988).The alpha level maintained was .05. Also, a Pearson correlation addressed the relationship between a participant’s GPA (M = 3.2, SD = .64) and rating about his or her college academic standards (M = 3.6, SD = .85). For an alpha level of .05, the correlation was found to be statistically significant (r (40) = -.313, p < .05), indicating that these two variables were positively related. Additionally, the Academic Performance 9 regression equation had an alpha level maintained at .05 which showed that the prediction of college academic success (GPA) with the use of a participant’s rating of his or her study skills, approximate SAT composite score, approximate number of study hours, and desired high level college degree was significant, (F (6, 35) = 2.462, p = .043). In addition, the estimated standard error of estimate was .5853. Discussion The present study supported the research hypothesis which stated that there were no differences in college academic success between public and non-public high school graduates. Also, there were many similarities and differences in results between the present study and past research studies. Gibbins and Bickel (1991) analyzed two SAT data sets from Florida and one national data sheet which contained a 10% random sample of US high school students. However, the present study non-randomly surveyed 47 participants’ approximate SAT composite scores. For this reason, Gibbins’ and Bickel’s (1991) statistical results for their participants’ SAT composite scores were more accurate than the present study’s statistical results. Similarly, both the present study and Gibbins’ and Bickel’s (1991) study did not find any sufficient evidence to prove that non-public high schools were more efficient than public high schools in instilling academic discipline and effective study skills in their graduates. Additionally, Thompson (1976) analyzed ACT composite scores from a random sample of 996 freshman and sophomores, to measure a college student’s mental capacity to do academic work. On other hand, the present study had a non-random selection of 47 participants who provided more SAT composite scores than ACT composite scores. As a Academic Performance 10 result, the present study only used the participants’ approximate SAT composite scores. The participants who did not take the SAT had their ACT composite scores converted into SAT composite score equivalents with the use a converter that was obtained from the Internet (College Board Online, 2001). In addition, results from the present study showed that nonacademic factors, such as a participant’s ratings about his or her study skills and desired high level degree, were found to be good predictors of college academic success. Moreover, the present study had many shortcomings. The survey in the present study did not have a sufficient number of questions that measured a participant’s non-academic factors, such as, academic attitudes, socioeconomic status, and their readiness to learn. In addition, the present study had a small sample with a small pool of variance. In addition, we predicted that future research would uncover evidence about the relationship between an individual’s non-academic factors and his or her academic success in college. Similarly, the present study has many practical implications. We believed that various college and university administrators throughout the US could use our findings to make better predictions about which of their applicants were more likely not to drop out of college after the first semester of their freshman year. Also, the present study’s primary objective was to provide sufficient evidence that would disprove the biased notion that non-public high school graduates achieved greater college academic success than public high school graduates. Equally important, analysis of variance showed that an individual’s approximate number of study hours that he or she spent on college academic study was the only variable that showed significance, for the measurement of college academic success. As a result, the extent of an individual’s study skills was a predictor of their college academic success. In Academic Performance 11 addition, the sample mean of study hours for non-public high school graduates was two points higher than the sample mean of public high schools graduates. We suggested that non-public high school graduates probably tended to study longer than public high school graduates do. Along with the theoretical implications of the present study, we suggested improvements and ideas for new future research. The present study lacked significant scores that generalized the total US population of public and non-public high school graduates who attended college. As a result, more participants need to be surveyed from various colleges or universities from either LA or other US states. Academic Performance 12 References Coleman, J.S., & Hoffer, T. (1987). Public and Private High Schools. New York: Basic Books, Inc. Publishers. Gibbins, N., & Bickel, R. (1991). Comparing public and private high schools using three SAT data sheets. The Urban Review, 23 , 101-115. McCausland, D. F., & Stewart, N. E. (1974). Academic aptitude, study skills, and attitudes and college GPA. Journal of Educational Research,67, 354-357. Mouw, J.T., & Khanna, R. K. (1993). Prediction of academic success: A review of the literature and some recommendations. Psychological Review,27, 328-336. Public and private schools: How do they differ? (July 1997). National Center for Education Statistics, http://nces.ed.gov/pubs97/97983.html Reiter, H.R. (1964). Psychological Reports,15, 23-26. SAT I-ACT Score Comparisons (March 2001). College Board Online, http://www.collegeboard.org/index_this sat/cbsenior/html/stat00f.html Thompson, M.E. (1976). The prediction of academic achievement by a British study habits inventory. Research In Higher Education, 5, 365-371. Academic Performance 13 Academic Performance 14 Academic Performance 15