Running Head: DEFENSIVE CONVICTION AS EMOTION REGULATION Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation: Goal Mechanisms and Interpersonal Implications 1 Denise C. Marigold, 2Ian McGregor, & 1Mark P. Zanna 1 University of Waterloo 2 York University Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 2 In this chapter we describe research on defensive conviction as a reaction to personal uncertainty and other poignant self-threats. We argue that uncertainty and self-threats are aversive because they block important personal goals, and that some people cope by exaggerating conviction about alternative goals and ideals. Goal-related, neuropsychological mechanisms are suggested for how such seemingly compensatory conviction can serve as a form of emotion regulation: Approaching a compelling goal or ideal may inhibit the distress that ensues when another meaningful goal is blocked. This mechanism has been proposed in recent articles (McGregor, 2006a, 2006b; McGregor, Gailliot, Vasquez, & Nash, in press), but here we delve into it further and discuss some unpublished supporting data. We also highlight the implications of defensive conviction for interpersonal relationships, which have not been discussed elsewhere. People often pursue relationships as goals and thus relationships may be a source of conviction after threat. Alternatively, relationships may suffer if conviction is focused on domains that are not shared with relationship partners. Most people have broad, important goals toward which they strive in their daily behavior. These may include, for example, goals to be correct and competent, to have a coherent identity, and to have close and satisfying relationships. According to Carver and Scheier (1998; elaborated from Powers, 1973), these kinds of broad goals or “system concepts” are the highest ideals and values about what to approach. Hierarchically nested under idealized system concepts are more specific principles and values, and then lower-level behavioral goals. The key to this chapter is that ideals and values are kinds of abstract goals that can be promoted by pursuing lower level behavioral goals. For example, studying hard for an exam would be one way to approach the goal of competence; doing something nice for one’s partner promotes the goal of being loving and lovable. One may even “approach” a high level goal simply by fostering Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 3 conviction for it. From this perspective, system concepts and value principles are crucial for selfregulation, and are thus of intense motivational significance because they act as “self-guides” (Van Hook & Higgins, 1988) for enactment of the pyramid of subordinate behavioral goals.1 Knowing how to prioritize self-guides can be a challenge, however. Conflict between valued selves is aversive (Van Hook & Higgins, 1988), and abstract quandaries about relative value cannot easily be solved by appeals to objective facts. People require interpersonal validation for setting such priorities (cf. Festinger, 1954; Hardin & Higgins, 1996; Shah, Kruglanski, & Thompson, 1998). Accordingly, consensual ideals promoted through groupidentifications and shared cultural meanings have powerful motivational significance and serve as pivotal guides for self-regulation. Members of one’s cultural group are a primary source of social comparison information and validation of personal beliefs and values (Festinger, 1954; Shah et al., 1998). Close relationship partners, too, can be a ready resource for construction and validation of personal identities (Swann, 1987; Swann & Predmore, 1985) and can shape movement toward one’s ideal self (Drigotas, Rusbult, Wieselquist, & Whitton, 1999). Because of their vital bolstering function, cultural or relational sources of consensus may be as crucial as the convictions that they support. Indeed, the research that will be described in this chapter indicates that when high-level self-goals are threatened by uncertainty or other selfthreats, people defensively exaggerate seemingly compensatory sources of both conviction (certainty that one’s opinions are correct) and consensus (certainty that other people agree with one’s opinions) with equivalent fervor.2 For example, they exaggerate conviction for attitudes about social issues after stewing in uncertainty about a personal dilemma or troubled close relationship (McGregor & Marigold, 2003; McGregor, Zanna, Holmes, & Spencer, 2001), they inflate estimates of consensus for their social issue opinions following a failure experience Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 4 (McGregor, Nail, Marigold, & Kang, 2005), and they display more ingroup bias than usual following mortality salience, uncertainty, and system injustice threats (McGregor et al., 2001; McGregor et al., in press; McGregor et al., 2005). Why do people react to uncertainty and other self-threats with apparently compensatory conviction and consensus? Goals Disruption and Uncertainty Our understanding of why goal-threats cause defensive conviction and consensus is based in Jeffrey Gray’s work on the neuropsychology of anxiety and the behavioral inhibition system (BIS; Gray & McNaughton, 2000). The BIS supports effective goal pursuit by raising anxiety and discouraging persistence at a compromised goal. BIS activation following any goal disruption is essentially a state of aroused uncertainty. When the BIS is activated, approach motivation remains prominent, but inhibition processes join in to create a motivational conflict. BIS anxiety and arousal are activated in scenarios in which an animal does not clearly need to immediately fight or flee from a threat. Clear fight, flight, and panic responses are mediated by a different brain system (Gray & McNaughton, 2000). Other unequivocal avoidance states are also mediated by different brain systems. For example, the BIS is active when rats smell a predator, but not when they are confronted with an actual predator (Gray & McNaughton, 2000).With BIS activation the animal remains in an inhibited approach orientation, with aroused attention vigilantly focused for awareness of threat cues, solutions, or tenable alternative goals. When the “alarm system” is alerted in this way, threatening cues attract indordinate perceptual attention, and elicit more intense emotional reactions than usual (Van den Bos, Euwema, Poortfliet, & Maas, in press; Van den Bos et al., 2007; Van den Bos, Van Ameidje, & Van Gorp, 2006). The anxious arousal also translates into an invigoration of efforts toward the focal goal, until it is Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 5 either accomplished, until flight becomes clearly necessary, or until an alternative goal pursuit is unequivocally re-engaged (Klinger, 1975). It is easy to see the adaptiveness of such a process for simple goals. If a hungry rat running toward food were to encounter an insurmountable obstacle, it would be adaptive for the rat to inhibit and disengage from its initial plan, scan for alternative routes, and then engage an alternative plan that skirted the obstacle. Vertebrates with lesions to brain areas related to the BIS fail to disengage from blocked goals. Various anti-anxiety medications have the same effect as these lesions. They alleviate anxiety associated with uncertainty and disrupted goals, but they can also compromise appropriate disengagement (Gray & McNaughton, 2000). Humans share the same BIS circuitry as rats, and our prefrontal cortex has elaborate projections to and from the BIS. These circuits allow for abstract, conceptual goals and ideals to be regulated the same way concrete goals are, by basic BIS processes. The main argument of this chapter is that the tendency for humans to exaggerate idealized convictions and consensus responses after various self-threats can be explained by the mechanics of the BIS. Our argument is rooted in Carver and Scheier’s (1998; cf. Powers, 1973) depiction of ideal system concepts, which map onto core values, relationship ideals, and cultural meanings, as high-level abstract goals. Such abstract ideals and self-guides are not only motivationally super-ordinate, they are also resistant to habituation, disillusionment, or factual refutation (cf. Klinger, 1977). As such, when humans have their BIS activated by important goal disruptions, they can reliably turn toward the alternative goals of ideological conviction and consensus to deactivate the BIS. It is important to note that the turn toward idealized conviction as an alternative goal does not function as a substitute for the blocked goal, but rather as a beacon for reallocation of motivational resources toward an unrelated and therefore wholly uncompromised alternative. Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 6 Accordingly, our main proposal is that ideological zeal is ubiquitously appealing for humans because it can provide a reliable beacon to approach when one is mired in uncertainty or anxiety. The defensive conviction reactions described in the following section represent this sort of reactive tendency of some individuals to quell the anxiety of goal disruption by fervent promotion of alternative goals and ideals in other domains. Doing so effectively re-engages the sanguine processes related to focused and unfettered goal pursuit, and relieves the distress associated with behavioral inhibition. Although we see uncertainty as a very poignant self-threat (Van den Bos & Lind, this volume), in this chapter we subsume it with other self-threats like failure, separation, and mortality salience, under the rubric of compromised high-level goals. The similarity of defensive responses to all these threats has prompted us to group them together in this manner (McGregor, 2006a). It is possible that these other self-threats also operate through uncertainty, but that point remains to be clarified in future research, and is not critical to the present argument.3 For our purposes, following Gray and McNaughton (2000), we will equate goal blockage and uncertainty as psychologically equivalent because they both activate the BIS and produce anxious discomfort. Defensive Conviction and Consensus A now considerable amount of research in our labs has shown that when goals are jeopardized by uncertainty or by some other threatening situation (e.g., a conflict with a partner threatens the goal for a close relationship; poor performance on a school task threatens an achievement goal), some people turn toward seemingly defensive sources of conviction and consensus. In some studies, a goal threat condition was created by reminding participants about an uncertain situation in their own lives. For example, after ruminating about an unsolved personal dilemma, conflicting life choices, or a troubled close relationship, participants had more Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 7 conviction about their self-concepts, values, personal projects, relationships, and attitudes about social issues (Marigold & Holmes, 2006; McGregor et al., 2001, Studies 1 and 2; McGregor & Marigold, 2003, Studies 1-3). In other studies, a goal threat condition was created in the lab. For example, after being asked to summarize a confusing excerpt from an advanced statistics textbook, or to imagine living in a foreign country separated from all their loved ones, undergraduate students inflated the extremity, conviction, and estimates of consensus for their opinions on issues like capital punishment, abortion, the US invasion of Iraq, and suicide bombing (McGregor et al., 2005, Studies 1 and 2; McGregor & Jordan, 2007). They also expressed greater determination to succeed at their most identity-defining goals (McGregor & Jordan, 2006). Another threat that has been studied extensively is mortality salience (Greenberg, Solomon, & Pyszczynski, 1997). This is particularly relevant to our conceptualization of defensive conviction as a response to blocked goals, as death is the ultimate goal disruptor. Concrete plans and most high-level goals that people pursue in daily life are terminated by death. Goals for certainty and understanding are undermined (despite sometimes fervent hopes, no one knows for sure what happens after death), and goals for self-worth, love, and inclusion are rendered absurd upon consideration of one’s personal transience. In our lab, mortality salience has been shown to increase identity-seeking, communal construal of identity, and determination to succeed at idiosyncratic personal goals (McGregor et al., 2001, Study 4; McGregor et al, in press, Study 2). In numerous other studies, mortality salience has been shown to promote ingroup favoritism, outgroup stereotyping, self-esteem striving (Solomon, Greenberg, & Pyszczynski, 2004), and motivation to develop relationships (Mikulincer, Florian, & Hirschberger, 2004). It seems that when goals are imagined to be blocked by death, people cling Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 8 more strongly to the same kinds of alternative goals and ideals that they turn to after other selfthreats: convictions, self-esteem, relationships, and groups (see McGregor, 2006a, for review). Further support for this interpretation comes from a recent study showing that mortality salience increased some people’s intentions to pursue personal goals (i.e., made them more likely to list as upcoming activities those that are in service of their long-term goals; Landau, Greenberg, & Kosloff, this volume). Related research paradigms also show that self-threats cause a variety of proud and idealistic “fluid compensatory” responses (cf. Steele, 1988). For example, uncertainty increases ingroup identification (Hogg, 2007) and failure increases outgroup derogation (Crocker, Thompson, McGraw, & Ingerman, 1987). Several theorists have noted the apparent substitutability of various defenses in response to self-threats and have explained these phenomena in terms of maintaining meaning, self-esteem, and security. In their “meaning maintenance model,” Heine, Proulx, and Vohs (2006) argue that when people’s sense of meaning is threatened by diverse threats, they reaffirm alternative sources of meaning. Tesser (2000) concluded from his self-evaluation maintenance research that when an aspect of the self is threatened, people maintain equanimity by affirming a different animal in their “self-zoo.” Hart, Shaver, and Goldenberg (2005) recently proposed a tripartite security system, based on evidence that close relationships, self-esteem, and cultural belief systems provide alternative means of boosting security and reducing anxiety about personal vulnerabilities, including death. They showed that threats to one component of the system elicit apparently compensatory defenses from the other components (e.g., attachment threats produced worldview and self-esteem defenses, and worldview or self-esteem threats produced attachment defenses). Similarly, system justification research shows that people defensively cling to conservative ideologies in the face Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 9 of situational threats or dispositional vulnerabilities (Jost, Glaser, Kruglanksi, & Sulloway, 2003; Kay & Gaucher, 2006). All of these perspectives are consistent with our view of defensive conviction. In every case, various self-threats cause a more tenacious adherence to some alternative personal agenda or ideal. We propose that basic BIS processes may underlie all of these models. When people’s important goals are disrupted, anxious vigilance is relieved by zealous promotion of reliable idealized goals. Emerging evidence about the roles of explicit and implicit self-esteem support this emotion-regulation view of defensive conviction. Who Is Most Defensive? The Roles of Explicit and Implicit Self-Esteem A recent unpublished study (Marigold & Holmes, 2006) will illustrate how explicit selfesteem (i.e., measured with self-report scales) typically moderates defensive conviction reactions to threats. In this study, 83 undergraduate psychology students were randomly assigned to either an identity threat or control condition. Participants in the threat condition were told to “Think about what your life will be like in five to seven years, some time after you have graduated from the University of Waterloo.” They were asked to list two different career options and describe the interests, values, and goals that might be given up by pursuing one over the other. They repeated this procedure for places they might live in order to fully immerse them in uncertainty about the future. Participants in the control condition received a modified version of this task. They were asked to complete the same procedure for someone else, specifically, a celebrity who was in the news often. They were asked to imagine what this person’s life would be like in five to seven years, and they completed materials parallel to the threat condition. For the main dependent variables, participants rated the intimacy of their two closest friendships: How positively their friends regarded them, and how understood they felt by their Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 10 friends. They also rated how certain they were that they would still be close to each of these friends five years from now and beyond, which invokes the ideal of true friendship. Results showed that all threatened participants reported more intimacy in their close friendships than did control participants. Importantly, however, only participants with high self-esteem exaggerated confidence about the ideal of true friendship with increased longevity ratings in the threat condition. This finding is consistent with our previous research on reactions to uncertainty and other threats as a function of explicit self-esteem (McGregor et al., 2005; McGregor et al., in press; McGregor & Marigold, 2003). In this previous research, individuals with high self-esteem have consistently been most inclined toward defensive promotion of zealous ideals after various threats (see also McGregor, 2006a). Other research suggests that it comes more naturally for high self-esteem individuals to mount approach-related defenses to threats. People with high selfesteem tend to be dispositionally approach-motivated (Heimpel, Elliot, & Wood, 2006) and promotion-focused (Leonardelli, Lakin, & Arkin, in press), which orients them toward identifying ideal goals and moving towards desired outcomes (Amodio, Shah, Sigelman, Brazy, & Harmon-Jones, 2004). As well, promotion focus increases self-esteem strivings (Leonardelli & Lakin, this volume). Our proposed link between high explicit self-esteem and defensive approach-motivation is further supported by a recent study indicating individuals with high self-esteem, but not those with low self-esteem, reacted to both mortality salience and uncertainty threats with heightened zeal for their most central personal goals (McGregor & Jordan, 2006). Moreover, the same study showed that the individuals with high self-esteem reacted to these threats with a pattern of brain activity that is characteristic of approach motivation (i.e., relative left frontal activation; to be Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 11 discussed later in this chapter). Similarly, other researchers have shown that individuals with high but not low explicit self-esteem responded to mortality salience with increased promotionfocus; they were more like to pursue an opportunity to demonstrate excellence despite a substantial risk of failure (Landau, Greenberg, & Kosloff, this volume). In addition to evidence that high explicit self-esteem is a predisposition toward defensive conviction reactions to threats, low implicit self-esteem appears to be a personal vulnerability that also inclines people toward defensive conviction. Low implicit self-esteem, as assessed by the Implicit Association Test (Greenwald & Farnham, 2000), measures experiential, less consciously elaborated associations of self with goodness. It is typically not correlated with explicit self-esteem. In three published studies we have found that various threats cause people with low implicit self-esteem, but not high implicit self-esteem, to exaggerate conviction, consensus, and extremity about ideological opinions (McGregor & Marigold, 2003, Study 3; McGregor et al., 2005, Study 1; McGregor & Jordan, 2007). Moreover, in two studies, participants with low implicit self-esteem were particularly likely to react to a self-threat with exaggerated zeal for their core personal goals, and also with patterns of brain activity characteristic of approach-motivation (McGregor & Jordan, 2006). These findings help to explain why, in two published studies, we have found three-way interaction effects with most defensive conviction among threatened participants with the combination of low implicit selfesteem and high explicit self-esteem (McGregor & Marigold, 2003, Study 3; McGregor et al., 2005, Study 1). Such individuals are not only dispositionally vulnerable to threats, but are also dispositionally inclined toward promotion of ideals. Our findings are consistent with foundational research which shows that individuals with high explicit self-esteem and low implicit self-esteem are particularly prone toward various forms of defensiveness, including Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 12 dissonance reduction and prejudicial reactions to outgroup members (Jordan, Spencer, & Zanna, 2005; Jordan, Spencer, Zanna, Hoshino-Browne, & Correll, 2003; Logel, Spencer, Wood, Holmes, & Zanna, 2006). These findings for explicit and implicit self-esteem are also consistent with an emotion-regulation view of defensive zeal. Defensive Conviction and Consensus as Emotion Regulation Our proposal that defensive conviction is a kind of promotion-focused emotionregulation4 is supported by evidence that people with high explicit self-esteem, who are most likely to engage in defensive conviction, are most motivated to repair their moods (Heimpel, Wood, Marshall, & Brown, 2002; Smith & Petty, 1995). They also tend to be more focused on the promotion and approach of ideal incentives (Heimpel, Elliot, & Wood, 2006; Leonardelli, Lakin, & Arkin, in press), particularly when threatened (Cavallo, Fitzsimons, & Holmes, 2006; McGregor et al., in press). In line with these findings on promotion focus, and consistent with the foundational work on the operation of the BIS, one promising explanation for how conviction relieves threats relates to Higgins’s (2005) theory of regulatory fit. Higgins has found that experiences seem most compelling when they resonate with individuals’ current regulatory focus. When individuals are dispositionally or situationally promotion-focused (i.e., oriented towards ideals and goals for hopes, aspirations, growth, and accomplishment), they overvalue phenomena related to approaching ideals and incentives, and undervalue phenomena related to preventing or avoiding unwanted outcomes. In our view, a corollary of Higgins’ theory of regulatory fit is that experiences should seem less compelling when they do not fit with active regulatory focus. That is, in a promotion-focused state, the distress of prevention-related anxiety arising from blocked goals should seem more remote. Indeed, some theorists have proposed that approach-motivation, Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 13 which is strongly related to promotion focus (Amodio et al., 2004) is a basic form of emotion regulation in humans and even in rats (Jackson et al., 2003; Sullivan, 2004; Tomarken & Keener, 1998). Such “offensive” strategies (McGregor, 2006a) may be superior to defensive attempts to simply suppress or distract oneself from unwanted thoughts, which can backfire and cause rebound hyperaccessibility of the unwanted thoughts (Wegner, 1994; cf. Greenberg, Arndt, Schimel, Pyszczynski, & Solomon, 2001). Accordingly, a number of studies have found that expressions of personal ideals take people’s minds off of threat-related thoughts. In one study (McGregor, 2006b), participants ruminated on a personal dilemma about which they were uncertain. Next, participants assigned to the “value conviction” condition were asked to write about their highest value and describe how they act consistently with it. Control participants wrote about their least important value and why it might be important to someone else. Participants in the value conviction condition later rated their personal dilemmas as less subjectively salient (i.e., less important and preoccupying) than did participants who were not given an opportunity to express value conviction. Interestingly, this effect was most pronounced on the last three items of the eight-item subjective salience scale. That is, the threat-muffling effects of conviction were actually strongest when the threat had been made highly accessible by repeated reminders. Thus, it seems that conviction does not simply function as a distracter, but rather it allows people to motivationally disengage from preoccupation with an accessible threat. Related studies have found that expressing conviction for idealistic opinions, romantic relationships, and self-worth also functions to relieve preoccupation with distressing thoughts (McGregor, 2004; McGregor, 2006b; McGregor & Marigold, 2003, Study 4).5 Similarly, terror management researchers have found that defensive expressions of consensual worldviews after Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 14 mortality salience serve to decrease the prominence of death-related thoughts (Greenberg, et al., 2001). Thus, avoidance-related anxiety arising from blocked goals does seem to be effectively reduced by promoting alternative goals and ideals with zealous enthusiasm. One might wonder whether people’s initial response to a blocked goal would be to increase effort and persistence at it. Indeed such anxious invigoration is the norm, at least immediately after the first signs of goal disruption, while anxiety is still manageable (Klinger, 1975). But when people begin to doubt their ability to overcome the obstacle, and anxiety has become highly uncomfortable, they are more likely to disengage from the goal, especially if an alternative is salient. In our model, we propose that initial invigoration is followed by withdrawal of motivational resources from the active pursuit of the blocked goal and reinvestment (via compensatory conviction) in the active pursuit of alternative goals (and convictions) in other life domains. Our research to date has not programmatically addressed the time course of compensatory conviction. However, based on related research we speculate that it would be more evident when there is a delay between the threat and goal conviction opportunity. Other researchers have found a delay/distraction task to be necessary to elicit defensive reactions following uncertainty threats (Wichman, Brunner, & Weary, 2006) and mortality salience (Greenberg, Arndt, Simon, Pyszczynski, & Solomon, 2000). Accordingly, we have always included a delay in our experiments. Future research should programmatically address the time course of compensatory conviction. It may be that there is an initially increased anxious perseverance at the blocked goal, during which time compensatory convictions are muted, followed by a period of compensatory conviction during which time concern about the blocked goal is muted (cf., Klinger, 1975). In our studies so far, participants have never been given the opportunity to demonstrate increased Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 15 resolve to overcome the blocked goal, but this could certainly be investigated in future research. For example, after a failure threat in which undergraduate psychology students are asked to summarize a highly complex and confusing statistical passage (shown to elicit a compensatory conviction response; McGregor et al., 2005), participants could be asked in another context about the desirability of signing up for a free statistics workshop offered by the department. The option could be offered immediately following the threat or after a delay. If threatened participants indeed showed greater interest in signing up for the workshop, particularly when asked immediately after the threat as opposed to after a delay, this would provide support for the idea that there is indeed an initial phase of invigorated goal perseverance before the compensatory conviction. Such a finding could provide an intriguing goal-regulation account for the divergent proximal (e.g., denial) and distal (i.e., compensatory) defenses that have been found in response to mortality salience threats (Pyszczynski, Greenberg, & Solomon, 1999). The Neuropsychology of Compensatory Conviction At a neuropsychological level, some insight into the appeal of conviction as an emotion regulation strategy can be gleaned from research on the dynamics of relative cerebral hemisphericity. A review by Martin and Shrira (2005) showed that across a number of studies, perceiving and ruminating about experientially threatening phenomena was associated with relative right frontal cerebral hemisphere activation, which is also associated with avoidance motivation. Relative left hemisphere activation, on the other hand, was associated with value affirmations, stereotyping, self-reported purpose and meaning in life, as well as approach motivation. Promotion focus (which predicts approach-related behaviors; Amodio et al., 2004) has also been shown to be positively related to EEG activity in the left frontal cortex and negatively related to EEG activity in the right frontal cortex (Amodio et al., 2004). Thus, zealous Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 16 ideals may serve to shift processing from right-hemisphere-mediated anxious rumination about threats to left-hemisphere-mediated approach-focus and insulation from threats. Indeed, chronic relative left hemisphere activation has been associated with attenuated startle responses after unpleasant stimuli, a dispositional tendency towards repression of negative thoughts, and greater happiness and meaning in life (Jackson et al., 2003; Tomarken & Davidson, 1994; Urry et al., 2004). Other evidence suggesting that people may mask threats by defensively engaging lefthemisphere-mediated approach motivation comes from research on anxiety. Threat and anxiety experiences are associated with relative right hemisphere activation (Friedman & Forster, 2005, Study 3; Lee et al., 2004; Nitschke, Heller, Palmieri, & Miller, 1999; Tucker, Roth, Arneson, & Buckingham, 1977), but dispositionally anxious people actually show chronic relative left hemisphere activation (see Heller, Nitschke, & Miller, 1998 for review). Dispositional insecurity is also associated with conservative and closed patterns of thinking (Jost et al., 2003), which are relatively left-hemisphere mediated (Martin & Shrira, 2005). Together, these finding are consistent with the idea that dispositionally anxious people may preemptively occupy themselves with patterns of thinking and acting that prime left hemisphere processes in order to minimize right-hemisphere-mediated anxious experience. Indeed, there is preliminary evidence that lefthemisphere-mediated approach motivation processes actively inhibit right-hemisphere-mediated avoidance motivation processes (Amodio et al., 2004; Drevets & Raichle, 1998; Jackson et al., 2003; Schiff & Bassel, 1996; Tomarken & Keener, 1998). Thus, when important goals are blocked, zealously promoting ideal values, goals, groups, attitudes, and relationships with conviction may be rewarding because doing so can reliably inhibit vigilant preoccupation with the threatened goal through asymmetrical hemisphere Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 17 activation (see also McGregor, 2006a). Accordingly, one recent study showed that after being confronted with personal uncertainty or mortality salience (which should be processed in the right hemisphere; Martin & Shrira, 2005), people with high explicit self-esteem (who, recall, are most invested in mood repair and most inclined toward defensive zeal) expressed more determination for their core personal goals, and showed an increase in relative left hemisphere activation (McGregor & Jordan, 2006). Consequences for Interpersonal Relationships Most research on the interpersonal consequences of defensive conviction and consensus has focused on implications for relations within and between groups. Self-threats like uncertainty and mortality salience have been shown to increase conviction for one’s own opinions and values and decrease tolerance for the opinions and values of outgroup members (see Hogg, 2007; McGregor, 2004; and Solomon et al., 2004, for reviews). To the extent that people are defensively cleaving to their ingroups to bolster their ideals and meanings, perspective-taking and dialogue between rival groups will be impeded. The threats experienced during inter-group conflicts may further inflame each group’s defense of their worldview and the dismissal of the other’s. Furthermore, intergroup anger is an approach-related phenomenon (Harmon-Jones & Sigelman, 2001), which may be recruited to further activate approach-related processes for relieving distress. Given that close relationships are a core source of uncertainty and conflict, and are also central to people’s high-level goals for belongingness, meaning, validation, and self-worth (see McGregor et al., 2001), similar processes may play out in the realm of close relationships as well. People may defensively enhance close relationships that support the self, but become defensively closed and hostile toward relationships that compete with salient goals. In this final Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 18 section, we review some research and speculate about the implications of defensive conviction for close relationships, for better and for worse. On the positive side, relationships may be the very goal or ideal that people fix their determination on after they have been threatened. In this way, defensive conviction may bolster established relationships. It may also allow people to hopefully exaggerate the potential of a new relationship in its early stages, when trusting in a partner’s caring and responsiveness to needs requires a leap of faith (Murray, Holmes, & Collins, 2006; Sorrentino, Holmes, Hanna & Sharp, 1995). Bolstering such faith could encourage people to commit to the relationship and behave in ways that foster security and closeness. For example, they might be more likely disclose selfdoubts and seek support, excuse or forgive partner transgressions, and view the relationship in the best possible light (Murray et al., 2006). Research has shown that when faced with a threat that is unrelated to relationships, people do often turn to their relationships for solace. In numerous studies, mortality salience has been shown to promote more communal identifications, greater willingness to initiate social interactions, a preference for love styles that foster intimate and committed relationships, and more commitment to current relationships (e.g., McGregor et al. 2001, Studies 2 and 4; see Mikulincer et al., 2004, for a review). The urge to affiliate is so strong after mortality salience, in fact, that people become more open to forming relationships with others they would not usually consider suitable: They show greater willingness to relax their typical mate requirements (Hirschberger, Florian, & Mikulincer, 2002) and prefer to sit with a worldview-threatening group than sit alone (Wisman & Koole, 2003). Although most people might seek out relationships when threatened, there are individual differences in the way they do so. As discussed earlier, one variable that often moderates Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 19 defensive conviction findings is explicit self-esteem. It is high, and not low self-esteem individuals who are most often willing to express increased confidence in their self-concepts, attitudes, and groups after threat (McGregor et al., 2005; McGregor & Marigold, 2003). In the study described earlier, in which people rated their quality of their close friendships after an identity threat manipulation, it was only people with high self-esteem who became more optimistic about true friendship when threatened (Marigold & Holmes, 2006). Similarly, in the work of Murray and colleagues, it was only for high self-esteem people that threats fueled greater commitment to romantic relationships (see Murray et al., 2006, for a review). Conviction about relationships may largely be self-fulfilling and increase relationship quality, but these defensive processes can also have some negative consequences. People might foreclose too early on inappropriate and frustrating relationships which are difficult to maintain. On the other hand, people who have little tolerance for uncertainty about their world (i.e., certainty-oriented individuals) or about relationship partners in particular (i.e., destiny theorists), are often too quick to dismiss natural feelings of ambivalence and end a potentially rewarding relationship before giving it a chance to develop (Knee, 1998; Sorrentino et al., 1995). There are other important ways in which people might respond to self-threats and manifest conviction in a manner that is problematic for their close relationships. Some research suggests that self-threats can decrease situational perspective-taking and empathy. For example, whereas priming attachment security increased empathic responses (Mikulincer et al., 2001), and compassion and helping (Mikulincer, Shaver, Gillath, & Nitzberg, 2005), an attachment threat (a forecast of a lifetime of social exclusion) led to decreased empathy with another person’s physical or emotional suffering (DeWall & Baumeister, 2006). Mortality salience has been shown to increase self-serving attributions for successes and failures (Mikulincer & Florian, Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 20 2002), which may suggest that threatened individuals would be less likely to take the perspective of others or notice their contributions. In our own research, we have shown that various self-threats (e.g., failure, separation) cause exaggerated conviction and consensus estimates for personal beliefs about unrelated social issues and a failure to recognize the validity of alternative perspectives (McGregor & Marigold, 2003; McGregor et al., 2005). For example, after just reviewing a list of 10 common and diverse opinions about a social issue, defensive participants responded to a threat by inflating social consensus estimates for their own opinion to over 70%. The assumption that most others agree with one’s point of view could make one less likely to listen to and consider partners’ expressions of their views. In fact, in one study people with high self-esteem were seen as more arrogant and rude after an ego threat, and were consequently rated as less likeable (Heatherton & Vohs, 2000). Being primed to powerfully pursue goal-directed behavior can also lead people to act in antisocial ways, as one’s personal agenda takes precedence over the consideration of others (Galinksy, Gruenfeld, & Magee, 2003). At a neuropsychological level, tenacious approach motivation and defensiveness have been associated with relative increases in left hemisphere processes (Martin & Shrira, 2005; McGregor & Jordan, 2006). It is right hemisphere activation, however, that seems more closely associated with social perspective-taking and sympathy (Decety & Chaminade, 2003; Heberlein, Adophs, Pennebaker, & Tranel, 2003; Henry, 1993; Hird & Kirsner, 2003). Thus, increased relative left hemisphere activation after threat may impede interpersonal sensitivity. The goal literature provides some support for the idea that approach-motivated pursuit of self-focused goals (which we have shown to be a defensive conviction type of response to selfthreats) sometimes leads people to downplay relationship goals, and consequently “block out” Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 21 relationship partners. To ensure that a goal of current focus receives sufficient attention, alternative pursuits are often put aside. Goal shielding theory assumes that this kind of inter-goal inhibition is applied automatically and without conscious awareness. Indeed, research has shown that simply priming a goal-related word inhibits recognition of alternative goals, particularly when one is highly committed to the focal goal. The inhibitory effect is more pronounced among individuals high in anxiety and need for closure (Shah, Friedman, & Kruglanski, 2002), which are states created by self-threats (Dechesne & Kruglanski, 2004; Gray & McNaughton, 2000). Such goal-shielding processes may have long-term effects: Successful pursuit of career-related goals might come at the expense of inhibiting relationship goals (Shah et al., 2002). Furthermore, prolonged inhibition may undermine one’s interest in, or commitment to, the inhibited goals. Chronic inhibition of relationship goals may not be very common, as several of our studies showed that defensive conviction was particularly pronounced for relationship goals and identities (Marigold & Holmes, 2006; McGregor et al., 2001, Studies 2 & 4). However, if the threat actually comes from a relationship, people will likely want to promote ideals in a different domain. Goal pursuit may also influence individuals’ feelings about their relationship partners in a more direct way. When actively engaged in goal pursuit, people automatically evaluate other people and objects that facilitate their current goal more positively (Ferguson & Bargh, 2004; Fitzsimons, 2006). Relationship partners vary in the extent to which they facilitate a person’s goals. Consequently, when a specific goal is primed, people feel closer to, see more importance in, are more motivated to approach, and more likely to intend to spend time with instrumental relationship partners. Conversely, they are more likely to avoid relationship partners than hinder their focal goal (Fitzsimons, 2006). Eagerly pursuing personal goals and ideals as a way of Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 22 coping with self-threats may thus lead people to inhibit thoughts of, and decrease commitment to, relationship partners who might interfere with goal pursuit. Summary and Future Directions In this chapter we have suggested that uncertainty and other self-threats like mortality salience, failure, and separation cause anxiety because they block important, high-level self-goals and activate the BIS. One response is to eagerly promote alternative, often idealized goals in the form of convictions about unrelated opinions, values, identities, and relationships. Idealized convictions are appealing in response to threats because they focus on ideal goals that are relatively immune to the kinds of frictions and frustrations that can block more concrete goals and cause anxious BIS activation. One can easily fail a test or lose at love, but moral ideals about Truth, Love, or Goodness can be more stable and resistant to evidence. Turning to such ideals in the face of anxiety may accomplish a kind of defensive approach motivation whereby activation of unequivocal approach processes inhibits avoidance processes. Individuals are thereby insulated from prolonged preoccupation with threatening experiences. Although this kind of motivational insulation certainly feels good for the threatened individual, it may negatively affect relationship partners who are shielded from consideration by the goal-driven individual. On the other hand, relationships may serve as a source of idealization and thus be strengthened as threats increase conviction about them. In future research we plan to look more closely at how the processes outlined in this chapter can affect relationships. Although from one perspective defensive approach-motivation reactions to self-threats may be seen as a resilient form of active coping that serves the individual’s emotion-regulation needs, it is also possible that it may take a toll on intimate and peer relationships. Pairs of participants will be instructed to converse warmly with each other Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 23 after a self-threat. We expect that defensive individuals in the glow of their post-threat zeal will report feeling connected and close to their interaction partners, and will assume that their interaction went well and that their partners were enjoying them. We expect that objective observers and the interaction partners might report a different experience, however. Lefthemisphere-charged, defensive resilience may come across as domineering, insensitive, and unable to vulnerably engage in the tentative perspective-taking that intimacy requires. In related research we plan to assess whether patterns of relative left-hemisphere EEG activity characteristic of approach-motivation coincide with both zeal and with interpersonal insensitivity. If our defensive-approach-motivation hypothesis is solidly supported by the proposed research, it could provide a parsimonious framework for integrating diverse theories of defensiveness and emotion-regulation. Debates over the motivational primacy of uncertainty, mortality salience, self-esteem, belongingness, control, security, or meaning could be dropped in recognition of a basic, underlying process model that operates according to ancient goalregulation mechanisms shared by all vertebrates. Looming threats to any important goal— whether for survival, success, or acceptance—cause anxious arousal and vigilance, until an alternative, uncompromised goal is identified for whole-hearted pursuit. For humans, idealized convictions are especially reliable in this regard because they are a kind of goal that cannot be easily blocked. Even if one’s daily goals are a disaster, idealized Truth or Goodness can reliably shimmer above the fray, as beacons for approach and insulation from concern with troubling temporal reality. Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 24 References Amodio, D. M., Shah, J. Y., Sigelman, H., Brazy, P. C., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2004). Implicit regulatory focus associated with asymmetrical frontal cortical activity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 225-232. Bartholomew, K. (1990). Avoidance of intimacy: An attachment perspective. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7, 147-178. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press. Cavallo, J.V., Fitzsimons, G.M., & Holmes, J.G. Unpublished raw data. University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Canada. Crocker, J., Thompson, L. L., McGraw, K. M., & Ingerman, C. (1987). Downward comparison, prejudice, and evaluations of others: Effects of self-esteem and threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 907-916. Decety, J., & Chaminade, T. (2003). Neural correlates of feeling sympathy. Neuropsychologia, 41, 127-138. Dechesne, M., & Kruglanski, A.W. (2004). Terror’s epistemic consequences: Existential threats and the quest for certainty and closure. In J. Greenberg & S. L. Koole & T. Pyszczynski (Eds.), Handbook of Experimental Existential Psychology (pp. 247-262). New York: Guilford Press. DeWall, C. N., & Baumeister, R. F. (2006). Alone but feeling no pain: Effects of social exclusion on physical pain tolerance and pain threshold, affective forecasting, and interpersonal empathy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 1-15. Drevets, W.C., & Raichle, M.E. (1998). Reciprocal suppression of regional cerebral blood flow Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 25 during emotional versus higher cognitive processes: Implications for interactions between emotion and cognition. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 353-385. Drigotas, S. M., Rusbult, C. E., Wieselquist, J., & Whitton, S. W. (1999). Close partner as sculptor of the ideal self: Behavioral affirmation and the Michelangelo phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 293-323. Ferguson, M. J., & Bargh, J. A. (2004). Liking is for doing: The effects of goal pursuit on automatic evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 557-572. Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison process. Human Relations, 7, 117-140. Fitzsimons, G. M. (2006). Pursuing goals and perceiving others: A self-regulatory perspective on interpersonal relationships. In K. D. Vohs & E. J. Finkel (Eds.), Self and relationships: Connecting intrapersonal and interpersonal processes (pp. 32-53). New York: Guilford Press. Friedman, R. S., & Forster, J. (2005). Effects of motivational cues on perceptual asymmetry: Implications for creativity and analytical problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 263-275. Galinsky, A.D., Gruenfled, D.H., & Magee, J.C. (2002). From power to action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 453-466. Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2000). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Greenberg, J., Arndt, J., Schimel, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (2001). Clarifying the function of mortality salience-induced worldview defense: Renewed suppression or reduced accessibility of death-related thoughts? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 70-76. Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 26 Greenberg, J., Arndt, J., Simon, L., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (2000). Proximal and distal defenses in response to reminders of one's mortality: Evidence of a temporal sequence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 91-99. Greenberg, J., Martens, A., Jonas, E., Eisenstadt, D., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (2003). Psychological defense in anticipation of anxiety: Eliminating the potential for anxiety eliminates the effect of mortality salience on worldview defense. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 14, 516-519. Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Pyszczynski, T. (1997). Terror management theory of self-esteem and cultural worldviews: Empirical assessments and conceptual refinements. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 61-139). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the implicit association test to measure selfesteem and self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 1022-1038. Hardin, C. D., & Higgins, E. T. (1996). Shared reality: How social verification makes the subjective objective. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition. (Vol. 3, pp. 28-84). New York: Guilford. Harmon-Jones, E., & Sigelman, J. (2001). State anger and prefrontal brain activity: Evidence that insult-related relative left-prefrontal activation is associated with experienced anger and aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 797-803. Hart, J., Shaver, P. R., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2005). Attachment, self-esteem, worldviews, and terror management: Evidence for a tripartite security system. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 999-1013. Heatherton, T., & Vohs, K.D. (2000). Interpersonal evaluations following threats to self: Role of Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 27 self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 725-736. Heberlein, A. S., Adophs, R., Pennebaker, J. W., & Tranel, D. (2003). Effects of damage to right-hemisphere brain structures on spontaneous emotional and social judgments. Political Psychology, 24, 705-726. Heimpel, S.A., Elliott, J.A., & Wood, J.V. (2006). Basic personality dispositions, self-esteem, and personal goals: An approach-avoidance analysis. Journal of Personality, 74, 12931319. Heimpel, S. A., Wood, J. V., Marshall, M. A., & Brown, J. D. (2002). Do people with low selfesteem really want to feel better? Self-esteem differences in motivation to repair negative moods. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 128-147. Heine, S. J., Proulx, T., & Vohs, K. D. (2006). The meaning maintenance model: On the coherence of social motivations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 88-110. Heller, W., Nitschke, J. B., & Miller, G. A. (1998). Lateralization in emotion and emotional disorders. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 26-32. Henry, J. (1993). Psychological and physiological responses to stress: The right hemisphere and the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis: An inquiry into problems of human bonding. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science, 28, 369-387. Higgins, E. T. (2005). Value from regulatory fit. Psychological Science, 14, 209-213. Hird, K., & Kirsner, K. (2003). The effect of right cerebral hemisphere damage on collaborative planning in conversation: An analysis of intentional structure. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 17, 309-315. Hirschberger, G., Florian, V., & Mikulincer, M. (2002). The anxiety buffering function of close relationships: Mortality salience effects on the readiness to compromise mate selection Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 28 standards. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 609-625. Hogg, M. A. (2007). Uncertainty-identity theory. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 39, pp. 69-126). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Jackson, D.C., Mueller, C.J., Dolski, I., Dalton, K.M., Nitschke, J.B., Urry, H.L., et al. (2003). Now you feel it, now you don't: Fontal brain electrical asymmetry and individual differences in emotion regulation. Psychological Science, 14, 612-617. Jordan, C. H., Spencer, S. J., & Zanna, M. P. (2005). Types of high self-esteem and prejudice: How implicit self-esteem relates to ethnic discrimination among high explicit self-esteem individuals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 693-702. Jordan, C. H., Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., Hoshino-Browne, E., & Correll, J. (2003). Secure and defensive self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 969-978. Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 339-375. Kay, A.C., & Gaucher, D. (2006, October). Needs for order and control and the palliative function of religious ideology. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Experimental Social Psychology, Philadelphia, PA. Klinger, E. (1975). Consequences of commitment to and disengagement from incentives. Psychological Review, 82, 1-25. Klinger, E. (1977). Meaning and void: Inner experience and the incentives in people's lives. Oxford: University of Minnesota Press. Knee, C. R. (1998). Implicit theories of relationships: Assessment and prediction of romantic relationship initiation, coping, and longevity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 360-370. Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 29 La Guardia, J. G., Ryan, R. M., Couchman, C. E., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Within-person variation in security of attachment: A self-determination theory perspective on attachment, need fulfillment, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 367-384. Lee, G. P., Meador, K. J., Loring, D. W., Allision, J. D., Brown, W. S., Paul, L. K., et al. (2004). Neural substrates of emotion as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 17, 9-17. Leonardelli, G. J., Lakin, J.L., & Arkin, R.M. (in press) A regulatory focus model of selfevaluation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (1998). The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 5-18. Logel, C., Spencer, S. J., Wood, J. V., Holmes, J. G., & Zanna, M. P. (2006). Guarded and responsive low self-esteem: How implicit self-esteem functions among low explicit selfesteem individuals. Unpublished manuscript, University of Waterloo. Marigold, D.C. & Holmes, J.G. (2006). [Friendship conviction in response to identity uncertainty]. Unpublished raw data, University of Waterloo. Martin, L.L., & Shrira, I. (2005). The cerebral hemispheres as a framework for social psychology theorizing. Unpublished manuscript. University of Waterloo. McGregor, I. (2004). Zeal, identity, and meaning: Going to extremes to be one self. In J. Greenberg & S. L. Koole & T. Pyszczynski (Eds.), Handbook of Experimental Existential Psychology (pp. 182-199). New York: Guilford Press. McGregor, I. (2006a). Offensive defensiveness: Toward an integrative neuroscience of Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 30 compensatory zeal after mortality salience, personal uncertainty, and other poignant selfthreats. Psychological Inquiry, 17, 299-308. McGregor, I. (2006b). Zeal appeal: The allure of moral extremes. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 28, 343-348. McGregor, I., Gailliot, M. T., Vasquez, N., Nash, K. A. (in press). Ideological and personal zeal reactions to threat among people with high self-esteem: Motivated promotion focus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. McGregor, I. & Jordan, C. H. (2006, October). Offensive defensiveness: Approaching zeal after self-threats. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Experimental Social Psychology, Philadelphia, PA. McGregor, I., & Jordan, C. H. (2007). The mask of zeal: Low implicit self-esteem, and defensive extremism after self-threat. Self and Identity, 6, 223-237. McGregor, I., & Marigold, D. C. (2003). Defensive zeal and the uncertain self: What makes you so sure? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 838-852. McGregor, I., Nail, P. R., Marigold, D. C., & Kang, S.-J. (2005). Defensive pride and consensus: Strength in imaginary numbers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 978996. McGregor, I., Zanna, M. P., Holmes, J. G., & Spencer, S. J. (2001). Compensatory conviction in the face of personal uncertainty: Going to extremes and being oneself. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 472-488. Mikulincer, M., Dolev, T., & Shaver, P.R. (2004). Attachment-related strategies during thought suppression: Ironic rebounds and vulnerable self-representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 940-956. Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 31 Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (2002). The effects of mortality salience on self-serving attributions: Evidence for the function of self-esteem as a terror management mechanism. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 24, 261-271. Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., & Hirschberger, G. (2004). The terror of death and the quest for love: An existential perspective on close relationships. In J. Greenberg, S.L. Koole, & T. Pyszczynski (Eds.), Handbook of Experimental Existential Psychology (pp. 287-304). New York: Guilford Press. Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., Halevy, V., Avihou, N., Avidan, S., & Eshkoli, N. (2001). Attachment theory and reactions to others' needs: Evidence that activation of the sense of attachment security promotes empathic responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1205-1224. Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., Gillath, O., & Nitzberg, R. A. (2005). Attachment, caregiving, and altruism: Boosting attachment security increases compassion and helping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 817-839. Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Collins, N. L. (2006). Optimizing assurance: The risk regulation system in relationships. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 641-666. Nitschke, J. B., Heller, W., Palmieri, P. A., & Miller, G. A. (1999). Contrasting patterns of brain activity in anxious apprehension and anxious arousal. Psychophysiology, 36, 628-637. Powers, W. T. (1973). Behavior: The control of perception. Chicago: Aldine. Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (1999). A dual-process model of defense against conscious and unconscious death-related thoughts: An extension of terror management theory. Psychological Review, 106, 835-845. Schiff, B. B., & Bassel, C. (1996). Effects of asymmetrical hemispheric activation on approach Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 32 and withdrawal responses. Neuropsychologia, 10, 557-564. Shah, J. Y., Friedman, R., & Kruglanksi, A. W. (2002). Forgetting all else: On the antecedents and consequences of goal shielding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1261-1280. Shah, J. Y., Kruglanksi, A. W., & Thompson, E. P. (1998). Membership has its (epistemic) rewards: Need for closure effects on in-group bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 383-393. Smith, S. M., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Personality moderators of mood congruency effects on cognition: The role of self-esteem and negative mood regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 1092-1107. Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (2004). The cultural animal: Twenty years of Terror Management Theory. In J. Greenberg, S.L. Koole, & T. Pyszczynski (Eds.), Handbook of Experimental Existential Psychology (pp. 13-34). New York: Guilford Press. Sorrentino, R. M., Holmes, J. G., Hanna, S. E., & Sharp, A. (1995). Uncertainty orientation and trust in close relationships: Individual differences in cognitive style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 314-327. Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 261-302). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Sullivan, R. M. (2004). Hemispheric asymmetry in stress processing in rat prefrontal cortex and the role of mesocortical dopamine. The International Journal of the Biology of Stress, 7, 131-143. Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 33 Swann, W.B. (1987). Identity negotiation: Where two roads meet. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1038-1051. Swann, W. B. J., & Predmore, S. C. (1985). Intimates as agents of social support: Sources of consolation or despair? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1609-1617. Tesser, A. (2000). On the confluence of self-esteem maintenance mechanisms. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 290-299. Tomarken, A. J., & Davidson, R. J. (1994). Frontal brain activation in repressors and nonrepressors. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 339-349. Tomarken, A. J., & Keener, A. D. (1998). Frontal brain asymmetry and depression: A selfregulatory perspective. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 387-420. Tucker, D. M., Roth, R. S., Arneson, B. A., & Buckingham, V. (1977). Right hemisphere activation during stress. Neuropsychologia, 15, 697-700. Urry, H. L., Nitschke, J. B., Dolski, I., Jackson, D. C., Dalton, K. M., Mueller, C. J., et al. (2004). Making a life worth living: Neural correlates of well-being. Psychological Science, 15, 367-372. Van den Bos, K., Euwema, M. C., Poortvliet, P. M., & Maas, M. (in press). Uncertainty management and social issues: Uncertainty as important determinant of reactions to osocially deviating people. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. Van den Bos, K., Ham, J., Lind, E. A., Simonis, M., Van Essen, W. J., & Rijpkema, M. (in press). Justice and the human alarm system: The impact of exclamation points and flashing lights on the justice judgment process. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 34 Van den Bos, K., Poortvliet, P. M., Maas, M., Miedema, J., & vandenHam, E.-J. (2005). An enquiry concerning the principles of cultural norms and values: The impact of uncertainty and mortality salience on reactions to violations and bolstering of cultural worldviews. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 91-113. Van den Bos, K., Van Ameijde, J., & Van Gorp, H. (2006). On the psychology of religion: The role of personal uncertainty in religious worldview defense. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 28, 333-341. Van den Bos, K., Stein, J., Brockner, J., Steiner, D. D., Van Yperen, N. W., & Dekker, D. M. (2007). Culture and the psychology of voice: The moderating influence of performance goal orientation and the Noblesse Oblige rule. Manuscript in preparation. Van Hook, E., & Higgins, E. T. (1988). Self-related problems beyond the self-concept: Motivational consequences of discrepant self-guides. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 625-633. Wegner, D.M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34-52. Wichman, A. L., Brunner, R. P., & Weary, G. (2006, October). On the importance of distracting people from their uncertainty: Distraction interferes with effortful inhibition of uncertainty. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Experimental Social Psychology, Philadelphia, PA. Wisman, A., & Koole, S. L. (2003). Hiding in the crowd: Can mortality salience promote affiliation with others who oppose one's worldviews? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 511-526. Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 35 Authors’ Note Denise C. Marigold & Mark P. Zanna, Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON, Canada; Ian McGregor, Department of Psychology, York University Toronto, ON, Canada. Correspondence concerning this chapter should be addressed to Denise Marigold, Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, 200 University Avenue West, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada, N2L 3G1; or Ian McGregor, Department of Psychology, York University, 4700 Keele Street, Toronto, ON, Canada, M3J 1P3. Electronic mail: dcmarigo@uwaterloo.ca or ianmc@yorku.ca. Footnotes Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 1 36 Although Higgins talks about ought guides as well as ideal guides, the ideal guides are most relevant here because future ideal selves are represented more abstractly (see also Liberman & Trope, 1998). 2 There is some evidence that properties of the threat determine, at least is part, whether people turn to conviction or consensus (or both). Threats that arise by highlighting existing intrapersonal conflict seem to steer people towards conviction, whereas threats that are delivered by the researcher during the experiment seem to steer people towards consenus (see McGregor, 2004, for a more detailed discussion). 3 Some researchers have found that uncertainty salience has a larger impact on people's reactions than does mortality salience (Van den Bos & Lind, this volume; Van den Bos et al., 2005). Other studies have found the opposite (Landau et al., this volume), or shown equal impact (McGregor et al., 2001). We suspect these outcomes depend on several factors, including the ways both uncertainty and mortality salience are manipulated, personality characteristics (McGregor et al., 2005; McGregor & Jordan, 2007) and cultural influences in the population in which the experiments are conducted (Van den Bos et al., 2007). 4 One argument against this emotion-regulation perspective might be that there is only occasional evidence for emotional distress after the various kinds of self-threats that cause defensive reactions (e.g., rejection, DeWall & Baumeister, 2006; mortality salience, Greenberg et al., 2003). In most cases, this appears to be due to the fact that affect is measured immediately after the threat, and some people’s first knee-jerk defense is thought-suppression (Greenberg et al., 2001; Mikulincer, Dolev, & Shaver, 2004). In at least one study in which uncertainty-induced affect was measured after a delay, after the suppressed threat had presumably had a chance to Defensive Conviction as Emotion Regulation 37 rebound into accessibility, emotional consequences of the threat were more apparent (McGregor et al., 2001, Study 1). 5 These findings are typically moderated by explicit self-esteem, such that only people with high explicit self-esteem seem to be able to use their convictions to quell distressing thoughts.