Dump-and-Chase Running Head: DUMP-AND-CHASE Dump-and-Chase: The effectiveness of persistence as a compliance-gaining strategy. Franklin J. Boster, Mikayla Hughes, Michael R. Kotowski, Renee E. Strom, Allison S. Shaw, Leslie Deatrick, and Chiharu Kato Michigan State University Keywords: Compliance-gaining, Sequential strategies, Persistence 1 Dump-and-Chase 2 Abstract Two field experiments were performed to assess the effectiveness of the dump-and-chase (DAC), a compliance-gaining technique that introduces a particular method of employing persistence to influence others. The outcomes of these two experiments demonstrate the effectiveness of the DAC relative to other compliance-gaining techniques known to be effective. In Experiment 1 the DAC was found to be more effective than the pooled data from the door-in-the-face (DITF) technique and the placebic information (PI) technique. In Experiment 2 the DAC was found to be more effective than the DITF, PI, and foot-in-the-door (FITD) techniques. The effect of the DAC was consistent across experiments, as well as producing relatively higher compliance-gaining rates. Moreover, because the contextual features of two experiments differed substantially, the effect occurred under heterogeneous conditions. Directions for subsequent tests of the DAC are discussed. Dump-and-Chase 3 Dump-and-Chase: The effectiveness of persistence as a compliance-gaining strategy. Although conventional wisdom suggests that persistence is an effective means of gaining compliance, little research has examined the matter (Boster, Levine, & Kazoleas, 1993). In a study examining the impact of argumentativeness and verbal aggressiveness on compliancegaining message selection, Boster et al. (1993) found that those who are highly argumentative, but low in verbal aggression, persist more than others when attempting to gain compliance. Other studies investigating social influence tactics have found message recommendations to be more effective when they are presented with little verbal aggression, but in a persistent manner. For example, Milgram’s (1974) participants were instructed to administer electric shocks when they thought that another participant (actually a confederate) had given an incorrect answer on a paired associate learning task. If the participant refused, Milgram followed with a probe asserting that the participant must continue for the sake of the experiment and administer another shock. If the participant continued to refuse, Milgram continued sending probes designed to gain the participant’s compliance. Uniformly, these probes were low in verbal aggressiveness, and the compliance rate in the experiment was extremely high (Milgram, 1974). The Boster et al. (1993) and Milgram (1974) studies suggest that if persons were more persistent, they would be more effective in their compliance gaining efforts. Building on these investigations, this study examines the issue of the effectiveness of persistence as a compliance-gaining strategy. The effectiveness of persistence, in the form of message repetition, has been investigated in persuasion experiments, and has been found to change attitudes substantially. Wilson and Miller (1968) provide evidence that an increase in message repetition increases conformity to message recommendations. Cacioppo and Petty (1979) report that as message repetition increased from one to three exposures, conformity to message recommendations increased, but Dump-and-Chase 4 an additional increase to a total of five exposures led to a decrease in conformity to message recommendations. These two studies demonstrate that at least moderate persistence is an effective way to persuade. Thus, one form of persistence, message repetition, enhances the effectiveness of social influence attempts (see, Weiss, 1969; Wilson & Miller, 1968; Zajonc, 1968). Moreover, structural features of persistence may affect whether compliance is gained. For example, increasing the number of people who endorse a position increases agreement with that position (e.g., Asch, 1951; White, 1975), and increasing the number of arguments associated with a position enhances agreement with that position (Calder, Insko, & Yandell, 1974). Furthermore, Harkins and Petty (1981) demonstrate that multiple messages transmitted from multiple sources produce more conformity to message recommendations than do single messages repeated by a single source, multiple messages from a single source, or single messages repeated by multiple sources. Although the reason(s) for the multiple source/multiple message effect has not been determined conclusively, it has been replicated (Harkins & Petty, 1981, 1987). At least two important consequences emerge from a compliance-gaining attempt. Obtaining the target’s compliance is one measure of success. On the other hand, the influencing agent’s ability to maintain a positive relationship with the target constitutes a second measure of success (Dillard, 1990; Goffman, 1952). The relationship between these two criteria is unknown. Nevertheless, one can certainly conjure up scenarios in which getting one’s way results in negative feelings between the influence agent and the target, and persistence, as a tactic, is susceptible to producing this outcome. Research on positive versus negative persistence is consistent with this reasoning (e.g., Ifert & Roloff, 1998; Kowalski, 2001; Roloff & Jordan, 1991). Nagging, an example of negative persistence (e.g., being rude by repeating a request until Dump-and-Chase 5 the person complies), is aptly referred to as an aversive interpersonal behavior (Kowalski, 1997, 2001). Evidence of the negativity of nagging can be seen in Heavy, Layne, and Christensen’s (1993) Conflict Rating System (CRS). The CRS consists of 15 behavioral dimensions, and nagging is grouped with pressure for change along with demands and requests as a negatively valenced conflict management tactic. If the influencing agent’s message behavior is viewed negatively, then exchanges can become progressively negative and retaliatory. Research on retaliatory exchanges demonstrates that parties will often exchange messages that are valenced similarly (e.g., a blunt request is followed by a blunt response) (Foa, Turner & Foa, 1972; Youngs, 1986). Put differently, if a negative request is made (e.g., someone requests a favor in a nagging tone of voice), it is likely that the target will respond in a negative manner (e.g., mimic the nagging tone in the reply). This segment of the interaction may affect the influencing agent’s subsequent message behavior such that a follow-up request may be more negative than the first. This response can produce an increasingly negatively valenced spiral of compliance-gaining attempts and refusals which can decrease the affect between the actor and target. The Process of Persistence Consider a compliance-gaining encounter in which the influencing agent begins with a direct request for compliance. In response to the direct request the target can choose either to comply with the request (i.e., success) or refuse it (i.e., lack of success). If the respondent complies with the request, there is no need to make additional compliance-gaining requests. A refusal, however, is more complex because the target can refuse in at least one of two ways. A rebuff is a blunt, unelaborated refusal; whereas, an obstacle is a refusal followed by the target providing a reason for not complying (Ifert & Roloff, 1998; Roloff & Jordon, 1991). Dump-and-Chase 6 The rebuff/obstacle distinction is important because the nature of the refusal has different effects on the compliance-gaining messages that follow. When presented with rebuffs, the influence agent’s response has a higher likelihood of being rude, aggressive, and forceful compared with the initial appeal, outcomes that are reportedly governed by the influence agent’s standards for acceptable messages (Ifert & Roloff, 1998). In contrast, when presented with obstacles, influence agents are more likely to respond in an adaptive manner (Francik & Clark, 1985). Thus, one potentially effective step in gaining compliance is attempting to transform a rebuff into an obstacle. In order to keep the rebuff from initiating a negatively valenced spiraling interaction, influencing agents need only ask targets why they cannot comply, e.g., “Why not?” Thus, the why not question allows the influencing agent to continue pursuing compliance in a manner unlikely to be perceived unfavorably by the target. This sequence is the crux of a compliance-gaining technique termed the dump-and-chase (DAC). Notably, this technique is sequential, as are a number of other known effective techniques (e.g., foot-in-the-door, door-inthe-face, low-balling, etc.), with the influencing agent’s message behavior contingent upon the target’s response. If the initial direct request gains compliance, then the influencing agent’s goal is attained. If the target raises an obstacle in response to the initial direct request, then the influencing agent generates a second message designed to overcome the obstacle and gain the target’s compliance. If the initial direct request yields a rebuff, then the influencing agent attempts to transform the rebuff into an obstacle by asking, “Why not,” and proceeds to address any obstacles that the target might mention. Figure 1 presents a visual depiction of the DAC strategy. Dump-and-Chase 7 Why the Dump-and-Chase May Be Effective At minimum, an explanation for the effectiveness of the DAC must specify the variables that represent the mechanism(s) mediating the relationship between the message and potential outcomes, particularly compliance. It is possible that there is more than one such mechanism. One possible mechanism is urgency. The structure of the DAC technique may require that the target be asked repeatedly to comply. Asking repeatedly for a favor can lead to the target forming the impression that compliance is urgent. A sense of urgency to complete the task implies time there is a limited time frame in which to perform the task. The scarcity principle posits that a decrease in an object’s availability results in a proportional increase in its value (Cialdini, 2001). Extending this principle and research involving scarce resources it follows that people can be expected to place more importance on completing tasks with a limited completion frame (see, Tversky & Kahneman, 1981). Therefore, to the extent that time is perceived as a scarce resource, the apparent importance of the request to the influencing agent increases with increasing scarcity, and, ceteris paribus, targets are more likely to comply with important requests. Therefore, one mechanism by which the DAC might increase compliance is the perceived pressing need for compliance implied in the structure of the technique. A second potential mechanism is guilt, an unpleasant emotional state characterized by feelings of perceived judgment about an action induced in the target by the DAC. Research has demonstrated that inducing guilt is an effective technique for gaining compliance (e.g., Carlsmith & Gross, 1969; Freedman, Wallington, & Bless, 1967) and helping behavior (Cunningham, Steinberg, Grev, 1980). Furthermore, after responding to a guilt appeal by helping, people report enhanced self-evaluations and being in more positive moods (Williamson & Clarke, 1989). Thus, the effectiveness of the DAC on compliance could be mediated by a sense of obligation and an Dump-and-Chase 8 ensuing sense of anticipatory guilt that is created when the target presents multiple obstacles to the influence agent’s request and the agent is persistent and effective in rebutting them. Put another way, a feeling of obligation in the target could arise as a consequence of the perception that the influencing agent’s persistence indicates a serious reason for requesting compliance. Subsequently, this sense of obligation could generate feelings of guilt associated with anticipated non-compliance with the influencing agent’s request. Compliance then serves as a means of avoiding the guilt that is anticipated from failure to comply with an important request which one has no unassailable reason to deny. Alternatively, the DAC may result in the target feeling sympathy for the influencing agent. Sympathy denotes feelings of understanding and compassion for another’s plight, and has been found effective in eliciting helping behavior (Konecni, 1972). The DAC may induce sympathy to the extent that the target attributes the influencing agent’s persistence to the request being of particular importance coupled with the absence of others to provide help. Complying with the request provides a means of fulfilling the influencing agent’s need, and conceivably producing positive affect in targets as a consequence of their altruistic action. Finally, the target’s desire to avoid cognitive dissonance provides another reason to expect the DAC to be effective. Cognitive dissonancy theory (Festinger, 1957) proposes that dissonance arises when one has two or more inconsistent cognitions. Because it is a psychologically uncomfortable state, people are driven to reduce dissonance. There are a number of ways in which dissonance reduction could occur, and one is behavior change. The structure of the DAC may produce dissonance in the target, and the target may comply with the request as a way to avoid dissonance. By presenting an obstacle, targets are claiming implicitly that they would comply with the request were if not for some condition preventing them from so doing. Dump-and-Chase 9 The influence agent’s response, or chase, to the obstacle addresses why condition this condition is insufficient so as to preclude compliance. Failing to comply after the obstacle has been addressed effectively by the influence agent produces an inconsistency in the target’s beliefs and actions and results in cognitive dissonance. In order to avoid such dissonance the target is expected to comply with the influence agent’s request after being exposed to responses that rebut effectively the presented obstacles. The Relative Effectiveness of the Dump-and-Chase The primary goal of this project is to elucidate the concept of the DAC and to test its effectiveness as a compliance-gaining technique relative to other techniques known to be effective. Comparison techniques with similar characteristics and structure were chosen as controls. Furthermore, these comparisons were made in two experiments so as to assess the generalizability of any existing effects. Experiment 1 compared the DAC with two known compliance-gaining techniques in an experiment involving a self-serving request for help. One comparison technique was the Doorin-the-Face (DITF) (Cialdini, Vincent, Lewis, Catalan, Wheeler, & Darby, 1975). Like the DAC, the DITF technique is sequential, i.e., it involves multiple messages in a planned order. The structure of the DITF requires that two requests be made: an initial request of a size sufficiently ample that most would find compliance with it unreasonable, and a second target request that is more moderate and is framed as a concession to the first (Hale & Laliker, 1999). At least two meta-analyses show the DITF to be an effective compliance-gaining technique (Dillard, Hunter, & Burgoon, 1984; Fern, Monroe, & Avila, 1986). The effectiveness of the DITF is often attributed to the norm of reciprocity. The influencing agent’s subsequent and more modest target request is expected to be viewed as a concession by the target. Normative pressure to match this Dump-and-Chase 10 concession by complying is presumed to affect the target (Cialdini et al., 1975). Disagreement exists over the psychological processes producing this effect (see Hale & Laliker, 1999 for a review), but its effectiveness is not disputed. The placebic information (PI) technique provides a second comparison. The PI request was discussed by Langer as part of her program of research on mindful and mindless action (Langer, Blank, & Chanowitz, 1978). In the Langer et al. (1978) experiment one of three requests for compliance was made: a direct request, a direct request with a relevant reason for the request, and a direct request with an irrelevant (placebic) reason for the request. In the PI request condition, the influence agent asked the target to comply with a direct request to allow them to move ahead in a line for a photocopier because they needed to make copies. For relatively small requests the placebic information condition yielded compliance rates (93%) similar to the relevant information request (94%). Notably, both requests produced more compliance than a simple direct request (60%). These results suggest that the DAC may be effective because a reason, any reason, for the request is provided in response to a target’s non-compliance. To examine this possibility the PI request was chosen as a control technique. Experiment 2 also made comparisons of the DAC with known compliance-gaining techniques. In addition to comparing the effectiveness of the DAC, DITF, and PI requests, Experiment 2 added the foot-in-the-door (FITD) technique (Freedman & Fraser, 1966). Like the DAC and DITF, the FITD is a sequential request strategy. Similar to the DITF, the FITD consists of two requests: an initial small request with which almost every target complies and a subsequent larger target request. The efficacy of this compliance-gaining technique has been documented (Dillard, Hunter, & Burgoon, 1984) and the need for consistency is among possible mechanisms accounting for its effectiveness (Dolinski, 2000). Particularly, it is posited that a Dump-and-Chase 11 target’s compliance with the initial request produces expectations that they will also comply with the subsequent request. Consequently, the target experiences pressure to behave consistently by complying with the second request. Including the FITD allows for an additional comparison of the DAC to a frequently studied sequential compliance-gaining technique. Given the finding, however, that sequential request strategies differ in their effectiveness based on whether they are pro-social or self-serving to the influencing agent (Dillard, Hunter, & Burgoon, 1984), the request in Experiment 2 examined pro-social proselytizing for a student organization. Experiment 1 Method Participants. The data in Experiment 1 consist of observations made on a randomly selected sample of 60 pedestrians who walked in front of the entrance of one of four pre-selected buildings in the greater Lansing, MI area. The sampling process for each pedestrian began with the random flip of a coin determining whether a pedestrian entering or leaving the building would be considered, constrained by the requirement that an equal number of observations of those entering and those leaving the building be made. Subsequently, a random selection of an integer (i) from one to six took place. After selecting the number the experimenter (E) counted each passing pedestrian until the ith pedestrian appeared, and that pedestrian became a participant (P) in the study. The sampling plan excluded persons carrying large parcels, those who were not alone, and those talking on a cellular telephone. With the constraint that an equal number of Ps be observed in each condition, each P was assigned randomly to one of three experimental conditions. These sampling procedures produced a sample consisting of 27 (45%) males and 33 (55%) females. Dump-and-Chase 12 Design. Experiment 1 involved contrasting the effectiveness of three different compliance-gaining strategies: the DAC, the DITF, and the PI. Each P was exposed to one, and only one, of the three compliance-gaining strategies. The DAC request consisted of the following initial request made by a confederate (C). “I need to take care of some business in __________ and I forget the lock for my bike. It should take about 10 minutes. Will you watch my bike for me until I return?” Rejection of the initial request by the P stating a rebuff or an obstacle elicited a response from the C that was consistent with the DAC strategy (see Figure 1). The P and C cycled through a maximum of three sequences of rejection and response before concluding the interaction. The following list presents some of the responses delivered by the C. “Why not?”, “I understand, I am in a hurry too…I will only be 10 minutes.”, “10 minutes is such a small part of the day.”, “I don’t know you either but I don’t know anyone around here.”, “This is my only means of transportation and bikes on campus get stolen all the time.”, “Oh, I have tried that before, and they don’t let you do that”, “You look like an honest person”, and “I think you are a better person than you pretend to be.” The initial DITF request was, “I need to take care of some business in __________ and I forgot the lock for my bike. It should take about 20 minutes. Will you watch my bike for me until I return?” Rejection of the initial request subsequently elicited the following concession from the C, “I won’t do everything I was planning on. I will make it in 10 minutes. Will you watch my bike for me until I return?” Finally, in the PI condition the C said, “I need to take care of some business in __________ and I forgot the lock for my bike. It should take about 10 minutes. Will you watch my bike for me until I return because I cannot watch it?” Dump-and-Chase 13 Procedure. The 20 observations made in each condition were divided equally across the four different locations (on the campus of Michigan State University, outside of a shopping mall, on the sidewalk of a suburban commercial district, and on the sidewalk of an urban business district). Furthermore, the 20 observations per condition were divided equally across three time periods (10:00a – 1:00p, 1:00p – 4:00p, and 4:00p – 7:00p). After selecting a location and time, the E followed the described sampling plan and selected a P. While one E performed the function of an unobtrusive observer (O), typically in a car or on a bench while appearing to read a newspaper, a second E performed the function of a confederate (C), waiting out of sight and ready to approach the P. The O and C communicated by telephone, allowing them to avoid contact while making observations. In the same manner the O communicated to the C which compliance-gaining message was to be delivered and which pedestrians were selected as Ps. When the C had identified a P, she approached the P on a bicycle. As the C approached the P, the C acknowledged the P by making eye contact, gesturing with the wave of a hand, and saying, “Excuse me.” When the C gained the P’s attention, the C immediately delivered the selected compliance-gaining message. After the request was made the O unobtrusively recorded the sex of the participant, whether or not the P complied with the C’s request, and how long the P watched the bicycle. In the DAC condition the C kept mental record of how many chases were used, and in the DITF condition she noted if the concession was presented. The C reported these results to the O when preparing for the next P. If the P complied with the request, the C entered the building, returned in 10 minutes, and retrieved the bicycle before riding away to prepare for the next P. If the P did not comply with the C’s request, the C rode away and prepared for the next P. Regardless of the P’s compliance, Dump-and-Chase 14 before leaving the C inconspicuously handed the P a small index card. This card included a debriefing statement explaining the study, and it provided contact information that could be used to address any of the P’s questions or concerns. Instrumentation. The measurement of a P’s sex was made by observing secondary sex characteristics. A verbal statement made by the P agreeing to watch the C’s bicycle constituted a compliant response. A response was considered non-compliant if, upon listening to the C’s request, the P verbally denied the request. The C mentally recorded the P’s response and the O watched for the P to remain by the bicycle after the C entered the building. The E and C compared these two observations after every P, observing no discrepancy for any case. For compliant responses measurement of how long the P watched the bicycle was performed with a stopwatch that the O started immediately as the C turned to enter the building and stopped either at 10 minutes or when the P stopped watching the bicycle prior to the C’s return. Furthermore, after the DAC request the C counted the number of refusal and chase sequences that were employed. Finally, in the DITF condition the C noted if the P complied with the initial request or the concession that followed. The C relayed this information to the O via telephone. Results Because two of the strategies are sequential, data on compliance with the initial request are pertinent and are presented in Table 1. From Table 1 one can observe that, consistent with what the technique implies, few of the Ps in the DITF condition complied with the initial large request (5%, one of 20). The initial request employed by the DAC strategy is a direct request, and 40% of the Ps in this condition complied with it (eight of 20). Notably, this compliance rate did not differ substantially from the initial compliance rate in the PI condition (45%, nine of 20). Dump-and-Chase 15 Tests of these observations indicate that the three compliance rates differed substantially from one another (χ2(2, N = 60) = 9.05, p < .05), primarily because the compliance rate in the DITF condition was substantially lower than the compliance rates in the DAC (χ2(1, N = 40) = 7.03, p < .01, r = .42, OR = 12.67) and PI conditions (χ2(1, N = 40) = 8.53, p < .01, r = .46, OR = 15.55). The DAC and PI conditions did not differ substantially from one another (χ2 (1, N = 40) = 9.05, ns, r = .05, OR = 1.22). Compliance rates for the target request are presented in Table 2. Overall, the three compliance rates differed from one another (χ2(2, N = 60) = 6.72, p < .05). A focused comparison indicated that the DAC technique resulted in a substantially higher compliance rate than did the DITF technique (χ2(1, N = 40) = 6.67, p = .01, r = .41, OR = 6.00). Additional focused comparisons showed that the hypothesis of no difference could not be dismissed for the comparison between the DAC and PI conditions (χ2(1, N = 40) = .90, ns, r = .15, OR = 1.83) and for the comparison between the PI and DITF conditions (χ2(1, N = 40) = 2.84, p < .10, >.05, r = .27, OR = 3.27), although the latter difference is not trivial. Comparing the DAC technique with the pooled data from the two comparison techniques (DITF and PI) resulted in a substantially higher compliance rate in the DAC condition (χ2(1, N = 60) = 4.15, p < .05, r = .26, OR = 3.12). Moreover, the compliance rate in the DITF condition was compared with the compliance rate of the pooled remaining conditions (DAC and PI), and the compliance rate in the PI condition was compared with the compliance rate of the pooled remaining conditions (DAC and DITF). In neither case did the compliance rate for the focal technique exceed substantially that of the pooled data for the remaining techniques. A comparison of Table 1 and Table 2 proves instructive in assessing the effectiveness of the sequential character of the DAC and DITF techniques. Specifically, of the 12 Ps in the DAC Dump-and-Chase 16 condition who failed to comply with the initial direct request, four (33%) complied after the C persisted in soliciting their compliance. Of these four Ps one complied after one additional request, two complied after two additional requests, and one complied after three additional requests. Of the 19 Ps in the DITF condition who failed to comply with the initial large request, three (16%) complied with the subsequent smaller target request. There was no evidence that whether the P was entering or leaving the destination, location, or time of day had an impact on the compliance rate. Male targets, however, were more compliant than female targets (55.6% vs. 30.3%, χ2(1, N = 60) = 3.90, p < .05, r = .26, OR = 2.88). There was no evidence that any of these factors combined non-additively with the type of compliance gaining strategy to affect the compliance rate. Discussion Experiment 1 demonstrated that the DAC is an effective compliance-gaining technique relative to other known techniques. Specifically, it produced a higher compliance gaining rate than the DITF technique, and was as effective as the PI technique. Moreover, it produced a substantially higher compliance-gaining rate than did the pooled rates of the comparison techniques. On the other hand, there were a number of contextual features of this experiment that were not varied but might have had an impact on experimental outcomes. One such feature involves the type of request employed. Some data suggest that sequential request strategies differ in their effectiveness depending on whether they are pro-social or self-serving to the influence agent (Dillard, Hunter, & Burgoon, 1984). Because the request in Experiment 1 was self-serving, it is important to discern if the relative effectiveness of the DAC generalizes to requests other than helping behavior intended to benefit the needs of the influence agent. Dump-and-Chase 17 A second factor of potential import is that a single chase response corresponding to each obstacle was employed. Because Ps sometimes cited the same reason for non-compliance multiple times, reciting the same chase response became artificial. Consequently, the compliance rate may have been attenuated in the DAC condition. A stronger DAC induction would more closely resemble naturally occurring chase behavior. Influence agents such as telemarketers or salespeople do not cite the same exact reason, or chase, over and over again in response to a customer’s non-compliance. Instead the same argument is stated with a slight difference or different arguments may be employed. A third potentially important feature of context is the immediacy of the request. In this experiment the request was made for immediate compliance. On the other hand, in many compliance-gaining situations requests are made for action that is to occur in the future. Such requests might be perceived as less pressured or urgent than the one made in this experiment and the compliance-gaining rate might decrease as a result. A fourth potentially important contextual feature is that a single confederate was employed. Clearly, employing additional confederates allows the generalizability of the results to be assessed. Moreover, influencing agents may make compliance gaining requests in the presence of other people. Expanding the context to include additional persons who are able to listen to the request and subsequent response would enhance the generalizability of this experiment to compliance gaining contexts in which multiple persons are present. A fifth potentially important contextual feature is that only two techniques were used to compare the effectiveness of the DAC. Clearly there are a host of potential comparison techniques. Thus, enthusiasm for the initial effectiveness of the DAC must be tempered by the knowledge that its effectiveness relatively to other important techniques has not been assessed. Dump-and-Chase 18 Experiment 2 As a result of these limitations a second experiment was designed in which several of these contextual factors differed from those present in the first experiment. In this experiment the compliance-gaining request involved donating time to an organization that promotes a pro-social cause. Thus, the influencing agent could be perceived as being other than self-interested. Furthermore, multiple confederates were employed, multiple responses to each obstacle were generated, the compliance behavior request was to be fulfilled at some point in the future, and an additional control technique (the foot-in-the-door) was added. Method Participants. Observations were made on 67 pedestrians who stopped to talk to two representatives (actually Cs) for a sexual health student organization who were sitting at an information table at one of three locations on the campus of Michigan State University. To be selected as a P in this experiment the pedestrian had to be alone, had to appear to be 18 years of age or older, and had to indicate a willingness to speak to one of the Cs. Those pedestrians meeting these criteria were greeted by one of the Cs who asked “Hello, do you have a minute?” Before proceeding, one of the Cs unobtrusively referred to a predetermined random number to assign the P to a condition. This sampling procedure yielded a sample consisting of 35 (52%) males and 32 (48%) females. Design. This experiment included conditions designed to evaluate the relative effectiveness of the DAC, the DITF, the foot-in-the-door (FITD), and the PI. Each P was exposed to one, and only one, of the four compliance-gaining techniques. The DAC consisted of the following initial request, “We really need people to help operate our recruitment booth next week. Basically, you would be doing what we are doing right Dump-and-Chase 19 now, handing out pamphlets about the group and seeing if people are interested. Can you help us operate the recruitment table anytime next week?” Rejection of the initial request by the P’s statement of a rebuff or an obstacle elicited a response from the C. The C selected the most relevant of four possible responses, each response intended to address a common obstacle identified in a pretest. Different chase messages for each obstacle were used so that, if the P referred to the same reason multiple times, the C would be able to provide a novel chase message. The P and C cycled through a maximum of three sequences of rejection and response before concluding the interaction. Table 3 presents a sample of the messages employed. In the DITF condition the C said, “We’ve been working in conjunction with Grand Valley and have a performance next Saturday in Grand Rapids which is about 50 minutes away. It’s our biggest performance ever and the State News is doing a special feature on the group. Unfortunately, the person that usually sets up our information table at the shows can’t be there and all our other members are participating in the show. We really need someone to operate the informational table at the show. Can you do it?” If the P rejected the initial request, the C followed up with the subsequent concession, “We really need people to help operate our recruitment booth next week. Basically, you would be doing what we are doing right now, handing out pamphlets about the group and seeing if people are interested. Can you help us operate the recruitment table anytime next week?” In the FITD condition the initial small request consisted of the following statement, “Would you be willing to wear this small sticker supporting our group?” (Shows sticker to the participant). If the P complied with this request the C then asked, “We really need people to help operate our recruitment booth next week. Basically you would be doing what we are doing right Dump-and-Chase 20 now, handing out pamphlets about the group and seeing if people are interested. Can you help us operate the recruitment table anytime next week?” Finally, in the PI request condition the C said, “We really need people to help operate our recruitment booth next week because it is essential that we have someone here to man the table. Basically you would be doing what we are doing right now, handing out pamphlets about the group and seeing if people are interested. Can you help us operate the recruitment table anytime next week?” Procedure. At each of the three data collection locations (two busy lecture halls and an on campus dining hall) the two Cs sat behind an table containing information about the group as well as pamphlets and handouts about general sexual issues (e.g., condoms, STDs, pregnancy). There was a sign up sheet on which persons could write their electronic mailing address if they were interested in having more information about the group sent to them, a pamphlet containing information both about safe sex and the group, and a handout with group contact information printed on it. Data were collected both during morning and afternoon hours. As each pedestrian approached the table the Cs delivered the following script while claiming to conduct a new member recruitment drive. “(C1) Hi, have you heard of the “In Your Face Theater Troupe”? The goal of our group is to increase awareness of sexual issues facing students. We try to do this by spreading information about STDs, safe sex, and other important things. Our approach is a little different because we try to educate and entertain. Our primary activity is putting on skits for students in dorms and then sharing information about sexual issues after the performance. We are looking for people to come join our troupe, we have a lot of fun and you can . . .” “(C2, interrupting C1) Just so you know, not all our members take part in performing the skits. There is no way I can get up in front of a bunch of people and act out the Dump-and-Chase 21 proper way to use a condom… but I really believe in the cause and the group needs people to do things like publicize performances, help write skits, and set up and run information booths.” Pedestrians not fitting the criteria of a P were given a flier consisting of basic information about the group. If the pedestrian fit the criteria, C1 took the role of an observer and C2 took the role of the influencing agent. C2 delivered one of the four possible randomly assigned requests and C1 observed whether or not the P complied with the request to help staff the recruitment table. After responding to the request for compliance Ps were asked if they had any previous knowledge about the group. Subsequently, there were debriefed. Immediately before leaving, Ps were asked their age. In order to retain the integrity of the experiment’s cover story the next P was not selected until the Cs were sure that any pedestrians in the area who may have overheard the debriefing had left. Instrumentation. The P’s sex was evaluated by C1 by observing secondary sex characteristics. Age and previous knowledge about the group were measured by the P’s selfreport. Ps were considered compliant when they indicated verbally that they would help staff the recruitment table; else they were considered noncompliant. Furthermore, a compliance-gaining index was formed by summing the number of pieces of available information (signing the contact information form, taking the group contact information handout, taking the information pamphlet) that were accessed. In the sequential request conditions (i.e., DAC, FITD, and DITF), compliance with the initial request was recorded. In the DAC condition, the number of chases employed was noted. Results Because three of the strategies in this experiment are sequential, data on compliance with the initial request are pertinent and are presented in Table 4. From Table 4 one can observe that Dump-and-Chase 22 consistent with what the technique requires few of those in the DITF condition complied with the initial large request (11.8%, two of 17). Furthermore, consistent with what the technique requires most of those in the FITD condition complied with the initial small request (94.7%, 18 of 19). The initial request employed by the DAC strategy is a direct request, and 18.8% of those in this condition complied with it (three of 16). Notably, this compliance rate did not differ substantially from the initial compliance rate in the PI condition (13.3%, two of 15). Tests of these observations indicate that the four compliance rates differed substantially from one another (χ2(3, N = 67) = 37.57, p < .001). This outcome occurred because the compliance rate in the FITD condition was substantially higher than the compliance rates in the other three conditions, none of which differed substantially from each other. Compliance rates for the target request are presented in Table 5. Overall, the differences among the four compliance rates were not statistically significant (χ2(3, N = 67) = 5.20, ns). A focused comparison indicated that the DAC technique resulted in a substantially higher compliance rate than did the PI technique (χ2(1, N = 31) = 4.76, p < .05, r = .39, OR = 6.50). Additional focused comparisons show that the hypothesis of no difference cannot be dismissed for all other comparisons, although in some cases the comparisons yield reasonable effect sizes (i.e., DAC vs. FITD, PI vs. DITF). Comparing the DAC technique with the pooled data from the three comparison techniques (DITF, FITD, and PI) resulted in a substantially higher compliance rate in the DAC condition (50% vs. 25.5%, χ2(1, N = 67) = 3.40, p = .065, r = .23, OR = 2.92), albeit one not statistically significant at the .05 level. Moreover, when the compliance rate in each of the remaining three cases is compared with the pooled data from the remaining techniques no substantial differences, or any approaching conventional levels of statistical significance, emerge. Dump-and-Chase 23 A comparison of Tables 4 and 5 proves instructive in assessing the effectiveness of the sequential character of the DAC, FITD, and DITF techniques. Specifically, of the 13 Ps in the DAC condition who failed to comply with the initial direct request, five (38.5%) complied after the C persisted in soliciting their compliance. Of these five Ps three complied after one additional request, one complied after two additional requests, and one complied after three additional requests. Of the 15 Ps in the DITF condition who failed to comply with the initial large request, four (26.7%) complied with the subsequent smaller target request. Of the 18 Ps who complied with the initial small request in the FITD condition, five (27.8%) complied with the subsequent larger request. An alternative method of assessing the relative effectiveness of the DAC involves employing the compliance-gaining index, the sum of the number of pieces of available information that Ps took or provided. Scores on this index were skewed negatively, and ranged from 0 to three with a mean of .63, a standard deviation of .98, and a reliability of α = .72 (standardized item α = .73). Summary statistics for this measure are partitioned by condition and presented in Table 6. Observing Table 6 indicates that the compliance-gaining index scores were substantially higher in the DAC condition than in the other conditions, and that scores in the other conditions do not differ substantially from one another. An unweighted means one-way analysis of variance performed on these data indicates substantial variance in the means (F(3, 63) = 3.21, p < .05, η2 = .14).1 Furthermore, contrasting the DAC condition with the data pooled from the other three conditions demonstrated that scores were substantially higher in the DAC condition relative to the DITF, FITD, and PI (F(1, 65) = 8.62, p < .01, r = .36).2 Moreover, residual explained variation not attributable to this contrast was trivial (F(2, 64) < 1.00, ns, η2 = .02). Attacking the same problem in another way, the results of least significant difference test Dump-and-Chase 24 post-hoc comparisons showed that the DAC was more effective than the PI technique (p < .05), the FITD technique (p < .01), and the DITF technique (p = .067). Because the compliance-gaining index was correlated substantially with the dichotomous measure of agreeing to staff the recruitment table, and because the impact of the treatment was stronger on the former than the latter, the possibility emerges that taking or providing material mediates the relationship between the experimental induction and agreeing to staff the recruitment table. This causal model is presented in Figure 2. From Figure 2 it may be observed that the path coefficient linking the induction and the compliance-gaining index (index) is substantial (.36, p < .01, two-tailed), and that the path coefficient linking the compliance-gaining index and agreeing to staff the recruitment table (compliance) is ample (.46, p < .01, two-tailed). Given the structure of the model and the parameter estimates, the predicted correlation between the induction and compliance is the product of the correlation of the induction with the compliance-gaining index and the correlation of the compliance-gaining index with the compliance measure, i.e., (.36)(.46) = .17. The obtained correlation between these two variables was .23, yielding an error in prediction of .06. Such an error is trivial, and well within sampling error of zero (χ2(1, N = 67) = .14, ns), indicating that the model and data correspond closely. General Discussion The results of the second experiment again demonstrate the promise of the DAC technique. Employing the cruder compliance measure (agreeing to staff the recruitment table), no technique was more effective than the DAC, the DAC was more effective than the PI technique, the DAC produced a much higher compliance rate than did the FITD technique (although the effect could be attributable to sampling error at the convention .05 level), and the DAC generated a substantially higher compliance rate than that obtained from pooling the data from the other Dump-and-Chase 25 three conditions. Using the more sensitive compliance-gaining index as a dependent measure, the DAC was more effective than the three comparison techniques. Across the two experiments the DAC produced a substantial and homogeneous compliance rate, 60% in Experiment 1 and 50% in Experiment 2. Two techniques, PI and DITF, were included in both experiments, and their compliance rates were lower and less consistent across these two experiments. Notably, these two techniques, as well as the third comparison technique (FITD), are believed to be very powerful methods of social influence (e.g., see Cialdini, 2001), and a substantial body of literature is consistent with this claim (e.g., see Dillard et al., 1984; Langer et al., 1978). The consistency of the DAC, and its effectiveness relative to known powerful methods of social influence, provides strong evidence of the promise of this instantiation of persistence. Also notable is the fact that the consistency of the DAC outcomes occurred despite substantial contextual variation across the two experiments. For instance, similar compliancegaining rates were obtained when the request was pro-social and when it was self-serving. The effect was produced when one C was employed and when several Cs were employed. It occurred when the target request was immediate and when it was to occur in the future. And, it occurred when the C employed one response per obstacle and when the C employed multiple responses per obstacle. Nevertheless, numerous additional contextual factors might be induced. It will be for subsequent experiments to vary these features, or to introduce new compliance-gaining situations that hold constant contextual features that challenge the efficacy of the DAC. Also, for future experiments is left the task of parsing out the accuracy of the proposed explanations of the DAC Dump-and-Chase 26 effect. The cumulative results from such a corpus will clarify the extent to which persistence is a powerful tool of social influence. Dump-and-Chase 27 References Asch, S. E. (1951). Effects of group pressure upon the modification and distortion of judgment. In H. Geutzkow (Ed.), Groups, leadership and men. Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie. Boster, F. J., Levine, T., and Kazoleas, D. (1993). The impact of argumentativeness and verbal aggressiveness on strategic diversity and persistence in compliance-gaining behavior. Communication Quarterly, 41(4), 405-414. Boster, F. J., Mitchell, M. M., Lapinski, M. K., Cooper, H., Orrego, V. O., and Reinke, R. (1999). The impact of guilt and type of compliance-gaining message on compliance. Communication Monographs, 66(2), 168-177. Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1979). Effects of Message Repetition and Position on Cognitive Response, Recall, and Persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(1), 97-109. Calder, T., Insko, C., & Yandell, B. (1974). The relation of cognitive and memorial processes to persuasion in a simulated jury trial. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 4, 62-93. Carlsmith, J. M., & Gross, A. E. (1969). Some effects of guilt on compliance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11, 232-239. Cialdini, R. B., Vincent, J.E., Lewis, S.K., Catalan, J., Wheeler, D., & Darby, B.L. (1975). Reciprocal concessions procedure for inducing compliance: The door-in-the-face technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 206-215. Cialdini, R. B. (2001). Influence: Science and practice (4th edition). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Cunningham, M. R., Steinberg, J., & Grev, R. (1980). Wanting to and having to help: Separate motivations for positive mood and guilt-induced helping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 181-192. Dump-and-Chase 28 Dillard, J. P. (1990). A goal-driven model of interpersonal influence. In J. P. Dillard (Ed.), Seeking compliance: The production of interpersonal influence messages. Scottsdale: Gorsuch Scarisbrick. Dillard, J. P., Hunter, J. E., & Burgoon, M. E. (1984). Sequential request persuasive strategies: Meta-analysis of foot-in-the-door and door-in-the-face. Human Communication Research, 10, 461-488. Dolinski, D. (2000). On inferring one’s beliefs from one’s attempt and consequences for subsequent compliance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 260-272. Fern, E. F., Monroe, K. B., & Avila, R. A. (1986). Effectiveness of multiple request strategies: A synthesis of research results. Journal of Marketing Research, 23, 144-152. Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford: Stanford University Press. Foa, E. G., Turner, J. L., & Foa, U. G. (1972). Response generalization in aggression. Human Relations, 25, 337-350. Francik, E. P., & Clark, H. H. (1985). How to make requests that overcome obstacles to compliance. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 560-568. Freedman, J. L., & Fraser, S. C. (1966). Compliance without pressure: The foot-in-the-door technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 195-202. Freedman, J. L., Wallington, S., & Bless, E. (1967). Compliance without pressure: The effect of guilt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 117-124. Goffman, E. (1952). One cooling the mark out: Some aspects of adaptation to failure. Psychiatry, 15(4), 451-463. Hale, J. L. & Laliker, M. (1999). Explaining the door-in-the-face: Is it really time to abandon reciprocal concessions? Communication Studies, 50(3), 203-210. Dump-and-Chase 29 Harkins, S. G., & Petty, R. E. (1987). Information utility and the multiple source effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(2), 260-268. Harkins, S. G., & Petty, R.E. (1981). Effects of source magnification of cognitive effort on attitudes: An information-processing view. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 401-413. Heavey, C. L., Layne, C., & Christensen, A. (1993). Gender and conflict structure in marital interaction: A replication and extension. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 16-27. Ifert, D. E., & Roloff, M. E. (1998). Understanding obstacles preventing compliance: Conceptualization and classification. Communication Research, 25, 131-153. Konecni, V.J. (1972). Some effects of guilt on compliance: A field replication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23, 30-32. Kowalski, R. M. (2001). Behaving badly: Aversive behaviors in interpersonal relationships. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Kowalski, R. M. (1997). Aversive interpersonal behaviors (pp. 1-9). New York: Plenum Press. Langer, E., Blank, A., & Chanowitz, B. (1978). The mindless of ostensibly thoughtful action: The role of “placebic” information in interpersonal interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(6), 635-642. Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority. New York: Harper and Row. Roloff, M. E., & Jordan, J. M. (1991). The influence of effort, experience and persistence in the elements of bargaining pleas. Communication Research, 18, 306-332. Tennen, H., & Herzberger, S. (1987). Depression, self-esteem, and the absence of self-protective attributional biases. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 72-80. Dump-and-Chase 30 Tversky, A., Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124-1131. Weiss, R. F. (1969). Repetition of persuasion. Psychological Reports, 25, 669-670. White, D. (1975). Contextual determinants of opinion judgments: Field experiments probe of judgmental relativity boundary conditions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 1047-1054. Williamson, G. M., & Clarke, M. S. (1989). Providing help and desired relationship type as determinants of changes in moods and self-evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 722-734. Wilson, W., & Miller, H. (1968). Repetition, order of presentation, and timing of arguments and measures as determinants of opinion change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9(2), 184-188. Youngs, G. A. (1986). Patterns of Threat and Punishment Reciprocity in a Conflict Setting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 541-546. Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9(2), 1-27. Dump-and-Chase 31 Footnotes 1 Observing Table 6 also indicates that there is a lack of homogeneity of variance (FL(3, 63) = 12.60, p < .001). Performing the contrast analysis employing the t distribution, and making the necessary corrections for the lack of homogeneity did not change the substantive conclusions. Moreover, the substantial correlation between the condition means and variances indicates that the lack of homogeneity may be attributed to a basement effect. 2 If the compliance-gaining index is dichotomized by contrasting those who took at least one of the indicators with those who took none, the resulting difference is non-trivial, although not statistically significant (χ2(1, N = 67) = 2.29, ns, r = .19, OR = 2.40. The contrast of this result with the analysis of variance results is explained by the fact that Ps in the DAC condition took more material than did those in other conditions. Dump-and-Chase Direct Request Compliance NonCompliant Rebuff NonCompliant Obstacle Ask Why Not Respond to Obstacle Quit NonCompliant Obstacle Respond to Obstacle Figure 1. Dump-and-Chase: A compliance-gaining process. 32 Dump-and-Chase Induction .36 Figure 2. A causal model of Experiment 2. Index .46 Compliance 33 Dump-and-Chase Table 1 Initial Compliance Rate: Experiment 1 Condition Compliance Complied Did Not Comply DAC DITF PI 8 (40%) 1 (5%) 9 (45%) 12 (60%) 19 (95%) 11 (55%) 34 Dump-and-Chase Table 2 Compliance Rate for the Target Request: Experiment 1 Condition Compliance DAC Complied 12 (60%) 4 (20%) 9 (45%) 8 (40%) 16 (80%) 11 (55%) Did Not Comply DITF PI 35 Dump-and-Chase 36 Table 3 Potential Responses to Obstacles Category of Obstacle Confederate’s Response P states they have a busy schedule. “Today (name of confederate 2) is helping out between classes. We’re all pretty busy but we don’t have absolute times people can help with the booth. We’re flexible. Can you help us?” “I understand, but it really won’t take a whole lot of time out of your schedule. Can you help us?” “I can relate, but I’m sure if you think about it you have some free time to help us out. Can you help us?” P states they don’t know very much about the group. “Oh, you wouldn’t have to do it alone. There would be some else who is not a new member to help operate the booth. Can you help us?” “This is only (name of confederate 2)’s second time helping out with the booth. You don’t have to be a long time group member to help out. Can you help us?” “All you really have to do is ask people if they are interested and give them a pamphlet. Can you help us?” P states they do not know their schedule. “That’s okay, you don’t have to give us an exact time and day you can do it right now. We just need to know if you can do it sometime next week and then you can let us know exactly when you can do it later. Can you help us?” “You don’t have to give us a specific time and date now but I’m sure you have a sense of what days and times you have free. Can you help us?” Dump-and-Chase 37 “Well you can just give us a general sense of when you might be available and we can work out the specifics later. Can you help us?” P states they are not interested. “What… do you not care about AIDS? This is an issue that affects everyone. Can you help us?” “You can’t deny how important it is for people to be more aware about safe sex. Can you help us?” “You might not be interested, but this is an issue that affects you no matter what. Even if you are abstinent or always have safe sex, someone you care about may not. Can you help us?” Dump-and-Chase Table 4 Initial Compliance Rate: Experiment 2 Condition Compliance DAC DITF Complied 3 (18.8%) 2 (11.8%) 18 (94.7%) 2 (13.3%) 13 (81.3%) 15 (88.2%) 1 (5.3%) 13 (86.7%) Did Not Comply FITD PI 38 Dump-and-Chase Table 5 Compliance Rate for the Target Request: Experiment 2 Condition Compliance DAC DITF FITD PI Complied 8 (50%) 6 (35.3%) 5 (26.3%) 2 (13.3%) Did Not Comply 8 (50%) 11 (64.7%) 14 (73.7%) 13 (86.7%) 39 Dump-and-Chase Table 6 Compliance Index Scores as a Function of Experimental Condition Condition Statistic DAC DITF FITD PI Mean 1.25 0.40 0.26 0.65 Standard Deviation 1.34 0.74 0.56 0.93 N 16 15 19 17 40