1 Date of submission: 11.9.2009 Running head: Lack of Nurturing Attitudes, Work Stress A Prospective Cohort Study of Lack of Maternal Nurturing Attitudes Predicting Adulthood Work Stress: The Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study Hintsanen, M.¹, Kivimäki, M.², Hintsa, T.¹, Theorell,T.³, Elovainio, M.4, Raitakari, O.T.5, Viikari, J.S.A.6, & Keltikangas-Järvinen, L.¹ ¹ Department of Psychology, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland ² Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, London, UK; and Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, Helsinki, Finland ³ Stress Research Institute, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden; and Department of Public Health Sciences, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden 4 National Research and Development Centre for Welfare and Health, Helsinki, Finland; and Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, London, UK 5 Department of Clinical Physiology, University of Turku, Turku, Finland; and Turku University Hospital, Turku, Finland 6 Department of Medicine, University of Turku, Turku, Finland; and Turku University Hospital, Turku, Finland 2 Correspondence should be addressed to Mirka Hintsanen, University of Helsinki, Department of Psychology, P.O. Box 9, FIN-00014 University of Helsinki, Finland. Email: mirka.hintsanen@helsinki.fi, Tel: +358-9-191 29517; Fax: +358-9-191 29521 Word count (body text): 3669, Tables: 4, Figures: 0. Key words: Childhood stress; effort-reward imbalance; job strain; nurturing attitudes; parenting; stress sensitivity; work stress. 3 Stressful childhood environments arising from lack of nurturing attitudes are hypothesized to contribute to later stress vulnerability. We examined whether lack of nurturing attitudes predict adulthood work stress. Participants were 456 women and 387 men from the prospective Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study. Work stress was assessed as job strain and effort-reward imbalance in 2001 when the participants were from 24 to 39 –year olds. Lack of maternal nurturance (intolerance and lack of emotional warmth) was assessed based on mothers’ reports when the participants were at age of 3 to 18 years and again at age of 6 to 21 years. Linear regressions showed that lack of emotional warmth in childhood predicted lower adulthood job control and higher job strain in both genders. Maternal intolerance predicted lower job control and higher job strain in women. Most associations were not explained by socioeconomic circumstances. Lack of nurturing attitudes in childhood may affect sensitivity to work stress and selection into stressful work conditions in adulthood. More attention should be paid to pre-employment factors in work stress research. 4 Research on animals suggests that less nurturing maternal behavior in rats is associated with more pronounced hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis responses to stress and higher behavioral stress responses in offspring. (Liu et al., 1997, Caldji et al., 1998) Existing research in humans also suggests that early stressful experiences may have long lasting effects on physiological stress systems (Bremner and Vermetten, 2001, Heim et al., 2002, Luecken, 1998, Evans and Kim, 2007) and recent review concluded that family environment characterized by frequent conflicts and cold, unsupportive, and neglectful relationships may predispose the individual to later stress and mental and physical health problems. (Repetti et al., 2002) In adulthood, a considerable part of time is spent in work settings. Work is a major source of stress and work stress has been repeatedly linked with a range of health problems, including cardiovascular disease (Kivimäki et al., 2006, Eller et al., 2009) and mental disorders. (Stansfeld and Candy, 2006) In spite of this only few studies have examined pre-employment determinants of work stress (Hintsa et al., 2007, Hintsa et al., 2006, Hintsa et al., 2008, Kivimäki et al., 2007, Hemmingsson and Lundberg, 2006, Elovainio et al., 2007) and to our knowledge, no previous studies are available on the association between childhood nurturance and adulthood work stress. Examining early roots of stress vulnerability is important for understanding the reasons for and consequences of work stress and for planning interventions that accurately take into account early predisposing factors. In the current study, we examine whether lack of nurturing attitudes by the mother (intolerance towards the child and lack of emotional significance of 5 the child to the mother) in the childhood predict work stress of the participants 18 - 21 years later in adulthood. We assess work stress based on the two most widely used conceptual models proposing that work stress arises from a combination of high job demands and low job control (Karasek, 1979) and effort-reward imbalance - a combination of low rewards received at work as compared to the level of efforts invested. (Siegrist and Peter, 1994) MATERIALS AND METHODS Participants The participants were derived from the ongoing prospective “Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns” study beginning in 1980. The subjects for the original sample in 1980 (n = 3,596) were selected randomly from six age cohorts (aged 3, 6, 9, 12, 15, and 18) in the population register of the Social Insurance Institution, a database that covers the whole population of Finland. The design of the study and the selection of the sample have been described in detail by Raitakari et al. (2008) The Young Finns cohort was enrolled into the study in 1980. The assessments of the present study were carried out in 1980, 1983 (maternal nurturing attitudes and mother’s years of education) and in 2001 when the participants were aged 24, 27, 30, 33, 36, and 39 (work stress variables and participants own education and occupational status). 2,105 participants (58.5% of the 1980 cohort) took part in the work stress assessments. Of them 691 were not employed in a full-time job in 2001 or did 6 not report employment status, and were therefore excluded. As working and possible work stress experienced by the participants at the time of maternal report of child rearing attitudes was thought as a potential source of confounding, participants who were known to work already in 1980 or in 1983 (n = 94, after other exclusions) were excluded from the analyses. There were 843 participants (387, 45.9% men and 456, 54.1% women) who had full data on all study variables in 1980, 1983 and 2001. All the analyses were conducted on these participants. The mean age of the included participants was 30.7 (in 2001). Participants gave written informed consent, and the study was approved by local ethics committees. Measurement of Job Strain and Effort-reward Imbalance Job demands and efforts were assessed with a same 3-item scale based on the Occupational Stress Questionnaire (OSQ; Cronbach’s alpha, α = 0.6) (Elo et al., 1992) developed at the Finnish Institute of Occupational Health. The OSQ has been widely used in Finland and the validity of the OSQ items has been satisfactory in studies involving a total of over 25,000 employees in various occupations. (Elo, 1994, Elo et al., 1992) The items used in the current study were: “Do you have to hurry to get your work done?” “Does your work have phases that are too difficult?” and “Is your work mentally strenuous?” These items correspond quite closely to demands in the Karasek´s Job Content Questionnaire. (Karasek, 1985) They also resemble efforts in the Siegrist’s 7 effort-reward imbalance questionnaire. (Siegrist et al., 2004) Responses were obtained on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (all the time). Job control was measured with the Job Content Questionnaire, (Karasek, 1985) which includes nine items for job control (α = 0.9). Responses were given on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). We constructed job strain as linear term, a continuous job strain variable obtained from the following equation: (job demand score) – (job control score). (Landsbergis et al., 1994) Reward was also measured with a 3-item scale (α = 0.6) from the OSQ. The items used in the current study were: “Do you get help and support from your superior if needed?” (1 [very little] to 5 [very much]) “How do your coworkers get along with each other on the workplace?” (Their relationship range is 1 [bad, tense, resentful, etc.] to 5 [very good]) “How satisfied are you with your current employment?” (1 [very unsatisfied] to 5 [very satisfied]). These items are similar to the reward items in the effort–reward imbalance questionnaire. (Siegrist et al., 2004) Effort–reward imbalance was constructed as a continuous variable, dividing effort by reward, as recommended by Siegrist et al. (Siegrist et al., 2004) A logarithmic transformation was performed to correct for skewness and kurtosis. All work stress scales were calculated with mean function. The participants were required to have answered to at least 50% of the items of each scale. Participants with more missing values were excluded. Lack of Nurturing Attitudes 8 Lack of nurturing attitudes (as indicated by intolerance of the mother towards the normal activity of the child, and lack of emotional warmth by the mother towards the child) were self-rated by the mothers in 1980 and in 1983 using a scale based on the Operation Family Study. (Makkonen et al., 1981) Intolerance (α = 0.7 and 0.7, for assessments in 1980 and 1983 respectively) was assessed with three items on a 5-point scale, e.g. “In difficult situations the child is a burden“, 1 = not at all, 5 = a great deal. Lack of emotional warmth (α = 0.7 and 0.8, for assessments in 1980 and 1983 respectively) was assessed with four items on a 5-point scale, e.g. “The child is significant to me“, 1 = very significant, 5 = not significant. Mean scores of these scales were formed so that high values represent lack of nurturing attitudes, i.e. high intolerance and high lack of emotional warmth. Here again, the participants were required to have information on at least 50% of items of each scale to be included in the study. Finally, the mean of intolerance in 1980 and 1983 and mean of lack of emotional warmth in 1980 and 1983 were calculated, and these scales were used in the analyses. Participant Socio-economic Status, Childhood Family Income, and Maternal Education Socio-economic status (SES) of the participants was indicated by educational level and occupational group. Educational level was classified as 1) low (comprehensive school), 2) intermediate (secondary education), or 3) high 9 (academic; graduated from a polytechnic or studying at or graduated from a university). Classification into occupational groups was based on the criteria of the Central Statistical Office of Finland. Three groups were formed: 1) manual, 2) lower non-manual, and 3) upper non-manual. Entrepreneurs, who formed a very heterogeneous group of their own in the original measure, were placed to the aforementioned occupational groups according to educational level (low, intermediate, and high education corresponding to manual, lower non-manual and upper non-manual occupational groups respectively). Childhood SES was assessed as it has previously predicted later stress. (Evans and Kim, 2007, Hintsa et al., 2006) Childhood SES was indicated by childhood family income (assessed in 1983 with an 8-point scale of annual income) and maternal education (indicated by mother’s years of education in 1983). Statistical Analyses All analyses were conducted for men and women separately, as maternal child rearing attitudes have been shown to be differently associated with various outcomes depending on the gender of the participant. (Pulkki et al., 2003, Flouri, 2004) Furthermore, gender differences in work stress are commonly found, e.g. (Hintsanen et al., 2005) Two linear regression models were constructed examining maternal nurturing attitudes predicting later adulthood work stress in the participants. The first model was adjusted for age, whereas the second model was adjusted for age, mothers years of education, childhood 10 family income, and SES of the participant in adulthood. Furthermore, these same associations were examined in men and women in two age groups (younger group: aged 3, 6, and 9 in 1980, and 6, 9, and 12 in 1983; older group: aged 12, 15, and 18 in 1980, and 15, 18, and 21 in 1983) because the age range of our participants is rather wide and vulnerability to environmental and social factors may vary at different ages. RESULTS Attritions The differences between the original sample (n = 3596) gathered in 1980 and the final study sample (n = 843) were examined with t-test and χ²-test. Excluded participants were somewhat older (mean ages 31.7 vs. 30.7 years, p < .001) and less educated (2.1 vs. 2.3, p < .001). Furthermore, they had lower occupational status (1.8 vs. 2.0, p < .001), their family had had lower income in their childhood (5.7 vs. 6.0, p < .001) and their mothers reported slightly higher intolerance (2.06 vs. 2.00, p = .005) and higher lack of emotional warmth towards them (1.60 vs. 1.56, p = .030). Excluded participants had lower rewards (3.7 vs. 3.8, p < .001) and a larger proportion of men than women were excluded (78.1% vs. 75.1%, p = .037). Characteristics and Bivariate Correlations 11 Table 1 presents characteristics of the study variables and the bivariate correlations in men and in women. Lack of maternal emotional warmth in childhood was correlated with higher adulthood job strain and lower job control in men, whereas higher maternal intolerance in childhood was correlated with higher adulthood job strain and lower job control in women. Intolerance and lack of emotional warmth were correlated with each other in both genders. Higher maternal intolerance also correlated with lower age of the participant in both genders. In women, lack of emotional warmth in childhood was correlated with participant’s lower adulthood education and lower occupational status, and with lower maternal education. In addition, maternal intolerance in childhood correlated with lower adulthood occupational status in women. Child rearing attitudes were not correlated with socio-economic measures in men. _______________________________ Insert Table 1 about here _______________________________ Lack of Maternal Nurturing Attitudes in Childhood Predicting Work Stress in Adulthood Table 2 presents the results of linear regression analyses on lack of maternal nurturing attitudes assessed in participants’ childhood / youth predicting job strain and effort-reward imbalance and their components in participants’ adulthood in men and in women. In men, in age adjusted models, lack of 12 emotional warmth by the mother predicted lower job control (β = -.138, p = .006) and higher job strain (β = .120, p = .018). Further adjustments for educational level, occupational status, maternal education and childhood family income did not attenuate these associations. Maternal intolerance did not predict work stress measures in men (p >= .149, for all age adjusted associations). In women, in age adjusted models, lack of emotional warmth and intolerance by the mother in childhood predicted lower job control (β = -.103, p = .027; β = -.110, p = .029, respectively) and higher job strain (β = .096, p = .040; β = .119, p = .019, respectively). Further adjustments for childhood and adulthood socio-economic measures attenuated the associations for lack of emotional warmth to non-significance. The additional adjustments did not attenuate the association between intolerance and job strain, but the association between intolerance and lower job control was attenuated to marginal significance (p = .058). Lack of maternal nurturing attitudes did not predict demands / efforts, rewards, or effort-reward imbalance in either gender. _______________________________ Insert Table 2 about here _______________________________ Lack of Maternal Nurturing Attitudes in Different Age Groups in Childhood Predicting Work Stress in Adulthood 13 To examine age differences, the participants were divided to two age groups formed by the three youngest and three oldest age cohorts. Tables 3 and 4 present linear regression analyses conducted in these age groups, in men and in women respectively. In men, in age adjusted models, childhood lack of maternal nurturing attitudes predicted work stress parameters only in the group of older participants. More specifically, childhood lack of emotional warmth by the mother predicted lower job control (β = -.249, p = .001) and higher job strain (β = .234, p = .003) in adulthood. Furthermore, maternal intolerance predicted lower rewards (β = -.221, p = .007). These associations were not attenuated when the analyses were additionally adjusted for childhood and adulthood socioeconomic measures. In women, in age adjusted models, childhood lack of maternal nurturing attitudes predicted work stress parameters in adulthood only in the group of younger participants although there were some nearly significant associations for the older group too (intolerance predicting lower job control and higher job strain). In younger women, lack of emotional warmth by the mother predicted lower job control (β = -.146, p = .023) and higher job strain (β = .152, p = .018). The additional adjustments for childhood and adulthood socioeconomic measures did not attenuate these associations. _______________________________ Insert Tables 3 and 4 about here _______________________________ 14 DISCUSSION Our main finding was that lack of nurturing attitudes by the mother in the childhood of the participant predicted participant’s lower job control and higher job strain 18 to 21 years later in adulthood. Lack of emotional warmth was an important predictor in men, whereas in women, higher intolerance was more important. These findings were not dependent on childhood or adulthood socioeconomic conditions. Much of the work stress literature focuses on work characteristics as a source of stress. Less attention has been paid to individual factors and even less to early experiences related to stress vulnerability. Our findings show that adulthood work stress may be predicted by variations in childhood environment. Our present findings are in line with previous research indicating that even ordinary variations in maternal nurturing are related to stress reactivity of the child. (Hane and Fox, 2006) Furthermore, our findings are in line with previous research suggesting that childhood early experiences may lead to long lasting stress vulnerability and dysregulation of physiological stress systems. (Evans and Kim, 2007, Heim et al., 2002, Luecken, 1998, Bremner and Vermetten, 2001) The gender differences we found are in line with previous research reporting that parenting styles may affect girls and boys differently. For example, depending on child’s gender parenting styles have been differently associated with child’s depressive symptoms, (Oldehinkel et al., 2006) as well as with child’s help seeking from the parent. (Puustinen et al., 2008) 15 Furthermore, it has been found that lack of nurturing attitudes predict health outcomes differently in boys and girls. In boys, strict maternal disciplinary style has been found to predict higher cardiovascular risk, whereas in girls, strict disciplinary style as combined with other attitudes indicating low nurturance interact with socioeconomic status in producing cardiovascular risk. (Pulkki et al., 2003) In addition to gender difference, we found age differences both in men and in women. Lack of nurturing attitudes in childhood of girls were found to predict later work stress, whereas in boys, more significant for later work stress were lack of nurturing attitudes in adolescence and young adulthood. In previous research on childhood and adolescent adjustment, adolescence has been found to be a more critical period for onset of problems in girls, whereas problems in boys often emerge in childhood as well as in adolescence. (Crick and Zahn-Waxler, 2003) The longitudinal nature and long follow-up time of our study may make our study somewhat incomparable to the previous findings especially as our outcome measure also differs from those previously reported. The gender differences found in the current study should be examined further in future research. The lack of nurturing attitudes by the mother explained approximately 1% to 1.5% of the variation in offspring’s later work stress (job control and job strain). As such, these proportions are not very high; they should not have a very large effect on a life of an individual. However, at the population level such an effect is still important. Furthermore, we assessed only one aspect of childhood environment, lack of maternal nurturing attitudes. There are many 16 other potential aspects (e.g. lack of paternal nurturing attitudes) that may as well affect later work stress, and their combined effect is likely to be more substantial. We found larger effects when the participants were divided into two age groups. In younger women, lack of nurturing attitudes explained approximately 2% of the variation in later work stress (job control and job strain). Effect sizes in older age group of men were still higher, explaining up to 6.5% of the variance in work stress (job control, job strain, and rewards). Based on these results, it seems that in relation to work stress, maternal nurturance inflicts its largest effects in male participants in their adolescence or young adulthood. It is possible that this result is related to gender role identity. Men are reported to have higher work involvement. (Parasuraman and Simmers, 2001) If early life stressors have led to stress vulnerability, its effects may be emphasized at work related matters in men. Furthermore, the older group of men was aged 33 – 39 in the end of the follow-up. This age period is likely to be more serious time for career development and competition as compared to the younger group (24 – 30) which consists of men who have recently graduated and are just beginning their careers. This is also supported by our data, as men in the older age group had higher demands / efforts and higher effort-reward imbalance (p = .001, p = .002, respectively). Furthermore, these age differences are in line with the Effort-reward imbalance model, which predicts that personal coping characteristic, overcommitment, leads to high efforts and to maintaining of efforts even in low reward conditions. (Siegrist, 1998) According to the model, high efforts are 17 maintained because being in control is self-rewarding for persons with overcommitment. In the long run, however, exhaustion may follow, (Siegrist, 1998) and this might lead to increased feelings of efforts and imbalance between efforts and rewards. In the current study we did not assess overcommitment and cannot therefore examine its effects. In the current study, work stress was assessed by self-reported questionnaires on work characteristics. Therefore, we have information on work characteristics as the participants perceived them. Both objective work characteristics and individual stress vulnerability are likely to affect these perceptions. Stressful childhood environment may predispose the individual to stress and therefore, to perceiving the characteristics of the work environment as more negative. Selection to more stressful jobs because of individual stress vulnerability is also possible, that is stress vulnerability induced by early experiences may lead to selection so that participants with stressful early experiences end up in more stressful jobs through lower educational attainment. In the present study, the association between lack of nurturance and work stress remained after educational level and occupational group were taken into account. In addition to affecting physiological stress vulnerability, lack of nurturance may have indirect stress inducing effects. For example, child rearing may affect development of social skills (Steelman et al., 2002) which are important in the current working life where team work and personal networks are becoming increasingly significant. Therefore, deficient social skills are a likely source of stress. 18 According to several reviews there is a relationship between psychosocial working conditions and cardiovascular disease (CVD). (Belkic et al., 2004, Eller et al., 2009, Kivimäki et al., 2006, Hemingway and Marmot, 1999) This is clearly established among men and particularly so among men below age 55. In the more recent studies, psychological demands seem to dominate in the associations. (Eller et al., 2009) The relationship between job strain and CVD among men seems to correspond to an etiological fraction in the order of 3 – 7%, and in men below age 55 the etiological fraction seems to be still higher (7 - 16%). (Karasek and Theorell, 1990) This means that the childhood nurturing factor that has been shown in the present study can explain only part of the job strain - CVD relationship. Accordingly, our results do not necessarily reduce the importance of improvement of psychosocial work environment although it brings a new dimension into the discussion. Limitations and Methodological Considerations In interpreting the current results, some limitations should be taken into account. Lack of nurturing attitudes was self-rated by the mothers and may therefore be affected by social desirability. Furthermore, mother’s nurturing attitudes may to some extent reflect the characteristics of the child, for example the child’s temperamental difficulty. Child rearing practices may be evoked by the characteristics and the behavior of the child. Another limitation is that our data included only maternal nurturing attitudes. At the time of the data collection 19 in early 1980s, the role of the fathers was not perceived to be as important as it is at present. As maternal nurturance is assumed to affect general stress sensitivity, it could be expected to be associated with all work stress variables and not just some as was the case especially in the analyses combining all age groups. Job demands, and the variables included in the effort-reward imbalance model were not assessed with original measures, which may have led to somewhat poorer measurement, which in turn could explain this phenomenon. However, in our previous studies we have found associations between the measures used in the current study and cardiac health (Hintsanen et al., 2005, Hintsanen et al., 2007) which gives credence to our measures. In this prospective longitudinal study, we were able to examine how lack of maternal nurturing attitudes assessed in the childhood of the participants affected participants’ work stress in adulthood. To our knowledge, no previously published studies on this subject exist. Overall, few previous studies have been able to examine the effect of childhood environment on adulthood stress vulnerability prospectively over such a long follow-up period. Conclusions According to our results, lack of maternal nurturing attitudes in one’s childhood may predict adulthood work stress 18 to 21 years later. Our findings imply that more attention should be paid to individual variance in stress vulnerability in work stress research. 20 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This study was supported by the Academy of Finland (grants 111056, and 124399 for LK-J; grants 117604, 124332, and 124271 for MK), the BUPA foundation (MK), the Signe and Ane Gyllenberg Foundation (LK-J and MH), and Yrjö Jahnsson Foundation (MH), Niilo Helander Foundation (MH). DECLARATION OF INTEREST The authors have no conflicts of interest. 21 REFERENCES Belkic KL, Landsbergis PA, Schnall PL & Baker D 2004. Is job strain a major source of cardiovascular disease risk? Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment and Health 30: 85-128. Bremner JD & Vermetten E 2001. Stress and development: behavioral and biological consequences. Development and Psychopathology 13: 473489. Caldji C, Tannenbaum B, Sharma S, Francis D, Plotsky PM & Meaney MJ 1998. Maternal care during infancy regulates the development of neural systems mediating the expression of fearfulness in the rat. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 95: 5335-5340. Crick NR & Zahn-Waxler C 2003. The development of psychopathology in females and males: Current progress and future challenges. Development and Psychopathology 15: 719-742. Eller NH, Netterstrom B, Gyntelberg F, Kristensen TS, Nielsen F, Steptoe A & Theorell T 2009. Work-related psychosocial factors and the development of ischemic heart disease: A systematic review. Cardiology in Review 17: 83-97. Elo A-L (1994) Assessment of mental stress factors at work. IN ZENZ, C., DICKERSON, O. B. & HORVATH, E. P. (Eds.) Occupational Medicine. 3 ed. St. Luis, Missouri, Mosby. 22 Elo A-L, Leppänen A, Lindström K & Ropponen T (1992) Occupational Stress Questionnaire: User's instruction (Reviews 19), Helsinki, Finnish Institute of Occupational Health. Elovainio M, Kivimäki M, Ek E, Vahtera J, Honkonen T, Taanila A, Veijola J & Järvelin M-R 2007. The effect of pre-employment factors on job control, job strain and psychological distress: A 31-year longitudinal study. Social Science and Medicine 65: 187-199. Evans GW & Kim P 2007. Childhood poverty and health: Cumulative risk exposure and stress dysregulation. Psychological Science 18: 953-957. Flouri E 2004. Psychological outcomes in midadulthood associated with mother's child-rearing attitudes in early childhood: Evidence from the 1970 British birth cohort. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 13: 35-41. Hane AA & Fox NA 2006. Ordinary variations in maternal caregiving influence human infants’ stress reactivity. Psychological Science 17: 550-556. Heim C, Newport J, Wagner D, Wilcox MM, Miller AH & Nemeroff CB 2002. The role of early adverse experience and adulthood stress in the prediction of neuroendocrine stress reactivity in women: A multiple regression analysis. Depression and anxiety 15: 117-125. Hemingway H & Marmot M 1999. Psychosocial factors in the aetiology and prognosis of coronary heart disease: systematic review of prospective cohort studies. British Medical Journal 318: 1460-1467. Hemmingsson T & Lundberg I 2006. Is the association between low job control and coronary heart disease confounded by risk factors measured in 23 childhood and adolescence among Swedish males 40-53 years of age? International Journal of Epidemiology 35: 616-622. Hintsa T, Kivimäki M, Elovainio M, Hintsanen M, Pulkki-Råback L & Keltikangas-Järvinen L 2007. Preemployment family factors as predictors of effort/reward imbalance in adulthood: a prospective 18-year follow-up in the Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study. Journal of occupational and environmental medicine 49: 659-666. Hintsa T, Kivimäki M, Elovainio M, Keskivaara P, Hintsanen M, Pulkki-Råback L & Keltikangas-Järvinen L 2006. Parental socioeconomic position and parental life satisfaction as predictors of job strain in adulthood: 18-year follow-up of the Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 61: 243-249. Hintsa T, Kivimäki M, Elovainio M, Vahtera J, Hintsanen M, Viikari J, Raitakari OT & Keltikangas-Järvinen L 2008. Is the association between job strain and carotid intima-media thickness attributable to pre-employment environmental and dispositional factors? The Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns study. Occupational and Environmental Medicine 65: 676682. Hintsanen M, Elovainio M, Puttonen S, Kivimäki M, Koskinen T, Raitakari OT & Keltikangas-Järvinen L 2007. Effort-reward imbalance, heart rate, and heart rate variability: the Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns study. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine 14: 202-212. Hintsanen M, Kivimäki M, Elovainio M, Pulkki-Råback L, Keskivaara P, Juonala M, Raitakari OT & Keltikangas-Järvinen L 2005. Job strain and early 24 atherosclerosis: The Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns study. Psychosomatic Medicine 67: 740-747. Karasek R & Theorell T (1990) Healthy Work: Stress, Productivity, and the Reconstruction of Working life, New York, Basic Books cop. Karasek RA 1979. Job demands, job decision latitude, and mental strain: implications for job redesign. Administrative Science Quarterly 24: 285308. Karasek RA (1985) Job content questionnaire and user's guide. Revision 1.1, Los Angeles, Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, University of Southern Los Angeles. Kivimäki M, Hintsanen M, Keltikangas-Järvinen L, Elovainio M, Pulkki-Råback L, Vahtera J, Viikari JSA & Raitakari OT 2007. Early risk factors, job strain, and atherosclerosis among men in their 30s: The Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study. American Journal of Public Health 97: 450452. Kivimäki M, Virtanen M, Elovainio M, Kouvonen A, Väänänen A & Vahtera J 2006. Work stress in the etiology of coronary heart disease - a metaanalysis. Scandinavian Journal of Work Environment and Health 32: 431-442. Landsbergis PA, Schnall PL, Warren K, Pickering TG & Schwartz JE 1994. Association between ambulatory blood pressure and alternative formulations of job strain. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment and Health 20: 349-363. 25 Liu D, Diorio J, Tannenbaum B, Caldji C, Francis D, Freedman A, Sharma S, Pearson D, Plotsky PM & Meaney MJ 1997. Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Science 277: 1659-1662. Luecken LJ 1998. Childhood attachment and loss experiences affect adult cardiovascular and cortisol function. Psychosomatic Medicine 60: 765772. Makkonen T, Ruoppila I, Rönkä T, Timonen S, Valvanne L & Österlund K (1981) Operaatio perhe - Isä ja synnytys. Lapsiraportti A 34. Helsinki, Mannerheimin lastensuojeluliitto. Oldehinkel AJ, Veenstra R, Ormel J, de Winter AF & Verhulst FC 2006. Temperament, parenting, and depressive symptoms in a population sample of preadolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 47: 684-695. Parasuraman S & Simmers CA 2001. Type of employment, work-family conflict and well-being: A comparative study. Journal of Organizational Behavior 22: 551-568. Pulkki L, Keltikangas-Järvinen L, Ravaja N & Viikari J 2003. Child-rearing attitudes and cardiovascular risk among children: moderating influence of parental socioeconomic status. Preventive Medicine 36: 55-63. Puustinen M, Lyyra A-L, Metsapelto R-L & Pulkkinen L 2008. Children's help seeking: The role of parenting. Learning and Instruction 18: 160-171. Raitakari OT, Juonala M, Rönnemaa T, Keltikangas-Järvinen L, Räsänen L, Pietikäinen M, Hutri-Kähönen N, Taittonen L, Jokinen E, Marniemi J, Jula 26 A, Telama R, Kähönen M, Lehtimäki T, Akerblom HK & Viikari JSA 2008. Cohort profile: The Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study. International Journal of Epidemiology 37: 1220-1226. Repetti RL, Taylor SE & Seeman TE 2002. Risky families: family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological Bulletin 128: 330-366. Siegrist J 1998. Reciprocity in basic social exchange and health: Can we reconcile person-based with population-based psychosomatic research? Journal of Psychosomatic Research 45: 99-105. Siegrist J & Peter R 1994. Job stressors and coping characteristics in workrelated disease: issues of validity. Work and Stress 8: 130-140. Siegrist J, Starke D, Chandola T, Godin I, Marmot M, Niedhammer I & Peter R 2004. The measurement of effort–reward imbalance at work: European comparisons. Social Science and Medicine 58: 1483-1499. Stansfeld S & Candy B 2006. Psychosocial work environment and mental health - a meta-analytic review. Scandinavian Journal of Work Environment and Health 32: 443-462. Steelman LM, Assel MA, Swank PR, Smith KE & Landry SH 2002. Early maternal warm responsiveness as a predictor of child social skills: Direct and indirect paths of influence over time. Applied Developmental Psychology 23: 135-156. 27 Table 1. Characteristics of Study Variables and Pearson Correlations between the Study Variables in Women and in Men. WOMEN (n = 456) Mean SD 1. 1. Age 30,9 ( 4,50 ) 1 2. Educational level 2,32 ( 0,55 ) -0,030 3. Occupational status 2,03 ( 0,66 ) 0,112 * 0,565 ** 4. Maternal education 10,0 ( 2,93 ) -0,168 ** 0,293 ** 0,240 ** 5. Childhood family income 5,90 ( 1,91 ) 0,253 ** 0,205 ** 6. Intolerance¹ 1,95 ( 0,53 ) 7. Lack of emotional warmth¹ 1,53 ( 0,42 ) 8. Job Demand 2,91 9. Job Control 0,031 -0,390 ** 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 1 0,389 ** -0,141 ** -0,114 * ( 0,66 ) 0,043 0,201 ** 0,123 ** 3,74 ( 0,78 ) 0,152 ** 0,295 ** 0,357 ** 0,204 ** 0,198 ** -0,152 ** -0,090 0,198 ** 10. Reward 3,81 ( 0,71 ) 0,000 0,044 0,108 * 0,083 0,127 ** -0,069 -0,053 -0,128 ** 0,444 ** 11. Job strain -0,84 ( 0,92 ) 12. Effort-reward imbalance² -0,12 ( 0,15 ) -0,108 * 0,108 * -0,217 ** 0,016 0,068 1 0,079 -0,002 1 -0,160 ** -0,067 0,320 ** -0,015 -0,035 0,013 1 0,015 1. Age 30,4 ( 4,30 ) 2. Educational level 2,27 ( 0,55 ) 0,042 3. Occupational status 1,97 ( 0,88 ) 0,131 ** 4. Maternal education 10,4 ( 3,30 ) -0,071 0,199 ** 0,233 ** 5. Childhood family income 6,02 ( 1,88 ) 0,066 0,199 ** 0,242 ** 6. Intolerance¹ 2,05 ( 0,59 ) 7. Lack of emotional warmth¹ 1,59 ( 0,50 ) 0,025 1 1 -0,194 ** 0,139 ** 0,088 0,546 ** -0,713 ** -0,471 ** -0,064 -0,106 * 0,043 0,035 0,782 ** -0,119 * -0,700 ** 1 1 0,663 ** 1 -0,184 ** MEN (n = 387) -0,335 ** 12. 1 -0,115 * 0,028 11. 1 -0,016 -0,099 * 10. 1 1 0,367 ** 1 -0,092 -0,061 0,033 -0,048 -0,023 -0,027 -0,054 -0,044 1 0,345 ** 1 1 0,662 ** 1 28 8. Job Demand 2,83 ( 0,62 ) 0,187 ** 0,128 * 0,209 ** 0,070 0,056 -0,046 9. Job Control 3,88 ( 0,72 ) 0,128 * 0,261 ** 0,433 ** 0,111 * 0,066 -0,075 -0,134 ** 10. Reward 3,84 ( 0,67 ) -0,075 0,057 0,155 ** 0,042 0,073 -0,044 -0,063 11. Job strain -1,05 ( 0,84 ) 0,027 -0,218 ** -0,044 -0,015 0,031 12. Effort-reward imbalance² -0,14 ( 0,13 ) 0,169 ** 0,020 0,000 -0,004 *Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed). **Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). ¹Mean of assessments in 1980 and 1983 ²Logarithmically transformed -0,131 * 0,062 0,065 0,008 1 0,218 ** -0,064 1 0,468 ** 1 0,121 * 0,546 ** -0,698 ** -0,448 ** 0,033 0,800 ** -0,105 * -0,631 ** 1 0,677 ** 1 29 Table 2. Linear Regression Analyses of Intolerance and Lack of Emotional Warmth Predicting Work Stress and Its Components in Men and in Women. Demands / Efforts Job control Adjusted R^2 Rewards Adjusted R^2 Job strain Adjusted R^2 Effrot-reward imbalance Adjusted R^2 Adjusted R^2 Beta p R^2 change Beta p R^2 change Beta p R^2 change Beta p R^2 change Beta p R^2 change Model 1 .035 .490 _.001 .001 _.110 .029 .029 .010 _.081 .111 .001 .006 .119 .019 .017 .012 .063 .215 <.001 .003 Model 2 .045 .364 .042 .002 _.088 .058 .172 .007 _.075 .137 .018 .005 .108 .028 .088 .010 .067 .186 .028 .004 Model 1 .012 .798 _.002 <.001 _.103 .027 .029 .010 _.053 .258 _.002 .003 .096 .040 .015 .009 .033 .483 _.003 .001 Model 2 .035 .462 .041 .001 _.037 .394 .167 .001 _.037 .441 .014 .001 .057 .219 .081 .003 .037 .430 .025 .001 Model 1 .019 .719 .030 <.001 _.036 .502 .013 .001 _.078 .149 .006 .005 .045 .406 _.003 .002 .060 .264 .027 .003 Model 2 .021 .685 .057 <.001 _.033 .501 .184 .001 _.081 .131 .030 .006 .044 .409 .039 .002 .063 .239 .021 .004 Model 1 .003 .954 .030 <.001 _.138 .006 .030 .019 _.061 .229 .004 .004 .120 .018 .010 .014 .028 .573 .024 .001 Model 2 .010 .841 .057 <.001 _.126 .006 .200 .016 _.056 .270 .028 .003 .115 .021 .051 .013 .030 .552 .019 .001 Women (n = 456) Intolerance¹ Lack of emotional warmth¹ Men (n = 387) Intolerance¹ Lack of emotional warmth¹ 30 ¹ The mean of assessements in 1980 and 1983 Model 1 - Controlled for age Model 2 - Controlled for age, educational level, occupational status, maternal education, and childhood family income. 31 Table 3. Linear Regression Analyses of Intolerance and Lack of Emotional Warmth Predictiong Work Stress and Its Components in Younger and Older Age Groups of Men. Job demand Beta p Job control Adjusted R^2 R^2 change Beta p Rewards Adjusted R^2 R^2 change Intolerance¹ Beta p Job strain Adjusted R^2 R^2 change Beta Effor-Reward imbalance Adjusted R^2 p R^2 change Beta Adjusted R^2 p R^2 change Younger age group (ages 24 - 30, n = 229) Model 1 .015 .817 _.006 <.001 .023 .728 .003 .001 .024 .725 _.005 .001 _.010 .877 _.005 <.001 .002 .981 _.003 <.001 Model 2 .017 .801 .024 <.001 .024 .701 .171 .001 .008 .906 .034 <.001 _.010 .882 .049 <.001 .012 .855 _.012 <.001 Model 1 _.023 .734 _.006 .001 _.070 .290 .007 .005 _.033 .620 _.004 .001 .048 .471 _.003 .002 _.004 .949 _.003 <.001 Model 2 _.006 .923 .023 <.001 _.049 .429 .173 .002 _.039 .562 .035 .001 .040 .546 .050 .002 .011 .870 _.012 <.001 Lack of emotional warmth¹ Intolerance¹ Older age group (ages 33-39, n = 158) Model 1 .037 .653 .005 .001 _.131 .105 .039 .016 _.221 .007 .036 .046 .131 .112 .006 .016 .139 .092 .009 .018 Model 2 .055 .511 .048 .003 _.120 .119 .187 .013 _.255 .002 .059 .057 .136 .110 .016 .016 .174 .041 .021 .027 Model 1 .051 .522 .006 .003 _.249 .001 .085 .062 _.095 .237 _.001 .009 .234 .003 .045 .054 .076 .343 _.003 .006 Model 2 .047 .557 .047 .002 _.259 <.001 .241 .065 _.116 .153 .013 .013 .238 .003 .056 .055 .084 .301 <.001 .007 Lack of emotional warmth¹ ¹ The mean of assessements in 1980 and 1983 32 Model 1 - Controlled for age Model 2 - Controlled for age, educational level, occupational status, maternal education years and family income in childhood 33 Table 4. Linear Regression Analyses of Intolerance and Lack of Emotional Warmth Predictiong Work Stress and Its Components in Younger and Older Age Groups of Women. Job demand Beta p Job control Adjusted R^2 R^2 change Beta p Rewards Adjusted R^2 R^2 change Intolerance¹ Beta p Job strain Adjusted R^2 R^2 change Beta Effor-Reward imbalance Adjusted R^2 p R^2 change Beta Adjusted R^2 p R^2 change Younger age group (ages 24 - 30, n = 241) Model 1 .038 .567 _.005 .001 _.077 .237 .007 .006 _.050 .442 _.006 .002 .095 .148 .004 .009 .043 .509 _.005 .002 Model 2 .035 .586 .034 .001 _.074 .220 .156 .005 _.043 .516 <.001 .002 .090 .158 .058 .008 .038 .563 .017 .001 Model 1 .032 .621 _.006 .001 _.146 .023 .023 .021 _.062 .338 _.004 .004 .152 .018 .019 .023 .048 .456 _.005 .002 Model 2 .056 .383 .035 .003 _.128 .033 .167 .016 _.079 .228 .004 .006 .153 .016 .073 .023 .074 .251 .021 .005 Lack of emotional warmth¹ Intolerance¹ Older age group (ages 33-39, n = 215) Model 1 .030 .676 _.005 .001 _.134 .053 .058 .017 _.103 .149 .003 .010 .130 .065 .024 .016 .076 .287 _.004 .005 Model 2 .049 .489 .045 .002 _.084 .194 .199 .006 _.079 .263 .037 .006 .104 .128 .103 .010 .073 .302 .038 .005 Model 1 _.001 .985 _.006 <.001 _.038 .572 .042 .001 _.040 .559 _.005 .002 .029 .666 .009 .001 .021 .762 _.009 <.001 Model 2 _.010 .891 .043 <.001 .059 .354 .196 .003 .022 .748 .032 <.001 _.055 .419 .095 .003 _.024 .730 .034 .001 Lack of emotional warmth¹ ¹ The mean of assessements in 1980 and 1983 34 Model 1 - Controlled for age Model 2 - Controlled for age, educational level, occupational status, maternal education years and family income in childhood