Knowledge Acquisition Theory 1 Running head: INSTRUCTIONAL COMMUNICATION MATTERS Instructional communication matters: Refinement of measures of perceived cognitive learning from a Knowledge Acquisition Theory perspective Robert J. Trader, PHD October 11, 2008 McDaniel College Dr. Robert J. Trader 405 Pleasanton Road, A32 Westminster, MD 21157 Email: rtrader@mcdaniel.edu Phone: 859-227-6703 Knowledge Acquisition Theory 2 Abstract This study provides further evidence that Knowledge Acquisition Theory’s receiver oriented message perspective provides valuable insights into instructional communication that were previously unavailable. This study tests three new measures of perceived cognitive learning. Results of this test indicate that the Data Obtained, Information Obtained, and Knowledge Obtained Measures were sensitive enough to represent these constructs in the minds of two distinct groups of learners. The study also provides evidence that message clarity and message relevance are more complex constructs than had previously been explored within instructional communication research. Knowledge Acquisition Theory accounts for 58-84% of the variance in undergraduate perceived cognitive learning. In submitting the attached paper or proposal, I/we recognize that this submission is considered a professional responsibility. I/we agree to present this panel or paper if it is accepted and programmed. I further recognize that all who attend and present at ECA's annual meeting must register and pay required fees. Knowledge Acquisition Theory 3 Instructional communication matters: Refinement of measures of perceived cognitive learning from a Knowledge Acquisition Theory perspective Introduction Cognitive learning is rather poorly measured and defined within instructional communication research. In fact, a clear vision of what higher education is for seems to be generally lacking (Sprague, 1992, 1993). In order to correct this lack of clarity in definitions for higher education and cognitive learning, this study introduces Knowledge Acquisition Theory (KAT) and new measures of cognitive learning based upon the definition of cognitive learning proposed in Knowledge Acquisition Theory. The dominant paradigm in instructional communication research holds that communication in the classroom is important because the difference between knowing and teaching is communication (Hurt, Scott, & McCroskey, 1978). From the perspective of this paradigm, higher education is teachers pouring their accumulated knowledge into the waiting and empty minds of students via communication in the classroom. Unsurprisingly, the measure associated with cognitive learning as defined within the dominant paradigm is the “learning loss” measure (Chesebro & McCroskey, 2000; Richmond, McCroskey, Kearney & Plax, 1987). Consistent with the view of higher education as a funnel system from teachers to students emphasizing mimicry and memorization, the learning loss measure attributes student learning to the presence or absence of an “ideal” instructor. Also consistent with this view, the instructional technique attracting the most research attention is the most teacher centered instructional method—the lecture. Message variables such as “clarity” and “relevance” also reflect the teacher centered perspective of the dominant paradigm. “Clarity” is “teacher clarity” defined as “a cluster of teacher behaviors that result in learners gaining knowledge or understanding of a topic, if they possess adequate interest, aptitude, opportunity, Knowledge Acquisition Theory 4 and time” (Cruickshank & Kennedy, 1986, p. 43). Relevance is the ability of teachers to persuade students that course content is somehow relevant to students’ lives (Frymier & Shulman, 1995; Frymier & Houser, 1998; Frymier, Schulman, & Houser, 1996) regardless of whether said content is truly in some way relevant. In contrast to the teacher centered traditional view of instructional communication, Knowledge Acquisition Theory adopts a receiver-oriented message perspective to understand the cognitive learning process within the higher education course. Communication in the classroom from a KAT perspective is more than just teacher talk. Students are exposed to a wide variety of messages within and outside of a higher education course and these messages can be more or less relevant to different aspects of a student’s life. These messages range from interactions with teachers and classmates to interactions with textbooks and other learning materials. While lectures have certainly not disappeared from higher education classrooms, other teaching and learning methods such as discussions are often designed into a course to facilitate some aspect of cognitive learning. It is not so much that lectures are outdated or that discussions are somehow inherently better (or worse) than lectures, it is more that different message dissemination techniques serve different functions. These functions are contingent upon beliefs about what higher education is designed to accomplish. Higher education does not occur within a vacuum. Rather, higher education reflects the needs and goals of society, government, and industry. The Commission on the Future of Higher Education in their 2006 report states, “With too few exceptions, higher education has yet to address the fundamental issues of how academic programs and institutions must be transformed to serve the changing needs of a knowledge economy” (p. 25). The 2007 Education Criteria for Performance Excellence put forth by the Baldridge National Quality Program of the National Institute of Standards and Technology proposes that higher education Knowledge Acquisition Theory 5 institutes can meet the demands of a knowledge driven society by adopting a learner-centered approach focusing on active learning and the development of problem-solving skills. The Boyer report (1998) stresses the need for integrating student and faculty research in order to better develop student problem solving skills. Ultimately, the needs and demands of life in the 21st century suggest that the goal of higher education is to produce knowledge workers: people who are able to identify problems and work out solutions to those problems. The smorgasbord approach to higher education in which students sample tidbits of information from a variety of disciplines in order to acquire a well rounded “liberal” education is somewhat outmoded. This model was put into a place during a time period when exposure to information was extremely limited. As little as 30 years ago, information exposure within a higher education context was restricted pretty much to courses students took and access to information outside of class was restricted to what was available in the institution’s academic library or the media (assuming that a student utilized either one of these sources). Students in the 21st century face an overload of information. There is an overload of information and data flooding our media, and creating congestion on our information superhighway, the Internet. The challenge is no longer gaining access to information. The challenge is to find and access quality information, to make sense out of this information, and to work out viable solutions to problems based on the highest quality information available. We need people who can synthesize information, create order out of the current chaos, and make connections between the information available (much of which lies dormant) and the problems that face us. Simply stated, our knowledge driven society creates a demand for creative problem solvers who know how to transform data into information into knowledge. Knowledge Acquisition Theory 6 KAT addresses the process for developing problem-solving skills. First, KAT building off of Bloom’s (1956) taxonomy of cognitive learning defines cognitive learning as a three part process: 1) acquiring data (encoding and memorization), 2) acquiring information (comparing and contrasting data, analyzing and synthesizing data; critical thinking), and finally 3) acquiring knowledge (testing what is believed to be true, applying information to the solution of problems and measuring the success of this application). Different types of interactions with messages are likely to produce different gains in data, information, or knowledge. KAT posits that the quality of lectures, notetaking, highlighting, listening, and reading (data acquisition behaviors) predict increases in data acquisition. Information is acquired through some combination of discussion, writing essays, and debating (information acquisition behaviors). Finally, knowledge is acquired through doing research, through creating finished products, and through successful completion of problem solving exercises (knowledge acquisition behaviors). The three happen in sequence. One cannot have knowledge without having acquired information, nor information without having acquired data. The real challenge is the quality of the interaction students have with instructional messages. First, data must be attended. Gibson (1950) points out that in order for a stimulus to be responded to, the stimulus must first be perceived. Scholars from the field of communication echo Gibson’s sentiment. Donohew, Palmgreen, and Duncan (1980) and later Donohew, Palmgreen, and Lorch (1994) in their Activation Model of Information Exposure (also Donohew et al., 1991, 1998) advance and offer considerable support for the claim that persuasion begins with attention. What is persuasion other than changes in affect, cognition, and/or behavior and hence learning? In Knowledge Acquisition Theory (KAT), data must first be attended in order for decoding to ensue. At the minimum, in order for data to be attended within a higher education course, a lecture must be listened to or a textbook must be read. Knowledge Acquisition Theory 7 While these behaviors do not guarantee that data has been attended, performance of these behaviors increases the probability that data (and hence information and knowledge) will be acquired. Second, data must be remembered. If data are not remembered, they cannot be processed. In other words, data must be stored long enough in memory for data to be able to be analyzed, evaluated, compared, contrasted, and integrated with other data. Behaviors related to positive increases in recall and retention include highlighting key data, notetaking, outlining, summarizing, and discussion (Doctorow, Marks, & Wittrock, 1978; Hooper, Sales, & Rysavy, 1994; Taylor & Berkowitz, 1980; Wittrock & Alesandrini, 1980). Again, performance of these behaviors increases the probability that data (and hence information) will be recalled, but does not guarantee data (or information or knowledge) acquisition. The first and second steps when combined equal decoding. In other words, decoding combines attending data with the lowest level of Bloom’s (1956) taxonomy of cognitive learning objectives, remembering data (Anderson & Krathwohl, 2001). Decoding is simply data acquisition, and there is a final step necessary for data to be transformed into information. The final step for data to be transformed into information is information processing. Information processing is roughly equivalent to the revised higher order cognitive learning objectives of Bloom’s taxonomy (Anderson & Krathwohl, 2001) namely analyzing, evaluating, synthesizing, and integrating. While these cognitive learning objectives are loosely based on Bloom’s taxonomy, they in no way follow Bloom’s prescribed order since these actions can occur simultaneously as well as in tandem. Behaviors associated with this final step include: a) critical thinking, b) summarizing, and c) thinking deeply about the relationships between data. Yet again, performance of these behaviors increases the probability that data will be Knowledge Acquisition Theory 8 transformed into information, but does not guarantee data (or information) acquisition. For information to be transformed into knowledge, data must first be acquired through decoding and data must be processed into information. Thus, knowledge acquisition is dependent on data and information acquisition. However, in order for information to be transformed into knowledge, information must be usable to students. There is a difference between knowing and doing. Data and information are known (decoded and analyzed). However, until data as information becomes usable and thus the basis for action, data as information is not knowledge. Usable information (knowledge) can be applied to problem solving or future learning. Behaviors likely to support this final transformation based partially on recommendations for higher education reform of research universities in the Boyer Report (1998) include: 1) doing applied research, 2) creating finished products, 3) giving presentations, 4) leading discussions, and 5) providing systematic accounts of how information could be used to solve hypothetical problems. Of course, performing these activities in no way guarantees that information will be transformed into knowledge. However, performing these activities increases the probability that information will be transformed into knowledge (usable information). In reverse engineering the challenge of student acquisition of knowledge in the higher education context, it becomes apparent that the knowledge acquisition behaviors described above are all commonly associated with communication. For data acquisition, the communication is predominantly receptive. Students listen to or read other people’s generated messages. For information acquisition, the communication is predominantly interactive. Messages are exchanged between instructors and students and/or between students and other students. For knowledge acquisition, the behavior is predominantly productive. Students become the producers of messages. Thus, for students to acquire knowledge, they have to move Knowledge Acquisition Theory 9 from rather passive message receivers to active message producers. Messages are, of course, the central communication science construct (Powers, 1995). This being the case and since messages are central to data, information, and knowledge acquisition, the next step is to consider how the characteristics of messages relate to student knowledge acquisition behaviors in the higher education context. To understand how messages and their design relate to student behavior, Knowledge Acquisition Theory (KAT) builds on Zipf’s principle of least effort (1949). The principle of least effort states that human beings have a tendency to lower their goals if meeting a higher goal requires more effort than the person currently wishes to make. For example, a student may knowingly turn in a lower quality paper if submitting a higher quality paper requires more effort than the student is current willing to make. Knowledge Acquisition Theory argues that messages can be designed and that interactions with messages can be designed to maximize outcomes while seemingly minimizing efforts. It seems obvious that students who make minimal effort to acquire data, information and/or knowledge are less likely to have obtained that data, information, and/or knowledge or to be able to utilize the information they have for the solving of problems. In fact, research in persuasion suggests that positive or negative attitude change is contingent upon the depth of information processing (Cacioppo & Petty, 1984; Kruglanski, 1989, 1990; Kruglanski & Orehek, 2007; Kruglanski & Thompson, 1999; Petty & Wegner, 1999). In other word, people have an internal monologue, and when people are exposed to some information, this monologue elaborates upon some physical or conceptual aspect of the message. According to the Elaboration Likelihood Model, people elaborate (the internal monologue focuses) on one of two sets of cues: a) central cues such as the argument and the evidence supporting the argument or b) peripheral cues such as appearance, credibility, or other cues not Knowledge Acquisition Theory 10 directly related to the argument that people can still give themselves messages about via their internal monologue. For example, when listening to a speech, audience members may focus on the appearance of the speaker (i.e. “This person looks intelligent and trustworthy. This person seems knowledgeable. So, I’ll accept what they say, at least for now.”). This is an example of the peripheral route to persuasion. Or, an audience member could focus on what the speaker is saying (i.e. “I read a book about this once that completely agrees with what this woman is saying. As a matter of fact, that last statement makes complete sense.”). This would be an example of the central route to persuasion. The ELM claims that deeper processing, central route processing, is more persuasive. In other words, the attitudinal change is more resistance to subsequent change when people process messages using the central route. If this is indeed the case, then we need to have our students deeply process instructional messages. Unfortunately, the ELM offers little advice on how to construct messages to encourage deep processing. The ELM does suggest that people with more background knowledge on a topic tend to process messages centrally and more deeply. Messages can be designed to make interaction with those messages appear easier or more appealing and this is likely to encourage depth of processing. The two variables suggested by the instructional communication research literature for making messages appear easier or more appealing are clarity and relevance. A clear message psychologically appears easier to understand, and thus students will more likely process a clear message deeply. A relevant message is more likely to motivate one to more deeply process that message since the message is more appealing, and potentially has a higher sense of importance. As stated earlier, existing instructional communication research has focused exclusively on teacher clarity and the ability of teachers to persuade students that content is relevant. Knowledge Acquisition Theory 11 Knowledge Acquisition Theory refines the clarity and relevance constructs. A higher education course consists of a variety of messages including: 1) what textbooks say (Textbook Clarity), 2) what instructors say (Instructor Clarity), 3) rules for class interaction and for evaluating progress (Procedural Clarity), and 4) the overall goals of the course usually in the form of a syllabus (Course Clarity). Relevant messages can be messages given in a course that are relevant to a student’s 1) present life (Primal Relevance), 2) possible future life (Distal Relevance), and/or 3) that have already proven useful (Generic Relevance). Trader’s (2007) unpublished dissertation asks the question which of these dimensions of clarity and/or relevance best predict student perceptions of knowledge acquired from the completion of a college course. The results showed that Generic Relevance, Course Clarity, Primal Relevance, Distal Relevance, and Textbook Clarity significantly predicted student perceptions of knowledge acquisition. In fact, these variables accounted for 63% of the variance in student perceptions of knowledge obtained from completion of an undergraduate course. In Trader’s (2007) unpublished dissertation, the measure used for cognitive learning failed to make the distinction between data, information, and/or knowledge obtained from successful completion of an undergraduate course. The current study offers new measures of data, information, and knowledge obtained. Thus, the first research question this study asks is: RQ1: Do the Data Obtained Measure (DOM), Information Obtained Measure (IOM), and Knowledge Obtained Measure adequately capture these constructs? Also, in Trader’s (2007) unpublished dissertation, Procedural Clarity and Presentation Clarity dropped out of the final model failing to be significant predictors of student perceived cognitive learning. Does this still hold true when cognitive learning is divided into data obtained, information obtained, and knowledge obtained? Knowledge Acquisition Theory 12 RQ2: Which message variables (out of the four dimensions of clarity and the three dimensions of relevance) best predict data obtained? RQ3: Which message variables (out of the four dimensions of clarity and the three dimensions of relevance) best predict information obtained? RQ4: Which message variables (out of the four dimensions of clarity and the three dimensions of relevance) best predict knowledge obtained? Knowledge Acquisition Theory 13 Methods In order to answer the research questions, data for this study were obtained using an online survey program. An Independent Samples T-Test was used to answer Research Question 1. Research Questions 2-4 were answered using stepwise regression. Sampling Students enrolled in upper level biology and communication courses at a small private liberal arts college on the east coast were asked to fill in the online survey. These students received no credit or incentives for participating in the study. Participation was completely voluntary and anonymous. This study had 56 participants, 34 females and 32 males. Twenty of these students were biology majors and 36 were Communication majors. There were 11 sophomores, 34 juniors, and 21 seniors. Procedures Students whose last names began with A, C, E, G, I, K, M, O, Q, S, U, W, or Y were asked to consider the course from which they felt they had learned the most while attending college (n = 32). And students whose last names began with B, D, F, H, J, L, N, P, R, T, V, X, or Z were asked to consider the course from which they felt they had learned the least while attending college (n = 24). Students were asked to verify whether they were considering the course from which they had learned the least or the course from which they ad learned the most. Students were then asked to fill in their answers to several Likert scales as honestly as possible. Measures The measures used in this study were: 1) Data Obtained Measure (DOM), Information Obtained Measure (IOM), and Knowledge Obtained Measure (KOM) to capture student perceptions of cognitive learning, 2) Textbook Clarity Measure (TCM), Instructor Clarity Measure (ICM), Procedural Clarity Measure (PCM), and Course Knowledge Acquisition Theory 14 Clarity Measure (CCM) to measure student perceptions of message clarity, and finally 3) Primal Relevance Measure (PRM), Distal Relevance Measure (DRM), and the Generic Relevance Measure (GRM) to measure student perceptions of message relevance. The clarity and relevance measures were used in Trader’s (2007) dissertation, but the three measures of perceived cognitive learning are new. Measures of Perceived Cognitive Learning Cognitive learning is measured in a variety of ways. One method is to give students a test. This test is course specific and cannot be used as a measure of cognitive learning in other courses. Realizing the limitations with measuring cognitive learning in this way, instructional communication scholars devised a measure of cognitive learning that is not course specific. This measure, called the “learning loss” measure, consists of the following two questions on a 0-9 scale: 1) “How much did you learn in class” and 2) “How much do you think you could have learned in the class had you had an ideal instructor” (Richmond, McCroskey, Kearney & Plax, 1987). The first item is subtracted from the second in order to compute the perceived learning loss. Chesebro and McCroskey (2000) claim that the learning loss measure is valid, and its use has been supported in one comparative experimental research study. Though this measure is convenient and can be applied to any classroom context, it oversimplifies perceptions of cognitive learning (Witt, Wheeless, & Allen, 2004) and posits student cognitive learning as the result of the efforts or existence of a perceived “ideal” instructor. While this approach is consistent with the traditional knowledge transfer model and its emphasis on pedagogy, it is an inadequate measure for KAT in which the burden for knowledge acquisition is largely placed on the student. Henning in his unpublished dissertation (2006-2007) offers the following measure of perceived cognitive learning to replace the learning loss measure. Henning’s Knowledge Acquisition Theory 15 measure, the perceived Cognitive Learning Aptitude Measure (CLAM), is an 8-item 5point Likert-type scale derived from Bloom’s (1956) original conceptualization of cognitive learning. The measure has a 1-factor structure and a strong reliability of items (α = .928), and in Henning’s study explains 66.58% of the variance in perceived cognitive learning aptitude. While Henning’s Cognitive Learning Aptitude Measure is a strong measure of student perceptions of cognitive learning in a general sense, Trader’s (2007) unpublished dissertation created and adopted the Knowledge Gained Inventory to more fully capture student perceptions of knowledge gained from undergraduate course. Henning’s Cognitive Learning Aptitude Measure includes items about linking course content to other course content, but not to past or projected learning experiences and objectives. Knowledge gain is partly a question of knowing and partly a question of doing something with the information known such as using it in present or future learning endeavors. The Knowledge Gained Inventory (KGI) included items to capture both knowing and doing. In Trader’s (2007) unpublished dissertation, the reliability for KGI using Cronbach’s alpha was .89 with an Eigenvalue of 4.29. KGI explained 62% of student perceptions of knowledge acquisition in that study. However, KGI fails to capture the data obtained, information obtained, and knowledge obtained distinctions in cognitive learning proposed in KAT. Last spring, a focus group of 5 undergraduate students met to discuss cognitive learning as defined within Knowledge Acquisition Theory. For one hour this group discussed data obtained, information obtained, and knowledge obtained and how these could be measured. The group generated a consensual list of five items for each aspect of cognitive learning that the group believed captured the meaning of these terms. All of the resultant scales in this study related to perceived cognitive learning are unidimensional. All scales in this study are 5-point Likert scales. The Knowledge Acquisition Theory 16 items and their factor loadings for each of the perceived cognitive learning scales are as follows: Table 1: Data Obtained Measure (n = 55, = .95, Eigenvalue = 4.212 with 84% of the variance explained) Item I remember key words from the course. I remember key facts from the course. I can recall the main points from the course. I can recall the main themes from the course. I can identify important terms from the course. Loading .902 .949 .931 .889 .918 Table 2: Information Obtained Measure (n = 56, = .95, Eigenvalue = 4.159 with 83% of the variance explained) Item I can see how the ideas from the course fit together. I can evaluate the pros and cons of what I learned. I can look at the course topic from multiple perspectives. I can distinguish similarities and differences between sections of the course. I can summarize course materials. Loading .937 .915 .934 .918 .854 Table 3: Knowledge Obtained Measure (n = 55, = .97, Eigenvalue = 4.461 with 89% of the variance explained) Item I can use information from this course to solve problems. The information is usable. I can apply what I learned in this course to understanding my experiences. I can use course concepts to help solve problems. I can apply what I have learned to future learning activities. Loading .953 .930 .948 .963 .927 Measurement of Content Relevance The Primal Content Relevance Measure, the Distal Content Relevance Measure, and the Generic Content Relevance Measure are derived from Frymier and Shulman’s (1995) Content Relevance Scale ( = .88). The problem with Frymier and Shulman’s measure is that it emphasizes teacher behaviors rather than potential relevance of content to student learners and fails to indicate clearly the way in which content can be relevant to students. In fact, Frymier and Shulman’s scale with its emphasis on examples is more closely related to instructor clarity than relevance. Knowledge Acquisition Theory 17 Relevance in KAT is primarily the degree of utility of course content, and has a temporal dimension in that content can be relevant to a learner’s past experiences (has already shown relevance), present life, and/or future experiences or expectations. Further, content can be relevant to a learner’s life outside of a course and to a learner’s perceived future goals as well as relevant to all people. In order to tap into the above mentioned dimensions of content relevance, the following three measures were created. The Primal Relevance Measure consists of three items to capture student perceptions of the relevance and applicability course content had to their present lives. The Cronbach’s alpha reliability for the scale in this study is .94 with an Eigenvalue of 2.679 and 89% of the variance explained. In Trader’s (2007) unpublished dissertation, the reliability was .90. The factor loadings for the Primal Relevance Measure items are as follows: Table 4: Primal Relevance Measure (n = 56, = .94, Eigenvalue = 2.679 with 89% of the variance explained) Item The course topic was relevant to my life at that time. I could apply the course content to problems in my life at that time. The content applied to my own life at that time. Loading .943 .948 .944 The Distal Relevance Measure consists of four items that capture student perceptions of potential relevance and applicability of course content to future life, career, and learning. The Cronbach’s alpha reliability for the scale in this study is .94 with an Eigenvalue of 3.523 and 88% of the variance explained. In Trader’s (2007) unpublished dissertation, the reliability of the scale was .90. Factor loadings for the items are as follows: Table 5: Distal Relevance Measure (n = 56, = .94, Eigenvalue = 3.523 with 88% of the variance explained) Item I believed the course content would help me find a job. I thought I might need the information/skills from this course someday. I believed the knowledge I gained in this course would help me with other courses. Loading .927 .963 .941 Knowledge Acquisition Theory 18 I believed the course content would help me in my future life. .922 The Generic Relevance Measure contains six items asking about student perceptions of the relevance of course content to understanding of the world in which they live. Several items ask if content has already proven useful for work, life, and learning. And, one item asks if everyone needs to know the content. The Cronbach’s alpha for the Generic Relevance Measure in the present study is .95 with an Eigenvalue of 4.285 and 72% of the variance accounted for. In Trader’s (2007) unpublished dissertation, the reliability was .89. Factor loadings for the items of the Generic Relevance Measure are as follows: Table 6: Generic Relevance Measure (n = 56, = .95, Eigenvalue = 4.285 with 72% of the variance explained) Item I understand why the content of the course was important. The course content was similar to my own experiences. Since taking the course, I have a better understanding of the world I live in. I have used knowledge gained from this course in my other courses. I have used knowledge gained from this course outside of school in my work or internship. Everyone needs to know the content in this course. Loading .874 .816 .789 .868 .913 .803 Measurement of Message Clarity Instructional message clarity in KAT is divided into four measures: textbook clarity, presentation clarity, procedural clarity, and course clarity. The Textbook Clarity Measure is based on Chesebro and McCroskey's (1998) Teacher Clarity Short Inventory ( = .92). “My textbook” was used in place of “my instructor”. Several items were moved to other scales. And, several items were added to more clearly specify the clarity of textbooks. The most important aspects of textbook clarity as revealed in the research on clarity are: 1) clearly presenting concepts, 2) content organization, 3) having clear objectives, 4) use of examples, 5) readability, and 6) inclusion of multiple entry points for finding information. Textbooks that include Knowledge Acquisition Theory 19 these things are more likely to be perceived as high in clarity, and those that do not as low in clarity. The Textbook Clarity Measure used in this study consists of seven items reflecting the important aspects of textbook clarity listed in the preceding paragraph. The Cronbach’s alpha reliability for the Textbook Clarity Measure is .94 with an Eigenvalue of 5.1 with 73% of the variance accounted for. In Trader’s (2007) unpublished dissertation, the reliability was .91. Factor loadings for the Textbook Clarity Measure items are as follows: Table 7: Textbook Clarity Measure (n = 55, = .94, Eigenvalue = 5.1 with 73% of the variance explained) Item My textbook clearly defined major concepts. In general, I understood the textbook. The objectives for each chapter in the textbook were clear. My textbook was well organized. My textbook provided clear and relevant examples. My textbook used relevant graphics to explain key concepts. The textbook had a good index or glossary to find necessary information. Loading .861 .827 .924 .905 .850 .793 .806 The Instructor Clarity Measure and the Procedural Clarity Measure are based on Simonds’ (1997) Teacher Clarity Scale ( = .93) with the two subscales (content clarity and procedural clarity) having reliabilities of .88 each. Important aspects of presentation clarity are: 1) use of examples, 2) use of visual aids to clarify explanations, 3) use of previews and summaries, 4) stressing and defining main points, 5) staying on topic, and 6) having clear objectives. Inclusion of these aspects is likely to lead to higher perceptions of presentation clarity. The Instructor Clarity Measure used in this study consists of ten items. The Cronbach’s alpha for the Presentation Clarity Measure is .94 with an Eigenvalue of 6.604 with 66% of the variance accounted for. In Trader’s (2007) unpublished dissertation, the reliability was .95. Factor loadings for the measure are as follows: Knowledge Acquisition Theory 20 Table 8: Instructor Clarity Measure (n = 54, = .94, Eigenvalue = 6.604 with 66% of the variance explained) Item My instructor My instructor My instructor My instructor class. My instructor My instructor My instructor My instructor My instructor presented. My instructor was clear when presenting content. used examples when presenting content. related examples to the concept being discussed. used the board, transparencies, or other visual aids during Loading .913 .935 .873 .536 gave previews of material to be covered. gave summaries when presenting content. stressed important points. stayed on topic. clearly explained the objectives for the content being .754 .841 .787 .724 .913 defined major/new concepts. .878 Item 4 had a factor loading below the .6 level. The item was kept for theoretical reasons. The important aspects of procedural clarity are: 1) having clear goals and objectives, 2) having clear assessments and procedures for assessment, 3) checking student understanding, 4) providing feedback, and 5) having clear classroom policies and consequences for violation. The Procedural Clarity Measure used in this study consists of ten items. The Cronbach’s alpha reliability for the Procedural Clarity Measure is .96 with an Eigenvalue of 7.373 and 74% of the variance accounted for. In Trader’s (2007) unpublished dissertation, the reliability was .95. Factor loadings for the Procedural Clarity Measure items are as follows: Table 9: Procedural Clarity Measure (n = 56, = .96, Eigenvalue = 7.373 with 74% of the variance explained) Item My instructor My instructor My instructor My instructor My instructor My instructor My instructor My instructor My instructor My instructor violation. communicated classroom processes and expectations clearly. described assignments and how they should be done. asked if we knew what to do and how to do it. prepared us for the tasks we would be doing next. pointed out practical applications for coursework. prepared students for exams. explained how we should prepare for an exam. provided students with feedback of how well they were doing. provided rules and standards for satisfactory performance. communicated classroom policies and consequences for Loading .882 .889 .899 .926 .758 .875 .872 .815 .860 .797 Knowledge Acquisition Theory 21 Finally, the Course Clarity Measure is a blend of the most relevant items from the Chesebro and McCrockey clarity scale and the two Simonds’ scales mentioned above applied to the clarity of courses as a whole. Aspects of course clarity include: 1) clear organization, 2) clear goals and objectives, 3) integration of parts and whole, 4) a clear syllabus, 5) clear instructor expectations, and 6) clear relationship between assessment and content. The Course Clarity Measure used in this study consists of ten items reflecting the six aspects listed in the preceding sentence. Cronbach’s alpha reliability for the Course Clarity Measure is .94 with an Eigenvalue of 7.618 and 76% of the variance accounted for. In Trader’s unpublished dissertation, the reliability was .95. Factor loadings for the Course Clarity Measure items are as follows: Table 10: Course Clarity Measure (n = 53, = .94, Eigenvalue = 7.618 with 76% of the variance explained) Item The course was well organized. I understood the purpose or goal of the course. The different parts of the course contributed to my understanding of the course as a whole. The different parts of the course were good examples of the course's main goal or purpose. The syllabus was clear. The syllabus outlined the content of the course well. I knew what the instructor expected of me in this course. Testing reflected what I was supposed to have learned in the course. The course was what I expected it to be. The course was well integrated. Loading .743 .752 .854 The above measures were used to test the research questions of this study. Results of those tests are reported in the next section. .861 .825 .836 .725 .768 .766 .798 Knowledge Acquisition Theory 22 Results Research Question 1 asks whether the Data Obtained, Information Obtained, and Knowledge Obtained Measures adequately capture these distinctions. This was tested using an Independent Samples T-Test. Group 1 consists of students considering the course from which they felt they had learned the most and Group 2 consists of students considering the course from which they have learned the least. If the Data Obtained, Information Obtained, and Knowledge Obtained Measures show significant differences between the two groups, then it seems likely that the measures adequately capture these distinctions in perceived cognitive learning. A t-test indicates that Group 1 (M = 4.23, SD = .52) and Group 2 (M = 2.58, SD = 1.06) significantly differ in their perception of data obtained from an undergraduate course [t(54) = 7.682, p < .001]. A t-test indicates that Group 1 (M = 4.26, SD = .51) and Group 2 (M = 2.41, SD = .93) significantly differ in their perception of information obtained from an undergraduate course [t(54) = 9.521, p < .001]. Finally, a t-test indicates that Group 1 (M = 4.30, SD = .52) and Group 2 (M = 1.94, SD = .71) significantly differ in their perception of data obtained from an undergraduate course [t(54) = 14.32, p < .001]. Research Questions 2-4 asked which of the 4 dimensions of perceived message clarity and the 3 dimensions of perceived message relevance best predict student perceptions of data obtained, information obtained, and knowledge obtained from successful completion of an undergraduate course. Stepwise regression as opposed to hierarchical linear regression was used to test this since no theoretical differences are proposed for message characteristics in KAT. Results (adjusted R-Square = .60, F (2, 53) = 68.810, p < .001) indicate that Generic Relevance (β = .693, t = 7.667, p < .001) and Textbook Clarity (β = .203, t = 2.247, p < .029) are the significant predictors of Data Obtained. Primal Relevance (β = -.114, t = -.613, p = N.S.), Distal Relevance (β = -.280, t = -1.766, p = N.S.), Knowledge Acquisition Theory 23 Instructor Clarity (β = .173, t = 1.538, p = N.S.), Procedural Clarity (β = .115, t = 1.089, p = N.S.), and Course Clarity (β = .177, t = 1.532, p = N.S.) are not significant. Results of the stepwise regression to test RQ2 (adjusted R-Square = .70, F (3, 52) = 42.790, p < .001) indicate that Generic Relevance (β = .533, t = 5.260, p < .001), Course Clarity (β = .320, t = 3.195, p = .002), and Textbook Clarity (β = .163, t = 2.099, p = .041) are significant predictors of perceived Information Obtained in this study. Primal Relevance (β = .013, t = .082, p = N.S.), Distal Relevance (β = -.203, t = -1.425, p = N.S.), Instructor Clarity (β = .105, t = .579, p = N.S.), and Procedural Clarity (β = -.008, t = -.052, p = N.S.) are nonsignificant. Stepwise regression was also run on the data as above with the inclusion of the Data Obtained Measure since data acquisition is a prerequisite for information acquisition. Results (adjusted R-Square = .83, F(3, 52) = 90.188, p < .001) indicate that Data Obtained (β = .602, t = 6.991, p < .001), Course Clarity (β = .218, t = 2.845, p = .006), and Generic Relevance (β = .195, t = 2.2122, p = .039) are significant predictors of Information Obtained. Primal Relevance (β = .026, t = .218, p = N.S.), Distal Relevance (β = -.071, t = -.649, p = N.S.), Textbook Clarity (β = .051, t = .836, p = N.S.), Instructor Clarity (β = .051, t = .375, p = N.S.), and Procedural Clarity (β = .008, t = .068, p = N.S.) are nonsignificant. Finally, stepwise regression was also used to test RQ3. Results (adjusted RSquare = .74, F(2, 53) = 80.174, p < .001) indicate that Generic Relevance (β = .666, t = 7.252, p < .001) and Course Clarity (β = .226, t = 2.896, p = .005) are significant predictors of Knowledge Obtained. Primal Relevance (β = .186, t = 1.280, p = N.S.), Distal Relevance (β = .065, t = .490, p = N.S.), Textbook Clarity (β = .059, t = -.829, p = N.S.), Instructor Clarity (β = -.041, t = -.244, p = N.S.), and Procedural Clarity (β = -.067, t = -.478, p = N.S.) are nonsignificant. Knowledge Acquisition Theory 24 Since data is a prerequisite for information and information is a prerequisite for knowledge, stepwise regression with the addition of Data Obtained and Information Obtained was once again calculated on the data. Results (adjusted R-Square = .84, F(3, 52) = 95.216, p < .001) indicate that Information Obtained (β = .618, t = 6.674, p < .001), Generic Relevance (β = .397, t = 4.467, p < .001), and Textbook Clarity (β = -.156, t = -2.642, p = .011) significantly predict Knowledge Obtained. Primal Relevance (β = .127, t = 1.104, p = N.S.), Distal Relevance (β = .156, t = 1.497, p = N.S.), Instructor Clarity (β = .036, t = .460, p = N.S.), Course Clarity (β = .088, t = 1.102, p = N.S.), Procedural Clarity (β = .031, t = .444, p = N.S.), and Data Obtained (β = .081, t = .687, p = N.S.) are nonsignificant. Knowledge Acquisition Theory 25 Discussion The results of the present study are encouraging. A receiver oriented message perspective offers insights into perceived cognitive learning in the higher education course that were not previously available. First, the results of the Independent Samples T-Test comparing students considering either the course they thought they had learned the most from or the course they thought they had learned the least from provides considerable support for the claim that the Data Obtained, Information Obtained, and Knowledge Obtained Measures do indeed measure what they were designed to measure. One expects that in any course students will remember certain words, facts, and even some arguments for or against an idea. Even in the Data Obtained and Information Obtained cases, there were statistically significant differences in what students believed they had learned in the most and least conditions. The measures were sensitive enough to capture these differences. The difference in student perceptions of cognitive learning was of course biggest in relation to perceptions of Knowledge Obtained since knowledge is much more dependent upon scaffolding. Next, the results indicated considerable support for the claims that 1) different aspects of message clarity and message relevance are important during different phases in the knowledge acquisition process, 2) knowledge acquisition is a building process (knowledge builds off of prior knowledge), 3) that instructional communication is more than mere teacher talk, and 4) that there is a generation effect. Generic Relevance is significant in the results for every regression run in this study. Generic Relevance is content that has already proven useful in a student's life. Obviously, knowledge builds upon prior knowledge and it is thus important to provide students with knowledge upon which to build. Solutions that have worked in the past are likely to be applied again in the future. However, this approach to problem Knowledge Acquisition Theory 26 solving can be problematic. There is the danger of trained incapacity, for example, in which solutions to problems are chosen based on convenience (the method of tenacity and least effort) and lack of recognition of viable alternatives (people are blinded by their prior training). In other words, people are trained to repeat the same mistakes from the past over and over again. The results suggest that it may be necessary to leave a certain amount of ambiguity in our messages in order to ensure that student thinking about problems goes beyond the obvious solution. This is especially true in regards to Knowledge Obtained. Textbook Clarity is negative and significant in the final regression equation. Textbook Clarity, Instructor Clarity, and Procedural Clarity are negative but nonsignificant in the fourth regression equation. This suggests that too much clarity stifles creativity and fails to stimulate the brain enough to think outside the box. This also provides support for a generation effect. A generation effect is the idea that the brain requires stimulation through omitting a certain amount of information that the brain then fills in. In other words, giving the complete answer is less stimulating than giving a partial answer. Robust self-generation effects have been found within a wide variety of contexts such as to increase reading comprehension and retention (Doctorow, Marks, & Wittrock, 1978; Hooper, Sales, & Rysavy, 1994; Taylor & Berkowitz, 1980; Wittrock & Alesandrini, 1980); the solving of mathematical problems (Lawson & Chinnappan, 1994; McNamara & Healy, 1995a, 1995b, 2000); the retention of nonwords (Begg, Snider, Foley, & Goddard, 1989; Brooks, Dansereau, Holley, & Spurlin, 1983; Foos, Mora, & Tkacz, 1994; Frase & Schwartz, 1975; Jacoby, 1978; Johns & Swanson, 1988; Nairne & Widner, 1987; Watkins & Sechler, 1988); the recall of advertising product information (Reardon & Moore, 1996; Sengupta & Gorn, 2002); and even the recall of answers to trivia questions (deWinstanley, 1995; Pesta, Sanders, & Murphy, 1999; Peynircioglu & Mungan, 1993). Clarity is a double edged sword. Sometimes it Knowledge Acquisition Theory 27 is good to be clear and sometimes it is necessary to be ambiguous in order to stimulate learner minds. In this study, clarity seems to be important in data and information acquisition. In particular, it seems that clear textbooks and clear course goals aid students in the acquisition of unfamiliar data and information. Textbooks provide the bulk of messages with which students interact and course goals frame the meaning of those messages. However, if students feel that the answers are too clear cut, that there is no room for improvement or no need for further inquiry into a topic, then students are less likely to feel like knowledge has been gained. Finally, prior instructional communication research has focused almost exclusively upon “teacher clarity” when considering the role message characteristics play in student cognitive learning. Findings in this study as well as in Trader’s (2007) unpublished dissertation suggest that Instructor Clarity is not an important factor in student perceived cognitive learning. Perhaps, it would be better to design undergraduate courses in which the onus of learning is placed on the students rather than on the instructor. While it will always be necessary to have instructors who have sufficient content knowledge by which to select course content to expose students to and sufficient knowledge of learning processes that inform appropriate interactions with content during the appropriate stage in the learning process, learning is predominantly an individual struggle. Of course, there are several limitations to this study that suggest cautious interpretation of the results. First off, the sample size is rather small thus increasing the risks of Type II errors. Second, the sample is a convenience sample and there is no way to determine if the participants were representative of all undergraduates. The study took place among biology and communication majors at a small liberal arts college, and thus there may be some unique qualities among these people and Knowledge Acquisition Theory 28 the types of courses they have experienced. However, the results of this present study are in alignment with the results from Trader’s (2007) unpublished dissertation. The results of this study are encouraging and suggest that a receiver oriented message perspective can yield insights into instructional communication that were not previously possible. First off, Knowledge Acquisition Theory provides us with a theoretical basis for understanding how instructional messages and student interactions with content can be optimized in relation to student cognitive learning. The surprising thing in this study is that despite the small sample size, KAT accounts for 58% of the variance in perceived data obtained from an undergraduate course, 70-83% of the variance in perceived information obtained, and 74-84% of the variance in perceived knowledge obtained from successful completion of an undergraduate course. Though the results of this study are encouraging, there is still much work to be done. Other message variables such as message complexity remain unexplored. It is also unclear if Knowledge Acquisition Theory holds true within a variety of content domains or within other educational contexts. The author of this study sincerely hopes that other instructional communication scholars will adopt a receiver oriented message perspective in their future research endeavors. Knowledge Acquisition Theory 29 References Anderson, L. W., & Krathwohl, D. R. (eds.) (2001). A taxonomy for learning, teaching, and assessing: A revision of Bloom's taxonomy of educational objectives. New York: Longman. Baldridge National Quality Program [NIST] (2007). Education criteria for performance excellence. Retrieved 4/27/2007 from: http://www.quality.nist.gov/PDF_files/2007_Education_Criteria.pdf. Begg, I., Snider, A., Foley, F., & Goddard, R. (1989). The generation effect is no artifact: Generating makes words distinctive. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 15, 977–989. Bloom, B.S. (Ed.), Engelhart, M.D., Furst, E.J., Hill, W.H., & Krathwohl, D.R. (1956). Taxonomy of educational objectives: Handbook I: Cognitive domain. New York: David McKay. Boyer Report (1998). Reinventing undergraduate education: A blueprint for America’s research universities. Retrieved 5/11/2007 from: http://naples.cc.sunysb.edu/Pres/boyer.nsf/. Brooks, L. W., Dansereau, D. F., Holley, C. D., & Spurlin, J. E. (1983). Generation of descriptive text headings. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 8, 103–108. Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1984). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. Advances in Consumer Research, 11, 673-675. Chesebro, J. L. (2002). Teaching clearly. In J. L. Chesebro & J. C. McCroskey (Eds.), Communication for Teachers (pp. 93-103). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon. Chesebro, J. L. (2003). Effects of teacher clarity and nonverbal immediacy on student learning, receiver apprehension, and affect. Communication Education, 52(2), 135-147. Knowledge Acquisition Theory 30 Chesebro, J. L., & McCroskey, J. C. (1998). The development of the teacher clarity short inventory (TCSI) to measure clear teaching in the classroom. Communication Research Reports, 15, 262-266. Chesebro, J. L., & McCroskey, J. C. (2000). The relationship between students' reports of learning and their actual recall of lecture material: A validity test. Communication Education, 49, 297-301. Chesebro, J. L., & McCroskey, J. C. (2001). The relationship of teacher clarity and immediacy with student state receiver apprehension, affect, and cognitive learning. Communication Education, 50(1), 59-68. Chesebro, J. L., & Wanzer, M. B. (2006). Instructional message variables. In T. P. Mottet, V. P. Richmond & J. C. McCroskey (Eds.), Handbook of instructional communication: Rhetorical & relational perspectives (pp. 89-116). Boston: Pearson. Civikly, J. M. (1992). Clarity - Teachers and students making sense of instruction. Communication Education, 41(2), 138-152. Commission on the Future of Higher Education (2006). Final report — A test of leadership: Charting the future of higher education. Retrieved 9/27/2008 from: http://www.ed.gov/about/bdscomm/list/hiedfuture/reports.html. Cruickshank, D. R. (1985). Applying research on teacher clarity. Journal of Teacher Education, 36(2), 44-48. Cruickshank, D. R., Kennedy, J. J., Bush, A., & Myers, B. (1979). Clear teaching What is it. British Journal of Teacher Education, 5(1), 27-33. Cruikshank, D. R., & Kennedy, J. J. (1986). Teacher clarity. Teaching & Teacher Education, 2, 43-67. deWinstanley, P. A. (1995). A generation effect can be found during naturalistic learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 538-541. Knowledge Acquisition Theory 31 Doctorow, M., Marks, C., & Wittrock, M. (1978). Generative processes in reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 109-118. Donohew, L., Lorch, E. P., & Palmgreen, P. (1991). Sensation seeking and targeting of televised anti-drug PSAs. In L. Donohew, H. E. Sypher, & W. J. Bukoski (Eds.), Persuasive Communication and Drug Use Prevention (pp. 209-226). Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Donohew, L., Lorch, E. P., & Palmgreen, P. (1998). Applications of a theoretic model of information exposure to health interventions. Human Communication Research, 24(3), 454-468. Donohew, L., Palmgreen, P., & Duncan, J. (1980). An activation model of information exposure. Communication Monographs, 47(4), 295-303. Donohew, L., Palmgreen, P., & Lorch, E. P. (1994). Attention, need for sensation, and health communication campaigns. American Behavioral Scientist, 38, 310-322. Foos, P. W., Mora, J. J., & Tkacz, S. (1994). Student study techniques and the generation effect. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(4), 567-576. Frase, L. T., & Schwartz, B. J. (1975). Effect of question production and answering on prose recall. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 628–635. Frymier, A. B., & Houser, M. L. (2000). The teacher-student relationship as an interpersonal relationship. Communication Education, 49(3), 207-219. Frymier, A. B., & Shulman, G. M. (1995). What’s in it for me - Increasing content relevance to enhance students’ motivation. Communication Education, 44(1), 40-50. Frymier, A. B., & Wanzer, M. B. (2006). Teacher and student affinity-seeking in the classroom. In T. P. Mottet, V. P. Richmond & J. C. McCroskey (Eds.), Handbook of instructional communication (pp. 195-211). Boston: Pearson. Knowledge Acquisition Theory 32 Frymier, A. B., & Weser, B. (2001). The role of student predispositions on student expectations for instructor communication behavior. Communication Education, 50(4), 314-326. Gibson, E. J. (2000a). Perceptual learning in development: Some basic concepts. Ecological Psychology, 12(4), 295-302. Gibson, E. J. (2000b). Where is the information for affordances? Ecological Psychology, 12(1), 53-56. Gibson, J. J. (1950). The perception of the visual world. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co. Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. Hooper, S., Sales, G., Rysavy, S. D. M. (1994). Generating summaries and analogies alone and in pairs. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 19, 53-62. Hurt, T. J., Scott, M. D., & McCroskey, J. C. (1978). Communication in the classroom. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Jacoby, L. L. (1978). On interpreting the effects of repetition: Solving a problem versus remembering a solution. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 17, 649–668. Johns, E. E., & Swanson, L. G. (1988). The generation effect with nonwords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 180–190. Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). Lay epistemics and human knowledge: Cognitive and motivational bases. New York: Plenum Press. Kruglanski, A. W. (1990). Layepistemic theory in social-cognitive psychology. Psychological Inquiry, 1, 181-197. Kruglanski, A. W., & Orehek, E. (2007). Partitioning the domain of social inference: Dual mode and systems models and their alternatives. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 291-316. Knowledge Acquisition Theory 33 Lawson, M. J., & Chinnappan, M. (1994). Generative activity during geometry problem solving: Comparison of the performance of high-achieving and lowachieving high school students. Cognition & Instruction, 12, 61-93. McNamara, D. S., & Healy, A. F. (1995a). A generation advantage for multiplication skill training and nonword vocabulary acquisition. In A. F. Healy & L. E. Bourne, Jr. (Eds.), Learning and memory of knowledge and skills: Durability and specificity. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. McNamara, D. S., & Healy, A. F. (1995b). A procedural explanation of the generation effect: The use of an operand retrieval strategy for multiplication and addition problems. Journal of Memory & Language, 34, 399-416. McNamara, D. S., & Healy, A. F. (2000). A procedural explanation of the generation effect for simple and difficult multiplication problems and answers. Journal of Memory & Language, 43, 652-679. Nairne, J. S., & Widner, R. L., Jr. (1987). Generation effects with nonwords: The role of test appropriateness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 164–171. Pesta, B. J., Sanders, R. E., & Murphy, M. D. (1999). A beautiful day in the neighborhood: What factors determine the generation effect for simple multiplication problems? Memory & Cognition, 27, 106-115. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1984). The effects of involvement on responses to argument quantity and quality: Central and peripheral routes to persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 69-81. Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1999). The Elaboration Likelihood Model: Current status and controversies. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 41-72). New York: Guilford Press. Knowledge Acquisition Theory 34 Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Goldman, R. (1981). Personal involvement as a determinant of argument based persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 847-855. Peynircioglu, Z. F., & Mungan, E. (1993). Familiarity, relative distinctiveness, and the generation effect. Memory & Cognition, 21, 367-374. Powers, J. H. (1995). On the intellectual structure of the human communication discipline. Communication Education, 44, 191-222. Reardon, R., & Moore, D. J. (1996). The greater memorability of self-generated versus externally presented product information. Psychology & Marketing, 13(3), 305-320. Richmond, V.P., McCroskey, J.C., Kearney, P., & Plax, T.G. (1987). Power in the classroom VII: Linking behavior alteration techniques to cognitive learning. Communication Education, 36, 1-12. Sengupta, J., & Gorn, G. J. (2002). Absence makes the mind grow sharper: Effects of element omission on subsequent recall. Journal of Marketing Research, 39(2), 186-202. Simonds, C. J. (1997). Classroom understanding: An expanded notion of teacher clarity. Communication Research Reports, 14(3), 279-281. Sprague, J. (1992). Expanding the research agenda for instructional communication - Raising some unasked questions. Communication Education, 41(1), 1-25. Sprague, J. (1993). Retrieving the research agenda for communication education Asking the pedagogical questions that are embarrassments to theory. Communication Education, 42(2), 106-122. Taylor, B., & Berkowitz, S. (1980). Facilitating children's comprehension of context material. In M. L. Kamil & A. J. Moe (Eds.), Perspectives on reading research and instruction (Twenty-ninth Yearbook of the National Reading Conference). Washington, DC: National Reading Conference. Knowledge Acquisition Theory 35 Trader, R. J. (2007). Instructional communication matter: A test of knowledge acquisition theory (KAT) from a message-oriented receiver perspective (Doctoral dissertation, University of Kentucky, 2007). Retrieved 9/27/2008 from: http://hdl.handle.net/10225/623. Watkins, M. J., & Sechler, E. S. (1988). Generation effect with an incidental memorization procedure. Journal of Memory & Language, 27, 537–544. Witt, P. L., & Wheeless, L. R., & Allen, M. (2006). The relationship between teacher immediacy and student learning: A meta-analysis. In B. M. Gayle, R. W. Preiss, N. Burrell & M. Allen (Eds.), Classroom communication and instructional processes: Advances through metaanalysis (pp. 149-168). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Wittrock, M. C. (1974). Learning as a generative process. Educational Psychologist, 19(2), 87-95. Wittrock, M. C. (1989). Generative processes of comprehension. Educational Psychologist, 24(4), 345-376. Wittrock, M. C. (1992). Generative processes of the brain. Educational Psychologist, 27(4), 531-541. Wittrock, M. C., & Alesandrini, K. (1990). Generation of summaries and analogies and analytic and holistic abilities. American Educational Research Journal, 27, 489-502. Wood, W., Rhodes, N., & Biek, M. (1995). Working knowledge and attitude strength: An information-processing analysis. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 283-313). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Zipf, G. K. (1949). Human behavior and the principle of least effort. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.