Dodd, B., Holm, A., Crosbie, S., & McIntosh, B. Core vocabulary intervention for inconsistent speech disorder. pp117-136 In L. Williams, S. McLeod, & R. McCauley (Eds.), Interventions for speech sound disorders in children. Baltimore: Brookes. Abstract Core vocabulary intervention establishes consistent word production for a functional SSD characterized by inconsistent pronunciation of the same lexical item, in the absence of CAS. The intervention is suitable for children from two years of age, including those from bilingual backgrounds and those with a cognitive disability. Children are seen twice weekly for thirty minutes, for about eight weeks. Carers are required to carry out daily practice. Clients select a vocabulary of around 70 functionally powerful words. Each week, children learn to produce their best pronunciation of up to ten of these words consistently, in isolation and connected speech. Non-treated probes measure generalization of consistency. Efficacy studies reveal gains in consistency and accuracy of speech production. Introduction Children with speech sound disorders differ in severity, the types of errors made and their response to specific intervention approaches. Given the complexity of speech processing, it is not surprising that deficits to different abilities cause differences in speech output (Duggirala and Dodd, 1991). There are three interactive domains: input processing (sensation, perception); cognitive-linguistic abilities (phonological representation, derivation of language specific phonological constraints); and, output processing (assembly of phonological plans or templates, phonetic planning and articulation). This chapter focuses on intervention for children who have a deficit in phonological assembly: an impaired ability to plan the sequence of phonemes that make up a word, in the absence of any oro-motor signs of childhood apraxia of speech (CAS). The deficit results in inconsistent pronunciation of the same word. Inconsistent speech errors Inconsistency characterized by multiple error types (unpredictable variation between a relatively large number of phones) indicates pervasive speech processing difficulties (Grunwell, 1982) and is a potential indicator of persistent SSD (Forrest, Elbert and Dinnsen, 2000). Children who make inconsistent speech errors pronounce the same words and phonological features differently not only from context to context, but also within the same context (McCormack and Dodd, 1996; Holm and Dodd, 1999). For example, one 7 year old boy (Dodd, Holm, Crosbie and McIntosh, 2007) was likely to pronounce the same word differently each time he said it (e.g., [bns], [dn], [bm] tongue; [bw::t], [bw], [bwt] witch; [deuwa], [jeia] [jedw] zebra). The proportion of children with SSD who make inconsistent errors is around 10%. For example, Broomfield and Dodd (2004) assessed 320 English-speaking children with SSD, finding that 30 (9.4%) pronounced at least 40% of 25 words differently when they were asked to name the same pictures on three separate trials in one assessment session, each trial separated by a different activity. Cross-linguistic evaluations have identified about the same proportion of children with inconsistent speech in a range of languages indicating that the deficit in the speech processing Dodd, Holm, Crosbie and McIntosh 2 chain is not dependent on the phonology being learned (see Holm, Crosbie and Dodd, 2005, for review). Differential diagnosis: inconsistent SSD and childhood apraxia of speech (CAS) Table 1 summarizes clinical opinion (Forrest, 2003) and research (reviewed by Ozanne, 2005) concerning criteria for the diagnosis of CAS. The characteristics of errors made by children with inconsistent speech disorder are drawn from Holm, Crosbie and Dodd (2005). Two symptoms are shared: inconsistent errors and increasing errors with increased word length. The two groups are discriminated, however, by two important differences. Children with CAS have oro-motor problems that result in inappropriate oral movements during speech (e.g. groping) and affecting speech prosody, rate and fluency. Children with inconsistent SSD do not show these symptoms. Children with CAS make more errors in imitation than they do in spontaneous production. The opposite is true of children with inconsistent SSD: they make fewer errors in imitation than in spontaneous production. Differential diagnosis is, then, straightforward. Table 1. Differential diagnosis of CAS and inconsistent speech disorder Childhood Apraxia of Speech Inconsistent Speech Disorder Inconsistent errors Inconsistent errors Increasing errors with increasing length Increasing errors with increasing length Poor sequencing of sounds (e.g., metathesis) Wrong choice of phoneme rather than order errors as in metathesis Inability to imitate sounds, better spontaneously Better in imitation than in spontaneous than in imitation production General oral-motor difficulties; Oromotor skills within normal limits Groping; silent posturing No groping, no silent posturing Prolongations and repetitions of speech sounds. No prolongations and repetitions of speech sounds Slow speech and diadochokinetic (DDK) rates Normal speech and DDK rates Intervention Describing and analyzing inconsistent speech errors in terms of error patterns is not possible and deciding what to target in intervention is difficult (Dodd and Bradford, 2000). Forrest et al. (2000) stated that intervening with children who make inconsistent errors is problematic because “one may not know the appropriate sound to use in contrast to the error. This may mean that children with a variable substitution will fare worse in treatment than other children because the available protocols for this population are not as effective as other procedures” (p. 529). An alternative intervention, the core vocabulary approach, has been developed that targets consistency, rather than accuracy, of whole word production (Dodd and Iacono, 1989). It has been trialed with children as young as two years as well as preschool and school aged children with a functional SSD who make inconsistent errors. The approach has also been successfully used with a bilingual child (Holm and Dodd, 1999) and children who have Down syndrome 2 Dodd, Holm, Crosbie and McIntosh 3 (Dodd McCormack and Woodyatt, 1994). Broomfield and Dodd’s (2005) RCT indicated that children with inconsistent SSD made most progress if core vocabulary intervention occurred when they were three years-old. Early intervention is possible for children who make inconsistent errors because their low intelligibility means that they tend to be referred early (Broomfield and Dodd, 2004). Identification of inconsistent SSD requires assessment of multiple productions of the same words in the same phonetic context. Assessing consistency Research indicates that children who score 40% or more on the Inconsistency Subtest of the Diagnostic Evaluation of Articulation and Phonology (Dodd, Crosbie, Zhu, Holm and Ozanne, 2002) benefit from a core vocabulary approach to intervention. In this assessment children are asked to name 25 pictures (e.g., girl, dinosaur, elephant) on three trials within one session. Each trial is separated by another activity. If a child says a word identically on all three trials, s/he receives a score of 0 for that word. If the child produces a word differently on at least one of the trials, then he receives a score of 1 for that word. The child’s score, for all words produced three times, is converted to a percentage, with a score of 40% being the criterion for diagnosis. The 40% criterion is justified by consistency of production data from typically developing and speech disordered children (Holm, Crosbie and Dodd, 2007; McCormack and Dodd, 2000). Ways of describing inconsistency in more detail are outlined in Holm, Crosbie and Dodd (2005). Theoretical Basis When children begin to produce recognizable words, at around 12 months, their phonology is idiosyncratic. It is characterized by a great deal of individual variation in the phones used, phonetic variability of production and inconsistency of error types (Grunwell, 1982). Ferguson and Farwell (1975) concluded that children’s initial phonology is whole-word based. They argued that ‘a phonic core of remembered lexical items and articulations which produce them is the foundation of an individual’s phonology’ (p. 437). Ingram (1976) claimed that once children’s vocabularies begin to expand past 50 words, phonological error patterns (e.g. stopping, fronting) begin to occur across lexical items, suggesting reorganization from a whole word to a segmental phonological system. Similarly Velleman and Vihman (2002) argued that while initially there are no consistent error patterns, this is followed by a stage characterized by dominant production patterns attributed to the development of word templates. The templates are abstract phonetic production patterns that integrate the adult target with the child’s most common vocal patterns, resulting in explicit word learning. Reorganization of their phonological system from whole words to phonemic segments seems to occur by most children’s second birthday. By three years mean inconsistency of word production is 13% and by four years, 6%. About 10% of those referred for assessment of suspected SSD, however, continue to make inconsistent errors while showing no oro-motor signs of childhood apraxia of speech. Their speech is often unintelligible, even to their mothers. Carers cannot learn how children say particular words, because their productions are inconsistent. What type of deficit in speech processing underlies inconsistent speech errors? A series of experiments compared groups of children who make inconsistent errors but have no symptoms of CAS, with typically developing children and those who make consistent speech errors (for a summary of published results see Dodd, Holm, Crosbie and McCormack, 2005). Case history factors 3 Dodd, Holm, Crosbie and McIntosh 4 Fox, Howard and Dodd (2002) examined parental reports of risk factors for 66 children with SSD according to types of surface speech errors and 48 controls. Positive family history was statistically higher for the SSD group than controls, but did not discriminate between subgroups. A reported family history of communication difficulty, however, was the only risk factor for children who consistently made atypical errors (e.g., backing, bilabial fricatives for clusters). According to parental report, children with delayed phonological development were more likely than other groups to have a history of multiple middle ear infections. Children who made inconsistent errors were more likely to have a reported history of prenatal or perinatal complications (e.g., resuscitation, maternal infections, prematurity). This finding is intriguing given the literature on phonological disorder in aphasia. A deficit in phonological planning (termed ‘phonological assembly’ in Berndt & Mitchum, 1994) is reported to underlie inconsistent speech errors in adults with acquired speech difficulties that cannot be attributed to dyspraxia. Input processing Studies of auditory processing abilities on children with SSD have yielded contradictory results (Bird and Bishop, 1992) probably due to the assessment of heterogeneous populations. One study (Thyer and Dodd, 1996), investigated the auditory processing skills of 30 children with SSD (10 phonologically delayed, 10 consistently using atypical error patterns and 10 making inconsistent errors) and 30 typically developing (TD) controls. None of the SSD groups performed differently from the matched controls on standard auditory-processing assessments of speech discrimination, auditory figure-ground separation or binaural integration. The findings indicate that auditory processing difficulties are unlikely to be a general explanation for SSD. Cognitive Linguistic processing Lexical impairment The relationship between phonology and the size of the lexicon is important given that consistent production of words occurs once the lexicon reaches a critical size, and that children with larger lexicons produce a wider range of speech sounds and speech-sound sequences (Storkel and Morrisette, 2002). One experiment (Dodd et al 2005) compared the expressive vocabulary performance of subgroups of SSD on the Hundred-Pictures Naming Test (Fisher and Glenister, 1992). Children with an inconsistent disorder performed more poorly than all the other subgroups. No other differences between the subgroups were significant. This finding might reflect a word-finding difficulty, since qualitative analysis of errors indicated that 46% of the inconsistent children’s errors were related to the target word (e.g., net, “a thing for catching fish in”; nest, “eggs”; owl, “bird that comes out at night”), compared with 28% of the controls’ errors, 33% of the delayed children’s errors, and 33% of the consistent children’s errors. That is, the children who made inconsistent errors often seemed to know what the picture was, but couldn’t access the phonological shape of the word. An impaired ability to access full phonological specifications of words might contribute to inconsistent word production. Linguistic knowledge An experiment tested the preference for phonological legality of subgroups of children with SSD and controls (Dodd, Leahy and Hambly, 1989). Children had to choose between two nonsense words to give names to pictures of animals. The 12 nonsense word pairs differed by one phoneme that made one of the words phonologically legal, and the other phonologically illegal. For 4 Dodd, Holm, Crosbie and McIntosh 5 example, // is legal, whereas // is illegal because /sv/ does not occur in Australian-English. Only the children who consistently made atypical errors showed no preference for legal nonsense words. There were no significant differences between the other three groups. A more recent experiment (Holm, Farrier and Dodd, 2007) measured the syllable segmentation, rhyme awareness and alliteration awareness of 61 preschool children: 46 with SSD and 15 TD controls. Children who made inconsistent errors performed poorly on the syllable segmentation task but no differently from controls on the other two tasks. In contrast, the children who made consistent atypical errors showed poor performance on rhyme and alliteration but appropriate performance on syllable segmentation. The findings of these two experiments indicate that children who make inconsistent errors had relatively strong phonological awareness skills. In contrast, children who consistently made atypical errors had phonological awareness deficits that are associated with reading difficulties. The children who make inconsistent errors, however, had specific difficulty with syllable segmentation. If their underlying deficit was one of phonological planning, they would have difficulty assembling words for subvocal rehearsal. Subvocal rehearsal is an essential step in a number of phonological awareness tasks. For example, it allows the counting of how many syllables there are in statistical. Output processing To investigate the nature of the deficit underlying the inconsistent sub-group’s variable production of words, Bradford and Dodd (1996) compared the sub-groups’ ability to establish motor plans for words. A nonsense-word learning task assessed children’s ability to generate and execute motor plans for the articulation of nonsense words. An attractively illustrated book, depicting the story of ‘The Three Little Pigs,’ was used. Each pig was given a legal disyllabic nonsense name (i.e., [pazi], [] and []), that was modified to allow accurate production for each child. To differentiate visually between the pigs, who were portrayed ten times throughout the book, one wore glasses, another, a tie, and the third, a hat. The children learned the names of the ‘Three Little Pigs.’ The story was told and children were encouraged to imitate the nonsense names. The children then had to tell the story. Spontaneous production of each test word was elicited five times. Children’s comprehension of the names was assessed by asking them to point to named characters. The inconsistent group performed more poorly than the other three groups on the expressive naming task. The control, delayed and consistent groups performed equally well. Comparison of the four sub-groups’ performance on the receptive task showed that the groups performed equally well, although there was a trend for the inconsistent group to perform more poorly. The finding that children who make inconsistent errors have a more general motor-planning problem raises the question about the nature of their deficit. Interpretation Velleman and Vihman (2002) argue for a word ‘template’ that contains the phonological specifications for word production – a phonological plan. The phonological plan assembled when children have derived the wrong phonological constraints results in a pronunciation that differs from both the target adult form and the developmental error form of a child, of the same age, 5 Dodd, Holm, Crosbie and McIntosh 6 following the normal course of development. Nevertheless, the errors made, and therefore the phonological plan assembled, is the same when children pronounce the same word on different occasions, and when the same phonological feature is produced in different words (e.g., using a bilabial fricative to mark consonant clusters). In contrast, the evidence reviewed in this section leads to the conclusion that children whose speech is characterized by inconsistent errors may have difficulty selecting and sequencing phonemes (i.e., in assembling a phonological template or plan for production of an utterance). Since their phonological knowledge seems intact, it is unlikely that they have an input or cognitive linguistic difficulty. Since they have no oro-motor difficulties, it seems unlikely that they have phonetic planning or implementation difficulties. Their deficit appears to be at the level of phonological planning. Level of consequences The World Health Organization (2007) classification of learning difficulties relevant for speechlanguage pathology specifies the need for clinicians to address impairment, activity and participation. Core vocabulary intervention focuses on the impairment underlying the functional limitation of unintelligible speech. Targeting the underlying deficit leads to improved intelligibility that enhances children’s communication (activity) and their academic and social skills (participation). Treatment Target Core vocabulary intervention targets an underlying deficit in the speech processing chain: the ability to generate consistent phonological plans for words. The long-term goal of intervention for children who make inconsistent errors is to establish consistent (as opposed to correct) production of words in spontaneous speech. Unlike phonological contrast therapy, it does not target surface error patterns. Learning to consistently say a set of high frequency, functionally powerful words targets the phonological planning deficit. The ability to create a phonological plan on-line is improved by providing detailed specific information about a limited number of words and drilling the use of that information with systematic practice. Empirical Basis The primary reason for targeting inconsistency is the impact it has on intelligibility. Children who make inconsistent speech errors usually have a high degree of unintelligibility even to family members. Consequently they are more likely to be referred for assessment of a speech disorder by their parents rather than another referral agent, and they are more likely to be referred at three, rather then four, years (Broomfield and Dodd, 2005). Intervention target selection is very difficult. A child with inconsistent speech disorder may use a range of sound substitutions that differ in manner of production, place of production or voicing. For example, Amy (the case study described later in this chapter) marked /s/ with a [b, f, v, t, d, s] or deleted the sound. It was impossible to select the appropriate error to contrast given the range of substitutions. Further, it would not be effective to take an articulatory approach that targets a single sound when a child has adequate oro-motor control and sometimes produces the target accurately or, if not, is stimulable for the sound. Children with inconsistent speech disorder are resistant to phonological contrast or articulation therapy. Forrest, Dinnsen and Elbert (1997) conducted a retrospective post-hoc analysis of 14 children with speech disorder. The children were divided into three groups: those who made 6 Dodd, Holm, Crosbie and McIntosh 7 consistent sound substitutions for sounds not present in their inventories (e.g., /k/ always produced as [t]), those who had inconsistent sound substitutions across word positions (e.g., /v/ substituted by [b] word initially, but [f] word finally), and those that used a different sound substitution within (word initial /s/ being substituted by /v, f, d, b/) and across word positions. The three groups were matched for severity of phonological impairment and all received phonological contrast therapy targeting a single error in a single word position. The children with consistent sound substitutions learned the sound and generalized it to other word positions. The children with inconsistent sound substitutions across word positions learned the sound but only in the treated position. The children with variable sound substitutions within and across word positions did not learn the sound in the treated or untreated word position. Another intervention study (Forrest, Elbert and Dinnsen, 2000) compared two groups of children: those with consistent sound substitutions and those with variable sound substitutions. The children with inconsistent substitutions did not respond to traditional articulation therapy. The children with consistent substitutions learned and generalized the taught sound. Forrest et al (2000) acknowledged the limitation of these approaches in treating children with inconsistent SSD, concluding that “the challenge is to develop treatment protocols that instill learning and generalization, despite the complex pattern of errors that these children demonstrate” (p. 530). One plausible treatment approach would be to target whole words, rather than segments. While Ingram and Ingram (2001) advocated a whole-word approach to phonological analysis and described how that approach would translate into a set of treatment goals, no treatment study was done. The core vocabulary approach for inconsistent SSD has included treatment case studies that explored the appropriateness of the approach; a group efficacy study comparing different treatments using a cross-over design; and, an effectiveness study that was part of a randomized control trial carried out in a Speech and Language Therapy service in the north-east of England. Case Studies Developing a novel therapy MW a boy, aged 4;2, who was part of a clinical research study evaluating phonological contrast treatment for phonological disorders (Dodd, and Iacono,1989) made no progress despite weekly therapy for eight months. The other six children in the study had improved and been discharged. He differed from them in that he made inconsistent errors (e.g., he pronounced tomato as [] and [] and TV as [] and []). He showed no oro-motor signs of CAS. To establish consistency of production of specific lexical items, a novel therapy approach was developed. The approach focused on whole words to establish a vocabulary of highly functional, powerful words that were produced consistently, although not necessarily correctly. After two months of weekly therapy establishing a consistent vocabulary of 50 words, consistency generalized and a phonological contrast approach was successfully reintroduced. Comparing different intervention approaches for different types of SSD Bradford-Heit (1996) reported six treatment case studies that compared three therapy methods for children with different types of speech disorder (consistent atypical errors, inconsistent errors and CAS). Two children in the study (see Dodd and Bradford 2000) presented with speech disorder characterized by inconsistent productions. A multiple baseline with alternating treatments evaluated the effect of phonological contrast therapy (targeting error patterns), core vocabulary therapy (targeting consistency) and PROMPT system therapy (targeting articulatory gestures). 7 Dodd, Holm, Crosbie and McIntosh 8 Children were seen individually, twice weekly, for 12 (30 minute) sessions in each treatment block with a 3-week withdrawal period between therapy blocks. The order of treatments was randomized across the six children in the study. MC, aged 4;3, received core vocabulary therapy first. Consistency of word production was established and generalized to untreated words (31% pre-treatment to 69% post treatment of untreated words). He also benefited from the second block of therapy that targeted a phonological process (liquid and glide contrast) although generalization to untreated words was limited. MC did not benefit from the PROMPT therapy approach. TN, aged 3;7, received core vocabulary followed by PROMPT and then phonological therapy. The results indicate that TN benefited from core vocabulary (consistency 36% pre-treatment, 75% post-therapy on untreated words). The other two blocks of therapy did not result in improvement. These findings suggest that once consistency is established, it may be possible to immediately target phonological contrasts effectively, as was done with MW, if accuracy is still at unacceptable levels. Core vocabulary intervention with ‘special’ children Three preschool boys, who were referred to an efficacy study focusing on phonological contrast intervention, did not meet the inclusion criteria because they made inconsistent speech errors. All three had additional complicating factors. Treatment case studies were undertaken to examine the effects on core vocabulary intervention outcome of previous intervention, the use of default preferred word plans and behavior disorder (McIntosh and Dodd, in press). Andrew had a history of previous intervention that had focused on the articulation of /s/ which had over-generalized. Ben had a behavior disorder and limited attention. Cameron appeared to have a number of pervasive default plans for his word production. He most often began words with /w/, used a velar plosive syllable finally, employed default syllables (with variable vowels) // or // word finally or as whole word substitutes (e.g., [] apple, [] orange, [] lighthouse, [] watch, [] swing, [] giraffe). All three boys showed gains in intelligibility, accuracy and consistency of word production, although their individual differences required clinical adaptation of the approach (see Table 2). While Andrew received only six hours of intervention, Ben received 13.5 hours and Cameron 19 hours. It proved difficult to suppress preferred word shapes, but progress was achieved by an intermediary step of accepting a consistent production that included an aspect of his default word plan (e.g. accept [] swing) before that was modified to [] and then []. The case studies indicated that previous treatment focusing on articulation did not affect the outcome of core vocabulary intervention, although it did make it difficult to diagnose because the inconsistent use of an intrusive /s/ might have reflected previous therapy rather than inconsistency. The cases with behavior disorder and default templates for words required more intervention hours than had previously been found necessary for positive outcome. Table 2. Case studies: percent increase in consistency and accuracy after core vocabulary intervention Children Consistency Accuracy (PCC, PVC) Pre Post Pre Post Andrew 3;8 44 90 46, 88 94, 99 Cameron 4;2 36 90 34, 78 69, 95 Ben 3;9 36 60 22, 53 52, 86 8 Dodd, Holm, Crosbie and McIntosh 9 Group design Efficacy study: alternating treatments design The study (Crosbie, Holm and Dodd, 2005) evaluated the effect of two different types of intervention on the speech accuracy and consistency of word production of children with consistent (N = 8) and inconsistent SSD (N = 10) SSD. The 18 children (aged 4;08 to 6;05 years) with severe SSD participated in an intervention study comparing phonological contrast and core vocabulary intervention. All children received two 8-week blocks of each intervention, receiving twice weekly, 30 minute sessions. A multiple baseline design with alternating treatments was used. Two pre-therapy baseline measures were taken three weeks apart and showed no spontaneous change in number or type of speech errors. Once eligibility was confirmed children were allocated to one of the two therapies by order of referral. Treatment 1 was implemented after the baseline period followed by a 4-week withdrawal period, followed by treatment 2. The method of allocation to treatment ensured children in both subgroups of speech disorder received the blocks of therapy in both possible orders (core vocabulary followed by phonological contrast; phonological contrast followed by core vocabulary). All of the children increased their consonant accuracy during intervention (see Table 3). Core vocabulary therapy resulted in greater change in children with inconsistent speech disorder and phonological contrast therapy resulted in greater change in children with consistent speech disorder. The results of this study provide strong evidence that treatment targeting the speechprocessing deficit underlying a child’s speech disorder will result in efficient system-wide change. Differential response to intervention across subgroups provides evidence supporting theoretical perspectives regarding the nature of SSD: different underlying deficits result in different types of speech errors requiring different types of therapy. Table 3. Group summary of change in inconsistency score and PCC following each intervention Inconsistency (% mean, SD) PCC (mean, SD) Group Core vocabulary Phonological contrast Core vocabulary Phonological contrast Consistent (n=8) 5.00 (7.63) 9.50 (12.99) 6.75 (3.01) 24.62 (12.88) Inconsistent (n=10) 24.6 (9.14) 4.20 (7.57) 15.80 (9.05) 9.70 (6.57) 15.89 (12.99) 6.56 (10.35) 11.78 (8.28) 16.33 (12.22) Overall Randomized Control Trial Effectiveness Study Broomfield and Dodd (2005) reported a randomized control trial that included 320 children with SSD who were referred to the pediatric Speech and Language Therapy Service in Middleborough UK, between January 1999 and April 2000. The study differentially diagnosed subtypes of speech impairment, specified the content of the therapeutic approaches implemented with each subgroup 9 Dodd, Holm, Crosbie and McIntosh 10 and described the clinical pathway (including service delivery). Statistical analyses investigated treatment versus no treatment outcome. In addition, the effect of a range of factors on outcome was reported: dosage, age for cost-effective intervention for specific impairments, gender, case history factors and co-morbidity with other language disorders. Only data relevant to children with inconsistent SSD are discussed here. The type and duration of the intervention offered was determined by the nature and severity of each child’s SSD. Thirty children had inconsistent SSD, twenty of them classed as having a severe or profound level of unintelligibility. Core vocabulary therapy was the specified treatment for a diagnosis of inconsistency and all clinicians (N = 12) employed by the service used the treatment approach when it was indicated by diagnosis. After assessment, children typically attended up to six ‘diagnostic’ group sessions focusing on attention, listening, speech sound awareness, language concepts and categories and oro-motor and speech skills to ensure all difficulties that a child was having had been identified by the standardized assessments routinely administered. These initial sessions also allowed children to become familiar with the clinicians and confident in the clinic environment. Children receiving core vocabulary intervention then received six individual, weekly, 30-45 minute treatment sessions. After a break from intervention (a consolidation period) children were reassessed and, if their consistency remained atypical, received a second episode of core vocabulary intervention. In order that a standard measure was available for comparison, assessment findings were converted to z scores. Each change in (accuracy) performance was measured in comparison with an age change, with six months passing between each RCT assessment. Consequently, when a difference in z scores showed improvement, the child had progressed more than would have been expected for their change in age. Two of the children moved away and did not complete intervention. The 18 children with inconsistent disorder who received intervention performed better (z = 0.37) than the 10 children who received no intervention (z = 0.10). There is evidence, then, that a core vocabulary approach is effective for inconsistent SSD at a clinical service level. The result suggests, however, that children who made inconsistent errors made less progress in treatment than other subgroups of speech disorder (e.g., treated consistent atypical disorders: z =0.59; untreated z = 0.03). A number of factors may contribute to this finding. Children who make inconsistent errors often have a severe, pervasive disorder that may need to be addressed by more intervention sessions An alternative explanation is that many of the children in this group were younger (around 3 years old) than children in the other subgroups. Consequently, some children may have had poorer attention or less motivation to comply with intervention tasks. Additionally, unlike other evaluations, treatment occurred once per week rather then twice. Further, clinicians had only recently been trained in the use of core vocabulary and may have been less confident in its implementation than for other treatment approaches. Finally, for comparison with other children with SSD, the outcome measure focused on accuracy rather than consistency. While not assessing the primary target of therapy, the findings demonstrate that improved accuracy is a ‘byproduct’ of intervention targeting consistency. Summary of Clinical Findings Robey and Schultz (1998) identify clinical reports and case studies of a treatment as the first steps in research on clinical outcomes. These studies can explore a treatment’s potential and allow 10 Dodd, Holm, Crosbie and McIntosh 11 precise definition of the treatment and the population the treatment will benefit. The outcome of the seven treatment case studies briefly described all had a positive outcome. The group efficacy study, which used a multiple baseline alternating treatments research design, also provided positive findings. This design has been argued to have greater power than RCTs (e.g., Dodd, 2007) because all participants receive the same amount of intervention and children act as their own controls. The highly controlled research design allows stronger conclusions to be drawn about the relative benefit of a particular therapeutic approach using a constant service delivery model. The benefit of core vocabulary therapy was confirmed by an effectiveness study: an RCT carried out in a clinical service. Practical Requirements The service delivery model recommended for core vocabulary intervention is individual, twice weekly sessions lasting around 30 minutes, with the parent present for at least one of those sessions. While the number of sessions varies according to severity and teacher and carer input, a clinician should not expect to exceed 16 half hour sessions to attain consistency of production, as well as enhanced PCC, that generalizes to untreated words. Some studies of older children have held one session per week at school and the other at the SLP health-based clinic to ensure liaison with both the child’s teacher and parents. Roles of Parent and Teachers The intervention approach depends on the child’s family and teacher to reinforce use of the core vocabulary and carryout daily practice. Both must be involved from the outset of intervention. For example, after assessment and diagnosis, but before the first intervention session, the child, family and teacher are asked to contribute to a list of 70 words that are frequently part of the child’s functional vocabulary. These words are used as the basis for the treatment sessions. Family and school must be aware of, and monitor the consistency of, their child’s best production of the words being targeted that week, in practice and spontaneously. It must be emphasized to carers that the primary target of the intervention is to make sure their child says a word in exactly the same way each time they attempt to say it, not necessarily an error-free production. The Clinicians’ Role The first session each week is devoted to eliciting the child’s best production of up to 12 selected words and establishing their production. The number of words targeted depends on the child’s ability to achieve a word’s best possible word production. The second weekly session focuses on drilling the newly learned words in order to monitor production, providing appropriate feedback when the best production is not produced. Parents and teachers must be encouraged to perceive the child’s productions accurately and learn how to provide appropriate feedback. Clinicians need to be sensitive to parent and teacher feedback that could influence the content of the therapy session (e.g., words or phrases needing additional teaching, new words to target). Finally, an essential clinical role is to regularly monitor generalization of consistency to untreated probes. Materials Equipment is minimal: pictures of the target words (with the written name beneath the picture); a box or bag for the vocabulary pictures; a wall-chart where words produced consistently can be glued; games that elicit multiple productions of target words rapidly. A DVD of text resources (e.g., teacher and parent information sheets, probe word lists) is available from the authors. 11 Dodd, Holm, Crosbie and McIntosh 12 Key Components The core vocabulary intervention approach should only be used for children who make inconsistent errors but have no oro-motor signs of CAS. The ultimate goal of intervention is intelligible speech. The long-term goal for a block of core vocabulary intervention is for the child to produce at least 50 target words consistently, that is, to produce a word exactly the same way each time it is produced. Generalization of consistency is expected once the child has mastered the consistent production of 50 words. The short-term goals are target specific, however two general goals can be applied to each set of target words. The first goal is for the child to achieve an appropriate productive realization of each target based on the child’s phonological system and phonetic inventory. This ‘best production’ may be correct or contain a developmental error. The second goal is for the child to consistently use the established ‘best production’. Selection of core vocabulary A core vocabulary of up to 70 functionally powerful is selected by the child, parents, and teacher. The types of words commonly included on children’s lists are names (e.g., teacher, friends, pet), places (e.g., address, toilet), function words (e.g., please, sorry), foods (e.g., cornflakes, juice, chips) and the child’s favorite things (e.g., Polly Pocket, Spiderman, games). The words are not selected according to word shape or segments. They are chosen because the child frequently uses these words in their functional communication. The child’s intelligible use of the functionally powerful words selected motivates the use of consistent productions. Establishing best production In the first session each week, the clinician teaches the child between up to ten target words, selected at random from a bag containing the vocabulary chosen. Words are taught sound-bysound, using cues such as syllable segmentation, imitation and cued articulation as outlined in Passy (1990). For example: to teach Joseph, the clinician would explain that Joseph has two syllables – [do] and [sf]. The first syllable [do] has two sounds, /d / and /o/, and the second syllable [sf] has three sounds /s/, // and /f/. The child attempts the first syllable - [do] - receives feedback and makes further attempts after being given models and receiving feedback about each attempt. When the child’s best production of the first syllable has been established, the second - [sf]’ is targeted, and then the two syllables are combined - [do-sf]’. For some children it is effective to link sounds to letters. Children with inconsistent SSD are usually able to imitate all sounds. If it is not possible to elicit a correct production then the best production may include developmental errors (e.g., [dosf] for Joseph, [tmra] for camera). Drill It is important that the child practises the target words daily as well as receiving feedback on those words in everyday communication situations. The second session each week with the clinician involves practice of the target words. Games are used to elicit a high number of repetitions. Any game that the child is highly motivated to participate in can be used to elicit productions. Initial picture naming games (e.g., stepping stones – with more than one picture on each stepping stone) can be followed by those requiring the target in a carrier phrase (e.g., picture lotto) and finally by story generation (asking for one, two or three of the target words). Elbert, Powell and Swartzlaner (1991) suggest a child should produce about 100 responses in 30 12 Dodd, Holm, Crosbie and McIntosh 13 minutes. Although this number may sound like a high rate of response it is not difficult to elicit 150 –170 responses in a 30-minute session of core vocabulary intervention. Treatment on error It is important to provide children with feedback when their best production is not uttered. Leahy (2004) reported that children do not always understand why they attend treatment or what they are required to do. Consequently it is important to be explicit about the purpose of intervention, the nature of the error made, and how it can be corrected. If a child produces a target that deviates from the best production the clinician can imitate the production and explicitly explain that the word differed and how it differed. For example if the child’s target word was ‘sun’ and he produced [] the clinician would say “[], that’s different to how we say it. That had a [] sound at the start but we need to make it a [s], []. Have another try at telling me what this picture is.” Clinicians should avoid simply asking for an imitation of the target word that provides a phonological plan. Instead, clinicians should provide information about the plan requiring children to generate their own plan for the word.. Monitoring consistent production Towards the end of the second session each week, the child is asked to produce, three times, the set of target words that have been the focus of therapy for the past week. Any word that the child can produce consistently is removed from the list of words to be learned. It may be placed on a chart showing what the child has achieved. Words produced inconsistently remain on the list (go back in the bag of words yet to be learned). Even though there are 50 target words that form a core vocabulary for the child’s 8 weeks of intervention, such monitoring allows for words that have not been mastered to be readdressed in another week. Assessment and Progress Monitoring to Support Decision Making Generalization Core vocabulary intervention aims to stabilize a child’s system leading to consistent word production. The therapy would not be beneficial if the effect of therapy was limited to the treated target items. To monitor generalization, a set of untreated items (10 words) should be elicited three times, once a fortnight in one therapy session, to monitor system change (i.e., determine when consistency has generalized). Once untreated probes become consistent the 25 Word Test (DEAP, Dodd et al 2002) should be re-administered to ensure generalization has occurred. What to do when the child’s speech is consistent Core vocabulary intervention will increase the consistency of a child’s speech production. The effect this has on the child’ speech system can vary. Case studies suggest that most children’s speech became both more consistent and more accurate, and that the children’s speech was characterized by developmental, not atypical, error patterns. For some children, however, more than one intervention approach may be necessary to achieve age-appropriate speech. For example, Dodd and Bradford (2000) report a case study of a boy with inconsistent speech production. Once consistency was established he benefited from phonological contrast therapy that targeted his remaining developmental error patterns. Assessment on a standardized phonological assessment, preferably after a consolidation break from treatment, would allow decisions about the need for further, different intervention. 13 Dodd, Holm, Crosbie and McIntosh 14 Children from culturally diverse backgrounds A bilingual Punjabi-English speaking child had speech was characterized by inconsistent errors (Holm and Dodd, 1999). Hafis was successfully treated using a core vocabulary approach, in English, which targeted consistency of production. Hafis received 16 (30 minute) sessions over an 8-week period. His consistency of production increased. On untreated items, his inconsistency in English fell from 56% to 21% and in Punjabi from 45 to 30%. His accuracy, measured by percent of consonants correct, increased significantly in Punjabi by 16% and in English by 26%. The significance of the generalization of intervention across languages is that it highlights the effect of targeting the underlying deficit rather than the surface speech error patterns. Case study (from Crosbie et al, 2005) Amy was assessed mid-way through her pre-school year when she was aged 4;8. She is monolingual, an only child, living with both parents. Her speech had always been difficult to understand. Amy’s birth, medical and developmental histories were normal. She had no hearing problems. Amy was reported to be conscious of her SSD, having difficulty establishing friendships at pre-school, and upset and frustrated when she was not understood. Standardized assessment indicated age-appropriate receptive language and non-verbal intelligence. The Articulation, Inconsistency and Phonology Assessments of the Diagnostic Evaluation of Articulation and Phonology (DEAP - Dodd et al., 2002) indicated that Amy could produce all speech sounds in imitated simple syllables or in isolation. Her oro-motor skills were age-appropriate. The Inconsistency Assessment indicated that her speech was 56% inconsistent (a standard score of 3). The Phonology Assessment indicated that Amy’s pre-intervention PCC was 50% and her PVC 92%, performance at the 1st percentile. Reassessment three weeks after initial assessment showed no spontaneous change. Intervention Treatment with the clinician occurred individually, twice weekly. Therapy sessions were alternately conducted in Amy’s home and pre-school to allow liaison with both her teacher and parents. There were 16 (30 minute) sessions over an eight-week period. Prior to intervention, Amy, her parents and her teacher collaborated to produce a list of 50 words that were functionally ‘powerful’ for her. The clinician explained the principles of core vocabulary therapy to Amy’s parents and teacher. Each week 10 words were drawn randomly from the set of 50 target words. Amy was taught the 10 words by the clinician, and then those words were targeted consistently by her parents and teacher throughout the week and revised in the second session with the clinician. Some of the taught words were correct. For others, developmental errors were accepted. Production was drilled sound by sound. After the initial session where Amy learned the target words, her parents and teacher consistently required her to produce those 10 words in the same way throughout the week. Practice of the ten words occurred three times each day, as well as being reinforced when they occurred in everyday communication situations. Her parents and teacher used the same teaching strategies as the clinician. During the second weekly session with the clinician the words were drilled, and then Amy had a “test” where she had to produce the 10 words three times, three trials separated by another activity. Untreated probes (a set of 10 untreated words) were also elicited three times to monitor generalization fortnightly. Amy’s progress was drawn on to her chart and her parents implemented a reward scheme linked to her progress on the weekly words. 14 Dodd, Holm, Crosbie and McIntosh 15 Amy found the intervention program difficult at times, particularly when learning the new words each week. However she was well supported at home and school and regularly did her practice. The intervention period was for a pre-determined eight-week period so that everyone involved was able to commit to it knowing when it was going to end. Progress Amy learned to produce 57 words consistently in eight weeks. When she was reassessed on the consistency tasks used in the initial and baseline assessments her inconsistency had decreased from 56% to 32%. Her consonant accuracy increased by 10%. In addition she was more consistent in her substitution patterns following intervention. Instead of the almost free variation between up to six phonemes evident in her pre-intervention assessments Amy generally only used either the correct phoneme or one other phoneme. Her speech was still affected by developmental phonological error patterns but there was no evidence of atypical error patterns in her speech at the final assessment. Future Directions For core vocabulary intervention to be accepted as a useful treatment approach, clinical efficacy and effectiveness studies need to be carried out by other groups of clinical researchers to validate the findings already made. Although the use of the approach has been anecdotally reported to be successful by clinicians, it still needs to be rigorously tested. Future research could address some issues needing clarification, listed below in the form of research questions. - Does client choice of the vocabulary lead to better outcome than clinician choice that would manipulate word shape or phoneme contrast? - How many words have to be taught before consistency generalizes? - Does accepting ‘best production’ rather than ‘correct production’ result in differences in outcome of therapy for accuracy of production? - For bilingual children, only one case study of a child with inconsistent SSD has shown generalization of core vocabulary intervention gains from the treated to the untreated language. This finding has important theoretical and clinical implications and needs repeating with other language pairs. References Berndt, R. & Mitchum, C. (1994). Approaches to the rehabilitation of ‘phonological assembly’. In G. Humphreys & M Riddoch (eds.), Cognitive Neuropsychology and Cognitive Rehabilitation, London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Bird, J. & Bishop, D. (1992). Perception and awareness of phonemes in phonologically impaired children. European Journal of Disorders of Communication 27, 289-311. Bradford, A & Dodd, B. (1996). Do all speech disordered children have motor deficits? Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 10, 77-101. Bradford-Heit, A (1996) Subgroups of children with developmental speech disorder : identification and remediation. Unpublished PhD Thesis: University of Queensland Broomfield, J. & Dodd, B. (2004) The nature of referred subtypes of primary speech disability. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 20, 135-151 15 Dodd, Holm, Crosbie and McIntosh 16 Broomfield, J. & Dodd, B. (2005) Clinical effectiveness. In: Dodd, B. (ed). Differential Diagnosis and Treatment of Children with Speech Disorder. London: Whurr, pp 211-230 Crosbie, S Holm, A & Dodd, B (2005) Intervention for children with severe speech disorder: A comparison of two approaches. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 40, 467–491. Dodd, B. (2007) Evidence-based practice and speech-language pathology. Folia Phoniatrica Logopedia, 59,118-129. Dodd, B. & Bradford, A. (2000) A comparison of three therapy methods for children with different types of developmental phonological disorders. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 35, 189-209 Dodd, B. & Iacono, T. (1989) Phonological disorders in children: changes in phonological process use during treatment. British Journal of Communication Disorders 24, 333-351 Dodd, B., Crosbie, S., Zhu, H., Holm, A. & Ozanne, A. (2002) The Diagnostic Evaluation of Articulation and Phonology. London: Psych-Corp Dodd, B. Leahy, J. & Hambly, G. (1989) Phonological disorders in children: underlying cognitive deficits. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 7, 55-71 Dodd, B., McCormack, P. & Woodyatt, G. (1994) An evaluation of an intervention program: The relationship between children’s phonology and parents communicative behavior. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 98, 632-645 Dodd, B., Holm, A., Crosbie, S. & McCormack, P. (2005) Differential diagnosis of phonological disorders. In: Dodd, B. (ed). Differential Diagnosis and Treatment of Children with Speech Disorder. London: Whurr, pp 44-70 Dodd B, Holm A, Crosbie S, McIntosh B: (2006). A core vocabulary approach for management of inconsistent speech disorder. Advances in Speech-Language Pathology, 8, 220-230. Duggirala, V. & Dodd, B. (1991) A psycholinguistic assessment model for disordered phonology . In: Congress for Phonetic Sciences, Aix-en-Provence, Universite de Provence, pp 342-345. Elbert, M., Powell, T. & Swartzlander, P. (1991) Toward a technology of generalization: How many exemplars are sufficient? Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 81-87. Fisher, J. & Glenister, J. (1992) The Hundred Word Pictures Naming Test. Melbourne: ACER. Ferguson, C. & Farwell, C. (1975) Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Language 51, 39-49. Forrest, K. (2003) Diagnostic criteria of developmental apraxia of speech used by clinical speechlanguage pathologists. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 12, 376-380. Forrest, K., Dinnsen, A. & Elbert, M. (1997). Impact of substitution patterns on phonological learning by misarticulating children. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 11, 63-76. Forrest, K., Elbert, M. & Dinnsen, D. (2000). The effect of substitution patterns on phonological treatment outcomes. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 14, 519-531. Fox A., Howard, D. & Dodd, B. (2002) Risk factors in speech disorder. International Journal Language and Communication Disorder, 37, 117-132. Grunwell, P. (1982). Clinical Phonology. London: Croom-Helm. Holm, A. & Dodd, B. (1999). An intervention case study of a bilingual child with phonological disorder. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 15, 139-158. Holm, A., Crosbie, S. & Dodd, B. (2005). Treating inconsistent speech disorders. In: Dodd, B. (ed.), Differential Diagnosis and Treatment of Children with Speech Disorder. London: Whurr, 182-201 16 Dodd, Holm, Crosbie and McIntosh 17 Holm, A., Farrier, F., & Dodd, B. (2007). The phonological awareness, reading accuracy and spelling ability of children with inconsistent phonological disorder. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 42, 1-23. Ingram, D. (1976). Phonological disability in children. New York: Elsevier. Ingram, D & Ingram, K. (2001). A whole-word approach to phonological analysis and intervention. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in School, 32, 271-283. Leahy, M. M. (2004) Therapy talk: analyzing therapeutic discourse. Language, Speech, Hearing Services in Schools 35, 70-81 McCormack, P. & Dodd, B. (1996). A feature analysis of speech errors in subgroups of speech disordered children. In McCormack, P. & Russell, A. (eds). Proceedings of the Sixth Australian International Conference on Speech Science and Technology. Adelaide: AASTA, pp 217-222. McCormack, P.F. & Dodd, B. (1998). Is inconsistency in word production an artefact of severity in developmental speech disorders? Child Language Seminar, Sheffield. McIntosh, B. & Dodd, B. (in press) Evaluation of Core Vocabulary intervention for treatment of inconsistent phonological disorder: Three case studies. Child Language Teaching and Therapy. Ozanne,A. (2005) Childhood apraxia of speech. In: Dodd, B. (ed.), Differential Diagnosis and Treatment of Children with Speech Disorder. London: Whurr, pp 71-82. Passy, J. (1990). Cued Articulation. Hawthorn, Victoria: ACER. Robey, R & Schultz, M (1998). ‘A model for conducting clinical outcome research: An adaptation of the standard protocol for use in aphasiology’ Aphasiology, 12: 787-810. Storkel, H. & Morrisette, M. (2002) The lexicon and phonology: Interaction in language acquisition. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 27, 215-230. Thyer, N. & Dodd, B. (1996). Auditory processing and phonologic disorder. Audiology 35, 37-44 Velleman, S. & Vihman, M. (2002). Whole-word phonology and templates: Trap, bootstrap, or some of each. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 33, 9-23 Word Health Organization (2007) International Classification of functioning and health. http://www.who.int/classifications/icf/en/ 17