Acta Psychologica 57 (1984) 165-192 North-Holland 165 VARIATION WITHIN CATEGORIES E.A. DAS-SMAAL and J.H. DE SWART * Free Unit'ersity, The Netherlands Accepted December 1983 Two aspects of variation within categories, relating to different models of categorization, were investigated - frequency of dimensional values and typicality differences within values. The influence of range of typicality experienced during learning and of informational value of feedback was also studied. Finally, differential forgetting of values was examined. In the experiment subjects learned to categorize faces, and then performed a classification test task and pairwise comparisons of faces. A variety of dependent variables was employed, including the galvanic skin response (GSR). Typicality and frequency of values appeared to influence categorization performance independent of each other. It was concluded that both prototype distance models and frequency models explain different aspects of variation within the same categories, and that models of classification should account for frequency of values in contrasting categories. Results showed furthermore (1) the influence of typicality range on the extension of a category; (2) no influence of specific feedback regarding representativeness of a face; (3) less decay with more important values; and (4) a positive relationship between uncertainty reduction and GSR. Traditional work on categorization treated all exemplars of a category as equally good examples of the category. This view has been criticized a.o. by Rosch (1973) and by Das-Smaal and De Swart (1981), who argued that categorization models must be capable of representing variation among exemplars within a category. Up to now, however, it is not clear what aspects of within-category variation are represented, nor how specifically they are represented (Palmer 1978). The present study investigates two forms of variation within categories. The first form concerns the similarity of a dimensional value variant to a prototypical value (typicality of variants). Exemplars having the same dimensional values may differ with respect to how typically they exhibit these * Mailing address: E.A. Das-Smaal, Vakgroep Functieleer en Methodenleer, Free University, De Boelelaan 1115, 1081 HV Amsterdam, The Netherlands. 0001-6918/84/$3.00 © 1984, Elsevier Science Publishers B.V. (North-Holland) values. For example, a basket full of big red apples will not usually contain apples all of which are equal in size and colour. The values big and red have variants that differ in typicality. The second form of variation relates to the frequency of values, irrespective of the variants with which they occur. For example, the frequency of the value red among the category of apples. The general purpose of this paper is to extend the knowledge about the effects of different aspects of withincategory variation. Two kinds of categorization models that are often contrasted are relevant to these aspects, prototype-distance models (Posner and Keele 1968; Reed 1972) and feature frequency models (Reitman and Bower 1973; Neumann 1974; Hayes-Roth and Hayes-Roth 1977). Prototypedistance models assume that subjects abstract a central representation (prototype) from the presented exemplars of a category; subsequent classification is based on distance from this prototype in a multidimensional space. On the other hand, frequency models assume that subjects encode the frequency with which dimensional values occur among members of a category, and that they base their classification judgments on these frequency measures. This paper aims to show that both types of within-category variation affect categorization independently, and that prototype-distance models and feature frequency models are complementary. Besides, this paper examines several related issues. Typicality differences within values Das-Smaal and De Swart (1981) showed that typicality of variants influenced learning performance in a traditional, welt-defined categorization task. The influence, however, differed for relevant and irrelevant dimensional values. Typicality of relevant values facilitated category learning, whereas typicality of irrelevant values did not affect performance. To further examine the typicality effects, the present experiment comprised besides a learning task also a test following learning. Influence of value typicality on classification was investigated in two ways. Firstly, typicality of value variants was varied. Exemplars composed of highly typical variants were predicted to be classified following learning more easily than exemplars with less typical variants. Secondly, the range of typicality variation experienced during learning was varied. Learning exemplars were composed of either a small range of typical variants only, or a broad range of both typical and atypical variants. Das-Smaal and De Swart (1981) found that instances composed of atypical variants slow down the learning rate. It is however possible, that experience with atypical instances results in a better representation of the category boundaries. That is, it may enhance the capability to correctly separate instances from non-instances in this area. With dot patterns, high instance variability has already been shown to enhance transfer to new exemplars (Posner and Keele 1968; Homa and Vosburgh 1976). In these studies, however, instance variability and number of learning trials experienced by the subjects were confounded. In the present experiment, this was avoided. It was predicted that small as compared with broad typicality range facilitates learning performance, whereas broad typicality range facilitates classification of new atypical exemplars. If broad range experience enhances transfer, the question further arises as to whether enhanced transfer implies only an extension of the boundary of the focal category, or additionally, a more fine-grained distinction between exemplars on both sides of a boundary. In the former case only a decrease in number of false negative errors has to be expected, whereas in the latter case a decrease in both false negative and false positive errors will appear with boundary exemplars. This issue was also investigated in the present study. Frequency Frequency of occurrence may be expressed in terms of cue validity, defined as the frequency with which a cue, or a value, is associated with one category, divided by the total frequency of that cue across all categories. Cue validity takes into account the resemblance within a category as well as distinctiveness from contrasting categories. Distinctiveness from other categories is accounted for in some models of categorization but not in others. For example, the property-set model proposed by Hayes-Roth and Hayes-Roth (1977) does, whereas Neumann's (1974) attribute-frequency model does not include this factor. In the present experiment, frequency and cue validity were investigated apart. The hypothesis was that categorization is facilitated by increasing simple frequency and by increasing cue validity of the values; that is, not only by a high frequency in the focal category, but also by low frequency in the contrasting category. Common and distinctive values and differential forgetting The relative importance of common and distinctive values to categorization is an issue closely related to frequency of occurrence. In abstracting the prototype, both common and distinctive values are involved, with distinctive values having the greater weight (Homa and Chambliss 1975). Following Homa and Chambliss, common values are defined as values which are found among many exemplars in one category, and distinctive values as values which are common within one category but do not occur in the contrasting category. In the present study, the relative influence of these two types of values was determined. Delay of testing has been shown to deteriorate performance on old non-prototypical instances but not on new and prototypical ones (Posner and Keele 1970; Strange et al. 1970; Homa et al. 1973). Homa et al. (1973) suggested that this could be attributed to a higher resistance to decay of the more frequent values, which compose the prototypes. In the present study this view was extended to the hypothesis that values which appear to be most important to classification are also the most resistant to forgetting. Because distinctive values were supposed to receive greater weight than common values, these values were expected to show less decay. Type of feedback In studies of category learning, subjects learn by feedback that an exemplar either belongs or does not belong to a particular category. Feedback usually gives no information about the degree of membership. The present experiment investigated the effects of information about degree of membership on learning and representation of categories. The feedback was either specific or non-specific. Specific feed-back informed the subjects about the representativeness of an exemplar, as determined by cue validity. Specific feedback was expected to facilitate category learning and to improve performance on subsequent test tasks. Physiological activity In previous studies on category learning, De Swart and Das-Smaal (1976, 1979a, b) and Das-Smaal and De Swart (1981) found a positive relationship between galvanic skin response (GSR) and informational value of the feedback. The studies suggested that during category learning, GSR primarily reflects uncertainty reduction about alternative hypotheses, and not information processing activities per se. The results were related to Sokolov's (1969) model of the orienting reflex (OR). The present study gathered additional information about this issue in two different ways. Firstly, the specific feedback offered an opportunity to gather further evidence about this relationship in a new way. The discrepancy between expected and actual feedback indicates the informational value of the feedback. The discrepancy between expected and actual feedback increases with increasing cue validity of a misclassified instance. A concomitant rise in GSR was predicted. Secondly, in replication of De Swart and Das-Smaal (1979 b), a relationship was predicted between GSR and the subjects' certainty estimation about their classification response. Experiment Method Subjects Ss were 48 male student volunteers. Ss who were tested immediately after learning received Dfl. 25. Those who had to return after 1 week were paid Dfl. 30 each. Stimuli Stimuli were faces, mounted on slides. The faces were constructed from Photo-Fit materials, European type (see fig. 1). Lower frame contour was the same for all faces. The faces were further composed from seven variants selected in a preliminary study [1] for each of three values of the dimensions eyes, nose and mouth. Table 1 gives mean [1] In a preliminary study, degrees of typicality for variants of dimensional values were determined. In a free-classification task, subjects partitioned sets of faces into groups showing the same family trait (dimensional value). In each set, one dimension varied. Subjects also judged the typicality of each variant within a group. Typicality appeared to be reliably rated in eight of the nine groups. The group with non-significant inter-subject agreement (eyes value 3) was made non-characteristic in the present study. c vy áy c7 0 ]) v CE 0 N E 0 y c aG >L 3 ó t) 0 s .~ v 4 'á ava E E O .r L c C y a EW Table 1 Mean typicality ratings of variants to their values. Variant Eyes Nose Value Value 1 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1.89 3.11 3.61 3.67 4.33 4.61 6.78 2 1.94 2.33 3.50 3.78 4.78 5.50 6.17 Mouth Value 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 3.11 3.61 3.67 4.11 4.22 4.39 4.89 1.67 2.61 3.00 4.11 4.56 5.67 6.39 2.78 3.06 3.39 3.89 4.28 4.83 5.56 2.89 3.33 3.61 3.78 4.28 4.78 5.00 1.78 2.56 3.39 4.00 4.89 5.39 6.06 2.22 2.94 3.39 4.28 4.44 4.94 5.72 2.94 3.50 3.61 3.78 4.00 4.50 5.67 typicality ratings of the variants to their values, as obtained from the preliminary study. Variants were numbered 1 to 7, from most to least typical, respectively. In the learning task, 18 different hairstyles were assigned randomly to the faces, using each hairstyle equally often. Non-specific feedback slides contained a + or a - , indicating that the preceding stimulus belonged to the focal or the contrasting category, respectively. Specific feedback slides showed +, + +, + + +, - -, - - -, indicating a less good, moderate, or good example of the focal category, or a less good, moderate, or good example of the contrasting category, respectively. In the test tasks, one hairstyle was used, which was the same as in the preliminary study. The classification test task was composed of faces like in the learning task. Slides for the pairwise comparison task contained two faces each. The left face was labelled A, the right face was labelled B. Apparatus The S was seated in a dimly illuminated, soundproofed room. Slides were projected onto a frosted-glass window in front of the S, via two carousel projectors located outside the room. A response panel on the arm rest of the chair contained a starter button below and two choice buttons above. The choice buttons could be labelled either + and -, or A and B by means of an interchangeable strip above the buttons. A and B were used in the pairwise comparison task, + and - in the classification tasks. A lever was fixed at the side of the panel and could be moved forwards or backwards. The range of this lever represented a continuum of certainty about the choice response. Response times (RT) were measured from the beginning of stimulus presentation to the S's choice response. Choice responses, certainty ratings and RT's were recorded automatically. Basic level GSR was measured DC and specific GSR's were measured AC by a constant 0.5 V voltage bridge. AC responses were registered with a time constant of 3 seconds. The greatest conductance change beginning between 1 and 4 seconds after the onset of feedback projection in the learning task was used to calculate the change in log conductance (4 log C). In order to reduce individual differences, J log C was taken as a measure for GSR. Ag/AgC I electrodes, 7.5 mm in diameter, were attached to the volar surface of the distal phalanx of the second and third finger of the S ' s non-preferréd hand. Interface medium was K-Y jelly. GSR, choice responses. certainty ratings and projector impulses from the timer were all recorded on a Beekman eight -channel polygraph (type R 411 dynograph). Design and procedure Following a training ta sk, Ss learned to categorize faces int() two categories: a focal ( + ) category of faces from one family, and a contrasting ( -) category of faces from other families. Subsequently, Ss were tested in a classification task and in a pairwise comparison task. The procedure was as follows: after informing the S about the experimental equipment, recording leads were attached and the S was seated in the soundproofed room. Ss were informed of the nature of the tasks, the means of responding and the meaning of the feedback. They were told that they would be presented with faces from men of different families, and that they had to learn to distinguish faces of one family from faces of other families. It was stated that hairstyle was irrelevant to classification. In the specific feedback condition Ss were told that the representativeness of a face for the family of the focal category was indicated by the number of plusses or minussen, with decreasing representativeness from + + + to - - -. With the classification task, Ss who were tested immediately were instructed to classify faces as in the learning task, the only difference being that this time no feedback would be given. Ss who were tested with one week delay received the same instruction, preceded by a short verba] re capitulation of the learning phase. In the pairwise comparison task, Ss were instructed to choose the more representa tive face for the focal category. The tasks were separated by a rest interval of about 5 minutes. After completing the test tasks, Ss were asked to describe the characteristic features of the family of the focal category. Trial composition Trials of the learning task started with presentation of the stimulus. The stimulus disappeared when the S pressed a choice button. If he did not respond within 20 seconds, the stimulus also disappeared and the S had to wake a response immediately. Then th e S had to give his certainty rating by pushing a lever to the point that corresponded with his estimation. Four seconds after stimulus switch -off a little red lamp came on, indicating that the response had to be completed. Again 4 seconds later, feedback was shown for 5 seconds. The red lamp remained on until feedback switch -off. Two seconds later, the next stimulus was presented. Trials of both test tasks were composed like the trials in the learning task, excluding the 5 seconds feedback period. In the p airwise comparison task Ss had to start each slide projection themselves by pressing the starter button. Training task. The training task consisted of 15 Afro -Asian faces, composed arbitrarily from different hairstyles, eyes, noses and mouths. Feedback of either the specific or the non-specific type was given randomly. The learning task was composed of 80 trials. Stimuli are given in table Al of the Appendix. Typicality of variants was varied, employing variants 1 to 6. Variants 1 to 3 were "typical", variants 4 to 6 were "atypical". Typicality range varied between Ss. Either a smal] range of faces with typical variants was learned, or a broad range of faces with both typical and atypical variants. Variants are indicated in table Al of the Appendix. In the small range condition, variants were chosen randomly, with the restriction that the three variants allowed in this condition had to be used in about equal numbers. In the broad range condition, 50% from the variants of the small range condition were replaced by atypical variants 4 to 6. Learning task. The three relevant dimensions (A, B and C) differed with respect to the frequency distribution of their values (see table 2). Values Al, Bl and Cl were characteristic of the focal category. Al was distinctive because of its non-occurrence in the contrasting category, BI and Cl were common values because of their high frequency in the focal category. The focal category was formed by faces containing Al and/or BI + Cl. All other faces belonged to the contrasting category. The particular assignment of the dimensions eyes, nose and mouth to A, B and C varied across Ss and was counterbal anced in three learning task problems, employing each dimension once as A, B or C. Each problem had its own accessory classification and comparison task. Frequency of the 27 combinations possible with 3 three-valued dimensions, varied from 5-15% within a category. The order of presentation was the same for each S and was randomized. Cue validities for each dimensional value for the focal category were computed by dividing the frequency of a dimensional value x in the focal category by the total frequency of x in the focal and the contrasting category. From table 2 it can be seen that Al and BI occur with equal frequencies in the focal category, but differ with respect to their cue validity. In contrast, BI and Cl have the same cue validity, but differ in frequency. Total cue validities (TCV's) of faces were computed by summation of the cue validities of their composing values. TCV's are given in table Al of the Appendix. With specific feedback, information was given about the TCV of a face. Therefore, in the Table 2 Frequency of occurrence and cue validity of dimensional values. Dimension Frequency in category + Cue validity A B Value 1 Value 2 28 0 6 20 1.00 0.23 3 6 20 0.23 C Value 1 2 3 6 13 20 10 15 10 15 0.32 0.67 1 2 3 28 6 13 0.67 0.32 14 10 0.40 0.40 focal and in the contrasting category three groups were distinguished according to TCV. The focal category contained five different TCV's. To equalize the frequencies of the stimuli within each group as much as possibble, one group was formed by the two highest TCV's (H), one group by two medium validities (M), and one group by the lowest validity (L). TCV's for the contrasting category were computed in the same way as described for the focal category and classified equally into a high, a medium and a low validity group. L, M and H groups of the focal and the contrasting category were indicated by one, two or three plusses or minusses, respectively. Classification task. The classification task was composed of 29 faces. The first two trials were habituation trials. The last three faces were faces of the highest TCV with most typical value variants, leaving out either distinctive value dimension A, or common value dimension B or C. The omitted value was replaced by a black rectangle. The trials in between consisted of three sets of eight faces. The eight faces represented different TCV's, five for the focal category (2 H, 2 M and 1 L) and three for the contrasting category (H, M and L). Each of these faces was chosen randomly from faces with the same TCV. One set of eight faces was composed of variants 2, which were typical and familiar to all Ss. Another set contained atypical variants 5, which were familiar to Ss in the broad typicality range condition, and novel to Ss in the small range. The third set contained atypical variants 7, which were novel for all Ss. The order of presentation of the trials in between was the same for each S and determined at random. The order of the last three trials varied between Ss and was counterbalanced. Pairwise comparisons. The comparison task consisted of 10 pairwise combinations of five faces, preceded by an habituation trial. The five faces which had to be compared represented the five TCV's for the focal category. They were all composed of highly typical variants (variants 1). The task was preceded by a training task of five pairs not used in the test task. The order of presentation of the stimuli was determined at random. The order was the same for each S. However, the place of the faces on a slide (left or right) was reversed for half of the Ss. Variables The experiment comprised four between-Ss variables. These were type of feedback, typicality range, time of testing, and learning task problem. Within-S variables in the learning task were learning phase and TCV. In the classification test task, typicality of variants, TCV, and omission of a dimension in a face varied within Ss, as did TCV in the pairwise comparison task. Dependent variables were number of errors (NE), certainty estimate (CE), reaction time (RT), and GSR. In the classification task also type of error (false positive or false negative) was measured. Ss were assigned randomly to one of the conditions formed by combination of the four between-Ss variables. To each possible combination of two types of feedback, two kinds of typicality range, two times of testing, and three learning task problems, two Ss were assigned. Resuits Certainty estimate, reaction time and number of errors Multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA's) were run on certainty estimate (CE), reaction time (RT) and number of errors (NE) data. The analysis used is described by Finn and Mattson (1978). Step-down results were computed in the order CE-RT-NE, because it was presumed that low certainty would result in slow reactions with a high error probability. When appropriate, data were collapsed across problems, across order of the three classification test exemplars that lacked one dimension, and across place of faces on the slides of the pairwise comparisons task. Post-hoc comparisons on interactions were carried out with Tukey's test, adjusted as per Cicchetti (1972). For all analyses, the level of statistic al significance was p < 0.05. Learning task Table 3 summarizes mean performance scores apart for beginning and end of the task. The table shows an improved performance at the end of learning in all conditions on all dependent variables. Nevertheless, learning proved difficult, with an average of 28% errors in the last quarter of the task. A MANOVA was computed for stimuli from the first (begin) and the last (end) 20 trials, for the variables mentioned in table 3. Six H, six M and six L TCV items were analyzed for each learning phase. Learning phase and TCV were both significant ( F (3, 42) = 44.66, and F (6, 39) = 10.03). The effect of learning phase represented an improved performance at the end. The TCV effect reflected facilitation by high TCV. Type of feedback was marginally significant (F (3, 42) = 2.65, p < 0.06), due to both a lower CE and a lower NE with specific feedback. Typicality range showed no multivariate main effect. However, this variable affected CE significantly, CE being higher with a small range of variants than with a broad range. The Learning phase X TCV interaction was significant (F (6, 39) = 9.77), as was the Typicality range x Learning phase X TCV interaction (F (6,39) = 2.42). At the end of the task, CE increased with increasing TCV more than in the beginning. This effect was stronger in Table 3 Mean performance scores in different phases of the learning task. Dependent Learning variable phase CE RT NE (%) Begin End Begin End Begin End Typicality range Type of feedback Total cue validity Smal] H 0.50 0.73 9.55 7.40 39.3 28.2 Broad Specific 0.44 0.43 0.63 0.64 11.33 7.89 44.7 27.5 Non-spec. M 0.43 11.08 9.81 10.36 8.22 39.7 26.7 7.08 6.96 7.61 44.5 34.3 13.2 42.7 0.50 0.63 10.43 8.38 49.0 26.7 43.8 29.2 0.74 0.48 0.66 L 0.50 0.72 10.54 the broad range condition (post-hoc analysis). None of the other interactions approached significance. Classification task Table 4 shows mean performance scores on trial 3-26. A MANOVA was performed on these trials for the variables summarized in table 4. Overall results of the MANOVA will be presented first, followed by the results on the main topics of interest. All main effects of the MANOVA were significant, except for Type of feedback. Typicality range (F(3, 38)=3.30) reflected a significantly higher NE for the small range condition. Delay of testing (F(3, 38) = 2.90) affected only RT significantly, with higher RT for the delayed condition. Typicality of variants (F(6, 35) = 11.63) represented a significant decrease in CE and increase in both RT and NE from variants 2 to 5 to 7. Finally, TCV (F(6,35) = 6.95) indicated a significant decrease in CE and increase in RT and NE with decreasing TCV. The interaction between Typicality range x Type of feedback x TCV was significant (F(6, 35) = 2.49). This was due to a relatively high NE for H TCV in the small range and non-specific feedback condition. Finally, the highest interaction was significant (F(4, 160) = 3.84), due to the NE variable. The estimated proportion of variance accounted for by this interaction, however, was one percent. None of the other interactions was significant. Table 4 Mean performance during the classification test. Condition Dependent variable Typicality range Smal] Broad Feedback Specific Non-specific Time of testing Immediately Delayed Variant typicality Variant 2 5 7 Total cue validity H M L CE RT NE (%) 0.66 0.62 8.05 7.61 31.6 23.4 0.61 0.67 8.29 7.38 26.9 28.1 0.64 0.64 6.64 9.02 29.5 25.5 0.70 0.63 0.59 7.26 7.90 8.34 21.4 25.8 35.4 0.68 0.63 0.58 7.46 7.76 8.38 21.1 31.7 35.8 T y p i c a l i t y . From the MANOVA, high typicality of value variants appeared to facilitate classification performance. However, this effect could have been confounded with novelty, because experience with the variants varied. Although all faces in the classification task were new to the Ss, lome of the variants were shown during learning in other combinations. Thus, variants 2 were old in the small, and variants 2 and 5 were old in the broad range condition. The unconfounded effect of variant typicality was analyzed within each category and typicality range condition. In the focal category, a typicality effect showed up in the small but not in the broad range condition. In the smal] range condition less errors were made with variant 5 than with variant 7 exemplars (sign test, ( p < 0.02). RT and CE were not significantly affected (Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed ranks test). In the broad range condition variant 2 and 5 exemplars were compared, and these showed no significant differences in CE, RT or NE (Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed ranks test and sign test, respectively). In the contrasting category, a typicality effect showed up in the broad but not in the small range condition. Variant 5 and 7 exemplars in the small range condition did not differ significantly in CE, RT or NE. However, variant 2 and 5 exemplars in the broad range condition did show the expected differences. CE was significantly higher and RT was faster with variants 2 than with variants 5 (Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed ranks test, both p < 0.01). NE tended to be smaller with variants 2 than with variants 5 (sign test, p < 0.09) . Typicality range. The extension of the focal category appeared to be larger following broad than following small range experience. More + responses were given in the broad than in the small range condition (Mann-Whitney U-test, U= 380.5, p < 0.03). To get more insight in the category extension, the boundary (cut-off) between both categories was determined. The cut-off was defined as the point at which faces were categorized 50% of the cases in the focal category. With small range experience, the cut-off turned out to fall between variants 5 and 7 of the focal category, whereas with broad range experience the cut -off was placed between variants 7 of both categories. Type of errors were analyzed to test the hypotheses of better classification of new atypical focal exemplars and of new atypical contrasting category exemplars in the broad than in the small range condition. Type of errors were analyzed by an ANOVA for the variables Typicality range, Type of feedback, Delay of testing, Typicality of variants, TCV and Type of classification error (false positive or false negative). The number of false positives and false negatives possible was made equal by taking mean NE both over the two H and over the two M TCV focal exemplars. The interaction Typicality range x Typicality of variants x Type of error (see fig. 2) was relevant to the hypotheses. This interaction was significant (F(2, 80) = 3.20). Regarding the focal exemplars, no differences between small and broad range condition were found for the typical exemplars (variants 2; t = 0 , df = 80). However, the number of false negatives was significantly higher with the small than with the broad range condition for the atypical exemplars (both variants 5 and 7; t = 2.85 and t = 2.24, dj= 80, p < 0.01 and p < 0.025, respectively). Fig. 2 also shows that typicality range does not affect the number of misclassifications of atypical contrasting category items. The number of false positives did not differ significantly for the three typicality levels nor for both typicality range conditions (post-hoc analysis). The ANOVA on errors showed furthermore the following results with respect to type of error. Type of error was significant (F(1, 40) = 19.91). False negative responses were given more often than false positive ones. Type of feedback interacted significantly with type of error (F(1, 40)=12.84). Post-hoc analysis indicated that this interaction reflected significantly more errors on focal than on contrasting category exemplars in the non-specific feedback condition ( p < 0.01), but a similar number of false positives and false negatives with specific feedback. No other interactions with type of error were significant. Apart from effects of type of error, the same results as with NE in the MANOVA mentioned above were obtained. Frequencv. Table 4 shows that CE increased and that RT and NE decreased with increasing TCV. Main effect of TCV in the MANOVA was significant, and all mutual differences between H, M and L TCV were significant for CE, RT and NE. H-L differences were also significant within the non-specific feedback condition (post-hoc analyses). Common and distinctive values and differential forgetting. The last three faces of the classification task were faces in which either dimension A (with distinctive value Al), or dimension B or C (with common value BI or Cl) was omitted. Performance was significantly worse when the distinctive than when a common value was omitted; NE was 15, 7 and 9 with omission of A, B and C, respectively (Cochran Q-test; Q = 7.13, dj= 2, p < 0.05). RT mirrored these results (Friedman two-way ANOVA; x2 = 9.28, F A L S E- , S M AL L ~ - -F A L S E - , BR O AD F A L S E+ , SM A LL FALSE+, BROAD c--- 2 5 7 TYPICALITY Fig. 2. Type of error as a joint function of typicality range during learning (small and broad) and variant typicality (variants 2, 5 and 7) in the classification task. d/= 2, p < 0.01). CE differences were non-significant (Friedman two-way ANOVA; x2 = 3.21, df = 2). Differential forgetting did occur. With immediate testing, no significant differences were found for either NE, or RT, or CE (Cochran Q-test; Q = 1.56, and Friedman two-way ANOVA; x2 = 1.00 and x2 = 1.94, respectively). However, with delayed testing, significant differences were found for NE (Cochran Q-test; Q = 7.14, df = 2, p < 0.05). NE was 8, 3 and 3 with omission of A, B and C, respectively. Again RT mirrored these results, while CE differences were marginally significant (Friedman two-way ANOVA; x2 = 11.09 and x2 = 4.75, df = 2, p < 0.01 and p < 0.10, respectively). Type of feedback. The expected facilitation of classification performance in the specific feedback condition did not show up. Main effect in the MANOVA was non-significant. However, as said before, the type of error analysis showed that Ss in the specific feedback condition made less errors in the focal but more in the contrastang category than Ss in the non-specific feedback condition. Pairwise comparison task From the pairwise comparisons the ranking of the five faces of different TCV's was established for each S. Agreement among rankings was significant (Kendall's Coefficient of Concordance, W = 0.38, x2 = 71.95, df = 4, p < 0.001). The mean ranking was the same as the ordering to TCV (experimenter-determined), except for the order of the two faces with the lowest TCV (see table 5). Faces of the type 2 1 1, which contained the two characteristic common values BI and Cl, were ranked higher than 1 3 3 type faces, containing only one characteristic, distinctive value (Al). Mean rankings were also calculated for the specific and the non-specific feedback condition separately (see table 5). In both conditions Ss agreed significantly in their rankings (W = 0.31 and W = 0.32, x2 = 29.77 and x2 = 30.56 for specific and nonspecific feedback, respectively, df = 4, p < 0.001). The order in the specific feedback condition was the same as the overall mean ranking. In the non-specific feedback Table 5 Objective and consensual rankings according to representativeness of five types of faces in the pairwise comparison task. Each face was composed of one value (1, 2 or 3) for each of three dimensions (A, B and C). Values Al, BI and Cl were characteristic of the focal category. Al was a distinctive value, BI and Cl were common values. Faces Total cue validity Objective ranking Consensual mean rankings Overall Specific feedback Non-specific feedback AIB1CI A1BIC3 AIB2C1 AIB3C3 A2BICI 2.34 1 2.07 2 1.99 3 1.72 4 1.57 5 1.76 1.78 1.75 2.55 2.48 2.63 3.28 3.29 3.27 4.10 4.02 4.19 3.30 3.44 3.17 condition, faces of the type 2 1 1 were ranked higher than both 1 3 3 and 1 2 1 type faces. Effect of TCV was further determined by examinating whether performance was influenced by the distance in TCV between two faces in a comparison. Therefore, a MANOVA was run on CE and RT data, with the main factors of typicality range, type of feedback, delay of testing and distance. The 10 distantes among the five faces were classified into three groups. The small distance group contained the four smallest, the medium the three intermediate, and the large distance group the three largest differences. Distance was significant (F(4, 37) = 4.20). The distance effect represented a significantly increasing CE and a significantly decreasing RT with increasing distance between the faces. Typicality range and Type of feedback were both non-significant. Delay of testing was significant ( F (2, 39) = 3.72), reflecting a higher RT in the delayed condition. There were no significant interactions. After completing the test tasks, Ss were asked to describe the characteristic features of the family of the focal category. The number of times a value was mentioned rightly was 43, 37 and 27 out of 48 each for the characteristic values Al, Bl and Cl, respectively. These numbers differed significantly (Cochran Q-test; Q = 16.33, d j - 2, p<0.001). GSR To test the first hypothesis regarding GSR, an ANOVA was performed on GSR data for misclassifications in the specific feedback condition. To avoid differences in GSR caused by disparity in frequency and habituation, the learning task was divided into four equal parts of 20 trials. From each part, an equal amount of H, M and L TCV disconfirming feedback trials were taken, which were the first to occur within each part. Main factors in the analysis were typicality range, delay of testing, and TCV. TCV was significant (F(2, 40) = 4.51), representing the predicted decrease in GSR from H to M to L TCV. The interaction Typicality range X Delay of testing was significant (F(1, 20) = 4.80). This reflected for the broad range condition a higher GSR with delay than with immediate testing, but for the small range condition the reverse. None of the other main effects or interactions approached significance. The second hypothesis on the relationship between GSR and informational value of feedback was tested by the relationship between GSR and CE on trials with confirming and on trials with disconfirming feedback. On the average, informational value is smaller with confirming than with disconfirming feedback (De Swart and Das-Smaal 1979a, b). Furthermore, it was assumed that informational value of confirming feed-back was smaller when it followed a high CE than a low CE, whereas the opposite would hold with disconfirming feedback. An ANOVA was run for the variables of typicality range, type of feedback, delay of testing, and CE with confirming and disconfirming feedback. The latter factor contained four levels: CE > 0.50 before confirmation (high conf), CE 0.50 before confirmation (low conf), CE 0.50 before disconfirmation (low disc), and CE> 0.50 before disconfirmation (high disc). It was hypothesized that GSR would rise in this order. The analysis showed that the only significant main factor was CE with confirming and disconfirming feedback (F(3, 120) = 10.02). Tukey's test indicated that GSR on high conf = low conf < low disc < high disc. Discussion Within category variation The present study established the effects of two forms of variation within categories. One form follows from prototype-distance models and concerns typicality differences within dimensional values; it is continuous in character. The other form relates to the frequency of discrete values, in conformity with frequency models. Frequency is not accounted for by distance models. Both frequency and variant typicality were varied in the same experiment on the same values. Each form of variation was shown to affect categorization performance in its own way. The forms did not interact. Therefore, it is concluded that prototype-distance and frequency models are complementary to one another; they explain different aspects of variation within the same categories. A viable model of categorization must be able to account for both variant typicality and frequency effects, and not for only one of them. Typicality differences within values Increasing typicality of variants highly improved classification performance following learning. There is a problem with the interpretation of this result. Typicality and experience with the variants were confounded in the experiment. To disentangle both variables, the effect of typicality was further determined separately for old and novel variants. In consequence of the experimental set-up, the effects for novel and old variants could be tested only within the small and within the broad typicality range condition of learning, respectively. In the small range condition, medium typical focal exemplars were classified better than atypical ones. This efffect did not show up in the contrasting category. The width of the focal category could account for the lack of effect in the contrasting category. Subjects in the small range condition formed a small extension of the focal category. The small extension, and the instruction to focus on the focal category, may have induced categorization by default with items of the contrasting category, resulting in a reduction of typicality effects in the contrasting category. In the broad range condition, typical and medium typical focal exemplars were classified equally, whereas in the contrasting category, typical exemplars were classified faster and with more certainty than medium typical ones. The Jack of typicality effect in the focal category can be explained also by category width. In the broad range condition, the focal category had a large extension. Subjects could have compressed the focal category to obtain a category of a more manageable size, causing a reduction of differences within the focal category. This explanation is consistent with a model of stimulus discrimination proposed by Gravetter and Lockhead (1973), which predicts that two stimuli are more likely to be confused when they are part of a broad range of stimuli than when they belong to a small range. Das-Smaal and De Swart (1981) demonstrated that typicality variation within values influenced ease of category learning with well-defined conceptual rules. The results were taken as evidence in support of the prototype view and against models of categorization that treat exemplars of a category as equivalent in their degree of membership of the category. The present results with ill-defined categories, amplify this support by showing that categories, once learned, still allow for degrees of membership. Classification is more difficult the less typical an exemplar is, but the effect is dependent on category and typicality range experienced during learning. Typicality range Regarding learning, small range experience did not show the expected facilitating effect, although it resulted in higher response certainty. In contrast, Das-Smaal and De Swart (1981) found faster learning with typical rather than atypical instances. In the present experiment, learning by typical instances was compared with a mixed condition of both typical and atypical variants. If learning with this mixed condition is easier than with atypical instances only, this can explain the smaller effect in the present study. Following learning, broad range experience resulted in less classification errors than small range experience, due to better classification of both medium typical and (new) atypical focal faces in the broad range condition. The result that small range subjects categorized the atypical faces more often in the contrasting than in the focal category, clearly indicates the smaller extension formed by these subjects. The results agree with Posner and Keele (1968) and Homa and Vosburgh (1976), who found that broad experience on dot patterns enhances transfer to new exemplars. The present study, however, excluded the alternative explanation of their results, that broad range subjects had more learn- ing experience than small range subjects. In the contrasting category, boundary exemplars were not classified better with broad than with small range experience. However, with small range experience, contrasting category items might have been rejected as focal exemplars relatively easily by default. This could have veiled the advantage of broad range experience. Categorization of atypical items of a contrasting category may be improved by broad experience in a situation where subjects are prevented from categorizing by default, for instance in a classification task with more than two categories. The present study demonstrated effects of typicality of variants that cannot easily be accounted for by discrete feature frequency. At the same time, the study indicates how category boundaries are affected by learning experience. Broad range experience results in a larger extension of the focal category, with better classification of atypical exemplars on the focal side of the boundary but not on the contrasting side. Frequency in focal and contrasting category The second form of within-category variation implied variation among dimensional values in frequency of occurrence both in the focal and in the contrasting category, as expressed in TCV. Like predicted, high TCV facilitated both category learning and classification after learning, and this yielded also when non-specific feedback was given. The importance of TCV is further evidenced by pairwise comparisons, from which rank orderings of focal exemplars were inferred. Subjects showed significant consensus on these rankings. The rankings followed the ordering according to TCV except for the ranking of the lower TCV exemplars. Furthermore, pairwise comparison performance improved with increasing TCV distance between the exemplars. The facilitating effect of increasing TCV implies that the exemplars which are categorized best are the ones which have the most in common with other exemplars of the same category, and at the same time share the least with items outside the category. The findings support the idea of Rosch (1973) that the categories which are formed are structured according to the principle of maximization of cue validity. Besides TCV, simple frequency of occurrence affected performance. Two common values had the same TCV but different simple frequencies. The more frequent value was mentioned more often as a characteristic feature than the less frequent one, although no effect of simple frequency was shown with classification of typical focal faces that misled one dimension. An important conclusion of the present study is that models of categorization that do not account for frequencies outside the focal category, are not supported by the differential performance on common as compared with distinctive values. Classification performance on faces appeared to be worse when the distinctive value dimension was left out than when the common value dimension with the same focal frequency distribution was omitted. This difference has to be attributed to different frequency distributions of values in the contrasting category. Furthermore, when asked to describe the characteristic features of the family of the focal category, subjects more often mentioned the distinctive value than the common values. This supports the idea that distinctive values are weighted more heavily than common values (Homa and Chambliss 1975). The results on delay of testing further specify this conclusion. With value omission in the classification task, delay had less effect on distinctive than on common values. This suggests that distinctive values become relatively more important to classification with the passage of time. The conclusion that models of classification should account for frequency of values in contrasting categories is supported by two studies on categories defined in terms of discrete values. Rosch and Mervis (1975) found that the best examples of a category were related least to members of other categories. Although Martin and Caramazza (1980) did not aim to investigate the issue of frequency in a contrasting category, the effect can be deduced from their results. The study employed binary values with equal frequencies in category x. In the contrasting category, one value occurred always, the other value never. The Jatter value facilitated performance on category x. This can only be an effect of the differential frequencies in the contrasting category. It can be argued that the effect is limited to their experimental set-up. Definingness of one value of a bi-valued dimension to one category, may induce the tendency to regard the other value as relatively more important to the other category. This objection was cancelled out in the present experiment, since discrete, defining and binary values were not used. Nevertheless, effects of frequency in the contrasting category were obtained. The experiment of Martin and Caramazza (1980) was aimed at comparing several models of categorization. None of the models was consistently supported by their data. However, the models were not properly tested. In predicting from the Rosch model, they failed to take into account frequency in the contrasting category. Furthermore, their predictions did not account for which category the subjects considered the focal one. Their results showed that the subjects focused on only one category and categorized instances of the second category by default. Task difference In the pairwise comparison task, lower TCV faces were ranked higher than expected. This result differs from that on the learning task and the classification test task, which showed better performance with medium than with low TCV faces. Task difference can explain the disagreement. In a classification task, differences between categories are relevant, and this stresses distinctive values. On the contrary, a withincategory comparison task asks for similarity judgments, stressing common values (Tversky and Gati 1978). This explains why medium TCV faces, with a distinctive value and either one or no common values, were classified better but were not ranked higher than the low TCV face, with two common values but no distinctive value. In addition to task difference, correlation of values could explain why face 2 1 1 was ranked even slightly higher than face 1 2 1 in the non-specific feedback condition. Importance of values to a category may be enhanced by conjoint frequency (Rosch 1975; Hayes-Roth and Hayes-Roth 1977; Medin and Schaffer 1978). In the learning task, B1 and Cl occurred together 40%, and Al and Cl 20%. With otherwise equal frequencies, this could have made face 2 1 1 more representative than face 1 2 1. Task difference also explains why Kellogg (1980) found representativeness ratings to be a function of focal frequency, regardless of frequency in a contrasting category. Distinction between categories - and therefore frequency in t contrasting category - is not of primary importance in judging representativeness of exemplars within one category. Task dependency of the weights assigned to values, implies theoretically that the represented information about occurrence in a category has to be available apart from information about occurrence in other categories. It depends upon the task whether or not occurrence in other categories is taken into account. It could be argued that for each value relevant to a category two weights are stored, one accounting for distinctiveness, the other only for commonality. However, this is unlikely because distinctiveness depends upon which category is the contrasting one. For instance, comparing dogs with Cats asks for other distinctive values than comparing dogs with wolves. This is a matter of context. Labov (1973) showed that weighting of values is influenced by context. Context altered the degree to which a value was considered important in naming cup-like objects. For "bowl" classification, more weight was attached to the value "wide" when an object was imagined to be filled with food than if it was judged without that imagination instruction. If weights of values depend on task and context, it is unlikely that weights per se are stored in memory. It would not be very parsimonious to store different weights for various types of tasks and possible contexts. It seems more likely that values, their frequency of occurrence in various categories, and perhaps their co-occurrences, are registered. Different operations are used to obtain and weigh the represented information that is needed in a particular context or task. When distinction between categories is relevant, weights are assigned that take into account frequencies in contrasting categories. Otherwise, resemblance within a category is weighted more heavily. Type of feedback The effect of a new, specific, kind of feedback was investigated in our study. Specific feedback did not show the expected facilitatory effect, either on learning, or on tests. In the learning phase, specific feedback tended to decrease the number of errors. However, contrary to our expectation, it also tended to make subjects less certain about the correctness of their responses. The Jatter tendency can tentatively be interpreted as an effect of confusion, which offers one explanation of the lack of positive results on specific feedback. With specific feedback, subjects have to evaluate their response and to process extra information on representativeness. The time to process all information was fixed at 7 seconds for both kinds of feedback. In the highly informative specific feedback condition this time might have been too short. Another explanation is that evaluation of information concerning frequency of occurrence is a relatively autonomous process. This process is not influenced by whether the information is presented implicitly or explicitly. Hence, specific feedback will not improve performance. In fine with this explanation is the suggestion of Kellogg et al. (1978) that subjects automatically create differences in representativeness in organizing a category. They found that typicality judgments were positively related to frequency of values, and that these judgments were not influenced by whether or not the subjects were specially instructed to learn which faces were better examples than others. An unexpected finding and potentially interesting result was the interaction between type of feedback and type of error. Following specific feedback, the number of categorization errors did not differ between the focal and the contrasting category. Following non-specific feedback, more errors were made on focal than on contrasting category items. Specific feedback, therefore, may favour performance on the focal category, but not on the contrasting category. Physiological activity Previous results of De Swart and Das-Smaal (1976, 1979a, b) and of Das-Smaal and De Swart (1981) have demonstrated that increasing informational value of feedback in concept learning tasks is accompanied by increasing GSR activity. The specific feedback condition of our study provided the opportunity to test the relationship in a new way. Informational value of feedback was assumed to be positively related to the discrepancy between expected and actual feedback. This discrepancy increased with increasing representativeness of misclassified instances to their category. Therefore, in the specific feedback condition, a rising GSR was predicted with increasing TCV of a misclassified instance. The results agree with this hypothesis, providing new evidence for the existence of a positive relationship between GSR and informational value of feedback. De Swart and Das-Smaal (1979b) found different relationships between certainty about classification responses and GSR for different types of feedback. The present study replicated these findings. It was assumed that informational value of confirming feedback was lower with high than with low CE. The reverse was assumed to hold with disconfirming feedback. Furthermore, on the average, disconfirming feedback supplies more information than confirming feedback. Hence, a higher GSR with disconfirming than with confirming feedback was predicted. Therefore, GSR was expected to increase from high to low CE confirmed responses, and further from low to high CE disconfirmed responses. GSR appeared to increase in this order, although the difference between low and high CE confirmed responses was in the predicted direction, but not significant. De Swart et al. (1981) found the same relationship with an EEG response (the P300) as an indicator of physiological activity. In total, the results clearly support the idea that the amount of uncertainty reduction in a category learning task is reflected in the change in autonomic activity, measured by GSR. Table Al Values and variants of faces from the learning task for the smal] and broad typicality range condition. With each dimension values are indicated first and variants are given next. Values are numbered 1 to 3. Variants are numbered - 1 to - 6 . Stimulus number Typicality range Total Category Small cue validity label Broad Dimension Dimension A B C A B C 1 2-2 1-3 1-1 2-5 1-6 1-4 1.57(L) + 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 2-3 1-2 2-1 3-2 1-2 1-2 2-1 1-1 1-2 3-3 1-3 1-3 2-3 1-3 1-3 3-2 2-2 2-2 3-3 3-3 3-1 1-1 1-3 3-1 2-1 3-2 2-2 1-3 1-3 2-1 3-3 1-1 3-3 2-1 3-3 3-1 3-2 2-1 1-1 3-2 1-1 3-1 1-3 1-3 3-1 1-2 2-1 3-2 3-2 1-2 2-3 2-1 2-1 1-2 1-2 1-1 1-2 1-2 3-3 2-3 1-1 1-2 3-3 2-3 2-1 1-2 1-1 1-1 1-3 2-1 3-2 2-1 2-3 2-3 1-2 1-3 2-2 1-1 2-2 1-3 1-1 1-2 3-3 3-1 2-2 3-1 3-1 2-3 2-1 2-1 2-1 2-2 2-2 2-3 1-3 2-3 3-1 1-2 2-2 1-2 3-1 1-1 1-3 2-3 1-3 3-1 1-2 2-3 3-1 1-2 1-2 3-2 3-3 3-1 1-3 2-2 2-1 2-3 1-2 2-1 3-5 1-5 1-5 2-1 1-1 1-2 3-3 1-6 1-3 2-3 1-6 1-3 3-2 2-2 2-2 3-6 3-6 3-4 1-1 1-3 3-4 2-1 3-5 2-2 1-3 1-3 2-4 3-6 1-4 3-3 2-1 3-6 3-1 3-5 2-4 1-4 3-2 1-4 3-1 1-3 1-6 3-4 1-2 2-4 3-2 3-2 1-2 2-3 2-1 2-1 1-2 1-5 1-1 1-2 1-5 3-6 2-6 1-1 1-2 3-6 2-6 2-1 1-2 1-4 1-1 1-6 2-4 3-5 2-4 2-3 2-3 1-2 1-6 2-2 1-4 2-5 1-3 1-1 1-2 3-3 3-1 2-5 3-4 3-4 2-6 2-4 2-1 2-4 2-2 2-5 2-6 1-3 2-6 3-4 1-2 2-5 1-5 3-4 1-4 1-3 2-3 1-6 3-4 1-5 2-3 3-1 1-5 1-5 3-5 3-6 3-1 1-3 2-2 2-4 0.95(H) 2.34(H) 1.22(M) 1.57(L) 2.07(H) 1.72(M) 1.30(L) 1.72(M) 1.72(M) 0.95(H) 2.07(H) 1.72(M) 0.95(H) 1.72(M) 2.07(H) 1.30(L) 1.57(L) 1.30(L) 1.30(L) 1.22(M) 0.95(H) 2.34(H) 2.07(H) 1.22(M) 1.22(M) 0.95(H) 1.57(L) 2.07(H) 2.34(H) 1.30(L) 0.95(H) 1.99(M) 1.22(M) 0.95(H) 0.95(H) 1.30(L) 1.57(L) 0.95(H) 2.07(H) + + + + + + + + + + + - + + + + + + - + + Table Al (continued) Stimulus number Typicality range Small Broad Dimension Total cue validity C a tego rv label Dimension A B C A 41 2-1 2-2 1-3 2-4 B 2-5 C 1-3 1.22(M) - 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 1-2 1-3 2-1 3-2 3-3 2-1 1-2 3-1 3-1 1-3 3-3 3-1 2-1 3-2 2-2 1-3 3-3 2-3 1-1 3-2 1-1 3-2 2-3 2-1 1-3 2-3 1-1 2-1 1-2 2-3 1-1 3-2 1-2 3-2 2-3 2-1 3-3 2-2 1-1 2-3 1-3 1-1 3-2 1-2 3-2 1-1 1-1 1-1 3-3 1-1 3-1 3-3 3-3 3-3 1-2 1-2 1-2 1-3 2-3 2-1 1-2 2-3 2-1 1-1 1-2 1-2 1-3 3-3 1-1 3-3 1-1 1-3 3-1 1-3 1-1 1-2 3-1 2-2 3-2 3-1 3-1 3-3 1-1 3-1 3-2 2-1 3-2 2-2 1-2 3-3 2-1 1-1 1-2 2-3 2-1 1-2 2-2 1-3 1-3 1-3 3-2 1-3 3-3 3-1 1-1 2-1 2-1 1-3 1-1 3-2 2-1 2-3 1-3 3-2 1-3 3-3 1-3 1-2 1-6 2-4 3-5 3-3 2-4 1-2 3-4 3-4 1-3 3-6 3-4 2-1 3-2 2-2 1-6 3-3 2-3 1-4 3-5 1-1 3-5 2-3 2-1 1-6 2-3 1-1 2-4 1-5 2-6 1-4 3-5 1-5 3-2 2-3 2-4 3-3 2-2 1-4 2-3 1-3 1-4 3-5 1-5 3-5 1-1 1-4 1-1 3-3 1-1 3-4 3-3 3-3 3-3 1-5 1-2 1-5 1-6 2-3 2-4 1-5 2-6 2-1 1-4 1-2 1-5 1-3 3-6 1-4 3-3 1-4 1-6 3-4 1-3 1-4 1-2 3-4 2-2 3-2 3-4 3-4 3-3 1-4 3-1 3-5 2-1 3-2 2-5 1-2 3-3 2-1 1-4 1-2 2-3 2-1 1-2 2-5 1-6 1-6 1-3 3-2 1-6 3-6 3-6 1-4 2-1 2-1 1-3 1-1 3-2 2-1 2-3 1-3 3-5 1-6 3-6 1-3 1.72(M) 2.07(H) 1.30(L) 0.95(H) 1.57(L) 0.95(H) 2.07(H) 1.30(L) 1.30(L) 1.72(M) 1.57(L) 0.95(H) 0.95(H) 1.22(M) 1.22(M) 2.07(H) 1.30(L) 1.57(L) 2.07(H) 1.22(M) 1.99(M) 1.57(L) 0.95(H) 1.22(M) 2.07(H) 1.30(L) 2.34(H) 1.30(L) 1.72(M) 1.59(L) 1.99(M) 0.95(H) 2.07(H) 0.95(H) 1.59(L) 1.30(L) 1.59(L) 0.95(H) + + + + + + + + - 1.99(M) + + + + + - + + + + + + + Note: High, medium and low TCV are indicated in parentheses by H, M and L, respectively. References Cicchetti, D.V., 1972. Extension of multiple-range tests to interaction tables in the analysis of variance: a rapid approximate solution. Psychological Bulletin 77, 405-408. Das-Smaal, E.A. and J.H. De Swart, 1981, Typicality, physiological activity and concept identification. Acta Psychologica 47, 1-24. De Swart, J.H. and E.A. Das-Smaal, 1976. Relationship between SCR, heart rate and information processing. Biological Psychology 4, 41-49. De Swart. J.H. and E.A. Das-Smaal. 1979a. Physiological activity in an attribute learning task. Acta Psychologica 43, 57-69. De Swart, J.H. and E.A. Das-Smaal, 1979b. 'Orienting reflex and uncertainty reduction in a concept learning task'. In: H.D. Kimmel, E.H. van Olst and J.F. Orlebeke (eds.), The orienting reflex in humans. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. De Swart, J.H., A. Kok and E.A. Das-Smaal, 1981. P300 and uncertainty reduction in a concept-identification task. Psychophysiology, 18, 619-629. Finn, J.D. and 1. Mattson, 1978. Multivariate analysis in education research. Applications of the MULTIVARIANCE program. Chicago, IL: National Educational Resources. Inc. Gravetter, F. and G.R. Lockhead, 1973. Criteria] range as a frame of reference for stimulus judgment. Psychological Review 80, 203-216. Hayes-Roth, B: and F. Hayes-Roth, 1977. Concept.learning and the recognition and classification of exemplars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 16, 321-338. Homa, D. and D. Chambliss, 1975. The relative contributions of common and distinctive information on the abstraction from ill-defined categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 104, 351-359. Homa, D. and R. Vosburgh, 1976. Category breadth and the abstraction of prototypical information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 2, 322-330. Homa, D., J. Cross, D. Cornell, D. Goldman and S. Schwartz, 1973. Prototype abstractions and classification of new instances as a function of number of instances defining the prototype. Journal of Experimental Psychology 101, 116-122. Kellogg, R.T., 1980. Simple feature frequency versus feature validity models of forma on of prototypes. Perceptual and Motor Skills 51. 295-306. Kellogg, R.T., L.E. Bourne and B.R. Ekstrand, 1978. Feature frequency and the acquisition of natural concepts. American Journal of Psychology 91, 212-222. Labov, W., 1973. `The boundaries of words and their meanings'. In: C.M. Bailey and R.W. Shuy (eds.), New ways of analyzing variations in English. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press. Martin, R.C. and A. Caramazza, 1980. Classification in well-defined and ill-defined categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Genera] 109, 320-353. Medin, D.L. and M.M. Schaffer, 1978. A context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review 85, 207-238. Neumann, P.G., 1974. An attribute frequency model for the abstraction of prototypes. Memory and Cognition 2, 241-248. Palmer, S.E., 1978. `Fundamental aspects of cognitive representation'. In: E. Rosch and B.B. Lloyd (eds.), Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Posner, M.I. and S. Keele, 1968. On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology 77, 353-363. Posner, M.I. and S.W. Keele, 1970. Retention of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology 83, 304-308. Reed, S.K., 1972. Pattern recognition and categorization. Cognitive Psychology 3, 382-407. Reitman, J.S. and C.H. Bower, 1973. Storage and later recognition of exemplars of concepts. Cognitive Psychology 4, 194-207. Rosch, E., 1973. `On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories'. In: T.E. Moore (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press. Rosch, E., 1975. Cognitive representation of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 3, 192-233. Rosch, E. and C. Mervis, 1975. Family resemblances: studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology 7, 573-605. Sokolov, E.N., 1969. `The modeling properties of the nervous system'. In: M. Cole and 1. Maltzman (eds.), A handbook of contemporary Soviet psychology. New York: Basic Books. Strange, W., R. Keeney, F.S. Kessel and J.J. Jenkins, 1970. Abstraction over time of prototypes from distortion of random dot patterns: a replication. Journal of Experimental Psychology 83, 508-510. Tversky, A. and J. Gati, 1978. `Studies of similarity'. In: E. Rosch and B.B. Lloyd (eds.), Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.