F10_L1_vowel-writeup

advertisement
Intro to Linguistics
Autumn 2009
[Ʀ] = uvular trill
[q] = voiceless uvular stop
Inuktitut
[iglumut]
‘to a house’
[pinna]
‘that one up there’
[ukiaq]
‘late fall’
[ani]
‘female’s brother’
[aiviq]
‘walrus’
[iglu]
‘(snow) house’
[aniguvit]
‘if you leave’
[panna]
‘that place up there’
[aglu]
‘seal’s breathing hole’
[aivuq]
‘she goes home’
[iglumit]
‘from a house’
[ini]
‘place, spot’
[anigavit]
‘because you leave
[ukiuq]
‘winter’
Inuktitut has 3 phonemic vowels: [i], [a], [u]. We know that [i] and [a] are phonemically
different because of the minimal pair [ini] ~ [ani], [i] and [u] are phonemically different because
of [iglumit] ~ [iglumut], and finally [u] and [a] are phonemically different because of [ukiaq] ~
[ukiuq].
Greenlandic Eskimo
[ivnaq]
‘bluff’
[qasaloq]
‘bark’
[ipeƦaq]
‘harpoon strap’
[ikusik]
‘elbow’
[imaq]
‘sea’
[qilaluvaq]
‘white whale’
[tuluvaq]
‘raven’
[qatigak]
‘back’
[itumaq]
‘palm of hand’
[sakiak]
‘rib’
[sava]
‘sheep’
[ugsik]
‘cow’
[nuna]
‘land’
[oƦpik]
‘tree’
[ine]
‘goose’
[nanoq]
‘bear’
[maƦƦaq]
‘clay’
[iga]
‘pot’
[isse]
‘eye’
[seƦmeq]
‘glacier’
[sako]
‘room’
[neƦdloq]
[iseƦaq]
[igdlo]
‘ankle’
‘house’
1
‘tool’
Intro to Linguistics
Autumn 2009
This is trickier; there don’t appear to be any minimal pairs here. The real reason minimal pairs
indicate that 2 sounds are meaningfully different is because they show that there’s no way to
always consistently predict the distribution of the two sounds. (In Inuktitut, [ani] ~ [ini] illustrate
that you can’t predict where [a] vs. [i] occur.) So we can also investigate whether sounds are
phonemically or allophonically different by coming at it from the other direction: is their
distribution predictable? If the distribution of any pair of sounds is predictable, that pair is
allophonically different; if the distribution between any pair is not predictable, that pair is
phonemically different.
Here are all the sounds that can precede and follow each of these 5 vowels in the data above:
__a
i
g
q
a__
t
k
q
Ʀ
Ʀ
v
s
m
n
l
v
s
l
#
__i
p
t
k
q
s
#
i__
a
p
t
k
g
v
s
m
n
l
__u
t
k
n
l
#
u__
g
v
s
m
n
l
__e
p
s
m
n
e__
q
Ʀ
#
__o
k
n
l
#
o__
q
Ʀ
#
These lists illustrate that, based on immediate neighboring sounds (which is the relevant
conditioning environment here), the difference between [a] and each other vowel is never
predictable. [a] and [i] can both follow [q] and [s], and they can both precede [t], [k], [v], [s], and
[l]. While there are some sounds that only precede [a] (e.g. [g], [n]), or only follow [i] (e.g. [p],
[m]), the fact that there are some environments (e.g. after [s], or before [t]) where it is impossible
to predict whether [a] or [i] should occur means that there’s no way to always consistently
predict which sound appears where, so the difference between these 2 sounds must be
meaningful, so phonemic. By similar logic, the differences between [a] and [u], [a] and [e], and
[a] and [o] must also be phonemic; make sure you understand why.
Okay, so [a] is a phonemically unique vowel. What about the rest? Well, [i] and [u] occur in lots
of the same contexts: after [t], [k], and # (word edge), and before [g], [v], [s], [m], [n], [l]. Since
you can’t ever predict the occurrence of [i] vs. [u] – since [i] and [u] aren’t in perfectly
complementary distribution – they must also be meaningfully different.
The difference between [e] and [o] is meaningful, too – both show up after [n], and before
exactly the same set of sounds – [q] and [Ʀ], and at the ends of words. These contexts are, finally,
predictably different from those where [i] and [u] show up: [e] and [o] occur only before [q],
[Ʀ], and #, while [i] and [u] never occur in these contexts. This suggests that [e]/[o] are
allophones of [i]/[u], or vice versa.
2
Intro to Linguistics
Autumn 2009
Now we have to sort out the complicated relationship between these four sounds. In the diagram
below, solid lines represent meaningful differences, while dashed lines represent predictable
differences. It looks at this point like there are two phonemic vowels here, and the other two are
predictable allophones of these variants. But which are the underlying representations, [i]-[u] or
[e]-[o]? And are the allophonic pairs [i]-[e] and [u]-[o] or [i]-[o] and [u]-[e]?
[i]
[u]
[e]
[o]
The underlying representations are [i] and [u]. These high vowels show up in lots and lots of
contexts, while [e] and [o] show up in extremely specific contexts: before uvulars and wordfinally. Rule (a) below is much simpler than (b); it’s much easier to imagine [i]-[u] becoming
[e]-[o] in these few contexts than [e]-[o] almost always changing their forms.
(a) The real rule (almost):
[i] and [u] become [e] and [o] when they occur before uvulars or
word-finally.
(b) Too complex to be the right rule: [e] and [o] become [i] and [u] when they occur before
labials, labiodentals, alveolars, velars, and vowels.
Finally, does the underlying representation [i] become the allophone [e] or [o]? Well, think about
the features:
[i]: high, front, unrounded
[e]: mid, front, unrounded
[u]: high, back, unrounded
[o]: mid, back, unrounded
Again towards the goal of writing a simple rule (as simple rules are most often phonologically
correct), if [i] changes to [e] before uvulars and word-finally, it only needs to change its height
feature; if [i]  [o], it needs to change both height and backness. So the real, complete rule looks
like this:
The whole real rule: [i] becomes [e], and [u] becomes [o], when they occur before uvulars or
word-finally.
3
Download