SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL FOR Fractionation of Spatial Memory in GRM2/3 (mGlu2/mGlu3) Double Knockout Mice Reveals a Role for Group II mGluRs at the Interface between Arousal and Cognition Louisa Lyon, Philip WJ Burnet, James NC Kew, Corrado Corti, J. Nicholas P Rawlins, Tracy Lane, Bianca De Filippis, Paul J Harrison, David M Bannerman SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL Subjects Age-matched (> 2.5 months old), male, wild-type and GRM2/3-/- mice were obtained from GlaxoSmithKline, Harlow, UK. GRM2-/- mice (Yokoi et al, 1996) were crossed with GRM3-/- mice (Corti et al, 2007a) to generate GRM2+/-GRM3+/- double heterozygous offspring. GRM2-/- mice had been backcrossed onto the C57Bl/6 line for 21 generations, and GRM3-/- mice backcrossed onto C57Bl/6 for 11 generations. The double heterozygous animals were thus considered to be N11. Double heterozygous mice were then crossed to generate 1:16 GRM2-/-GRM3-/- double knockout (‘GRM2/3-/-‘ mice, 1:16 GRM2+/+GRM3+/+ wild-type (wt) mice, and 14:16 mice that were heterozygous for GRM2 and/ or GRM3. To avoid the prohibitive wastage of animals that would occur if all double knockout and wild-type mice were derived from double heterozygous crosses, separate lines of true breeding double knockout and wild-type mice were established. Separate wild-type and knockout lines were bred for up to 6 generations. The knockout line was then out-crossed onto C57Bl/6 in order to generate mice that were heterozygous at both loci and these mice were then inter-crossed to produce new wild-type and double knockout lines, and thus refresh the colony. The animals used in these behavioural experiments were obtained from between the F2 - F6 generations of these separate lines (for details, see below). Animals were housed in groups of 2-4 and kept on a 12-hour light-dark cycle (lights on at 07:00 and off at 19:00), with all testing conducted during the light phase. For all appetitively motivated tasks, mice were maintained on a restricted feeding schedule at 1 not less than 90% of their free-feeding weight. For several days prior to the start of each appetitive test, mice were habituated to the maze and to drinking the sweetened condensed milk (Carnation) (diluted 50:50 with water) that was used as a reward. Habituation was conducted in a room other than the experimental test room. For all aversively motivated tasks, mice were given ad libitum access to food and water. Order of testing Four separate cohorts of experimentally naïve mice were used. The first cohort (N15;F2 generation) completed the 2 hr test of spontaneous locomotor activity, followed by experiments 1-8. The order of testing for the cognitive tasks was as follows: appetitive spatial reference memory (SRM) Y-maze (Expt 1), appetitive spatial working memory (SWM) T-maze (Expt 2), appetitive visual discrimination (Expt 6), watermaze (Expt 4), appetitive six-arm radial maze (Expt 3), swimming Y-maze (Expt 5), spontaneous spatial novelty preference (Expt 7). The second cohort (N15;F5 generation) was used in Experiment 5, to replicate the SRM swimming Y-maze and appetitive Y-maze with the order of testing and the room in which testing was performed counterbalanced with respect to cohort 1. Cohort 2 also completed experiment 9 (effects of injection stress on appetitive T-maze SWM performance) and experiment 8 (amphetamine challenge – low dose, 2.5 mg/kg). The third cohort (N16;F2 generation) was used to study the effects of a high dose of amphetamine (10 mg/kg) on locomotor activity. Cohort 4 (N16;F6 generation) was used for the assessment of locomotor activity during the diurnal cycle over 70 hr (Expt 8), followed by the replication of the T-maze spatial working memory task with recording of latencies for sample and choice runs (Expt 2). All experiments were conducted under the auspices of the UK Home Office project and personal licences held by the authors. Supplementary Methods Genotyping The absence of GRM2 mRNA in founding members of the GRM2-/- population was confirmed by the use of in situ hybridisation histochemistry (Yokoi et al., 1996), as was the absence of GRM3 mRNA in the founding members of the GRM3-/- population 2 (Corti et al., 2007a). Genotypes of all animals were confirmed by the “Mouse genotyping group” at GSK, Harlow, using separate PCRs for GRM2 and GRM3 mRNA. GRM2 fragments were amplified using forward oligonucleotide primers (CTG TCT CTC TAT CTC TCT GC) and reverse primers (TGT GTG TGT GTA ACA TGA TGG). PCRs were performed with a denaturing step at 95 °C (15 mins) then 94 °C (30 s), followed by annealing at 60 °C (90 s) and extension at 72 °C (1 min). After 35 cycles, the reaction was maintained at 72 °C for a further 10 mins. PCR product was then resolved onto a 2% agarose gel. The wildtype product was a single 900 bp band, and the ko product a 450 bp band. GRM3 genotyping yielded a wildtype product that was 2 kbp long, and a knockout product that was 500 bp. The large disparity in size prevented the two fragments from being amplified in a single multiplex PCR. Two separate PCRs were therefore conducted, one for the wildtype product (forward primer: GTT TCT AGG ACT TCC TAT GG; reverse primer: AAC GAT GCT CTG ACA AAC TCC) and a second for the knockout product (forward primer: CGT ACG TCG GTT GCT ATG G; reverse primer: GTC AGA TAT AGT GAG AGC AGG). Both PCRs were performed with a denaturing step at 95 °C (15 mins) then 94 °C (30 s), followed by annealing at 56 °C (90 s) and extension at 72 °C (150 s). After 35 cycles, the reaction was maintained at 72 °C for 10 mins. PCR product was resolved onto a 2% agarose gel. Spatial memory tests Hippocampus-dependent, spatial learning was assessed using a battery of appetitively and aversively motivated spatial reference memory (SRM) and spatial working memory (SWM) tasks. SRM tasks are those in which the correct spatial response remains constant from trial to trial. SWM tasks, by contrast, are characterised by their flexible stimulus response requirements with different spatial responses being variably correct and incorrect. Experiment 1: SRM on the elevated Y-maze The elevated Y-maze consisted of three identical wooden arms, each 50 cm long by 9 cm wide, with a low wall (0.5 cm), connected by a central polygonal platform (14 3 cm diameter). A food well was positioned at the end of each arm. Each mouse was assigned a goal arm, defined by its position relative to extramaze spatial cues, which was baited with 0.1 ml sweetened condensed milk on all trials. On each trial, the mouse was placed at the end of one of the two non-baited arms (the “start arm”), facing the experimenter; 50% of trials began from the arm to the right of the goal arm, and 50% from the arm to the left. Neither arm was used as the start arm for more than three consecutive trials. Allocation of start and goal arms was counterbalanced across groups. Having been placed at the end of the start arm, the mouse was allowed to choose one of the remaining arms. If it chose the goal arm, it was allowed to consume the milk reward before being returned to the home cage. Mice that chose incorrectly were returned to the home cage immediately. Previous work in this laboratory, using the same maze in the same room with the same spatial cues, has demonstrated that this task is hippocampusdependent (Deacon et al., 2002). To prevent the use of intra-maze cues, the entire maze was rotated periodically (approximately every 5 trials). Mice received ten trials per day for nine days, with an inter-trial interval (ITI) of approximately five minutes. The last block of ten trials was conducted using post-choice reinforcement: the condensed milk reward was added to the food well only after the mouse had made a choice, to ensure that the animals were not locating the milk by virtue of its odor. Experiment 2: SWM on the elevated T-maze The T-maze consisted of a wooden start arm (47 x 10 cm) and two identical goal arms (35 x 10 cm), surrounded by a 10 cm high wall. A food well was positioned 3 cm from the end of each goal arm, and the whole maze was surrounded by prominent distal extramaze cues. Mice received five trials per day for ten days, with an ITI of approximately ten minutes. Each trial consisted of a sample run followed by a choice run. On the sample run, mice were forced either left or right (chosen pseudorandomly with equal numbers of left and right turns, and no more than three consecutive turns in any direction) by the presence of a large wooden block, closing off one of the goal arms. At the end of the goal arm the mouse collected a reward of 0.1 ml sweetened condensed milk. The block was then removed and the mouse placed back in the start arm, facing the experimenter, for the choice run. The mouse could now select either goal arm but was 4 rewarded only for choosing the arm that had not been visited on the sample run, i.e., it was rewarded for alternating (non-matching to place). The interval between the sample run and the choice run was approximately 5 seconds. SWM performance on the T-maze is also dependent on the hippocampus (Deacon et al., 2002). In a separate cohort of mice we replicated this study but, in addition, we recorded latencies for both the sample runs and the choice runs. We recorded the latency of the mice to run (i) from the beginning of the start arm to the food well on the sample trial, and (ii) from the beginning of the start arm until making a choice into one of the goal arms on the choice trial. Mice received five trials per day for twelve days, with an ITI of approximately ten minutes. Experiment 3: SRM and SWM in the six-arm radial maze The radial maze consisted of six arms (60 cm x 7 cm) radiating out from a central platform. Each arm was surrounded by a 1 cm raised edge and contained a food well located at the end. The central platform was surrounded by a transparent Perspex cylinder (18 cm diameter, 30 cm high). At the entrance to each arm was a Perspex door (6 cm wide, 7 cm high), which could be controlled manually by the experimenter using a series of strings. The maze was positioned 80 cm above the floor, and was surrounded by prominent distal extramaze cues. The radial arm maze can be used to assess SRM and SWM separately using a two-stage, within-subjects and within-task design (Schmitt et al., 2003). SRM acquisition: in the initial acquisition phase, three of the six arms were baited with 0.1 ml sweetened condensed milk. The three baited arms were allocated such that two were adjacent, and the third was between two unbaited arms. Combinations of baited arms were counterbalanced across all mice with respect to genotype. At the start of each trial, the mouse was placed on the central platform and given a free choice of arms. After visiting the food well in the chosen arm and consuming the milk reward/ discovering that the arm was not baited, the mouse would return to the central platform. The door to the visited arm was then closed and remained so for all subsequent choices on that trial, preventing the mouse from re-entering that arm (see Schmitt et al., 2003). After 10 seconds, all other doors were opened and the mouse allowed a second choice. This was 5 repeated, with five doors open for the second choice, four for the third, three for the fourth, and so on, until the mouse had visited all three baited arms. Each entry into an unbaited arm was scored as a SRM error (maximum number of errors = 3 per trial). SRM acquisition on the radial maze is prevented by cytotoxic hippocampal lesions in mice (Schmitt et al., 2003). Simultaneous assessment of SRM and SWM: When all mice had successfully acquired the SRM task, the SWM component was introduced. Entries into previously visited arms were now no longer prevented but the food rewards were not replaced within a trial. The doors were now used only to retain the mouse on the central platform for 10 seconds in between choices. Three types of error were scored (Jarrard, 1993), including spatial reference memory (SRM) errors in which the mouse visited a never-baited arm, and spatial working memory (SWM) errors in which the mouse visited a baited arm that had already been visited on that trial. If second and subsequent visits to a never-baited arm occurred, these were scored separately as spatial reference memory repeat (SRM-R) errors, because these might be regarded as involving failure in both SRM and SWM. In practice, there were very few SRM-R errors made by either group. Experiment 4: SRM in the watermaze Mice received no swim pretraining prior to this task. Testing was conducted in a large circular tank (diameter 2.0 m, depth 0.6 m) containing water at 20 + 1°C to a depth of 0.3 m. To escape from the water, mice had to locate a platform (diameter 21 cm, covered in wire mesh) hidden approximately 1 cm below the surface. The water was made opaque by the addition of 2 litres of whole milk to prevent the mice from seeing the platform. The pool was surrounded by prominent distal extramaze cues that could be used as landmarks (shelves, table, posters on walls, etc). Swim paths were tracked by a video camera mounted in the ceiling, and relayed to a computer for image analysis using specialised software (HVS Image Analyse, Hampton, UK; Archimedes hardware). Mice were assigned to one of four platform positions (NE, NW, SE, SW), with the platform located in the centre of the quadrant. Platform assignation was counterbalanced according to genotype. For each individual mouse, however, the platform remained in the same position across all trials. Each animal was given four trials per day (1 block) for nine 6 days. Mice were placed into the pool facing the sidewall at one of eight starting positions (N, S, E, W, NW, NE, SW, SE) that varied pseudorandomly across trials. On each trial, the mouse was allowed to swim in the pool until it found the platform, or for a maximum of 90 seconds. Mice that failed to find the platform after this time were placed there by the experimenter. All mice remained on the platform for 30 seconds before beginning the next trial. On day 7 (24 hours after training trial 24) and day 11 (24 hours after training trial 36), transfer (probe) tests were conducted. The platform was removed from the pool and the mouse allowed to swim freely for 60 seconds. The percentage of time spent in each quadrant was recorded, together with the number of times the mouse swam across the former location of the platform. We have previously shown using this watermaze, in this laboratory, with the same spatial cues, that acquisition of this fixed location, hidden platform task is prevented by cytotoxic hippocampal lesions (Deacon et al., 2002). Experiment 5: Aversively motivated spatial reference memory in the Y-maze The Y-maze was made from transparent Perspex, and consisted of three 30 cm long, 8 cm wide arms with 20 cm high walls, connected by a central junction. The maze was filled with water (temperature 21 ºC ± 1 ºC) to a depth of approximately 12 cm which obliged the mice to swim. Mice could escape from the water by climbing onto a platform (8 cm by 8 cm) hidden approximately 1.5 cm below the water surface. Milk was added to the water to prevent the mice from seeing the platform. Mice received five trials per day in this deep water escape Y-maze for six days. On each trial the mouse was allowed 90 seconds to find the platform; any that failed to do so were guided there by the experimenter. Mice were allowed to rest on the platform for 30 seconds before being transferred to a heated cage. On day seven (24 hours after training trial 30), a transfer test was performed, analogous to that used in the watermaze, in order to assess the extent of any spatial memory for the platform location. The platform was removed from the maze and the mouse allowed to swim freely for 30 seconds. Time spent searching in each arm was recorded. Experiment 6: appetitively motivated visual discrimination 7 This experiment compared visual discrimination learning for a food reward. The T-shaped maze consisted of three arms (30 x 10 x 29 cm): a light gray start arm plus two removable goal arms. The walls and floor of one of the goal arms were painted with black and white stripes, while those of the other goal arm were plain dark gray. Food wells were positioned 3 cm from the end of each goal arm. Mice received ten trials per day for five days. Each mouse was assigned a particular goal arm (“black/ white stripes” or “gray”), which was baited with 0.1 ml sweetened condensed milk on all trials. Allocation of goal arms was counterbalanced with respect to genotype. On 50% of trials, the rewarded goal arm was positioned to the right of the start arm, and on the remaining 50% it was positioned to the left. The rewarded goal arm did not occupy the same physical location for more than three consecutive trials. On each trial, the mouse was placed at the end of the start arm facing the experimenter and allowed to choose one of the goal arms. If the mouse chose the correct arm (defined by its visual appearance), it was allowed to consume the milk reward before being returned to the home cage. Mice that chose incorrectly were returned to the home cage immediately without reward. Postchoice reinforcement was used in block 5. Experiment 7: spontaneous spatial novelty preference task GRM2/3-/- and wild-type mice were also compared on a spontaneous, spatial novelty preference task in which behavior is driven, not by an overt unconditioned stimulus (US; e.g. a food reward), but instead relies upon animals’ natural exploratory drive. This task therefore provides a non-aversive experimental context but performance does not rely on the motivating or rewarding effects of food. The apparatus used was identical to the paddling and swimming Y-mazes, but without the water. Instead, a thin layer of sawdust covered the floor of the maze. Each mouse was assigned two arms (the “start arm” and the “other arm”) to which they were exposed during the first phase of the task (the “exposure phase”). Allocation of arms to specific spatial locations was counterbalanced within each genotype. During the 5-minute “exposure” phase, the entrance to the third, “novel”, arm was closed off by the presence of a large Perspex block. The mouse was placed at the end of the start arm, facing the experimenter, and allowed to explore the start arm and the other arm freely for five 8 minutes, beginning as soon as the mouse left the start arm. The number of entries into each arm and the length of time spent there were recorded. At the end of the five minutes, the mouse was removed from the maze and returned to the home cage for one minute. During this time, the Perspex block closing off the novel arm was removed and the sawdust redistributed throughout the maze to minimise the use of odor cues. The mouse was then returned immediately to the start arm, facing the experimenter, for the 2-minute test phase. This consisted of two minutes free exploration during which the mouse could enter all 3 arms, beginning as soon as the mouse left the start arm. The amount of time that the mouse spent in each arm, and the number of entries into each arm, were recorded, during both the exposure and the test phase. For the test phase, a discrimination ratio [(novel arm/ (novel + other arm)] was calculated both for number of arm entries and time spent in each arm. Previous work in this laboratory has demonstrated that wild-type mice display a marked preference for the novel arm during the test phase, and that this preference relies on extramaze cues, whereas preference for the novel arm is abolished in mice with cytotoxic hippocampal lesions (Sanderson et al., 2007). Experiment 8: spontaneous and amphetamine-induced locomotor activity Spontaneous locomotor activity was measured during a two-hour period in the light phase (12pm – 2pm). All mice were placed singly into a transparent plastic cage (26 cm x 16 cm x 17 cm) with a ventilated lid. Two infrared photocell beams crossed the cage 1.5 cm above the floor, with each beam 7 cm from the centre of the cage. Mice were left in a quiet room with the lights on for 2 hours. The number of beam breaks made by each mouse was recorded in 24 bins of 5 minutes. A separate cohort of mice were then tested in a threshold activity monitoring system for spontaneous locomotor activity over a 70 hr period (1 hr bins). Threshold pressure pads were used to measure the activity of mice across the diurnal cycle. The Threshold system (Version 3, Med Associates) converts changes in pressure on pads into changes in voltage. Dedicated software records these changes in voltage and allows threshold values to be set to give sensitive readings of locomotor activity. Mice were 9 placed in cages of 22.5x12.5x13cm at approximately 3 p.m. (4 hours before lights off), and left with ad libitum food and water (Supplementary Material, Figure S2). A further, separate cohort of drug-naïve mice, with no previous experience of the photocell activity boxes, was used for the amphetamine challenge experiments. Mice were habituated to the activity boxes for 20 minutes prior to receiving an i.p. injection of either saline or 2.5 mg/kg amphetamine (injection volume 10 ml/kg body weight). A latin square within-subjects design was used, in which half of the animals received an injection of amphetamine followed 3 days later by an injection of saline, and vice versa for the other half. Mice were left undisturbed and their activity levels (number of beam breaks) measured for 2 hours. Higher doses of amphetamine are known to induce stereotypy, which reduces locomotor activity. An additional cohort of drug-naïve mice was habituated to the activity boxes for 20 minutes prior to receiving an i.p. injection of either saline or 10 mg/kg amphetamine (10 ml/kg body weight), using a between-subjects design. Mice were returned immediately to the activity boxes and their activity levels measured for 7 hours. Every 10 minutes throughout the first 4 hours, stereotypy was scored for each animal by an experimenter, using an established rating scale (Creese & Iversen, 1974) (Supplementary Material, Figure S3). 10 Supplementary Figure Legends Fig. S1. GRM2/3-/- mice did not differ from wild-type mice in the watermaze SRM task. A, Mean number of annulus crossings (i.e., swims across the former location of the platform, or the equivalent location in the other quadrants) (± sem) in probe test 1 (day 7). B, Mean number of annulus crossings (± sem) in probe test 2 (day 11). Fig. S2. GRM2/3-/- mice are hypoactive across an extended testing period. Mean locomotor activity (arbitrary units) in wild-type and GRM2/3-/- mice across a 70 hr test session. Error bars are not shown for clarity. There was a significant group by time block interaction (F(69, 2553) = 2.09; p < 0.0001), and analysis of simple main effects revealed that the activity levels of the groups differed significantly in blocks 2,3,4,9,11,12,32,33,34,36,38,56,58 and 62 (all p < 0.05). In all but block 36 the wildtypes were more active than the GRM2/3-/- mice. Fig. S3. 10 mg/kg amphetamine induced stereotypy in both wt (n = 5) and GRM2/3 dko (n = 6) mice, relative to saline-injected controls (n = 6 wt; n = 6 dko). Stereotypy began to decrease in amphetamine-treated GRM2/3 dko mice approximately 140 minutes postinjection, and in wt mice approximately 190 minutes post-injection. Data shown are median stereotypy score as rated by an experimenter using an established scale (maximum = 5) (± IQR). 11 Supplementary References Corti C, Battaglia G, Molinaro G, Riozzi B, Pittaluga A, Corsi M, et al (2007a). The use of knock-out mice unravels distinct roles for mGlu2 and mGlu3 metabotropic glutamate receptors in mechanisms of neurodegeneration/neuroprotection. J Neurosci 27: 82978308. Yokoi M, Kobayashi K, Manabe T, Takahashi T, Sakaguchi I, Katsuura G, et al (1996). Impairment of hippocampal mossy fiber LTD in mice lacking mGluR2. Science 273: 645-647. Other references cited in Supplementary Materials can be found in the main reference list. 12