Individual variability of wild juvenile Atlantic salmon activity patterns: effect of flow stage, temperature and habitat use Mathieu L. Roy1, André G. Roy1, James W. A. Grant2, Normand E. Bergeron3 1 Département de géographie, Université de Montréal, 520 Côte Ste-Catherine, Montréal, QC, H2B 2V8, Canada. 2 Department of Biology, Concordia University, 7141 Sherbrooke St. West, Montréal, QC, H4B 1R6, Canada. 3 INRS-Eau, Terre et Environnement, 490 rue de la Couronne, Québec, QC, G1K 9A9, Canada. 1 Abstract 2 The magnitude of variation of diel activity patterns and habitat use of wild 3 Atlantic salmon parr was examined during the summer and autumn through a gradient of 4 declining temperature. Fish were marked with passive integrated transponders and 5 tracked using a large network of flat-bed antennas. High inter-individual variability was 6 observed, as some individuals were predominantly nocturnal whereas others frequently 7 changed their daily activity pattern. Overall fish activity decreased with decreasing 8 temperature and increasing flow stage, but most of these changes in daily activity 9 occurred during crepuscular periods. Parr used habitats with lower velocity at night than 10 in the day during the summer, but not in the autumn. Furthermore, there was no 11 difference between day and night habitats for fish that were cathemeral (active both day 12 and night during a given day), so differences between day and night habitats were the 13 result of individuals adopting different activity patterns. These results suggest that habitat 14 interacts with activity pattern, as individuals using suboptimal habitats seem to increase 15 daytime foraging to secure sufficient energy. Temporal and among fish variability of 16 activity patterns illustrate the dynamic nature of foraging decisions that may partly result 17 from tradeoffs experienced at the microhabitat scale. 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 Introduction Juvenile salmonids exhibit complex and variable diel activity patterns (Reebs 26 2002). During the summer, juveniles are active and feed during the day, but as 27 temperature drops to 8-12oC in the autumn and winter, they tend to become nocturnal and 28 shelter during the day (Rimmer et al. 1983; Cunjak 1988; Bremset, 2000). This switch to 29 nocturnal activity at low temperature may be explained by reduced food requirements and 30 higher predation risk. However, between-population differences in seasonal activity 31 patterns have been observed, including nocturnal summer activity (Gries et al. 1997; 32 Johnston et al. 2004), and diurnal winter activity (Hiscock et al. 2002). Such variability 33 among populations may be related to differences in overall food availability, or its 34 differential abundance during the day and night (Metcalfe et al. 1999; Orpwood et al. 35 2006), and in perceived predation risk (Orpwood et al. 2010). Variation in predation risk 36 and food availability associated with flow stage can also cause differences in diel activity 37 patterns at the reach level (Bradford and Higgins 2001). 38 Previous field studies have shown variation in activity patterns between groups of 39 fish but also reported that behaviour was not uniform among all individuals in a group 40 (e.g. Bremset 2000; Bradford and Higgins 2001). This variability may result from 41 individual variation in the relative benefits of rapid growth for fish facing a complex 42 trade-off between growth and mortality risk (Metcalfe et al. 1999). The level of risk 43 tolerated by individuals varies with their recent foraging success, but also on their state 44 along their growth trajectory (i.e. above average fish taking less risk) (Imre and Boisclair 45 2004). Combined with dominance and hierarchy constraints, the state of a fish may 46 explain why different individuals react differently to environmental fluctuations (e.g. 47 photoperiod and temperature) (Breau et al. 2007). Along the same lines, microhabitat use 48 may have an important impact on the foraging behaviour (Nielsen, 1992), but also on the 49 type of activity pattern adopted by an individual fish. For instance, a fish constrained to 50 use suboptimal habitat may have to forage for longer periods during the day and night to 51 meet its energy demands. Conversely, activity patterns may induce diel variation in 52 habitat use, as salmonids tend to use different habitats for feeding and sheltering 53 (Heggenes et al. 1999). For instance, at night young salmon generally use lower velocity 54 habitats with shallower depths and coarser substrates, perhaps to offset a decrease in 55 foraging efficiency (Heggenes et al. 1993; Metcalfe et al. 1997; Valdimarsson et al. 1997; 56 Mitchell et al. 1998). 57 Juvenile salmonid activity has been a topic of great interest. Numerous controlled 58 experiments have revealed the effects of particular variables on activity patterns, 59 presumably through an indirect effect on the growth vs mortality risk tradeoff (e.g. 60 Metcalfe et al. 1999). Concomitantly, field observation studies have described the 61 seasonal trends in activity patterns of populations or groups of fish (Bremset, 2000; 62 Johnston et al. 2004) as well as their habitat use (Heggenes et al. 1999; Armstrong et al. 63 2003). Previous studies have described general patterns of activity and reported 64 differences in habitat use between day and night (e.g. Metcalfe et al. 1997). However, the 65 previous field research has focused on unmarked individuals. Therefore, it is uncertain if 66 habitat use patterns are performed by most individuals within a given day or if they 67 emerge as an average of individuals with dramatically different daily patterns. 68 Furthermore, to our knowledge, a description of the temporal variation of individual 69 activity patterns and its potential relation with microhabitat is lacking. 70 In this study, we monitored the habitat use and activity of 66 Atlantic salmon parr 71 marked with passive integrated transponders (PIT) using a reach scale antenna grid in a 72 natural environment providing both high temporal and spatial resolution. The main goal 73 was to quantify the magnitude of variation in activity patterns, habitat use and their 74 interaction and to identify if individuals were adopting different activity patterns on a 75 daily and seasonal scale. The study was carried out during the summer and autumn 76 through a gradient of declining temperature ranging from 18 to 3oC. Specifically, we 77 tested the predictions that: 1) parr are predominantly nocturnal during both the summer 78 and the autumn; 2) individual activity patterns (i.e. nocturnal, diurnal, cathemeral - day 79 and night active, or crepuscular) vary on a short term basis, such that individuals are more 80 nocturnal in higher flows and in colder temperatures; 3) parr habitat use interacts with 81 activity patterns – fish use lower velocity habitat at night because their reaction distance 82 is reduced, whether they are nocturnal or cathemeral. 83 84 Materials and methods 85 Study site 86 This study was undertaken on Xavier Brook, a tributary of the Ste-Marguerite 87 River in Saguenay, Québec, Canada (48°2591799 N; 69°5394899 W). The stream was 88 10-15 m wide and flowed in a forested watershed. Data were collected in a channel 89 characterized by two pools separated by a steep riffle, providing high habitat diversity 90 (Fig. 1). In the thalweg at low flow (0.4 m3/s), depth ranged from approximately 0.1 m in 91 the riffle to 1.65 m in the upstream pool. Median substrate size (B-axis, i.e. particle 92 width) varied from cobble-boulders in the riffle, to gravel-cobble (classification, after 93 Wolman 1954) in the deep portion of the pools, and gravel-sand in the pool recirculation 94 zones. 95 Fish tracking system 96 A large flatbed antenna grid was used to monitor fish locations during 97 days, from 24 97 July to 1 November 2008. The tracking system consisted of an array of 149 circular 98 antennas of 50 cm in diameter. Each antenna, designed to detect the presence of PIT tags 99 (Texas Instruments (TIRIS) model RI-TRP-RRHP, 134 2 kHz, length: 23.1 mm, 100 diameter: 3.9 mm, weight in air: 0.6 g), was buried in the river bed and covered with 101 substratum. The antennas were distributed systematically along transects of five antennas 102 each. Overall, the detection field of the antenna grid covered 19% of the wetted area of 103 the site at a discharge of 0.4 m3 s-1. Each group of five antennas was linked to a tuning 104 capacitor, which was wired to a CYTEK multiplexer (JX/256 series, mercury wetted 256 105 single poles relay, www.cytec-ate.com). The multiplexer was connected to an Aquartis 106 controller (custom made by Technologie Aquartis; www.aquartis.ca) composed of a 107 TIRIS S-2000 reader, a datalogger and a custom-made controller unit. The system was 108 powered by three solar panels connected to four 6V batteries plugged in series and two 109 12V batteries plugged in parallel. Each antenna was activated every 34 s successively for 110 the detection of PIT tag presence. When a PIT tagged fish was detected, the date 111 (dd/mm/yy), time (hh/mm/ss), antenna ID (multiplexer card and port number) and fish ID 112 (tag number) were recorded. Detection range varied from 300-400 mm in height and 600- 113 800 mm in diameter. 114 115 During the study period, all antennas detected at least one individual. The failure to distinguish two fish located in the same antenna field is a limitation of PIT tracking 116 systems (Armstrong et al. 1996; Linnansaari et al. 2007). Tag collision (i.e. multiple tags 117 blocking the detection of each other) can especially be a concern at high fish density. 118 However, the high density of antenna of this tracking system reduced the probability of 119 having several fish simultaneously in the same antenna field. When fish are moving, they 120 are likely to be detected sequentially by the same antenna or by adjacent antennas. 121 Nevertheless, the presence of a sheltering fish might prevent other fish from being 122 detected by a specific antenna for a period of time. Although this is a potential issue for 123 consideration, previous frequent snorkeling and portable antenna surveys in the summer 124 and autumn of 2007 never found multiple fish on the same antenna. Furthermore, errors 125 in tag reading of this tracking system for juvenile Atlantic salmon were 0.07% of the data 126 recorded at a similar tagged fish density (Johnston et al. 2009). For more detailed 127 information on the antenna grid, see Johnston et al. (2009). 128 129 Fish tagging 130 A total of 69 Atlantic salmon parr (1+) were captured in the study reach on two 131 occasions using a backpack electrofishing device: 44 fish were caught on 24 July 2008, 132 and 25 on 28 August 2008. In the first session, fish were captured at the tracking system 133 location and up to 50 m upstream. For the second session, fish were caught up to 200 m 134 upstream from the reach to avoid recapturing tagged fish. Fish were brought from outside 135 the boundary of the tracking system to increase the number of study individuals, as the 136 study area had a low fish density in 2008. Juvenile Atlantic salmon of fork length < 80 137 mm were not tagged to avoid potential effects on growth rates (Sigourney et al. 2005; 138 Bateman and Gresswell, 2006) and survival (Roussel et al. 2000). Average and maximum 139 tag size to body weight ratio were 5.9% and 7.8%, which is lower than 8%, recommended 140 by Lacroix et al. (2004) to minimize effects on swimming performance.. Juvenile brook 141 trout (Salvelinus fontinalis) and longnose dace (Rhinichthys cataractae) were released 142 upon capture. The retained fish were anesthetised in a clove oil solution (3 ml/10 L) and 143 were surgically implanted with 23 mm PIT tags (Texas Instruments) in the abdominal 144 cavity secured with surgical tissue adhesive (Vetbond©). Tagged fish were kept for a 145 maximum recovery period of two hours in fish tanks within the river before being 146 released on the study site. Two and one individual perished during the first and second 147 tagging sessions, respectively. On each occasion, fish were measured (fork length, mean 148 ± SD, LA: 98 ± 7.4 mm, LB: 109 ± 8.3 mm) and weighed (MA= 9.7 ± 1.7 g, 10.7 ± 2.3 g). 149 150 151 Habitat survey A pressure transducer was used to monitor flow stage and water temperature 152 fluctuations every 15 min. Flow stage was estimated by subtracting atmospheric pressure 153 fluctuations from the signal using data obtained from the closest meteorological station. 154 Then, we subtracted the minimum value recorded during the study period, so flow stage 155 was defined as the water level above minimum summer low flow (Fig. 2). Considerable 156 precipitation was received during the summer and autumn 2008 (486 mm from July to 157 October). A high magnitude flow event occurred at the beginning of August, followed by 158 a stage decrease in the following month. A prolonged low-flow period lasted for 10 days 159 until the end of October, but was interrupted by a few precipitation events and remained 160 about 10 cm above minimum between these events, which corresponded approximately 161 to the median flow stage. 162 During the study period, water temperature decreased from 19.0 oC to 2.8 oC (Fig. 163 2). In the first part of the study period, water temperature remained relatively stable, with 164 slight fluctuations around 15 oC. Around 3 September, water temperature started 165 decreasing, reaching 12 oC on 11 September, which corresponds to the upper boundary 166 of the temperature range when juveniles suppress their daytime activity (Rimmer et al. 167 1983; Fraser et al. 1993). Therefore, this date was chosen to define the boundary between 168 the summer (12-18 oC) and the autumn (3-12 oC) periods. 169 The physical characteristics of the study reach were characterized in detail. 170 Micro-topography was surveyed using a robotic total station (Trimble 5600DR) by 171 combining a systematic sampling along transects 1m apart, with the characterization of 172 individual roughness elements that protruded 10 cm above the local mean bed elevation. 173 This strategy optimized sampling effort, as sampling point density increased 174 proportionally with bed complexity. From 6250 sample points, we created a digital 175 elevation model (DEM) using a triangular irregular network (TIN) interpolation with 176 pixel size of 10 cm. Depth was obtained by removing the general longitudinal slope of 177 the channel using linear regression, and then by subtracting the water level at average 178 flow (Q = 0.07 m3·s-1, stage: 10 cm) to all values of the bed elevation DEM. The general 179 slope being removed, the remaining differences in depth within the reach were attributed 180 to the channel shape, particularly to the steep pool riffle sequence. At median flow, the 181 maximum depth of 1.8 m was recorded in the upstream pool and the minimum depth in 182 the thalweg was 0.30 in the riffle (Fig. 1). 183 Flow velocity measurements were sampled extensively along the reach on three 184 occasions, close to median flow periods (stages of 13, 14 and 16 cm, Fig. 2). Three- 185 dimensional flow velocity measurements were sampled using four acoustic Doppler 186 velocimeters (ADVs, Sontek, San Diego) simultaneously at 10 cm above the bed at a 187 density of 3 samples.m-2. An aluminum frame was used as a support and reference grid 188 for ADV measurements. Each flow measurement was referenced using the total station 189 and mapped. At median flow, mean flow velocity was approximately 0-30 cm·s-1 in the 190 pool recirculation zones, 0.40-0.60 cm·s-1 in the pool tails and from 75-125 cm·s-1 in the 191 riffle. 192 At each antenna location, velocity and depth were averaged within the antenna 193 detection range (0.35 m radius). Although depth and velocity fluctuated during the study 194 period, in this study we focused on habitat use at close to base flow and removed fish 195 detections that were recorded during flood events (flow stage > 20 cm). We assumed that 196 a particular habitat would be ranked similarly in terms of depth and velocity across minor 197 flow stage fluctuations. Since flow velocities were measured at median flow stage, 198 reported habitat use values are slightly higher than previously reported values, generally 199 estimated at base flow. 200 201 202 Diel activity patterns First, the data set was explored for temporal trends in occurrences of detection of 203 each individual in different periods of the day. For each individual, for each hour, we 204 examined the periods during which fish were continuously detected every 34 s (tracking 205 frequency) without interruptions for more than 30 minutes and removed this hour for this 206 fish from the analysis, as it would be likely indicating that a fish was inactive. Large parr 207 are often mobile over short time scales (Ovidio et al. 2007) and have feeding territories 208 exceeding the detection range of the antennas (Armstrong et al. 1997). Hence, foraging 209 parr should be detected moving at least once in a 30 minute period. For all fish, a total of 210 175 h were removed this way over the entire study period, including 48 hours for a single 211 fish, and 127 h for 15 other individuals. Afterwards, for each day that a fish was detected 212 for at least 1h, each individual was assigned a “presence/absence” status for each hour. 213 When a fish was not detected during a specific day, no ‘presence/absence’ status was 214 recorded because we could not assess if the fish had emigrated from the reach or was 215 sheltering. To examine within-individual variability, we first selected the individuals that 216 were detected on the reach a minimum of five days in either the autumn or the summer 217 period or both. We then assumed that presence recordings reflect periods of fish activity, 218 as movements are likely to increase the frequency of fish detection by the system. 219 To examine the overall temporal patterns of fish activity, we estimated relative 220 frequency of detection for each hour of the day for each individual. For instance, if a fish 221 was detected during 20 days, but only five times between 5h00 and 6h00, its average 222 relative frequency of detection for 5h00 would be 25%. The frequency of detection for 223 each hour were square root transformed and used as datum in the analysis (n = 34 for 224 summer and n = 14 for autumn). Activity patterns of fish captured during the first and 225 second capture sessions were compared for a period during which a maximum of fish 226 from the two capture sessions were simultaneously tracked (25 Aug. – 25 Sep.). Repeated 227 measures general linear model with hour as a within-individual factor and capture 228 session as a between individual factor showed no significant difference in activity 229 patterns between the two groups of fish for that period (F=1.82, df=1,23, P=0.191). 230 Therefore, individuals from the two capture sessions were combined in further analyses. 231 To test for the presence of an overall diel activity pattern and for a seasonal interaction, 232 we used a repeated measures general linear model using hour as a within-individual 233 factor and season, mass and fork length as between-individual factors or covariates. Only 234 hour and season remained in the final model, as the other variables did not have 235 significant effects (i.e. p>0.05). 236 To examine activity patterns of each individual, we used logistic regression with 237 hour as the explanatory variable and average relative frequency of fish detection as a 238 response variable. Quadratic terms were added to the regression, as the variable hour was 239 ordered from 1 to 24; the hours of darkness occurred in two non-contiguous portions. 240 Then, based on the circadian cycle and on visualization of activity patterns, periods of 241 24h were divided into four daily periods: night (0100-0300 and 2200-2400), dawn (0300- 242 0700), day (0700-1800) and dusk (1800-2200). To present the within-individual variation 243 in activity pattern for each individual, for each day, each fish was categorized as 1) 244 nocturnal: active during night and possibly dawn and dusk, 2) diurnal: active during day 245 and possibly dusk and dawn, 3) cathemeral: active both night and day and possibly dusk 246 and dawn 4) crepuscular: active only during dusk and dawn periods. 247 248 Activity patterns vs. flow and temperature 249 Next, we investigated the temporal variation and the effect of flow stage and 250 temperature on daily activity (i.e. daily frequency of fish detection), diurnal activity, 251 nocturnal activity and crepuscular activity. The range of values for daily averaged 252 temperature and flow stage were divided into nine and seven discrete categories, 253 respectively. We estimated the percentage of time an individual was active at a particular 254 daily period at a particular temperature and flow level. We used four distinct mixed- 255 effects models to test for the effects of flow and temperature as fixed factors on fish 256 activity (daily, diurnal, nocturnal and crepuscular). Comparison of different fixed effect 257 structures was done using AIC values (Burnham et al. 2011). 258 259 260 Habitat use diel patterns For each hour of each day an individual was considered active, an average depth 261 and velocity values were estimated. To test for the presence of an overall habitat use 262 effect and a seasonal interaction effect, we used a general linear model with repeated 263 measures using daily period (i.e. night, day, crepuscular) as a within-individual factor and 264 season as a factor . Then, to examine and visualize the individual daily habitat use pattern 265 of each fish, for each individual for each season, quadratic regressions were carried out 266 using hour as an explanatory variable and average habitat use for that hour as the 267 response variable. 268 269 Diel habitat use vs. diel activity patterns 270 For each of the habitat variables, four distinct mixed-effects models were 271 performed using fish as subjects, days as repeated measures and diel activity pattern and 272 season as fixed factors. Hence, we compared 1) day and night habitat use within days 273 when individuals were cathemeral, 2) night habitat use when individuals were nocturnal 274 to night habitat use when individuals were cathemeral, 3) day habitat use when 275 individuals were diurnal to when individuals were cathemeral. All analyses were carried 276 out using Matlab R2010a (The Mathworks Inc., Natick, Massachussets) and SPSS 17.0 277 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, Illinois). 278 279 Results 280 Fish recordings 281 In this study, a total of 66 fish were captured, PIT-tagged and released in the 282 reach. From the total, four individuals (6%) were never detected by the tracking system 283 and 12 individuals (18%) were detected in the study reach for less than 24 hours, as they 284 most likely left the study site. Ten individuals (15%) were detected on site for a single 285 day, 13 individuals (24%) stayed in the reach between one and four days, and 27 286 individuals remained in the reach between 5 and 70 days (37%). As we were interested in 287 individual variability, only fish present for five days or more were investigated herein. Of 288 those fish, 13 and two individuals were only detected during the summer and autumn 289 periods, respectively, and 12 were detected during both periods. 290 During the study period, each antenna of the reach detected between 2 and 20 291 different individuals during the study period, with an average of 7.5 individuals. The 292 upstream shallow recirculating zone was used by the fewest individuals, followed by the 293 downstream part of the riffle (Fig. 3). The transition between the upstream pool and the 294 riffle, and the entire downstream pool were visited by the highest number of tagged 295 individuals. The highest frequency of fish detections was recorded in the upstream pool 296 tail, the transition between the upstream pool and the riffle and in the margins of the 297 downstream pool. Relatively few fish were detected in riffle habitats, the upstream 298 recirculation zone and the deep pool. 299 300 301 Diel and seasonal activity pattern In both summer and autumn, the frequency of fish detection exhibited a clear 302 daily pattern, with fish activity peaking at dawn and dusk, and being higher at night than 303 during the day (Two-way repeated measures GLM (hours); F = 11.854, df = 23, 851, 304 p < 0.001) (Fig. 4a). During the summer (stars), frequency of fish detection peaked at 305 approximately 0.45 between 0500 and 0600 and between 2000 to 2100, which 306 corresponded with sunrise and sunset. Average frequency of fish detection was 0.25 307 during the night and decreased to 0.15 to 0.20 during the day. A similar pattern was 308 observed during the autumn period (Fig. 4a, circles), except the peaks in fish detections at 309 sunrise and sunset occurred one hour later (between 0600 and 0700) and earlier (between 310 1900 and 2000), respectively. Mean frequency of fish detection was lower in autumn than 311 in summer (GLM (season), F = 4.652, df = 1, 37, p = 0.038), particularly during the dawn 312 and dusk peaks of activity. 313 Although the above results suggested an overall predominance of nocturnal 314 behaviour with activity peaks at twilight, substantial variability among parr was observed. 315 Logistic regressions with added quadratic terms revealed three types of daily patterns: 316 nocturnal, concave quadratic curves; diurnal, convex quadratic curve; or, equally active 317 during day and night, not significant (Fig 4b). During the summer, only 11 of 25 318 individuals exhibited the predominant nocturnal pattern, whereas five individuals (20%) 319 exhibited diurnal behaviour, and nine individuals (36%) showed no significant daily 320 pattern. During the autumn, nocturnal behaviour was adopted by 10 of 14 individuals 321 (71%), with four individuals showing no clear pattern (29%) and no parr exhibiting 322 diurnal behaviour. However, the difference in frequencies of diel patterns between 323 summer and autumn was not significant (χ2 = 4.20, df = 2, p = 0.12). 324 Although some individuals exhibited a predominant diel activity pattern, others 325 exhibited considerable within-individual variability. Fish that did not exhibit a significant 326 nocturnal or diurnal pattern were either active both day and night within the same day or 327 were active only at night on some days and only during the day on other days (Fig. 5). 328 Juveniles differed in how they adopted different activity patterns over time. Several 329 individuals were only nocturnal or crepuscular, whereas others frequently changed daily 330 patterns between days (Fig. 5). 331 During the summer, all fish adopted a nocturnal pattern on at least 10% of the 332 days, but the majority of fish were nocturnal on more than 50% of the days. While seven 333 individuals were never detected during daylight hours, 15 individuals (60%) were active 334 only in the day at least once during the summer. Three individuals (12%) were highly 335 crepuscular and were only detected at twilight periods on more than 50% of the days. 336 Within individual variability of activity pattern decreased during the autumn, as most fish 337 were nocturnal and crepuscular. However, four individuals (30%) still exhibited daytime 338 activity (i.e. either diurnal or both) over 15% of the days. 339 340 341 Daily activity patterns vs flow stage and temperature Daily patterns of activity varied among fish within days, with all types of activity 342 patterns being observed most days. Daily activity increased significantly with 343 temperature and was the highest at 13 oC (Fig. 6a; GLM, F = 2.08, df = 8, 104.50, p = 344 0.046). This relationship was mainly due to a significant increase in crepuscular activity 345 (GLM, F = 3.81, df = 8, 107.03, p = 0.001), as no significant effect of temperature was 346 observed on diurnal and nocturnal activity (GLM, F = 1.63, df = 8, 103.87, p = 0.126; F 347 = 0.791, df = 8, 106.75, p = 0.612). Daily activity also decreased significantly with flow 348 stage (Fig. 6b GLM, F = 3.98, df = 6, 107.10, p = 0.001), primarily due to a decrease in 349 nocturnal (GLM, F = 2.58, df = 6, 110.39, p = 0.022 ) and crepuscular (GLM, F = 5.46, 350 df = 6, 110.27, p < 0.001) activity, with no significant effect of flow on diurnal activity 351 (GLM, F = 2.14, df = 6, 106.87, p < 0.055). 352 353 354 Diel and seasonal habitat use patterns Average velocity used by parr varied significantly between day, night, and 355 crepuscular periods (Fig. 7a; GLM within fish repeated measures, F = 3.76, df = 3,111 356 p=0.013); parr used lower velocity habitats during the night than during dawn, day and 357 dusk periods. During the summer, velocity used ranged from 50 to 60 cm˖s-1 during hours 358 of darkness (2000 to 0500) and from 65 to 86 cm˖s-1 during the rest of the daily cycle. 359 However, this diel pattern of habitat use was not observed during the autumn period 360 (GLM within fish days x season interaction, F = 3.78, df = 3,111 p = 0.013), as average 361 velocity used varied irregularly between 64 and 70 cm·s-1 for the four daily periods. 362 Nevertheless, overall average velocity used did not differ between the summer and 363 autumn season (between fish GLM, F=0.033, df = 1, 3, p = 0.857). Although the use of 364 lower velocity at night was the predominant pattern, substantial variability among 365 individuals was observed. During the summer, 10 individuals (40%) exhibited a convex 366 daily pattern of current velocity used, whereas three individuals (12%) adopted the 367 opposite behaviour and 12 (48%) did not show a clear pattern (Fig. 7c). During the 368 autumn period, only three fish used lower velocity habitats at night, whereas the 369 remaining 10 individuals did not exhibit significant daily patterns (Fig.7e). 370 Average flow depth used did not differ significantly between day, night and 371 crepuscular periods (F = 1.46, df =3, 111 p = 0.23) or between summer and autumn 372 (GLM, F=0.85, df = 1, 37, p=0.364), nor was there a significant interaction between daily 373 period and season (Fig. 7b; GLM season x daily periods interaction, GLM, F = 0.25, df = 374 3,111 p = 0.863). However, average patterns obscured interesting variability between 375 fish: six fish (24%) used shallower areas at night than during the day; six others 376 exhibited the opposite pattern; and, half did not exhibit any clear daily depth use pattern 377 (Fig. 7d). During the autumn, six individuals out of 14 (43%) exhibited a different depth 378 use between day and night periods: two using deeper and four using shallower habitats at 379 night than during the day (Fig. 7f). Again, a large proportion of individuals (57%) did not 380 display any particular pattern of depth use. 381 382 383 Diel habitat use pattern vs. activity pattern Contrary to the average daily patterns of habitat use, cathemeral individuals did 384 not use lower velocities at night than in the day (within the same day) in the summer (F = 385 2.58, df = 1, 145, p = 0.11) nor autumn (F = 1.31, df = 1, 40, p = 0.26). Therefore, 386 average differences in daily habitat use were caused by individuals exhibiting different 387 daily patterns (diurnal vs. nocturnal). During the summer, nocturnal individuals used 388 night habitats with significantly lower velocities and shallower depths than cathemeral 389 individuals (U: F = 31.97, df = 1,271 p < 0.001; F = 15.94, df = 1,263 p < 0.001). 390 However, diurnal individuals did not use different day habitats than cathemeral fish (U F 391 = 0.95, df = 1, 84, p = 0.33; Y F = 2.57, df = 1, 82 p = 0.113). In the autumn, fish 392 exhibiting different daily activity patterns did not differ significantly in their habitat use 393 during the day and night (data not shown). 394 395 Discussion 396 Diel and seasonal activity pattern 397 Our results showed that the probability of detecting a fish, which is assumed to be 398 closely associated with fish activity, peaked during twilight periods, particularly during 399 the summer. Activity was also higher at night than during the day during both summer 400 and autumn. Although parr have been observed switching from a predominantly diurnal 401 to a nocturnal behaviour as temperature drops in the autumn between 8-12 oC (Rimmer et 402 al. 1983; Fraser et al. 1993; Bremset 2000), our results support more recent field studies 403 reporting a predominance of nocturnal behaviour during the warmer temperatures in 404 summer (Gries et al. 1997, 13-23oC; Imre and Boisclair 2004, 16-21 oC) and in the 405 autumn (Johnson et al. 2004). Nocturnal behaviour might be associated with a size 406 dependant trade-off between growth and predation risk. Post young-of the year parr tend 407 to be more nocturnal to minimize predation risk from diurnal predators, whereas young- 408 of-the-year tend to maximize food intake to increase their growth rate and chances of 409 surviving the winter (Johnston et al. 2004; Imre and Boisclair 2004). While we observed 410 only post YOY in this study, body size had no significant effect on diel activity pattern, 411 suggesting that small differences in size might be less important than other factors in 412 inducing a particular activity pattern. 413 Although the crepuscular activity of juvenile salmonids is generally 414 acknowledged (Brittain and Eikeland 1988), most studies compare only day and night 415 activity. However, our results suggest that dawn and dusk are important, often 416 representing the only periods of activity in a daily cycle for some individuals. 417 Furthermore, the overall low probability of fish detection of 23% suggests low activity 418 levels, even in the summer. Similarly, 1+ Atlantic salmon are active only 37% of the time 419 in the summer (Breau et al. 2007). Low activity in the summer may be explained by 420 individuals gaining enough energy during short periods of activity to survive the next 421 winter without unduly increasing their predation risk (Cunjak et al. 1998). Low activity 422 levels might also have been induced by an abundance of food (Metcalfe et al. 1999; 423 Orpwood et al. 2006). 424 In this study, fish activity was most likely underestimated due to several 425 limitations of the tracking system. First, fish could sometimes be active in areas that were 426 not covered by the antenna grid. However, this issue is shared with other methods, such 427 as snorkeling, that assume “absent” fish to be inactive (e.g. Breau et al 2007). Hence, 428 probability of detection in this study, as in others, was primarily a way of comparing 429 activity between periods, rather than an estimate of absolute activity. Secondly, multiple 430 individuals simultaneously located on the same detector for extended periods of time 431 could not be recorded due to tag collision. However, since large parr are generally mobile 432 and territorial (unpublished data), it is unlikely that multiple fish would stay for extended 433 periods at the same location. Though it was unlikely that an active fish would be recorded 434 at a location without interruption, this possibility should be considered when interpreting 435 our results. 436 437 438 Individual variability of parr activity The high within-individual variability in activity pattern confirmed our second 439 hypothesis that parr frequently changed activity patterns. Despite a predominantly 440 nocturnal activity pattern, important variability between individuals was observed, 441 confirming previous studies (Breau et al. 2007). In our study, only 44% of the fish 442 followed the average concave pattern in the summer, which was associated with lower 443 activity in the day than in other periods. These results highlight the importance of 444 examining individual variation in salmonid behaviour, rather than reporting average 445 trends. Our results also suggest that fish behaviour was more homogeneous in the 446 autumn, as the majority of individuals adopted a predominantly nocturnal activity pattern 447 and no fish were predominantly diurnal. 448 Several individuals exhibited the consistent diel activity pattern of being active 449 only during crepuscular periods and at night. This activity pattern likely provides basic 450 energetic needs while minimizing predation risk: dusk and dawn are typically times of 451 high food abundance (Brittain & Eikeland 1988), without the high predation risk of 452 daytime (Clark and Levy 1988). However, many individuals exhibited considerable 453 variability in activity patterns, sometimes being active at night only, during the day only, 454 during both the day and night, and during twilight periods only. 455 Numerous controlled laboratory experiments have examined how various factors 456 affect the tradeoff between growth and mortality risk and how this translates to changes 457 in the activity patterns of juvenile salmonids (e.g. Fraser et al. 1993; Alanärä and 458 Brannas, 1997). These studies have shown that an abundance of food and cover favour 459 night foraging, as it provides sufficient growth rates while minimizing the risk of 460 predation. Hence, the spatial and temporal variability of food and cover will contribute to 461 the variation in activity pattern observed in the reach. Furthermore, when food and space 462 is limited, dominance status may influence activity patterns; dominant individuals feed at 463 the most beneficial times, whereas subordinates feed at other time periods (Alanärä and 464 Brannas 1997; Alanärä et al. 2002). Social status and competition may combine with 465 short term variation in energy requirements and individual foraging success (Metcalfe et 466 al. 1999) to explain the observed high temporal and intra-individual variability of activity 467 patterns. 468 469 470 Activity vs. flow and temperature Contrary to the prediction, temperature did not significantly affect diurnal and 471 nocturnal activity. However, total activity increased with temperature, mostly due to an 472 increase in crepuscular activity. Our results suggested, therefore, that older parr in the 473 wild are predominantly nocturnal in the summer regardless of temperature (Imre and 474 Boisclair 2004, Johnston et al. 2004), but also modulate their crepuscular activity levels, 475 perhaps to benefit from higher food availability at higher temperatures during dusk and 476 dawn and to fulfill higher metabolic costs during the summer Furthermore, at high 477 temperatures, a higher variability in activity patterns among individuals was observed 478 than at low temperatures, when fish were almost all nocturnal. This result is analogous to 479 those of Alanärä et al. (2002), who suggested that temporal segregation occurred at high 480 temperatures, resulting from higher competition due to higher energetic requirements. 481 Also contrary to the prediction, nocturnal and crepuscular activity decreased with flow 482 stage. Perhaps the increased food availability in faster flows decreased the time required 483 to fulfill energetic demands while keeping predation risk to a minimum. However, unlike 484 in Bradford and Higgins (2001), daytime activity did not decrease with flow stage. 485 486 487 Seasonal and diel habitat use pattern On average in the summer, parr were detected in lower flow velocities at night 488 than during the day, perhaps to offset a decrease in their ability to catch prey at night in 489 fast flows (Metcalfe et al. 1997). Interestingly, in the autumn, there was no difference 490 between the flow velocities where fish were detected during the day and night. Similarly, 491 Riley et al. (2006) reported that young-of-the-year salmon used lower flow velocity at 492 night than during the day, but not for older juveniles. Therefore, differences in day/night 493 habitat use might be related to differences in day and night foraging. Young-of-the-year 494 salmon continue to feed later in the year than older juveniles (Johnston et al. 2004, Breau 495 et al. 2007). If diel patterns in velocity-related habitat use were associated with diel 496 activity patterns, a suppression of activity during daylight would affect the type of 497 habitats used during the day and night. In contrast with previous studies reporting the use 498 of low-velocity habitats at low temperatures (Metcalfe et al. 1997; Cunjak et al. 1998), 499 we observed no significant differences between summer and autumn. Consistent with 500 previous studies (Riley et al. 2006), parr did not exhibit a significant diel pattern in the 501 use of flow depth. Flow depth may be of less importance than velocity in habitat 502 selection. 503 504 Parr exhibited high inter-individual variability, as only 40% of individuals exhibited the predominant pattern of a shift to slower flows at night, with some 505 individuals exhibiting the reverse pattern. Such behaviour might arise as a result of lower 506 velocity habitats being limited by competition. Also, habitat selection might affect short- 507 term foraging success, which indirectly affected activity pattern decisions. 508 509 510 Activity pattern and habitat use patterns In contrast to the general pattern of lower flow velocity used at night, cathemeral 511 fish did not use different habitats at different times of day. This observation contrasts 512 with previous experiments that showed a preference for lower velocity at night (Metcalfe 513 et al. 1997; Valdimarsson and Metcalfe 1998). However, these studies were conducted 514 under controlled conditions in the absence of competition, which might prevent some 515 individuals from using already occupied habitats. The overall differences between day 516 and night habitat use observed in this study can therefore be attributed to individuals 517 being nocturnal or diurnal, which supports the hypothesis that habitat use influences the 518 activity pattern adopted by individuals. 519 When fish were nocturnal, they used habitats with lower flow velocity than when 520 they were cathemeral. Given the relatively high velocities observed on the reach at 521 median flow, we suggest that average velocity selected by nocturnal fish (53 cm·s-1) were 522 more profitable than the higher average velocity selected at night by cathemeral fish (68 523 cm·s-1). Perhaps low foraging success at night in fast flows induced a need to forage also 524 during the day. Individuals exhibiting nocturnal activity used lower velocity habitats than 525 other individuals during crepuscular periods, perhaps because low flow velocity habitats 526 were limited in the reach. 527 In this study, because the initial number of fish on the study site was relatively 528 low, a fraction of the study fish were brought into the detection area from upstream. 529 Furthermore, a second group of fish from upstream was added a month after the 530 beginning of the study period to balance a decrease in fish density over time. The causes 531 of the declining density may include mortality and emigration due to a density of large 532 parr exceeding the carrying capacity of the reach, a response to storm events and homing 533 behaviour to their capture locations (Huntingford et al. 1998). Moreover, large parr at this 534 location occasionally show bouts of high mobility, moving across the reach typically 535 from one pool to another (Roy et al. 2012). Such mobility likely increased the chances for 536 fish to relocate outside the boundary of the tracking system. However, despite no 537 significant difference in activity patterns between the groups of fish tagged on two 538 occasions, a potential impact of relocating fish and of combining resident and immigrant 539 fish on the behaviour noted here cannot be completely ignored. 540 In summary, most juvenile Atlantic salmon frequently changed activity patterns, 541 although some remained mostly nocturnal. Fluctuations in temperature and flow stage 542 were associated with changes in activity, mostly during crepuscular periods. Habitat use 543 differed between nocturnal fish and diurnal fish, but not between the night and day 544 habitats of fish that were active both day and night. Previous controlled laboratory studies 545 have shown how numerous factors can induce changes in day/night activity of individual 546 fish through a tradeoff between growth and mortality risk. While previous field studies 547 have reported nocturnal or diurnal activity trends at the reach scale, this study has shown 548 how variable activity patterns are among individuals and how part of this variability 549 might be associated by microhabitat scale processes. The high temporal variability in 550 activity patterns illustrated the dynamic nature of fish foraging decisions in a natural 551 system. 552 553 554 Acknowledgements We are very grateful to Francis Bérubé and Marc-André Pouliot for their help in 555 the development of the tracking system and for essential logistical and technical 556 assistance. We also thank Patricia Johnston, André Boivin, Claude Gibeault, Marie-Eve 557 Roy and René Roy for help in the field. We would also like to thank Geoide, NSERC and 558 the Canada Research Chair program for the funding of this research, I. Imre and two 559 anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments. 560 References 561 Alanärä, A. and Brannas, E. (1997). Diurnal and nocturnal feeding activity in Arctic char 562 (Salvelinus alpinus) and rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss). Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci., 563 54: 2894-2900 564 Alanärä A, Burns MD, Metcalfe NB. Intraspecific resource partitioning in brown trout: 565 the temporal distribution of foraging is determined by social rank. J. Anim. Ecol. 566 2001;70: 980–986 567 Armstrong, J.D., Braithwaite, V.A., and Huntingford, F.A. 1997. Spatial strategies of 568 wild Atlantic salmon parr: exploration and settlement in unfamiliar areas. J. Anim. Ecol. 569 66: 203-211. 570 Armstrong, J.D., Kemp, P.S., Kennedy, G.J.A., Ladle, M., and Milner, N.J. 2003. Habitat 571 requirements of Atlantic salmon and brown trout in rivers and streams. Fish. Res. 62: 572 143-170. 573 Bateman, D.S., and Gresswell R.E. 2006. Survival and growth of age-0 steelhead after 574 surgical implantation of 23-mm passive integrated transponders. N. Am. J. Fish. Manage. 575 26:545–550. 576 Bradford, M.J., and Higgins, P.S. 2001. Habitat-, season-, and size-specific variation in 577 diel activity patterns of juvenile chinook salmon (Oncorhynchus tshawytscha) and 578 steelhead trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss). Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 58: 365-374. 579 Breau, C., Weir, L.K., and Grant, J.W.A. 2007. Individual variability in activity patterns 580 of juvenile Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar) in Catamaran Brook, new brunswick. Can. J. 581 Fish. Aquat. Sci. 64: 486-494. 582 Bremset, G. 2000. Seasonal and diel changes in behaviour, microhabitat use and 583 preferences by young pool-dwelling Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar, and brown trout, 584 Salmo trutta. Environ. Biol. Fish. 59: 163-179. 585 Brittain, J.E., and Eikeland, T.J. 1988. Invertebrate drift - A review. Hydrobiologia 166: 586 77-93. 587 Burnham, K.P., Anderson, D.R., and Huyvaert, K.P. 2011. AIC model selection and 588 multimodel inference in behavioral ecology: Some background, observations, and 589 comparisons. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65: 23-35. 590 Clark, C.W., and Levy, D.A. 1988. Diel vertical migrations by juvenile sockeye salmon 591 and the antipredation window. Am. Nat. 131: 271-290. 592 Cunjak, R.A. 1988. Behavior and microhabitat of young Atlantic salmon (Salmo-salar) 593 during winter. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 45: 2156-2160. 594 Cunjak, R.A., Prowse, T.D., and Parrish, D.L. 1998. Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar) in 595 winter: "The season of parr discontent"? Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 55: 161-180. 596 Fraser, N.H.C., Metcalfe, N.B., and Thorpe, J.E. 1993. Temperature-dependent switch 597 between diurnal and nocturnal foraging in salmon. Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. 252: 598 135-139. 599 Gries, G., Whalen, K.G., Juanes, F., and Parrish, D.L. 1997. Nocturnal activity of 600 juvenile atlantic salmon (Salmo salar) in late summer: Evidence of diel activity 601 partitioning. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 54: 1408-1413. 602 Heggenes, J., Bagliniere, J.L., and Cunjak, R.A. 1999. Spatial niche variability for young 603 atlantic salmon (Salmo salar) and brown trout (S-trutta) in heterogeneous streams. Ecol. 604 Freshw. Fish 8: 1-21. 605 Heggenes, J., Krog, O.M.W., Lindas, O.R., Dokk, J.G., and Bremnes, T. 1993. 606 Homeostatic behavioral-responses in a changing environment - brown trout (Salmo- 607 trutta) become nocturnal during winter. J. of Anim. Ecol. 62: 295-308. 608 Hiscock, M.J., Scruton, D.A., Brown, J.A., and Pennell, C.J. 2002. Diel activity pattern of 609 juvenile Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar) in early and late winter. Hydrobiologia 483: 161- 610 165. 611 Huntingford, F.A., Braithwaite, V.A., Armstrong, J.D., Aird, D. and Joiner, P 1998. Homing in 612 juvenile salmon in response to imposed and spontaneous displacement: experiments in an 613 artificial stream. J. Fish Biol. 53: 847-852. 614 615 Imre, I., and Boisclair, D. 2004. Age effects on juvenile Atlantic salmon: parr are more 616 nocturnal than young-of-the-year. J. Fish Biol. 64: 1731-1736. 617 Johnston, P., Bergeron, N.E., and Dodson, J.J. 2004. Diel activity patterns of juvenile 618 Atlantic salmon in rivers with summer water temperature near the temperature-dependent 619 suppression of diurnal activity. J. Fish Biol. 65: 1305-1318. 620 Johnston, P., Bérubé, F., and Bergeron, N.E. 2009. Development of a flatbed passive 621 integrated transponder antenna grid for continuous monitoring of fishes in natural stream. 622 J. Fish Bio. 74: 1651-1661. 623 Linnansaari, T., J.-M. Roussel, R.A. Cunjak,. Halleraker J.H. 2007. Efficacy and 624 accuracy of portable PIT-antennae when locating fish in ice-covered streams. 625 Hydrobiologia, 582: 281-287 626 Metcalfe, N.B., Fraser, N.H.C., and Burns, M.D. 1999. Food availability and the 627 nocturnal vs. diurnal foraging trade-off in juvenile salmon. J. of Anim. Ecol. 68: 371-381. 628 Metcalfe, N.B., Valdimarsson, S.K., and Fraser, N.H.C. 1997. Habitat profitability and 629 choice in a sit-and-wait predator: Juvenile salmon prefer slower currents on darker nights. 630 J. of Anim. Ecol. 66: 866-875. 631 Mitchell, J., McKinley, R.S., Power, G., and Scruton, D.A. 1998. Evaluation of Atlantic 632 salmon parr responses to habitat improvement structures in an experimental channel in 633 Newfoundland, Canada. Regul. Rivers. 14: 25-39. 634 Nielsen, J. L., 1992. Microhabitat-Specific Foraging Behavior, Diet, and Growth of 635 Juvenile Coho Salmon, Trans. Am. Fish. Soc. 121,: 5 636 Orpwood, J.E., Griffiths, S.W., and Armstrong, J.D. 2006. Effects of food availability on 637 temporal activity patterns and growth of Atlantic salmon. J. of Anim. Ecol. 75: 677-685. 638 Orpwood J. E. Armstrong J.D. Griffiths S. W. 2010. Interactions between riparian 639 shading and food supply: a seasonal comparison of effects on time budgets, space use and 640 growth in Atlantic salmon Salmo salar. J. Fish Biol. 77, 1835–1849. 641 Reebs, S.G. 2002. Plasticity of diel and circadian activity rhythms in fishes. Rev. Fish 642 Biol. Fisher. 12: 349-371. 643 Riley, W.D., Ives, M.J., Pawson, M.G., and Maxwell, D.L. 2006. Seasonal variation in 644 habitat use by salmon, Salmo salar, trout, Salmo trutta and grayling, Thymallus 645 thymallus, in a chalk stream. Fish. Manag. Ecol. 13: 221-236. 646 Rimmer, D.M., Paim, U., and Saunders, R.L. 1983. Autumnal habitat shift of juvenile 647 Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar ) in a small river. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 40: 671-680. 648 Roussel, J.M., Haro, A. and Cunjak, R. A. 2000. Field test of a new method for tracking 649 small fishes in shallow rivers using passive integrated transponder (PIT) technology. 650 Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 57, 1326–1329. 651 Roy, M.L., Roy, A.G., Grant, W.J., Bergeron, N.E. 2013, Individual variability in the 652 movement behaviour of juvenile Atlantic salmon, Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 70: 339-347 653 Sigourney, D.B., Horton, G.E., Dubreuil, T.L., Varaday, A.M., and Letcher, B.H. 2005. 654 Electroshocking and pit tagging of juvenile Atlantic salmon: Are there interactive effects 655 on growth and survival? N. Am. J. Fish. Manage. 25: 1016-1021. 656 Valdimarsson, S.K., and Metcalfe, N.B. 1998. Shelter selection in juvenile Atlantic 657 salmon or why do salmon seek shelter in winter? J. of Fish Biol. 52: 42-49. 658 Valdimarsson, S.K., Metcalfe, N.B., Thorpe, J.E., and Huntingford, F.A. 1997. Seasonal 659 changes in sheltering: Effect of light and temperature on diel activity in juvenile salmon. 660 Anim. Behav. 54: 1405-1412. 661 Wolman, M.G. 1954. A method of sampling coarse river-bed material. Trans. Am. 662 Geophys. Union 35: 951-956. 663 664 665 666 667 668 669 670 671 672 673 674 675 676 677 678 679 680 681 682 683 684 Fig. 1. Flatbed antenna grid buried in the bed of a reach of Xavier Brook; 144 antennas 685 are displayed in transects (dots), connected to a controller and multiplexer on the bank 686 (rectangle) powered by solar panels (suns). Contours illustrate bathymetry at median flow 687 during the study period. 688 689 690 691 692 693 694 695 696 697 698 699 700 701 702 703 704 705 Fig. 2. Time series of water level and water temperature. Gray shading indicates periods during 706 which river stage was 20 cm over the minimum base level observed during the study period. The 707 vertical dashed line denotes 12oC, defining a warmer summer period and a colder autumn period. 708 Asterisks and numbers indicate fish tagging sessions and the days of flow measurement, 709 respectively. 710 711 712 713 714 715 716 717 718 719 720 721 722 723 724 725 Fig. 3. Maps of a) depth (stage:10 cm) b) the number of different tagged individuals detected at 726 each antenna over the entire study period c) the frequency of fish detections observed. Frequency 727 of fish detections was estimated from time decimated data (i.e. only one location per fish per hour 728 was kept at a single antenna location). Flow is from left to right. 729 730 731 732 733 734 735 736 737 738 Fig. 4. a) Average probability of fish presence per hour during the summer (stars, N=25) and 739 autumn (open circles, N=14). Horizontal line shows average periods of daylight during summer 740 and autumn. Quadratic logistic regression models of probability of fish presence as a function of 741 hours during the b) summer and c) autumn. Solid lines indicate significant curves, whereas 742 dashed lines non-significant curves. 743 744 Fig. 5. Number of days each fish was present in the reach subdivided into diel activity pattern 745 adopted by each individual in the (a) summer and (b) autumn. 746 747 748 749 750 751 752 753 754 755 756 757 758 759 760 761 762 763 Fig.6. Mean + SE activity (i.e. relative frequency of detection) of Atlantic salmon parr 764 during the entire day (24 h) (stars –solid line), crepuscular (diamonds – large dashed 765 line), night (circles – small dashed line) and day (square) periods in relation to a) water 766 temperature and b) flow stage above minimum recorded for a period of three months in 767 the summer and autumn. Lines indicate significant trends. Each datum was the mean 768 activity for each marked fish at each temperature and flow stage interval. For each 769 increasing temperature category, n = 3, 6, 14, 14, 16, 18, 22, 26, 17 and for flow category 770 n= 17, 27, 25, 25, 20, 12, 12. 771 772 773 774 775 776 777 778 779 780 781 Fig. 7. Average ± SE (a) flow velocity and (b) depth used by Atlantic salmon parr in the summer 782 (stars) and autumn (open circles) per hour. Quadratic regressions of individual average (c) flow 783 velocity and (d) use (c) per hour during the summer (n=25) (significant relationship: solid, non 784 significant: dashed) and (e) and (f) are the same relationships for the autumn period (n=13). 785 786 787