Chapter 2 The Contexts of Development Learning Objectives When students have studied the material in the chapter, they will be able to answer the following: • Introduction 1. What is known about the effects of environmental deprivation on children's development? 2. What aspects of the contexts provided by the Williams, Gordon, and Polonius families seem likely to be especially significant for their children’s development? 3. How have the contexts in which you grew up influenced your development? • An overview of developmental contexts 4. Describe Bronfenbrenner's model of developmental contexts and explain what is included at each level in the model. • The child’s biological makeup 5. Explain the three basic components of the child’s biological makeup and how they interact. • The child's immediate environment 6. Explain how the family, day care, peers, neighborhoods, and schools influence children's development. • The social and economic context 7. Summarize the ways American families have changed in recent decades and how these changes have affected children’s development. 8. Explain how socioeconomic status influences children’s development. • The cultural context 9. What is the same about child rearing across all cultures? What varies? 10. Summarize the impacts of culture on children’s development. • Development as context 11. Explain two ways in which development itself provides a context for further development. • Contexts in interaction 12. Explain how contexts interact to influence children's development. 17 Chapter 2 Chapter Summary Outline Note: Terms in bold print are chapter vocabulary words. Introduction • • Development, both physical and mental, requires an appropriate context. An abnormal context may result in abnormal development. Some examples: 1. Victor, “the wild boy of Aveyron” 2. Genie 3. Infants raised in institutions. Recent examples are from Eastern Europe. • Contexts of development include variations in genes, culture, family relations, and economic circumstances, which all contribute to developmental differences. An Overview of Developmental Contexts Urie Bronfenbrenner’s Ecological Theory outlines the complexity of contextual influences on development by means of a model depicting three concentric rings around the child at the center, where each ring influences all the rings inside of it. 4. Biological Makeup 5. Immediate Environment 6. Social and Economic Context 7. Cultural Context The Child’s Biological Makeup • Three components influence the center of the model, the child’s biological makeup: 8. The evolutionary heritage shared by all humans. 9. The child’s individual genetic inheritance. 10. The biological results of interactions between genes and environment. • The Human Evolutionary Heritage 1. We have a fairly precise timetable for many developmental milestones. 2. We have a strong disposition to act upon the environment, to be curious rather than passive. 3. We have an innate propensity for learning simple (e.g., sucking) and complex (e.g., language) skills. 4. We have a predisposition to be social, to form bonds. • • Individual genetic differences have allowed our species to meet environmental challenges over hundreds of thousands of years. There are direct and indirect effects of individual genetic makeup. There are interactions between genes and the environment. An important question pertains to the degree to which genes constrain environmental influences (canalization). 18 The Contexts of Development The Child’s Immediate Environment • The Family Context 1. The family is the dominant part of a child’s immediate environment, forming first social relationships and providing models for behavior. The Family as a System 1. Researchers have moved beyond a focus on maternal caregiving to now include fathers, siblings, and the extended family. 2. The family is an interconnected system—each member’s behavior depends in part on the behavior of the others. 3. Bidirectional effects – parents shape children’s behavior and children’s characteristics influence parent’s behavior. 4. Sameroff’s Transactional Model – cumulative effects of ongoing bidirectional influences. Characteristics of Family Systems a. Made up of many subsystems joined together in a coherent, interlocking network. b. A family is a dynamic, open system, subject to change as well as continuity. c. They are subject to cyclical influences that can be repeated across generations. Fathers in the Family System a. Direct (e.g., attachment) and indirect influences (e.g., marital harmony) are considered important. Siblings in the Family System a. Aid in development of social understanding. Immediate Contexts Outside the Family • The Day-Care Setting – increase in use of day care in the U.S. 1. 1 in 5 are cared for by grandparent or other relative. 1 in 6 are cared for by father at home. About 1 in 4 are cared for by a nonrelative such as an individual babysitter or a family day-care setting. 1 in 4 are in a child-care center or preschool. 2. Researchers have noted no negative effects of full-time day care for toddlers and preschoolers. It can promote cognitive and social development if high quality. The debate continues regarding day care for those under one year of age. • The Peer Group 1. Teaches how to interact in equal-status, or symmetrical relationships. 2. Reinforces values, beliefs, and behavior standards that are part of the child’s culture. • The Neighborhood 1. Community income levels have been a research focus, particularly community poverty. 2. Influence via collective socialization where adults provide role models and monitoring for local children; social networks. • The School 1. Instruction in cultural norms and values, such as gender roles, neatness, discipline, punctuality, competition, hard work, and material success. 19 Chapter 2 2. How the school is run and how teachers interact with students can affect how positive the school experience is for children. The Social and Economic Context • • • The second ring of Bronfenbrenner’s model contains the following: community in which the child’s immediate environment exists; social institutions, health-care systems, and religious organizations; social and economic conditions in the community and in the larger society (e.g., birth rates, income levels). The social and economic context affects children directly and indirectly. Family Changes Caused by Social and Economic Factors 1. Maternal Employment and Its Effects How maternal employment effects the child depends on the child’s age and gender, amount of time mother spends at work, quality of shared time remaining, quality of substitute care, strength of the parent-child relationship, and the meaning of the woman’s employment to both herself and other family members Maternal employment during first year associated with slightly lower performance on cognitive measures, but maternal employment after that has generally positive effects, especially on girls. Mothers who are unhappy with their situation, especially dissatisfied nonworking mothers, have more problems child rearing than those who are satisfied. If the husband is displeased with the wife’s employment, he may have more negative feelings toward his children and parenting responsibilities. Positive effects on mothers and children when single mother moves from welfare to paid employment. 2. Single Parenting and Its Effects Unmarried Mothers a. Rate of births to unmarried mothers increased from 11% in 1970 to 33% in 2000. 29% of these births are to women under age 20. b. Children of unmarried adolescent mothers often have developmental problems, show higher rates of school failure, delinquency, early sexual activity, and pregnancy. Much of these outcomes could be influenced by poverty factors. About 40% of the children in American families heading by single mothers are poor. c. Social support, family support, and finishing high school are important for reducing potential developmental problems. Divorced Parents a. Each year 1 million U.S. children are involved in divorce. b. Outcome issues depend on child’s age, sex, personality, quality of home life and parenting, and resources available to both parents and child. c. Divorce that ends parental conflict is generally better for children than a conflictbased marriage. Divorce with continued conflict is worse than marriage with conflict. 20 The Contexts of Development d. Ongoing contact with the noncustodial parent (usually father) generally reduces the negative consequences of divorce (with low conflict between the parents). e. Children’s adjustment to parental remarriage can take several years. Conflict is higher between children and stepparents, especially stepmothers. f. Adjustment to stepparents is more difficult for adolescents. 3. Other Nontraditional Families Homosexual parents are as involved with their children as are heterosexual parents. • Being raised by homosexual parents results in no obvious differences in gender identity, sex-role behavior, sexual orientation, and does not appear to place the child at risk for psychological problems. Socioeconomic Status and the Family 1. Socioeconomic Status (SES), the grouping of people within a society on the basis of income, occupation, and education, is another important aspect of social and economic context. Differences in parenting styles have been noted between working-class and middleclass parents. Poor quality childcare cuts across socioeconomic lines. 2. Poverty and Child Development Nearly 11.6 million U.S. children lived in poverty in 2000. This is 1 in 6 children overall but the rate is nearly twice that for Hispanic and African American children than for White children. Persistent poverty has consequences for child development. These children score lower on IQ, vocabulary, and achievement tests. They are more likely to repeat grades, be placed in special education, drop out of high school, have higher rates of behavior problems and delinquency. Why? Inadequate prenatal care, prenatal drug exposure, and low birth weight set the stage for later cognitive deficits. Lead exposure and lower cognitive stimulation at home intensify the early effects. Poor families experience more stress. They are more likely to be exposed to a string of negative events and to chronic problems. Parents then may be more depressed, irritable, and distracted which has negative consequences for parenting. Poverty can be a self-perpetuating cycle. This has been highlighted in research on the inner city African Americans living in ghettos. Intervention programs like Head Start can make a difference. 3. Homelessness Homelessness carries a particularly strong set of risks for children. In 2000 in the US, 40% of those in homeless shelters were families with children. Homeless women are least likely to receive prenatal care and have higher rates of low birth weight infants as well as higher infant mortality rates. 21 Chapter 2 Homeless children suffer from more health problems and are least likely to receive proper immunizations, they experience more stress, more disruptions in school and friendships, and have higher rates of behavior problems. 4. Unemployment and Family Relationships Job loss and its hardships tend to increase conflict and violence in families, including child abuse. The Cultural Context • • The cultural context is the third ring in Bronfenbrenner’s model. Culture – a system of beliefs, attitudes, values, and guidelines for behavior. Different cultures tend to produce different patterns of personality characteristics, cognitive skills, and social relationships. 1. Cultural Influences We all have the same human biological inheritance and the same fundamental need for care. Adults in all cultures must 1) provide for infants with basic nurturance needed for development, and 2) must prepare children to function as adults in their particular social world by passing on rules, standards, and values of the culture via socialization. Socialization occurs by means of explicit and implicit instruction. The values of a culture are often reflected in the structure of the settings in which children spend their time (e.g., Chinese vs. U.S. nursery school environments). Children’s behavior also reflects the values and demands of their culture (see Madsen’s study on competition vs. cooperation in a game). Children in nonindustrialized cultures show more nurturant and responsible behaviors due to the tasks they are given while those in industrialized cultures are more dependent and self-centered since most do not contribute to family survival. Egoism may be an asset in cultures that depend on personal profit for economic growth. 2. Cultural Change and Child Development Parenting practices have changed over the decades and centuries as economic life patterns have changed. China is a recent example of rapid cultural change (family-centered to state-centered shift). 3. Subcultures Subcultures – groups whose beliefs, attitudes, values, and guidelines for behavior differ in some ways from those of the dominant culture. Sometimes a child’s subculture clashes with that of the dominant culture, which can result in difficulties in the classroom, for example. Development as Context • Development provides a context in two ways: 1. It gives each person a developmental history, which influences the course of future development (e.g., Erikson’s theory). 2. Development provides a context for future development because children change physically and intellectually as they mature. These transformations dramatically influence how children interact with their environments. 22 The Contexts of Development Contexts in Interaction • Central message of the chapter: human development always occurs within a set of contexts. None of the contexts exists in isolation. An example of this interaction is maternal employment. • Certain environmental factors tend to be grouped. • All the environmental influences are funneled through the family to some extent. Lecture Topics The following are lecture topic suggestions to complement Chapter 2 reading material. Topic 1: The Concept of Family Systems • Research Questions: 1. Interdependence of subsystems in the family. Families are made up of subsystems (husbandwife, mother-daughter, brother-sister, etc.). How are these subsystems interrelated and in what ways do these interrelationships manifest themselves? 2. Bidirectionality and changes in family systems. The concept of bidirectionality can be extended beyond the parent-child dyad to the family as a whole. Children are influenced by the family context in which they live, but they also influence that context by their very presence and by the stages of development they pass through. Family systems are not static, as often assumed, but are constantly adjusting to changes in family composition and in the developmental status of individual members. How do family systems change? When a child is born, the family into which he or she comes is already an organized system. If there is another child or children, the family consists of several subsystems--husband-wife, mother-child, father-child, and sometimes sibling relationships. In addition to these dyadic relationships, there are also triadic relationships, if the family already has three or more members. A new child forces the reorganization of the existing system and the formation of new relationships. The existing pattern of interactions in the family must change to accommodate the new member, with repercussions for everyone in the family. For example, older siblings must adjust to the mother's preoccupation with the new baby, the new demands on her time, and her resulting limited availability. As a result, they often seek and receive more attention from their father, changing the previous patterns of interaction in the family. This particular change in family organization is one that often confronts children during their preschool years. It is not just the addition of new family members that changes families' organization and interaction patterns, however. As children develop, their families change in response to their changing needs and abilities. A new baby demands a very different type of caregiving than a toddler, a preschooler, an 8-year-old, or an adolescent. A child also makes a different contribution to the family as a newborn than at later points in development. As a result, families pass through developmental changes and face various developmental tasks that in some ways parallel those faced by individuals. 3. Intergenerational aspects of systems. How are relationship patterns transmitted from one generation to the next? How can knowledge about the family in which a person grew up illuminate that individual's current behavior as a spouse and parent? • Background Literature: 1. Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss. Vol. 1: Attachment (2nd ed.). New York: Basic Books. (Chapters 1, 12). 23 Chapter 2 2. Brody, G. H., Stoneman, Z., & McCoy, K. (1992). Associations of maternal and paternal direct and differential behavior with sibling relationships: Contemporaneous and longitudinal analyses. Child Development, 63, 82-92. 3. Brody, G. H., Stoneman, Z., & McCoy, K. (1994). Forecasting sibling relationships in early adolescence from child temperaments and family processes in middle childhood. Child Development, 65, 771-784. 4. Dunn, J., & Plomin, R. (1990). Separate lives: Why siblings are so different. New York: Basic. 5. Dunn, J., Stocker, C., & Plomin, R. (1990). Nonshared experiences within the family: Correlates of behavioral problems in middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 113-126. 6. Kowal, A. M., & Kramer, L. (1995, March). Why mom liked you best: Children's understanding of parental differential treatment. In L. Kramer & N. Howe (Chairs), Sibling relationships and emotional development. Symposium conducted at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN. 7. Kreppner, K., Paulsen, S., & Schuetze, Y. (1982). Infant and family development: From triads to tetrads. Human Development, 25, 373-391. 8. Quittner, A. L., & Opipari, L. C. (1994). Differential treatment of siblings: Interview and diary analyses comparing two family contexts. Child Development, 65, 800-814. 9. Sroufe, L. A., Jacobvitz, D., Mangelsdorf, S., DeAngelo, E., & Ward, M. J. (1985). Generational boundary dissolution between mothers and their preschool children: A relationship systems approach. Child Development, 56, 317-325. 10. Sroufe, L. A., & Fleeson, J. (1988). Relationships within families: Mutual influences. In R. A. Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), The coherence of family relationships. Oxford: Oxford University Press. • Methods and Results: More information is available at http://www.mhhe.com/socscience/devel/kidc/resources/1-top.htm. 1. Interdependence of subsystems in the family. See the discussion of Sroufe and Fleeson (1988) results and Sroufe et al., 1985 available at the Web site listed above. 2. Bidirectionality and changes in family systems. See the discussion by Kreppner and his colleagues on birth order. See also the Brody, Stoneman, and McCoy discussion of differences in parent-sibling subsystems within the same family, part of a growing literature on intrafamily variation. 3. Intergenerational aspects of systems. Relationship patterns often are transmitted from generation to generation. This has been most obvious in the case of abuse, where maltreated children often grow up to mistreat their own youngsters. A further illustration of the power of a relationship systems perspective comes from two of our stories in the textbook. A systems view adds perspective to the question of why Karen becomes pregnant and why Frank so desperately wants a son. A piece of information not included in the vignette is the fact that Karen's mother also had become pregnant as a teenager, marrying a man who shortly thereafter abandoned her. While Karen's pregnancy may be explained by societal trends in teenage pregnancies or by carelessness in using birth control, a further explanation lies in her reenactment of her mother's life. Clinical psychologists speak of "invisible loyalties" that children have to their parents. That is, despite the fact that Karen's pregnancy made her mother angry, in a basic (though unconscious) way it expressed a "loyalty" or "obligation" to her family relationship system. Likewise, Frank Gordon's desire for a son can be explained at the cultural level (our patrilineal culture's prizing of males and valuing children of both genders in a family) and at the subcultural or socioeconomic level (macho Frank's need to have a son to engage in manly activities). But it can also be explained at a more personal family systems level. Frank 24 The Contexts of Development Gordon lost his father when he was 11. For him, a son would represent the re-creation of a father-son relationship that was prematurely fractured. And you will see that he winds up separated from his own son through divorce when his son is also in middle childhood. • Discussion Questions: What are some of the methodological difficulties in carrying out research on family systems? Which methods seem to be best suited to this kind of topic and why? Topic 2: Contexts in Interaction • • • Research Question: How can the various contexts in Bronfenbrenner's model help us understand the impact of poverty on black families and children? Huston, McLoyd, and Garcia Coll (1994) discuss the complex relationships between social and economic factors, parents' psychological well being, and children's socioemotional functioning. Their analysis helps us understand how the contexts in Bronfenbrenner's model interact to shape children's development. Background Literature: 1. Brody, G. H., Stoneman, Z., Flor, D., McCrary, C., Hastings, L., & Conyers, O. (1994). Financial resources, parent psychological functioning, parent co-caregiving, and early adolescent competence in rural two-parent African American families. Child Development, 65, 590-605. 2. Chase-Lansdale, P. L., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Zamsky, E. S. (1994). Young African American multigenerational families in poverty: Quality of mothering and grandmothering. Child Development, 65, 373-393. 3. Garrett, P., Ng'andu, N., & Ferron, J. (1994). Poverty experiences of young children and the quality of their home environments. Child Development, 65, 331-345. 4. Grantham-McGregor, S., Powell, C., Walker, S., Chang, S., & Fletcher, P. (1994). The long-term follow-up of severely malnourished children who participated in an intervention program. Child Development, 65, 428-439. 5. Hashima, P. Y., & Amato, P. R. (1994). Poverty, social support, and parental behavior. Child Development, 65, 394-403. 6. Huston, A. C., McLoyd, V. C., & Garcia Coll, C. (1994). Children and poverty: Issues in contemporary research. Child Development, 65, 275-282. 7. Huston, A. C., McLoyd, V. C., & Garcia Coll, C. (Eds.) (1994). Children and poverty [Special issue]. Child Development, 65. 8. McAdoo, H. P., & McAdoo, J. L. (Eds.) (1981). Black families. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. 9. McLoyd, V. (1990). The impact of economic hardship on black families and children: Psychological distress, parenting, and socioeconomic development. Child Development, 61, 311346. 10. Spencer, M., Brookins, G., & Allen, W. (Eds.) (1985). Beginnings: The social and affective development of black children. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. • • Additional Reading: Kotlowitz, A. (1991). There are no children here: The story of two boys growing up in the other America. New York: Doubleday. Analysis of Contexts: Further background information is available at http://www.mhhe.com/socscience/devel/kid-c/resources/1-top.htm [Note: One effective way to present Huston et al.'s ideas is to develop this example beginning at the outermost ring in the model--the cultural context--and working your way in to the biological context.] 1. The cultural context. North American culture holds that success is the result of individual effort and that people who fail have only themselves to blame, adding psychological insult to financial hardship for families in poverty. For black families, cultural attitudes about race create additional social and economic barriers and contribute to psychological distress. 25 Chapter 2 2. The social and economic context. Increasing numbers of inner city black children are growing up in chronic poverty because of: The transformation of cities from manufacturing to information-processing centers and the accompanying decline in traditional working-class jobs. Low wages in the jobs that are available to inner city residents with low levels of education. The increase in households headed by women with little income and resources. 3. The immediate context. Poverty, writes McLoyd (1990), "defines the child's immediate environment, almost in its entirety" (p. 314). Refer to the above listed Web site for more detailed discussion of poverty and its impact on development. 4. The biological context. Finally, the quality of parenting a child receives depends in part on the child. Because poor children are more likely to be born to young, malnourished mothers who received little prenatal care and may have used drugs or alcohol, and more likely to be born prematurely, they are more likely to be "difficult" babies who require extra care and attention to thrive. Frequent illness and injury and poor nutrition in infancy and childhood may also contribute to the developmental problems many of these children exhibit. One long-term follow-up study (Grantham-McGregor et al., 1994) found that severely malnourished children maintained poorer levels of development than their peers for up to 14 years after an acute episode of malnutrition. These developmental problems improved, however, when the children participated in an intervention program that included psychosocial stimulation. • Discussion Questions: What are the implications of Bronfenbrenner's model for intervention? If you wanted to help the children McLoyd (1990) and Huston et al. (1994) describe, in which context(s) would you try to make changes? Explain your answer. Topic 3: A Cross-Cultural Perspective on Family Sleeping Arrangements • • • • Research Questions: What is the basis for family sleeping arrangements? Should parents and children sleep together or not? Shweder, Jensen, and Goldstein (1995) assert that "the practice of determining who sleeps by whom in a family household is a symbolic action that simultaneously expresses and realizes some of the deepest moral ideals of a cultural community" (p. 21). In all cultures, whether parents and children sleep together or not, sleeping arrangements reflect childrearing goals and values for interpersonal relations. In the U.S., middle-class parents typically furnish a nursery for their newborn infants and provide separate beds (and, whenever possible, separate bedrooms) for their children throughout childhood. Middle-class parents in the U.S. believe that infants are born dependent and need to be socialized to become independent, beginning with learning to sleep alone. Pediatricians in the U.S., such as Brazelton and Spock, have traditionally advised parents that children should be taught to sleep alone in order to promote autonomy; these experts have also cautioned that children who routinely sleep with their parents are at risk of being sexually abused or may develop inappropriate sexual fantasies. Recently, some pediatricians have advocated the shared "family bed," but this practice has not been widely adopted by the white middle-class in the United States. In most other parts of the world (in places as different as Guatemala and Japan), children grow up cosleeping with their parents or other family members, even when space available in the home would afford separate beds in nonshared sleeping quarters. Moreover, parents in these cultures express shock and concern about the well being of infants and children who sleep alone. Japanese parents, for example, believe that infants are born as separate beings who must develop interdependent relationships with community members to survive; co-sleeping is thought to facilitate this process. 26 The Contexts of Development • • See Shweder et al.'s discussion of Brazelton's (1990) conflict over co-sleeping, the moral perceptions of the Anglo-American middle class, and Abbott's (1992) and Mandansky and Edelbrock's (1990) studies of sleeping practices among other ethnic groups and social classes in the United States. Research Literature: 1. Abbott, S. (1992). Holding on and pushing away: Comparative perspectives on an Eastern Kentucky child-rearing practice. Ethos, 1, 33-65. 2. Brazelton, T. B. (1990). Parent-infant cosleeping revisited. Ab Initio: An International Newsletter for Professionals Working with Infants and Their Families, 1, 1-7. 3. Mandansky, D., & Edelbrock, C. (1990). Cosleeping in a community sample of 2- and 3-year-old children. Pediatrics, 86, 197-203. 4. Morelli, G. A., Rogoff, B., Oppenheim, D., & Goldsmith, D. (1992). Developmental Psychology, 28, 604-613. 5. Shweder, R. A., Jensen, L. A., & Goldstein, W. M. (1995). Who sleeps by whom revisited: A method for extracting the moral goods implicit in practice. In J. J. Goodnow, P. J. Miller, & F. Kessel (Eds.), Cultural practices as contexts for development. New Directions for Child Development, 67, 21-39. • • Methods and Results: Background information is available at http://www.mhhe.com/socscience/devel/kid-c/resources/1-top.htm. 1. Morelli, Rogoff, Oppenheim, and Goldsmith (1992) examined the decisions of middle-class U.S. and Highland Mayan parents regarding sleeping arrangements during their child's first 2 years and their explanations for their differing practices. See the Web site for results and discussion. 2. Shweder et al. (1995) investigated sleeping arrangements among high-caste families in a Hindu town in India and among white middle-class families in Chicago. Shweder et al. used three types of tasks in their study: (1) a "sleeping arrangement task" in which subjects sorted members of a hypothetical seven-person family into sleeping spaces under various hypothetical resource constraints; (2) a "preference conflict task" in which subjects evaluated and ranked various culturally deviant arrangements of members of the hypothetical seven-person family in terms of the relative seriousness of the breach; and (3) reports about who slept by whom. See the Web site listed above for results and discussion of this study regarding the three types of tasks. 3. Taken together, these studies strongly suggest that sleeping arrangements in the family are not simply based on personal preferences or arbitrary habits. Instead, as Shweder et al. argue, they are social practices, deliberate acts "of rational choice motivated by an analysis of probable psychological and social costs and benefits" (p. 37). Discussion Questions: Should parents and children sleep together? Why or why not? Topic 4: Cultural Differences in Education • • • • • A lecture topic on Cultural Differences in Education could be compiled from the following: Items 4 and 8 at http://www.mhhe.com/socscience/devel/kid-c/resources/1-lecture/l-18.htm. Information in item 4 explores the differences between American versus Japanese schools. Discussion questions that are explored are: How do the Japanese achieve such high educational standards? What are the costs and benefits of this system? What would it take for U.S. schools to compete? If there are Japanese students in the classroom, perhaps they would be willing to offer their experiences as insights into this lecture topic. Information in item 8 at the above listed Web site briefly mentions cultural differences in the classroom with Hawaiian, Navajo, Asian American, and European American students. Discussion questions that may follow could tap into issues of the difficulties children experience when their subculture is at odds with the dominant culture. 1) students may discuss issues regarding the design of educational programs by “white, middle-class, educated” persons and how this bias may affect children in the various subcultures or social strata of the U.S. 2) have students consider 27 Chapter 2 how subcultural differences spill into other domains of society and psychology, such as provision of mental health services or the legal ramifications of various parenting practices. • At the above listed Web site are additional Lecture Topic options available for this chapter: 1. Egalitarian motive (item 6) 2. Rites of passage: O-Kee-Pa, a torture ritual (item 7) Classroom Discussion Topics and Activities 1. Adoption Issues: Adoptions are becoming part of public knowledge, with around 40,000 adoptions or more taking place each year in the US from domestic and foreign agencies and around 50,000 adoptions of “waiting” children in the US foster care system. In the year 2000, 131,000 children were waiting to be adopted in the US. It is important, from a context of development perspective, to explore adoption beliefs that students have and to look at facts, figures, and potential problems. • • • • • • • Ask students, either in the class setting or on paper, to address how they feel about adoption. To facilitate discussions, have students explore some Web sites on adoptions. One is http://www.adoptivefamilies.com/articles/adoption_myths.php that presents some of the myths of adoption. Information also is available at http://www.geocities.com/ adoptionabc/domestic.html and at http://adoption.about.com. A recent survey showed that there has been an increase in favorable opinions about adoption. 63% of Americans think very favorably about adoption (a seven percentage point increase since in 1997), and 64% have a close experience with adoption. 78% of Americans believe the country should be doing more to encourage adoption and 95% think adoptive parents should receive the same maternity and paternity benefits from employers as biological parents. Considering that in the US in 2000, the number of children in the foster care system was 556,000, and over 100,000 of these children were/are available for adoption, increasing adoption awareness is a positive step. If students are comfortable with the idea, ask if anyone is the class is adopted or whether any families have adopted. First person accounts help flesh out issues more clearly. This can also lead to discussions about telling children the truth. A number of people adopted did not encounter information about the fact that they were adopted until they were adults! Have students consider what questions and concerns children, adolescents, and adults may have about being adopted. Have students consider issues parents may have about adopting children. Many in the US have chosen to adopt AFTER having spent thousands and thousands of dollars, and huge emotional energy, on fertility options which failed. Have students discuss reasons that people choose adoption and reasons that people choose fertility treatments. In the US, the trend is to have open or partial open adoptions. Have students talk about what they think are the pros and cons of open vs. closed adoptions. Have students think about issues involving domestic vs. international adoptions. There are estimates that 20,000 or more U.S.-born infants are placed for adoption every year. In 2002, there were over 20,000 immigrant visas issued to orphans coming to the US from a number of countries (http://travel.state.gov/orphan_numbers.html). Americans adopted 5,053 infants and children (mostly girls) from mainland China, 4,939 from Russia, 2,210 from Guatemala, 1,779 from South Korea, and 1,106 from Ukraine (the top five countries). Think about reasons that so many families choose international adoption. Discuss the pros and cons of transracial adoptions, both foreign and domestic. In the US in 2000, 56,000 (43%) African American children were available for adoption and 17,400 (13%) Hispanic children were available for adoption. Discuss the pros and cons of transcultural adoptions. Age of the child is an important consideration in cross-cultural placements. Many children placed into permanent homes in the US have to learn a new language as well as learning to adapt to a family environment vs. an institutional environment, and within a different culture. Besides the case in China, where 1.7 million girls are placed into orphanages each year, most international countries have more boys to place than girls, and many more adoptive families wish 28 The Contexts of Development to adopt girls than boys (source “How to adopt internationally”). In the US, 52% of “waiting children” are male, and 50% of the adoptions were for male children, a much more even statistic than that found in other countries. Discuss issues of child’s sex and how it relates to child development, culture, and world events. 2. Multiculturalism: “A light-skinned African American girl comes home from preschool in tears because her Latino playmate, who has approximately the same skin tone, says he won't marry her because she is too dark. During play time, she refuses to play with dark-skinned dolls.” “A Latina mom drops her daughter off at preschool in a formal dress and patent leather shoes, hair bedecked with ribbons. The teacher, who had planned a painting project, says, ‘Gosh, can't you just put on some jeans?’” “At preschool Sarah says to Ng, ‘You're stupid. You don't know how to talk.’ Ng's home language is Vietnamese.” Have students consider how to prepare ourselves and our children for a multicultural world. Read and comment upon the article at http://www.4children.org/news/999divrs.htm. • There are more than 700,000 interracial married couples in the United States and an estimated hundreds of thousands to almost five million interracial or multicultural children. Have students comment on these statistics. How does this impact the portrait of America? How does this impact the educational system? In addition, have students explore the article on “Teaching with a Multicultural Perspective” at http://www.ericfacility.net/ericdigests/ed339548.html. Discuss the points in class. 3. Gay and Lesbian Parenting: Researchers estimate that the total number of children nationwide living with at least one gay parent ranges from six to 14 million. “One belief that often underlies both judicial decision-making in custody litigation and public policies governing foster care and adoption has been the belief that lesbians and gay men are not fit to be parents. In particular, courts have sometimes assumed that gay men and lesbians are mentally ill, that lesbians are less maternal than heterosexual women, and that lesbians' and gay men's relationships with sexual partners leave little time for ongoing parent-child interactions.” This is a quote from an article at the APA web site, http://www.apa.org/pi/parent.html, that reviews literature about lesbian and gay parenting. Have students read this article, and form groups to comment on their own beliefs and stereotypes, and how these matched/mismatched the results from investigations. Additional points for this topic are available at the ACLU’s Web site http://www.aclu.org/LesbianGayRights/LesbianGayRights.cfm?ID=9212&c=104. 4. Book Review: Have students explore issues of how poverty affects child development by summarizing points from Jonathon Kozol’s 1995 book titled Amazing Grace: The Lives of Children and the Conscience of a Nation about poor families living in the South Bronx. Poignant tales of the inequities of poverty in health care and education are particularly valuable for students to identify. It is a good book for exploration of Bronfenbrenner’s model. 5. Video: In class, have students watch the video called Life’s Lessons from the Childhood Series, available from Ambrose Video. This is one of seven videos about child development in families living in several countries, including Japan, Russia, and the US. It is a great look at the individual, the family, the social context, and the cultural context. This particular video is about children between ages five to seven. Have students, in groups, explore the various rings of Bronfenbrenner’s model to see how they can be applied to the families and the individual children that are depicted in the video. 6. Cultural Impacts: Have students generate examples of how cultures impact one another. The U.S. infiltrates many cultures worldwide by means of television and cinema. A recent example of how strong of an impact U.S. television images can have on other cultures comes from research by Anne Becker, director of research at the Harvard Eating Disorders Center of the Harvard Medical School. Dr. Becker and colleagues researched body image and prevalence of eating disorders among people of Viti Levu of Fiji where a well-rounded body is the norm for men and women. The study began in 1995, one month after satellite television with U.S. programs became available to the islanders. At this time, only 3% of the sample of high school girls (average age 17) reported inducing vomiting to control weight. In 1998, the number rose to 15%, and 29% scored highly on a test of eating disorder 29 Chapter 2 risk compared to 13% in 1995. In 1998, those who watched television three or more nights per week were 50% more likely to describe themselves as “too big or fat” and 30% more likely to diet than girls who watched television less frequently. Have students comment on the implications of this research for standards of beauty and how these standards may foster development of attitudes toward bodies. 7. Working Moms: Have students list ways that maternal employment outside of the home can alter the context of young children's development. In particular, have students list the practical changes it produces in the ecology of early childhood at all three levels of Bronfenbrenner's model. 8. Bias in Research: It has been remarked that the phrase "of white, middle-class, American children" should be added to the titles of most of the research articles published in the field of child development. Have students consider what kinds of bias a reliance on research produced in one cultural setting can introduce into our ideas about what children are like and how they develop. Are there facts about the course of child development that we are likely to get wrong as a result? Are there factors whose importance we are likely to overestimate? 9. Family Changes: Have students generate examples of how family changes can result from social and economic changes in a country. The text has discussions of the impact of maternal employment on family change. Many examples are available from history (e.g., historical notes of family disintegration from the Great Depression are numerous). However, recently we have had opportunity to see these impacts from the economic fall of Russia where we have heard tales of educators and other workers in Russia being paid only in vodka as the government has no money to offer its employees (the alcoholism rate is 15% in Russia). In a Life magazine article (July 1999), a grim picture was painted of the lives of many children, where it is estimated that two million children have been abandoned by their parents. More than 80 % of these children become criminals. The orphanages are bankrupt and to most Russians these children are simply known as young wolves (volchata). Interestingly, as discussed in the article, some Russian Army officers have been chipping in funds from their own meager wages to set up units of “youngers”, orphaned children between the ages of 11 and 18 who are enlisted as soldiers. These children live in the barracks and have combat training. How do your students feel about this situation? The children are fed regular meals and have to attend school, care for livestock, and take training classes. They are not roaming the street. They have structure and are cared for by the older soldiers. In many ways, the boys are quite fortunate to have ended up as “youngers”. Have students discuss their thoughts on child soldiers (it is estimated that 300,000 child soldiers exist in the world) and the plight of children in Russia after social and economic upheaval. 10. Activity Sheet 1: At the end of this chapter is an activity sheet that could be copied for students to work on in groups or individually. The goal of this activity sheet is to have students apply Bronfenbrenner’s model to the characters in the family vignettes (Williams, Gordon, and Polonious families) presented in this text, particularly to the infants who are about to be born into these families. On the activity sheet, students will be asked to (a) note what each of Bronfenbrenner's contexts may be like for each baby; (b) suggest how each baby's development may be affected by the contexts he or she will be born into, drawing wherever possible on research discussed in Chapter 2. Have students bring their responses to discuss in class. 11. Activity Sheet 2: Walton (1987) describes an exercise designed to encourage students to think about the ways values affect the application of developmental research to practical problems. Use the exercise in Activity Sheet 2 as a way of summing up at the end of a term or reinforcing the importance of context in children's development with Bronfenbrenner's notions of immediate environment and social/economic context. The exercise requires class time on two different days. On day 1, students choose two statements from two sets of alternatives. They turn their choices in, signed. Before the next class, divide the class into groups (four minimum) made up of those who gave the same answers to the two questions. On day 2, each group should spend class time deciding on a group answer to the following: (1) What five factors in the child's immediate environment are the most necessary for optimal human development? (2) What five factors in the social and economic context are the most necessary for optimal human development? When they have finished, collect the lists and compare the responses of the groups. Groups will generally emphasize different aspects of the 30 The Contexts of Development environment, leading to interesting discussions of how values influence perceptions of developmental influences and developmental research. [Adapted from Walton, M. D. (1987). Science and values: Addressing practical issues in developmental psychology. Teaching of Psychology, 14, 50-51.] Films, Videos, and Internet Resources Films and Videos: • • • • • • • • • • • • • • Culture, Time, and Place (1992, 30 min., Insight Media). Shows how language, school, and relationships bring about acculturation about attitudes, values, and beliefs. Development and Diversity (1992, 30 min., Insight Media). Explores historical and cultural definitions of childhood. Probes the prolongation of infancy and childhood. Looks at children in many countries. The Ecology of Development (1992, 30 min., Insight Media). Profiles children of 12 families in five countries to illustrate the influence of family, peers, school, culture, and history on development. Defines the terms of Bronfenbrenner’s ecological theory. Family Patterns, Program 2 (1989, 17 min.). Describes family types, formal and informal roles, and dysfunctional aspects of family patterns. This program is slides on video. Family Therapy Theories, Program 3 (1989, 23 min.). Discusses the origin of family theory and family therapy, including models of Bowen, Satir, and Minuchin. The House of Tomorrow (1991, 57 min., Ambrose Video). Childhood Series - Features the ways various cultures celebrate the passage from childhood to adolescence. Includes Baka boys from Camaroon and a young girl in Brazil. Life's Lessons (1991, 57 min., Ambrose Video). Childhood Series - Shows children of different cultures between the ages of 5 and 7 as they shift to new abilities and behaviors. Features children starting school from Moscow, Japan, and America. Love's Labors (1991, 57 min., Ambrose Video). Childhood Series - Explores the developmental period of children in different cultures between the ages of 6 months and 3 years. Myths of Childhood: New Perspectives on Nature and Nurture (1997; 55 minutes each in a 3 part series; Films for the Humanities and Sciences). This provocative three-part series confronts the notion that children are naturally innocent, asks why some adults blame childhood events for their adult unhappiness, and questions the ambitions of parents who strive to raise the perfect child. Children and parents from the U.S., Australia, and the United Kingdom share their experiences. Home movies, photographs, and scenes from television and films portray the mythic childhoods we imagine, while best-selling authors discuss changing ideas about childhood and the raising of children. The series includes: Innocence: What Is a Child?, Damage: The Effects of a Troubled Childhood, Perfection: The Pressure on Parents and Children. Not All Parents Are Straight (1987, 58 min.). Examines the dynamics of the parent-child relationship within several different households where children are being raised by gay and lesbian parents. Preschool in Three Cultures (1989, 55 min., Yale University Films). Shows a typical day in a preschool in Japan, China, and the United States, with evaluations by Japanese, Chinese, and American teachers of their own and each other's schools. Secret of the Wild Child (1994, 60 min., Insight Media). This NOVA program profiles Genie, a girl whose parents kept her imprisoned in near-total isolation from infancy. It includes footage of Genie during her rehabilitation, exploring how and when people learn the skills that make them “human.” 7up in the Soviet Union (1989, 70 min., Shanachie Entertainment). Part of the 7up Around the World project--where children from many countries are revisited every seven years to document their growth and development. 7up in the Soviet Union presents an intimate portrait of 20 children. 7up in South Africa (1993, 80 min., Shanachie Entertainment). Part of the 7up Around the World project. Shows 19 children responding to basic questions regarding their present lives and future aspirations. 31 Chapter 2 • Shooting Back (1990, 26 min.). Documents the lives of homeless children in shelters in the Washington, DC, area. Provides a comprehensive look at shelter life through the eyes of the children. Discusses feelings and self-esteem issues. A-V Resource List Information: • List of providers for most of the videos listed above: 1. Ambrose Video at www.ambrosevideo.com or 800-526-4663 2. Davidson Films at www.davidsonfilms.com or 888-437-4200. 3. Films for the Humanities and Sciences at www.films.com or 800-257-5126. 4. Insight Media at www.insight-media.com or 212-721-6316. 5. Public Broadcasting Service at 1-800-949-8670 or www.shop.pbs.org. 6. Pyramid Film and Video at 213-828-7577. 7. Shanachie Entertainment at 201-579-7763. Additional Internet Resource Options for Chapter 2: http://www.mhhe.com/dehart5 A variety of teaching tools for this textbook are available from the Web site for McGraw-Hill: http://www.apa.org American Psychological Association – main site as the lead organization in psychology. This site is excellent for background material on historical and current issues in developmental psychology http://quiz.ivillage.com/home/html/ Online Psychological Web site of links to a variety of topics on child development and particularly on issues relative to family, health, and well-being. http://www.clearinghouse.net/ This site provides a juncture to issues relative to schooling for children K –12. By accessing this site, it will provide links to other sites relative to your search. It is an effective tool to shorten the search process. http://www.nacd.org/ National Academy for Child Development – private site doing research on developing learning skills. http://www.childcare-ppin.com/links.htm This site gives the state-by-state license requirements for day care centers. http://www.naeyc.org/ This is the main site for the National Association for the Education of Young Children. The site provides outlines to some of their journals as well as links to other sites on child development. Membership is required for complete article downloads. http://www.zerotothree.org An excellent site providing a myriad of links to dozens of topics relative to children aged birth to three years. Among some topics are: Sudden Death Syndrome, child abuse, cultural diversity, drug abuse, and brain development. Many of these sites are parents’ information lists. 32 The Contexts of Development Name: _________________________________ Activity Sheet 1 – Applying Bronfenbrenner’s Model Re-read the three families vignettes. For each of the families portrayed, consider how the components in each ring of Bronfenbrenner’s model may impact the babies of the Williams, Gordon, and Polonious families. Briefly write your answers in the space provided. Be prepared to discuss your responses in class. Baby Williams: Important factors to consider in your analysis: parents are heterosexual, married, living in the same home; two-wage earners; extended family involvement; emotional support of family members; two older siblings; older parents; urban environment; financial concerns; African American; male; physically strong baby; active infant. • Child’s biological make-up • The family context (family as a system, fathers in the family, siblings in the family) • Day-care setting; the neighborhood • Social and economic context (maternal employment and its effects, SES) Baby Gordon: Important factors to consider: parents are heterosexual, married, living in the same home; father is main wage earner; mother wishes to have greater employment options; extended family involvement; conflict between parents; female sibling a few years older; male; slow to warm-up to new situations; father encourages “all boy” behavior; working-class SES. • Child’s biological make-up • The family context (family as a system, fathers in the family, siblings in the family) • Day-care setting • Social and economic context (maternal employment and its effects, SES) Baby Polonius: Important factors to consider: single, adolescent mother; no contact with father, limited emotional support; mother feels isolated; female; difficult infant with feeding problems; large financial concerns; family patterns of unplanned and early pregnancies; mother feels insecure about parenting. • Child’s biological make-up • The family context (family as a system, fathers in the family, siblings in the family) • Day-care setting • Social and economic context (maternal employment and its effects, SES) 33 Chapter 2 Name: _________________________________ Activity Sheet 2: How Values Influence Perceptions of Child Development? Day 1 Circle one alternative or the other for the two pairs of statements below. • • "Which is more important: the development of individual uniqueness or the development of the ability to relate to and get along with others?" "Which is more important: the development of emotional security or the development of cognitive and intellectual skills?" Day 2 (Your Group # _________) In your groups (as determined by your instructor) discuss the following and generate the lists that you are asked to consider. As a group, you should decide on a group answer to the following two questions: • What five factors in the child's immediate environment are the most necessary for optimal human development? 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. • What five factors in the social and economic context are the most necessary for optimal human development? 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 34