Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 1 Running head: REPETITIVE SELF-MUTILATION Media and Other Social Influences on Adolescent Repetitive Self-Mutilation Laura E. Peek, Karen O’Brien Lightner, and Amy R. Murrell University of North Texas, Laura S. Howe-Martin University of Texas Southwestern Medical School Online Publication Date: July 10, 2010 Journal of Media Psychology, Volume 15, No. 3, Summer, 2010 Abstract The rise of adolescent self-harm behaviors, specifically repetitive self-mutilation, has become a major point of interest to both researchers and treatment providers in recent years. Repetitive self-mutilation is discussed in terms of its function, with an emphasis on social function. A review of literature discussing various influences on this behavior is explored, with consideration of both the Experiential Avoidance Model (Chapman, Gratz, & Brown, 2006) and the Four Factor Model (Nock & Prinstein, 2004) of self-harm behavior. Additionally, the role of the media, a social force which has increased the availability of knowledge on self-harm behaviors, is examined, and potential impacts of publicity on this subject are discussed. A brief review of findings from a recent study on adolescent self-mutilation is introduced as a source of empirical support for the impact of social models on self-mutilation in this age group. Future Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 2 research ideas are proposed, as little empirical research that examines the effects of the media on repeated self-mutilation has been conducted Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 3 The prevalence of self-harm behaviors among adolescents is on the rise (Gratz, Conrad, & Roemer, 2002; Lloyd, 1997), increasing almost 10% in recent years (Olfson, Gameroff, Marcus, Greenberg, & Shaffer, 2005). It is now estimated that as many as 2.1 million adolescents engage in self-harm behavior each year (Miller & Smith, 2008). Even more alarming, this behavior is not limited to inpatient and outpatient mental health settings (Howe-Martin, 2007). Self-harm behavior spans a number of contexts and can be found among individuals admitted to general hospitals (Hawton et al., 2003; Olfson et al., 2005) as well as among those in the broader community (Briere & Gil, 1998; Favazza, DeRosear, & Conterio, 1989; Gratz, 2001; Gratz et al., 2002; Hawton, Rodham, Evans, & Weatherall, 2002; Ross & Heath, 2002). In addition, selfharm has been portrayed in various forms of media (Baker & Fortune, 2008; Howe-Martin & Murrell, 2007; Whitlock, Powers, & Eckenrode, 2006). Before proceeding any further, it is important to define the particular self-harm behavior of interest to this article. The term “self-harm” has been broadly used to define self-destructive acts ranging from risk-taking behaviors to suicide (Skegg, 2005). However, the behavior of interest in this article is more specific and better captured by the term “repetitive self-mutilation” (RSM). Although some have argued the term “self-mutilation” is more stigmatizing (Gratz, 2001), this term captures several key aspects of the specific behavior in question (Pattison & Kahan, 1983; Suyemoto, 1998). First and foremost, RSM is repetitive and involves conscious intent to directly cause mild to moderate physical injury. RSM as defined in this study is not associated with developmental impairments (e.g., Lesch-Nyan syndrome) or the type of major tissue damage associated with intense psychotic episodes. In addition, although RSM has also been used synonymously with “parasuicide” (Linehan, 1993), the latter term can be misleading, as RSM behavior by definition lacks suicidal intent. However, a history of engaging in this Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 4 behavior is correlated with suicidal ideation and heightened suicide risk (Andover, Pepper, Ryabchenko, Orrico, & Gibb, 2005; Cyr, McDuff, Wright, Theriault, & Cinq-Mars, 2005; Darche, 1990; Langbehn & Pfohl, 1993; Lloyd, 1997; Nixon, Cloutier, & Aggarwal, 2002) and should be taken seriously by treatment providers and other healthcare professionals, as well as by parents, teachers, and peers of adolescents who engage in this behavior. In order to establish effective behavioral intervention strategies, the functions of RSM must first be understood. One approach to this issue is the Experiential Avoidance Model (EAM; Chapman, Gratz, & Brown, 2006), which proposes that RSM belongs to a set of experientially avoidant behaviors that serve the similar function of allowing escape or avoidance from unwanted inner experiences, such as thoughts, feelings, or bodily sensations. Indirect support for this theoretical model derives from studies that indicate RSM and experiential avoidance (EA) share a number of correlates, including alexithymia (Zlotnick et al., 1997), psychological impairment (Darche, 1990), borderline personality disorder (Chapman, Specht, & Cellucci, 2005; Kwawer, 1980; Leibenluft, Gardner, & Cowdry, 1987; Sansone, Gaither, Songer, & Allen, 2005; Shearer, 1994), depression (Ghaziuddin, Tsai, Naylor, & Ghaziuddin, 1992; Nixon et al., 2002), suicidal ideation (Andover et al., 2005; Cyr et al., 2005; Darche, 1990; Nixon et al., 2002), anxiety (Andover et al., 2005; Favazza, 1996; Langbehn & Pfohl, 1993; Ross & McKay, 1979; Strong, 1998; Turp, 2003), obsessive compulsive-disorder (Langbehn & Pfohl, 1993), posttraumatic stress disorder (Zlotnick et al., 1997; Shearer, 1994; Cyr et al., 2005), dissociation (Skegg, 2005; Turell & Armsworth, 2000; Zlotnick et al., 1996), eating disorders (Claes, Vandereycken, & Vertommen, 2005; Davis & Karvinen, 2002; Dulit, Fyer, Leon, Brodsky, & Frances, 1994; Favaro & Santonastaso, 1999, 2000; Favazza & Conterio, 1988), and substance abuse (Langbehn & Pfohl, 1993; Nixon et al., 2002; Parker et al., 2005; Romans, Martin, Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 5 Anderson, Herbison, & Mullen, 1995; Shearer, 1994). Additionally, more direct empirical research lends support to the specific underlying tenets of the EAM (Chapman et al., 2006; Howe-Martin, Adcock, Murrell, & Guarnaccia, 2008; Rosenthal, Cheavens, Lejuez, & Lynch, 2005) and clearly indicates that experiential avoidance is involved at some level. Still, the rising pervasiveness of RSM across settings, as well as findings supporting the role of peer and media influences in RSM, suggest that this behavior serves functions in addition to experiential avoidance. The Importance of Social Environment Peers, Contagion, and RSM Social acceptability, competition, and peer influence appear to be important components in the instigation and repetition of adolescent self-mutilation. In general, adolescent behavior is greatly influenced by the perceived degree of similarity between personal and peers’ symptoms, behaviors, or attitudes (Heilbron & Prinstein, 2008). Kandel (1978a) explained this influence as selection and socialization effects. Adolescents want to be around similar others, and how these “similar others” act influences those around them. This influence can occur within dyadic relationships (Simon, Aikins, & Prinstein, 2008; Urberg, Luo, Pilgrim, & Degirmencioglu, 2003) and even extend to the relationship between grade mates (La Greca, Prinstein, & Fetter, 2001). Therefore, acquaintances of adolescents who self-mutilate may be influenced to do the same in order to gain social acceptance within their peer group. This influence may be exacerbated by EA, as the need to avoid feelings of not fitting during adolescence is common. It appears, though, that there is something unique to be gained, as opposed to simply gotten rid of, by RSM in peer interactions. Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 6 Numerous studies have documented self-harm contagion in inpatient settings, in which one incident of self-harm was associated with another, even without any prior history of selfmutilating behaviors (see Heilbron and Prinstein, 2008, for review). Early descriptions of institutionalized self-cutters indicate that such individuals may cluster as a social group and form a strong, long-lasting group identification as “cutters.” As early as the 1960s, Grunebaum and Klerman (1967) referred to “co-conspiratorial dyads” between two individuals who treated wrist cutting as a form of bonding, as well as a form of competition. There may be competition to be the “chief cutter” and to prove oneself as the most unhappy person in an inpatient treatment ward (Graff & Mallin, 1967; Grunebaum & Klerman, 1967; Rosen & Walsh, 1989). These findings are consistent with more recent observations that self-mutilative acts are often “clustered” in time, indicating that RSM behavior in others may trigger repetition of selfharm among individuals who already do it, or even trigger an initiation of self-mutilation among previously naïve individuals (Taiminen, Kallio-Soukainen, Nokso-Koivisto, Kaljonen, & Kelenius, 1998; Walsh & Rosen, 1985). Individuals who do not engage in such behavior before inpatient admission may first learn this behavior from fellow patients. Among a small sample of inpatient adolescents, approximately half of the patients began to self-cut for the first time while in the hospital (Ghaziuddin et al., 1992). Gardner and Gardner (1975) reported a similar finding, in that 14 of 22 non-psychotic female self-cutters in their study engaged in their first episode of self cutting after inpatient admission. This behavior is evident beyond institutionalized and inpatient youth. Anecdotal accounts by school counselors, teachers, and youth ministers who work regularly with adolescents indicate that RSM is becoming more common among middle and high school students. These students often do not demonstrate any other form of pathology, and the behavior is often instigated by a Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 7 “ringleader” and/or a small, close-knit group of peers. When the leader or one member of the group stops engaging in RSM, moves away, or graduates, this behavior in the others appears to dissipate (Brand, personal communication, August 2006; Hamilton, personal communication, June 2006; Martin, personal communication, January 2006). Empirical evidence supports these anecdotal accounts. Hawton and colleagues (2002) reported that awareness of self-harming peers was a strong risk factor for RSM in their schoolaged sample. Nock and Prinstein (2005) also found that a large proportion of adolescents in their sample had friends who engaged in self-mutilation. In addition, the number of RSM incidents among friends was associated with self-reported positive reinforcement aspects of RSM behavior (e.g., engaging in RSM to feel like part of a group; see also Nock & Prinstein, 2004). Ross and McKay (1979) observed that 86% of the girls in an adolescent correctional facility carved words, letters, or insignia on their skin to indicate group membership and control. Ross and McKay (1979) also commented on the “carving parties” held at the Grandview School for delinquent adolescent girls, and Favazza (1996) reported recent interactions with a teenager who belonged to a “cut of the month” club (p. 240). Therefore, given that self-mutilation is influenced by social factors, group process variables should be emphasized in any understanding of RSM (Walsh & Rosen, 1988). The Four Factor Model Acceptance and belonging are not the only social functions of RSM. The four factor model (FFM; Nock & Prinstein, 2004, 2005; Nock, 2008) of non-suicidal self-injury helps explain repeated self-mutilation in terms of automatic versus social, as well as positive versus negative reinforcement, and presents RSM as a means of social communication. Nock (2008) elaborated on the social functions piece of the four factor model by first proposing that RSM can Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 8 be understood as either an effort to communicate distress or as an effort to communicate strength and fitness. In both cases, these efforts to communicate can be either positively or negatively reinforced by the social environment. For example, if an individual receives positive reinforcement for crying (e.g., attention and soothing), then more intense distress signals, such as RSM, might result in even more positive reinforcement. Self-mutilation can therefore be used to influence other’s behavior (Nock & Prinstein, 2004). Alternately, negative reinforcement (e.g., escape from demands) can also serve to maintain RSM. RSM may also be used as an effort to communicate strength or fitness. Signals of strength and fitness, like athletic performance, can lead to behaviors, such as punching walls, in which people place themselves in harm’s way. Likewise, tattooing can lead to self-mutilation in order to produce “battle scars” (Nock, 2008). Though Nock (2008) asserts that language is generally a better form of communication than overt behavior, the opposite may be true for some adolescents. That is, adolescents may revert to self-mutilation in order to communicate via overt behavior, because they have difficulty communicating with words or their language fails altogether (Nock, 2008). In addition, adolescents who self-mutilate are more likely to have issues with problem-solving abilities. Instead, they choose to use more harmful or maladaptive social responses in comparison to those who do not self-mutilate (Nock & Mendes, 2008). Even though research has demonstrated that social functions might explain only a minority of self-mutilation cases (Brown, Comtois, & Linehan, 2002; Nock & Prinstein, 2004, 2005; Rodham, Hawton, & Evans, 2004), Nock (2008) points out that this paucity of evidence is likely influenced by adolescent social desirability. Certainly, reporting that one engages in RSM out of a desire to influence others sounds less socially acceptable than self-mutilation for affect regulation. Nock also proposed that those who self-mutilate may not want to accept the fact that Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 9 a social function is the motivation behind their behavior. This would be a sign of weakness that is not often accepted in Western cultures and would imply dependency (Nock, 2008). Media Influences on RSM The four factor model and socialization function of repeated self-mutilation lead one to wonder what effect various media sources have on this increasingly prevalent issue. Media influences may play a role in the peer contagion effect of self-mutilation, for example when the subgroup of choice is found via the internet, in movies, or television shows. As previously mentioned, self-mutilation by one individual has been observed to influence other individuals to self-mutilate for the first time (Ghaziuddin et al., 1992; Rada & James, 1982; Raine, 1982; Rosen & Walsh, 1989; Taiminen et al., 1998; Walsh & Rosen, 1985). Thus, self-mutilation via peer contagion may be affected by the availability of the media portrayals and asynchronous communication in today’s society. Although the media is encouraged to downplay focus or sensationalism regarding suicidal deaths (Hawton & Williams, 2002), there is not a similar unspoken policy for RSM. Several “role models” within popular media have admitted to engaging in self-mutilating behaviors, including Princess Diana, Johnny Depp, Courtney Love, and Angelina Jolie (Favazza, 1996; Hooley, 2008; Howe-Martin, 2007). Books by Patricia McCormack (Cut, 2002), Emma Forest (Thin Skin, 2001), and Shelley Stoehr (Crosses, 1991) discuss the topic of repeated selfmutilation in books written for teens. Movies have also introduced adolescents to the concept of self-mutilation. Individuals in the movie Thirteen (London, Levy-Hinte, Hardwicke, & Reed, 2003) were displayed wearing wrist bands as a technique for covering up scars from cutting. Popular musicians, such as Avril Lavigne, have mimicked this trend. Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 10 This leaves one wondering what message is being sent to adolescents about their favorite musicians, actors, and similar-aged fictional peers. If they perceive themselves to be in the subgroup of a celebrity, one might expect socialization effects to occur, and the adolescents might potentially mimic these self-harming behaviors in order to fit in. Adolescents’ social scenes are changing as more asynchronous forms of communication are developed. They are exploring their identities online through instant messaging, e-mail, social networks, chat rooms, online forums, and video conferencing. Each new generation is exposed to different forms of communication, and the role of the internet in the self-harm trend has yet to be fully understood. The impacts of this effect on adolescents, as well as other effects, need to be assessed if our society wants to address self-mutilation. Despite the intention to educate the public and raise awareness regarding the negative aspects of RSM, such media inadvertently introduces the concept to adolescents and increases their exposure. The internet is one source that provides mixed messages regarding repetitive self-mutilation. Whitlock and colleagues (2006) brought to focus internet message boards and their use in self-injury discussions. Their study found that these message boards are a “powerful vehicle for bringing together self-injurious adolescents” and “may also expose vulnerable adolescents to a subculture in which self-injury is normalized and encouraged” (Whitlock et al., 2006, p. 415). One website, “Pro-Self-Injury” (http://razorblades.tribe.net; now dismantled), allowed the posting of pictures, experiences, techniques for self-mutilating, and ideas on how not to get caught (Howe-Martin, 2007). Though this site has been shut down, many internet forums that support self-injury still exist for adolescents to potentially frequent. Fischoff (2006) mentions another potential problem concerning violence on the internet, the third-person effect, which asserts that the viewer feels immune to the effects of the mass media. Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 11 Additionally, given that exposure to media portrayals of suicide may be a risk factor in adolescent suicide (Insel & Gould, 2008), exposure to RSM in the media may be a risk factor for adolescent RSM. As individuals who engage in repeated self-mutilation are more at-risk for engaging in separate suicidal behaviors (Howe-Martin, 2007; Langbehn & Pfohl, 1993; Lloyd, 1997), it is important to note the availability of information about suicide on the internet as well. Given the complex relationship between RSM and suicide (Rodham et al., 2004; Simpson & Porter, 1981; Walsh & Rosen, 1988), suicide websites may impact those who self-mutilate. Baker and Fortune (2008) found that users of internet websites on suicide viewed them as communities of understanding, empathy, sources of coping, and even friendship. The participants in their study felt socially valued as a result of participating in these websites and perceived having a broader support system. This is similar to the findings of Miller and Gergen (1998), who found suicide site users experienced validation, encouragement, and sympathy. The participants in the Baker and Fortune (2008) study viewed the “support” of their families and friends as “judgement,” but indicated internet contacts who also engaged in self-mutilation were supportive due to the existence of a shared identity. The implications of these Baker and Fortune (2008) findings are two-fold. If an adolescent feels “understood” and “supported” on a self-harm website, they will be willing to disclose more about their personal experiences, which leads to a greater feeling of unity (Vinogradov & Yalom, 1990). Secondly, the forming of this group may prevent the adolescent from seeking help elsewhere, which could potentially be hazardous. The individual may feel that their similar peers have all of the “answers” and that anyone else would not understand them. Interestingly, many of the participants in the Baker and Fortune (2008) study viewed the websites as therapy alternatives. Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 12 With the exception of a few recently published studies (e.g., Baker & Fortune, 2008; Whitlock et al., 2006), little empirical evidence exists regarding the impact of the media on repeated self-mutilation. If repeated self-mutilation can be spread through peer contagion “offline” (Taiminen et al., 1998; Walsh & Rosen, 1985), one might wonder if RSM or even suicidal behaviors could spread online as well. Existing research on media influences and adolescent socialization alludes to the fact that viewing information about self-mutilation on the internet and in other media forms or engaging in socially supportive websites could potentially increase the risk of engaging in these behaviors. The Importance of Examining Social Influences As noted, the Experiential Avoidance Model designates RSM as behavior whose function it is to help avoid painful private experiences. Some of this avoidance may relate to negative peer factors; however, the EAM does not specifically pinpoint the role of the social influence in perpetuating the cycle of RSM. Although researchers have evaluated the role of peer influence in self-injury among inpatients (e.g., Rosen & Walsh, 1989; Taimenan et al., 1998) and the roles of positive and negative reinforcement among adolescent psychiatric inpatients (Nock & Prinstein, 2004, 2005), there is very little research on peer influence in community samples or the impact of media on this behavior. Peer and Media Influences in Community Samples One exception is Whitlock and colleagues’ (2006) study on internet message boards. A second exception is Howe-Martin’s (2007) study, which aimed to fill this gap in the EAM by exploring the social aspect of reported self-mutilation among a community sample. A broad overview of the results of this study is provided, in efforts to lend empirical support for the information addressed in the literature; interested readers should consult the Howe-Martin paper Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 13 for more details. Adolescents (N = 211; ages 13 to 18), from three educationally diverse school environments, were administered anonymous surveys that measured exposure to self-mutilation by peers and through the media, as well as historic and current self-mutilation. A large percentage of these adolescents reported at least some exposure to self-mutilation via peers (44%) or media sources (85%). Adolescents with a history of repeated self-mutilation behavior reported significantly more of this exposure than their non-mutilating peers (Howe-Martin, 2007). These results follow with Nock and Prinstein’s (2004, 2005) conclusions that the number of self-injury incidents among friends was associated with a positive reinforcement function of self-mutilation behaviors. However, it should be noted that a majority of adolescents in the Howe-Martin (2007) study reported exposure to some form of self-mutilation among peers or in the media, but did not report engaging in repetitive self-mutilation (only about 16% of the total sample engaged in RSM, defined as self-harm 5 or more times). Thus, it appears that simple exposure to one or two sources was not enough to prompt repeated mimicry of the behavior. There were several findings from the Howe-Martin (2007) study alluding to the existence of a temporal relationship between exposure to self-mutilation among peers/media and RSM. First, there were no differences between male and female adolescents regarding the amount of self-mutilation peer/media contacts reported prior to initiation of self-mutilating behavior. However, there was a significant increase in self-mutilation peer/media contacts following initiation of self-mutilation behaviors for female adolescents. In addition, the severity of selfmutilating behaviors was positively correlated (r = .50, p < .01) with reported peer/media contacts after initiation of self-mutilation. The correlation was higher within the female subsample, indicating this effect as particularly strong for girls. Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 14 One potential interpretation of these findings is that peer/media contacts interact with self-mutilation in a causal feedback loop (Howe-Martin, 2007). For example, adolescents who engage in self-mutilation may be initially exposed to one or two poignant models of selfmutilation. Such contacts, coupled with psychological distress and/or practices of experiential avoidance, could prompt the initial self-mutilation episode. It is possible that adolescents who engage in an initial episode of self-mutilation may seek out further models and images of such behaviors. These models and images could take a variety of forms, depending on one’s context and preferences (e.g., internet images for adolescents who are more computer-oriented and/or introverted). If self-mutilation is supported within an inner social network and this behavior functions to temporarily provide avoidance of unwanted inner experiences, it would be expected that the behavior would then become repetitive. In addition, results that indicate a stronger trend for female adolescents may be because females are more susceptible, due to the tendency for females to solicit more social support than males (Fuhrer, Stansfield, Chemali, & Shipley, 1999; Whitlock et al., 2006). Suggestions for Further Research Clearly, more research is needed to gain a clearer picture of the functions of RSM with respect to social influence during adolescents. Empirical longitudinal tests of the causal feedback loop proposed by Howe-Martin (2007) are needed for a more precise understanding of the temporal relationship between the development of RSM, various peer/media influences, life events, and psychological distress. In addition, a greater understanding of the actual influence of self-mutilation images among peers and in the media on adolescents’ RSM, through the use of experimental paradigms, would provide the empirical support necessary to implement the public Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation policy changes necessary to better monitor the content and disclaimers related to portrayals of self-mutilation in various forms of media. 15 Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation Acknowledgements Funding was received from the University of North Texas Department of Psychology. The authors would like to thank the UNT Contextual Psychology Group for data collection, editing, and other support. 16 Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation References Andover, M. S., Pepper, C. M., Ryabchenko, K. A., Orrico, E. G., & Gibb, B. E. (2005). Selfmutilation and symptoms of depression, anxiety, and borderline personality disorder. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 35(5), 581-591. Baker, D., & Fortune, S. (2008). Understanding self-harm and suicide websites. Crisis, 29(3), 118-122. Brown, M. Z., Comtois, K. A., & Linehan, M. M. (2002). Reasons for suicide attempts and nonsuicidal self-injury in women with borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(1), 198–202. Chapman, A. L., Gratz, K. L., & Brown, M. Z. (2006). Solving the puzzle of deliberate selfharm: The Experiential Avoidance Model. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 371394. Chapman, A. L., Specht, M. W., & Cellucci, T. (2005). Borderline personality disorder and deliberate self-harm: Does experiential avoidance play a role? Suicide and LifeThreatening Behavior, 35(4), 388-399. Claes, L., Vandereycken, W., & Vertommen, H. (2005). Self-care versus self-harm: Piercing, tattooing, and self-injuring in eating disorders. European Eating Disorders Review, 13, 11-18. Cyr, M., McDuff, P., Wright, J., Theriault, C., & Cinq-Mars, C. (2005). Clinical correlates and repetition of self-harming behaviors among female adolescent victims of sexual abuse. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 14(2), 49-67. Darche, M. A. (1990). Psychological factors differentiating self-mutilation and non-selfmutilation adolescent inpatient females. Psychiatric Hospital, 21(1), 31-35. 17 Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 18 Davis, C., & Karvinen, K. (2002). Personality characteristics and intention to self-harm: A study of eating disordered patients. Eating Disorders, 10, 245-255. Dulit, R. A., Fyer, M. R., Leon, A. C., Brodsky, B. S., & Frances, A. J. (1994). Clinical correlates of self-mutilation in borderline personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151(9), 1305-1311. Favaro, A., & Santonastosa, P. (1999). Different types of self-injurious behavior in bulimia nervosa. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 40(1), 57-60. Favazza, A. R. (1996). Bodies Under Siege: Self-mutilation and Body Modification in Culture and Psychiatry (2nd ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. Favazza, A. R., & Conterio, K. (1988). The plight of the chronic self-mutilators. Community Mental Health Journal, 24(1), 22-30. Fischoff, S. (2006). Can we understand media and politics when the internet is silent? PsycCRITIQUES, 51(46). Retrieved September 2, 2009. Forrest, E. (2001). Thin Skin. New York: Pocket Books. Fuhrer, R., Stansfeld, S. A., Chemali, J., & Shipley, M. J. (1999). Gender, social relations and mental health: Prospective findings from an occupational cohort (Whitehall II study). Social Science and Medicine, 48(1), 77-87. Gardner, A. R., & Gardner, A. J. (1975). Self-mutilation, obsessionality and narcissism. British Journal of Psychiatry, 127, 127-132. Ghaziuddin, M., Tsai, L., Naylor, M., & Ghaziuddin, N. (1992). Mood disorder in a group of self-cutting adolescents. Acta Paedopsychiatrica, 55, 103-105. Graff, H., & Mallin, R. (1967). The syndrome of the wrist cutter. American Journal of Psychiatry, 124(1), 36-42. Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 19 Gratz, K. L. (2001). Measurement of deliberate self-harm: Preliminary data on the Deliberate Self-Harm Inventory. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 23(4), 253-263. Gratz, K. L., Conrad, S., & Roemer, L. (2002). Risk factors for deliberate self-harm among college students. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 72(1), 128-140. Grunebaum, H. U., & Klerman, G. L. (1967). Wrist slashing. American Journal of Psychiatry, 124(4), 113-120. Hawton, K., Harriss, L., Hall, S., Simkin, S., Bale, E., & Bond, A. (2003). Deliberate self-harm in Oxford, 1990-2000: A time of change in patient characteristics. Psychological Medicine, 33, 987-955. Hawton, K., Rodham, K., Evans, E., & Weatherall, R. (2002). Deliberate self harm in adolescents: Self report survey in schools in England. British Medical Journal, 325, 1207-1211. Hawton, K., & Williams, K. (2002). Influences of the media on suicide. British Medical Journal, 325, 1374-1375. Heilbron, N., & Prinstein, M. J. (2008). Peer influence and adolescent nonsuicidal self-injury: A theoretical review of mechanisms and moderators. Applied and Preventative Psychology, 12, 169-177. Hooley, J. M. (2008). Self-harming behavior: Introduction to the special series on nonsuicidal self-injury and suicide. Applied and Preventative Psychology, 12, 155-158. Howe-Martin, L. S. (2007). Adolescent Self-Mutilating Behaviors: Experiential Avoidance Coupled with Imitation? Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of North Texas, Denton. Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 20 Howe-Martin, L. S., Adcock, A., Murrell, A. R., & Guarnaccia, C. A. (May 2008). Adolescent self-mutilating behaviors: The role of experiential avoidance. Paper presented at the 34th Annual Convention of the Association for Behavior Analysis International, Chicago, IL. Howe-Martin, L. S., & Murrell, A. R., (August 2007). Adolescent self-mutilation: The role of media exposure. Poster presented at the 2007 Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, San Francisco, CA. Insel, B. J., & Gould, M. S. (2008). Impact of modeling on adolescent suicidal behavior. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 31(2), 293-316. Kandel, D. B. (1978a). Homophily, selection, and socialization in adolescent friendships. American Journal of Sociology, 84, 427–436. Kwawer, J. S. (1980). Some interpersonal aspects of self-mutilation in a borderline patient. Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis, 8(2), 203-216. Langbehn, D. R., & Pfohl, B. (1993). Clinical correlates of self-mutilation among psychiatric patients. Journal of Personality Disorders, 13(1), 75-89. La Greca, A. M., Prinstein, M. J., & Fetter, M. D. (2001). Adolescent peer crowd affiliation: Linkages with health-risk behaviors and close friendships. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 26, 131–143. Leibenluft, E., Gardner, D. L., & Cowdry, R. W. (1987). The inner experience of the borderline self-mutilator. Journal of Personality Disorders,1(4), 317-324. Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive-Behavioral Treatment of Borderline Personality Disorder. New York: Guilford. Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 21 Lloyd, E. E. (1997). Self-mutilation in a community sample of adolescents. (Doctoral dissertation, Louisiana State University, 1997). Dissertation Abstracts International, 58, 5127. London, M. (Producer), Levy-Hinte, J. (Producer), Hardwicke, C. (Director/Screenwriter), Reed, N. (Screenwriter). (2003). Thirteen [Motion picture]. United States: 20th Century Fox Home Entertainment. McCormack, P. (2000). Cut. New York: Scholastic. Miller, A. L., & Smith, H. L. (2008). Adolescent non-suicidal self-injurious behavior: The latest epidemic to assess and treat. Applied and Preventative Psychology, 12, 177- 188. Miller, J. K., & Gergen, K. J. (1998). Life on the line: The therapeutic potentials of computer mediated conversation. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 24(2), 189-202. Nixon, M. K., Cloutier, P. F., & Aggarwal, S. (2002). Affect regulation and addictive aspects of repetitive self-injury in hospitalized adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(11), 1333-1341. Nock, M. K. (2008). Actions speak louder than words: An elaborated theoretical model of the social functions of self-injury and other harmful behaviors. Applied and Preventative Psychology, 12, 159-168. Nock, M. K., & Mendes, W. B. (2008). Physiological arousal, distress tolerance, and social problem-solving deficits among adolescent self-injurers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76(1), 28-38. Nock, M. K., & Prinstein, M. J. (2004). A functional approach to the assessment of selfmutilative behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72(5), 885-890. Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 22 Nock, M. K., & Prinstein, M. J. (2005). Clinical features and behavioral functions of adolescent self-mutilation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(1), 140-146. Olfson, M., Gameroff, M. H., Marcus, S. C., Greenberg, T., & Shaffer, D. (2005). National trends in hospitalization of youth with intentional self-inflicted injuries. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(7), 1328-1335. Parker, G., Malhi, G., Mitchell, P., Kotze, B., Wilhelm, K., & Parker, K. (2005). Self-harming in depressed patients: Pattern analysis. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 39, 899-906. Pattison, E. M., & Kahan, J. (1983). The deliberate self-harm syndrome. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140(7), 867-872. Pro-Self-Injury. Retrieved August 1, 2006, from http://razorblades.tribe.net/. Rada, R. T., & James, W. (1982). Urethral insertion of foreign bodies: A report of contagious self-mutilation in a maximum-security hospital. Archives of General Psychiatry, 39(4), 423-429. Raine, W. J. (1982). Self-mutilation. Journal of Adolescence, 5(1), 1-13. Rodham, K., Hawton, K., & Evans, E. (2004). Reasons for deliberate self-harm: Comparison of self-poisoners and self-cutters in a community sample of adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(1), 80–87. Romans, S. E., Martin, J. L., Anderson, J. C., Herbison, G. P., & Mullen, P. E. (1995). Sexual abuse in childhood and deliberate self-harm. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 13361342. Rosen, P. M., & Walsh, B. W. (1989). Patterns of contagion in self-mutilation epidemics. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146(5), 656-658. Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 23 Rosenthal, M. Z., Cheavens, J. S., Lejuez, C. W., & Lynch, T. R. (2005). Thought suppression mediates the relationship between negative affect and borderline personality disorder symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(9), 1173-1185. Ross, R. R., & McKay, H. B. (1979). Self-Mutilation. Lexington, Massachusetts: Lexington Books. Ross, S., & Heath, N. (2002). A study of the frequency of self-mutilation in a community sample of adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 31(1), 67-77. Sansone, R. A., Gaither, G. A., Songer, D. A., & Allen, J. L. (2005). Multiple psychiatric diagnoses of self-harm behavior. International Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical Practice, 9(1), 41-44. Shearer, S. L. (1994). Phenomenology of self-injury among inpatient women with borderline personality disorder. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 182(9), 524-526. Simon, V. A., Aikins, J. W., & Prinstein, M. J. (2008). Romantic partner selection and socialization during early adolescence. Child Development, 79(6), 1676-1692. Simpson, C. A., & Porter, G. L. (1981). Self-mutilation in children and adolescents. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 45(5), 428-438. Skegg, K. (2005). Self-harm. Lancet, 366, 1471-1483. Stoehr, S. (1991). Crosses. New York: Delacorte Press. Strong, M. (1998). A Bright Red Scream: Self-Mutilation and the Language of Pain. New York: Viking. Suyemoto, K. L. (1998). The functions of self-mutilation. Clinical Psychology Review, 18(5), 531-554. Taiminen, T. J., Kallio-Soukainen, K., Nokso-Koivisto, H., Kaljonen, A., & Kelenius, H. (1998). Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation 24 Contagion of deliberate self-harm among adolescent inpatients. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 37(2), 211-217. Turell, S. C., & Armsworth, M. W. (2000). Differentiating incest survivors who self-mutilate. Child Abuse and Neglect, 24(2), 237-249. Turp, M. (2003). Hidden Self-Harm: Narratives from Psychotherapy. Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley. Urberg, K. A., Luo, Q., Pilgrim, C., & Degirmencioglu, S. M. (2003). A two stage model of peer influence in adolescent substance use: Individual and relationship-specific differences in susceptibility to influence. Addictive Behaviors, 28, 1243-1256. Vinogradov, S., & Yalom, I. D. (1990). Self-disclosure in group psychotherapy. In G. Stricker, & M. Fisher (Eds.), Self-disclosure in the therapeutic relationship (pp. 191204). New York: Plenum Press. Walsh, B. W., & Rosen, P. (1985). Self-mutilation and contagion: An empirical test. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142(1), 119-120. Walsh, B. W., & Rosen, P. M. (1988). Self-mutilation: Theory, Research, and Treatment. New York: Guilford. Whitlock, J. L., Powers, J. L., & Eckenrode, J. (2006). The virtual cutting edge: The internet and adolescent self-injury. Developmental Psychology, 42(3), 407-417. Zlotnick, C., Shea, M. T., Recupero, P., Bidadii, K., Pearlstein, T., & Brown, P. (1997). Trauma, dissociation, impulsivity, and self-mutilation among substance abuse patients. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 67(4), 650-654. Influences on Repetitive Self-Mutilation Laura E. Peeka, , Karen O’Brien Lightnera, and Amy R. Murrell a,c a University of North Texas, Department of Psychology, 1611 W. Mulberry, Terrill Hall Room 151, Denton, TX, USA 76203 Laura S. Howe-Martinb b University of Texas Southwestern Medical School, Department of Psychiatry, 5323 Harry Hines Blvd., Dallas, TX, USA 75390 c Corresponding Author: University of North Texas, Department of Psychology, 1611 W. Mulberry, Terrill Hall Room 358, Denton, TX, USA 76203, Phone: 001- 940-565-2967, Fax: (940) 565-4682, E-mail: amurrell@unt.edu 25