The Effects of Education and Direct Contact on Explicit and Implicit Attitudes Towards Older People A submission presented in partial fulfilment of the requirements of the University of Glamorgan/Prifysgol Morgannwg for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy April 2011 Paul Nash University of Glamorgan Acknowledgements ‘At times our own light goes out and is rekindled by a spark from another person. Each of us has cause to think with deep gratitude of those who have lighted the flame within us.’ (Albert Schweitzer – Theologian) As I am sure is the case for all PhD candidates, upon reflection a doctorate is just as much about the process as it is about the completed product. The journey to completion has encompassed both good times -that I will remember forever because they were aplenty, and not-so-good times- that I will remember forever because of the unwavering support I have received. This assiduous hike would have remained untrodden had it not been for the tremendous help and support I gratefully received from many individuals as highlighted below. Prof. Ian Stuart-Hamilton, my Director of Studies, deserves special thanks. Not only has he provided continued academic support and advice but he has always had the right words of encouragement (most of which could not be repeated), given when needed most. Dr Peter Mayer, as my Second Supervisor, has acted as mediator and voice of reason when Prof. Stuart-Hamilton and I have been swept away at tangents intangible to anyone else. Without him I fear this thesis may have reflected a somewhat chaotic mindset. As a friend, confidante and academic sounding board Dr Rachel Taylor has never failed to be there for me both at times when requested but more often than not without any request being needed. Without the support of these three fantastic individuals I would not have known where to start, where to finish or which path to take to get there. Thank you. 2 Data collected in this thesis has been the culmination of efforts from a number of people. Without Jill Kneath-Jones in the nursing school at the University of Glamorgan, my access to the Nursing students population would have been impossible. She has provided continued support for the duration of the longitudinal study and for this I am truly thankful. Thanks are also extended to the University of Glamorgan and the Aneurin Bevan Health Board who have facilitated data collection at different stages of the thesis. I would also like to extend my thanks to all of my participants without whose time, none of this would have been possible. In addition to the academic side of the PhD, my friends and family have provided support and understanding from the first days of proposal writing until the final edits before submission. I have a plethora of friends who have shown understanding when things have been hard for me. I cannot mention by name all of those who have supported me, however, special mention needs to be given to Katy Walden, Joe Draper and Dr Ian Pepper. Katy is a true ray of light, always there with a smile so infectious you can’t help but have your mood lightened by her. Joe has more recently been of support; functionally looking after me when I have been writing, ensuring that I have remained fed, watered and that a life exists outside the thesis. Ian has been there to boost my confidence and ensured that I take a realistic perspective on all things academic and personal. He has been my most ardent supporter and my fiercest critic, but above all else I know I can count on him. Thanks for keeping me sane! Foremost however, my family have been the one constant source of support from undergraduate through post graduate studies. Despite their geographical distance I 3 couldn’t feel closer to them. My Parents have provided financial and emotional support, never questioning my chosen path, simply ensuring they have been with me at every turn. My sister has always been close to me, knowing me better than many others and as such her understanding and guidance has been invaluable. Without my family I would not have been able to get this far and it is with their continued strength that I strive to go much further. I have mentioned but a few, however, my thanks extend to many others whom it would be impossible to name individually. Each and every one of these people has rekindled that spark within me in their own way and without that help this accomplishment would not have been half as sweet. 4 Table of Contents Page List of Tables …………………………………………………………………… 9 List of Figures …………………………………………………………………... 9 Chapter One Attitudes ………………………………………………………………… 10 Implicit Attitudes ………………………………………….......... 15 The Implicit Association Test ……………………………………........... 20 Predicting Behaviour …………………………………………… 28 Prejudice ………………………………………………………………... 34 Stereotypes ……………………………………………………... 40 Chapter Two Chapter Three Chapter Four Ageism …………………………………………………………………... 48 Chapter Five General Methodology …………………………………………………… 70 Materials ………………………………………………………… 70 Ethical Considerations ………………………………………….. 76 Procedure ………………………………………………….......... 77 Scoring …………………………………………………….......... 78 5 Chapter Six Empirical Studies ……………………………………………………….. 81 A Longitudinal Cohort Study with Nursing and Psychology Undergraduate Students Introduction …………………………………………….. 84 Participants ……………………………………………… 86 Methodology ……………………………………………. 87 Results …………………………………………………... 88 Discussion ………………………………………………. 92 Conclusions ……………………………………………... 100 A Cross-sectional Study Using Early Years Students to Ascertain Directional Preference in the IAT Introduction ……………………………………………... 103 Participants …………………………………………….... 105 Methodology ……………………………………………. 105 Results …………………………………………………... 105 Discussion ………………………………………………. 106 Conclusions ……………………………………………... 110 A Cross-sectional Study Assessing Attitudes Held by Hospital Nurses working in A&E and Geriatric Medicine Introduction ……………………………………………... 111 Participants ……………………………………………… 114 Methodology ……………………………………………. 114 Results …………………………………………………... 114 Discussion ………………………………………………. 116 Conclusions ……………………………………………... 119 6 A Cross-sectional Study Assessing the Effects of Higher Level Education on Implicit and Explicit Attitudes Towards Older People Introduction ……………………………………………... 121 Participants ……………………………………………… 122 Methodology ……………………………………………. 123 Results …………………………………………………... 123 Discussion ………………………………………………. 125 Conclusions ……………………………………………... 128 A Longitudinal Study Assessing the Effects of Specific Age Education on Attitudes Towards Older People Introduction ……………………………………………... 130 Participants ……………………………………………… 132 Methodology ……………………………………………. 132 Results …………………………………………………... 133 Discussion ………………………………………………. 135 Conclusions ……………………………………………... 141 A Cross-sectional Study Assessing the Levels of Implicit and Explicit Ageism Held by Older People Introduction ……………………………………………... 142 Participants ……………………………………………… 145 Methodology ……………………………………………. 145 Results …………………………………………………... 146 Discussion ………………………………………………. 147 Conclusions ……………………………………………... 150 7 A Meta-analysis of Study One-Six Data Introduction ……………………………………………... 152 Participants ……………………………………………… 153 Methodology ……………………………………………. 153 Results …………………………………………………... 154 Test of Counterbalancing ……………………………….. 159 Implicit vs Explicit Measures …………………………... 160 Discussion ………………………………………………. 161 Conclusions ……………………………………………... 168 Chapter Seven General Discussion ……………………………………………………… 170 General Implicit Ageism ………………………………………... 172 General Explicit Ageism ………………………………………... 175 Effects of Education on Attitudes Towards Older People …….... 177 Effects of Direct Contact on Attitudes Towards Older People …. 183 Test Stability Over Time ………………………………………... 184 Future Research Directions ……………………………………… 188 References ………………………………………………………………………. 193 Appendices ……………………………………………………………………… 229 8 List of Tables Table 1 - A tabular depiction of the IAT programme …………………… 19 Table 3 - The order of presentation for the IAT ………………………… 75 Table 4 - Table of longitudinal means …………………………………... 88 Table 5 - Table of Nursing students explicit measure correlations ……... 91 Table 6 - Table of Psychology students explicit measure correlations ….. 91 Table 7 - Table of Nursing students implicit measure correlations ……... 92 Table 8 - Table of Psychology students implicit measure correlations …. 92 Table 9 - Table of comparable means for Psychology and Nursing Students …………………………………………………………. Table 10 - Table of comparable means for Qualified Nurses and Nursing Students ………………………………………………... Table 11 106 115 - Table of aggregated comparable means for Qualified Nurses and Nursing Students ……………………………………. 116 Table 12 - Table of means comparing the effects of education …………... Table 13 - Table of means comparing Psychology of Ageing course and General Adult data sets …………………………………………. Table 14 124 133 - Table of means comparing the effects of ageing on attitudes towards older people ……………………………………………. 146 Table 15 - Table of means for each sample population …………………... 154 Table 16 - Table of significance values between samples for the implicit D-Score Measure ………………………………………………... 155 Table 17 - Table of significance values between samples for the FSA Explicit Measure ………………………………………………... Table 18 156 - Table of significance values between samples for the non-normed implicit scores ……………………………………... 158 Fig 1 - A screenshot of the congruent condition IAT ………………… 22 Fig 2 - Graph illustrating explicit longitudinal data …………………... 89 Fig 3 - Graph illustrating implicit longitudinal data …………………... 90 List of Figures 9 Chapter One Attitudes An attitude may be defined as an internal affective orientation explaining an individual’s action (Reber, 1995). An attitude is constructed of four components; cognitive, affective, evaluative, and conative. The cognitive component refers to the opinions / schema held about an object. The affective component refers to the emotion or salience towards the attitude object. The evaluative component refers to the direction of the feeling, whether the object evokes a positive or negative emotion. Finally, the conative component of the attitude is the disposition for action (Maio, Esses & Bell, 2000). It is the combination of these components that determines the attitude held by an individual. There are also several characteristics that define an attitude within the above framework. First, an attitude is learned. Attitudes can be learned in many ways, the most prominent being personal experience, observation of salient others and societal influence. Each of the aforementioned methods exposes the individual to attitudes and information about the attitude object, which they then appraise within their own belief systems (and those held within society and salient in-groups) to form and update their own opinions and attitudes (Ruys & Stapel, 2009). Second, attitudes are predispositions. Attitudes are inclinations and tendencies for action, thus, an attitude and its direction contain motivational qualities. If the attitude is such that the salience towards the attitude object is high and the object is encountered, then be it negative or positive, as long as the conative response is satisfied, an individual will act upon their 10 held beliefs. However, this is a predisposition and the association with behaviour is not a causal one (Ruys & Stapel, 2009). Third, attitudes are consistent. This does not mean that attitudes cannot be changed (discussed within the Ageism chapter, pp.48), just that they have a consistency in expression and measurement over time and across contexts. The way in which the attitudes are expressed may however change, dependent on the social situation and on the pervasive attitudes of any salient others in the same social setting1 (Krosnick, 1988). Finally, attitudes are directed towards an object that is referred to as the attitude object. This does not mean to say that an attitude is formed only with respect to a tangible item, but can also be a characteristic i.e. attitude objects can be physical like cars or non-physical like sarcasm. There are several theories of attitude formation, but two have become prominent. These are the summation (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1974) and averaged (Anderson, 1971) models. The summation approach argues that an attitude is the sum of evaluations associated with salient outcomes of observed behaviours (Betsch, Kaufmann, Lindow, Plessner, & Hoffman, 2006). Thus, the attitude (either positive or negative) is the result of the total exposure to an attitude object / target. Using this approach, an attitude can be equally strong if the salience and outcome are high but observed infrequently, or if the salience and outcome are low but the frequency of observation is high. This approach would appear to lend support to the mere exposure effect (Zajonc, 1968; Auty & Lewis, 2004). This refers to the contact an individual has with a particular attitude object. The more frequent the exposure or contact with the object, the stronger the associated attitude becomes. The effect is observable for both positive and negative attitudes and affects both explicit and implicit attitude formation and 1 social desirability and self presentation are discussed further within the ageism framework in the Ageism chapter on pg. 48 11 maintenance. Another facet of the mere exposure effect is that unlike other methods of acquiring information and behaviours, it is simply the exposure that builds the effect and that there need not be any explicit reward or reinforcement. In contrast, the averaged model proposes that attitudes are subject to a process of normalisation. Thus, the attitude is formed from the average evaluations of the attributes associated with an attitude object. Rather than the summation theory that simply adds together all of the experiences, this theory posits that the direction of the attitude is a reasoned average based on evaluations from each exposure. This theory, unlike the summation approach, would incorporate outlier responses or opinions and ‘dilute’ them into the previously held knowledge about a certain attitude object. More recently it has been established that there are, however, differences in effects of different exposure types. Prestwich, Kenworthy, Wilson and Kwan-Tat (2008) demonstrated that in terms of racist attitudes, exposure to the target group did indeed alter the attitudes held. They found that the quantity of the contact improved an individual’s implicit attitude and the quality of said contact affected the explicitly expressed attitudes. This has also been shown to be the case more specifically in the field of ageing. With an intergenerational study, Tam, Hewstone, Harwood, Voci and Kenworthy (2006) illustrated the same pattern of implicit and explicit attitude change based on quality and quantity of contact. This is important when considering that implicit attitudes reflect spontaneous behaviour and explicit attitudes are predictive of planned behaviour. In order to improve the implicit attitudes towards older people and the resulting behaviour, it is important that there is a high quantity of planned quality exposure and contact time. 12 In a review of the literature, Betsch, Plessner and Schallies (2004) argue that both models are only applicable in certain circumstances, and an integrated model is more appropriate. They called this the value-account model, which argues that implicit attitudes are formed by summation and explicit attitudes by the averaged procedure. Betsch et al. state that the model is applicable in any situation where the stimulus has the potential to evoke an affective reaction. The model also takes into consideration the four components of attitudes, explaining the salience and motivational aspects of attitudes whilst centring on the cognitive evaluations made by the individual, and their awareness of the process. The value-account model (Betsch et al., 2004) appears to create an inclusive model of attitude formation, stating that intuitive evaluative judgments reflect the total value of prior encounters. But what of measures of attitudes themselves? Much of the existing body of research is based on self report measures. These are typically Likert-style questionnaires with positive and negative statements towards an attitude object (e.g. smoking, racial issues, gender issues). These measures generally have high levels of consistency and reliability. However, they only measure explicit attitudes whereas attitudes can also be implicit as well as explicit. As defined by Greenwald and Banaji (1995, p.8), implicit attitudes are ‘introspectively unidentified (or inaccurately identified) traces of past experience that mediate favourable or unfavourable feeling, thought or action toward social objects’. Conversely, explicit attitudes are defined as ‘consciously expressed actions, thoughts or feelings under the performer’s control’ (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995, p.8). Perugini (2005) demonstrated that implicit attitudes are stable and enduring, allowing prediction of spontaneous behaviour after exposure to unexpected stimuli. He also demonstrated that explicit attitudes were less enduring, 13 more malleable and predicted only planned behaviour. He believed that because the two attitude types predicted behaviour at different times, they may in fact be manifestations of a single root attitude. This research does not suggest that explicit measures are incorrect, nor that the implicit measures are innately better. The research emphasises that the ability now exists to collect data on both forms of attitude and as such, allows collection of the complete attitude as opposed to one or other subset. Explicit attitude testing uses self report measures and as such is prone to self presentational bias (Goffman, 1959). When reporting information about oneself, an individual is more likely to tell the other person: (i) what it is that they believe they want to hear; and/or (ii) what will place the speaker in the most socially acceptable light. Jones and Pittman (1982) observed self-monitoring and impression management in self reporting measures. They demonstrated that when reporting details about themselves, participants were careful about what information they volunteered and monitored the answers given to provide a consistently positive view. Explicit attitudes can be measured in several ways, including Likert scale self report questionnaires, semi-structured interviews, and focus groups. The latter two methods allow for further probing into answers and as such, by using a qualitative approach, provide a deeper insight into proclaimed attitudes, but socially desirable rather than truthful responding is still possible. These findings support the theory of reasoned action (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975) which posits that in addition to the individual’s belief, an attitude is heavily influenced by the subjective norm. The subjective norm is the belief that attitudes are often influenced by the expectations of others as well as 14 the individual’s desire to engage in actions that are favourably viewed by significant others. The other subset of attitude is implicit. This is different to the explicit attitudes as it is not under the conscious control of the individual and as such is not as easily measured, assessed or altered. Implicit Attitudes ‘I cannot totally grasp all that I am… For that darkness is lamentable in which the possibilities in me are hidden from myself: so that my mind, questioning itself upon its own powers, feels that it cannot rightly trust in its own report’ St. Augustine, Confessions Despite being written around 397AD the above quotation from the Confessions of St Augustine could have been taken from work within social psychology of the 21 st century. He suggests that there are parts of himself that he is not totally aware of and that this in turn means that his self report (explicit attitudes) cannot be wholly believed. From more recent works this idea has been expanded as understanding grows and much more is understood about the implicit side to attitude formation and expression. Implicit attitudes are “introspectively unidentified (or inaccurately identified) traces of past experience that mediate favourable or unfavourable feeling, thought or action toward social objects” Greenwald & Banaji, 1995 p.8 15 Greenwald and Banaji illustrate here that an implicit attitude may indeed reveal thoughts, feelings and emotions that may normally be explicitly rejected as they conflict with outwardly held values and beliefs. Alternatively, they may be hidden as their explicit expression may have negative social consequences. More likely, however, is that implicit attitudes reveal information which is not available to the individual through introspection however motivated or able a person is (Wilson, Lindsey & Schooler, 2000). An example of this can be seen in ageism. Where an individual genuinely believes they are not ageist and outwardly expresses accepting explicit attitudes, they may harbour negative implicit attitudes based on subtle reinforces from their social interactions and environments. Implicit measures by their very nature are not subject to the same shortfalls as explicit measures. In part this is because implicit attitude measures are often relatively opaque. By this it is meant that unlike explicit tests where it is often clear what is being measured and what socially acceptable responses are, implicit tests are less obvious. In addition, implicit measures assess automatic social cognition, and participants are unaware of the causal underpinnings of their performance even if they can guess that their performance was influenced by underlying associations. They have been shown to be less biased by deliberate attempts to conceal the attitude and in turn reflect the attitude which may indeed be unknown to the individual (Dovidio & Fazio, 1992; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995). It is exactly this quality of the implicit test that enables them to measure thoughts and processes outside of the conscious control of the individual (De Houwer & Moors, 2007). 16 There are several types of implicit measure that have been developed, namely: the Affective Priming Task where primes are introduced to assess the valence of decisions and influence the evaluation of target concepts (Fazio, Jackson, Daunton & Williams, 1995). The Implicit Association Test where response latencies are measured for pairings between congruent and incongruent pairings of targets and evaluations (IAT – Greenwald & Banaji, 1995). The Extrinsic Affective Simon Task (EAST) which is similar to the IAT, however, in affective Simon studies, participants are asked to choose between a positive or negative response on the basis of a nonevaluative feature of valenced words. For instance, they might be asked to respond “GOOD” whenever a person word is presented and to respond “BAD” when an animal word is presented (De Houwer & Eelen, 1998). Also there is the Go/ No-go Association Task (GNAT) which is again similar to the IAT, however, the IAT requires that an attitude toward one category (insects) be assessed relative to a second category (flowers). With the GNAT, experimenters can vary whether insects are evaluated in the context of a single category (flowers), a superordinate category (animals), a generic category (objects), or with no context at all (Nosek & Banaji, 2001). Nosek, Greenwald & Banaji (2007) conducted a review of research using implicit measures in the seven years following the release of the IAT. They identified that each test varied in different measures of reliability and consistency mainly depending on the type of stimuli being used e.g. pictures, words and pairing categories. They concluded that when no comparable opposite category to the attitude object can be found e.g. attitudes towards television (single category assessments) the Go/No-go association task or the Extrinsic Affective Simon Task provide superior results even 17 when compared to the IAT. When there are comparable categories (e.g. old/young) the IAT provides results superior to the others in power, reliability, validity and replicability. Some evaluative priming measures demonstrated weak internal consistency with a split-half r = .06 (Olson & Fazio, 2003). The Go/No-go Association Test (GNAT) has also demonstrated similarly weak reliability when implementing its signal detection method (split-half r = .20; Nosek & Banaji, 2001). The IAT, however, has observed split-half internal consistency scores consistently recording r = .69 (Bosson et al., 2000). On average, the internal consistency estimates for the IAT have been shown to range from .7 to .9 (Schmuckle & Egloff, 2004). When looked at in direct comparison, the IAT significantly outperforms the EAST in measures of reliability (EAST a = .19; IAT a > .75; Teige, Schnabel, Banse & Asendorpf, 2004). It is clear from this that the IAT far outperforms other latencybased implicit measures and as such demonstrates far greater reliability. In a review, Monteith, Voils & Ashburn-Nardo (2001) found the IAT to be different from the other implicit measures in that participants were often aware of the difference in time taken in responding to congruent and incongruent pairings. Their results revealed strong implicit racial biases that were moderately related to explicit prejudice but unrelated to proneness to discrepancies. By this it is meant that IAT scores were moderately linked to the explicit measures but not to the differences between what participants explicitly stated and their IAT scores. The majority of participants detected this bias, and they felt guilty about it to the extent that they attributed the bias to race-related factors. Participants with smaller discrepancies were more prone to misattribute their IAT bias to non-racial factors and not feeling guilty. These latter findings suggest that people who typically experience success at avoiding 18 prejudiced responses might, paradoxically, be least likely to detect subtle racial biases when they do occur. Thus, the IAT not only allows researchers to assess implicit biases but also gives participants the ability to ‘observe’ their own biases. Due to this and other qualities discussed below, this study and review will centre on the IAT. 19 Chapter Two The Implicit Association Test (IAT) The IAT as developed by Greenwald, McGhee and Schwartz (1998) is a tool for measuring the strength of associations between concepts of an attitude. By this it is meant that response latencies are measured between category pairings within an attitude object. In this case response latencies between categorisations of old and young faces. When a score has been calculated (described below) then the strength and direction of the association can be identified thus illustrating the strength and direction of the attitude held. The test involves sorting stimuli (words and/or pictures) from four concepts (two sets of opposite concepts i.e. old/young and good/bad) using two response options, each assigned two of the concepts. A representation of the IAT programme is displayed in Table 1. The premise behind the IAT is that sorting stimuli in categories where the concepts are more strongly associated will be quicker than those where the association is weaker. 20 Table 1: A tabular depiction of the IAT programme Block No. of Trials Items Assigned to Left Key Items Assigned to Right Key B1 12 Old Faces Young Faces B2 16 Good Words Bad Words B3 28 Old Faces + Good Words Young Faces + Bad Words B4 28 Old Faces + Good Words Young Faces + Bad Words B5 12 Young Faces Old Faces B6 28 Young Faces + Good Words Old Faces + Bad Words B7 28 Young Faces + Good Words Old Faces + Bad Words The IAT requires participants to categorise target concepts with descriptors representing positive and negative poles of an attribution dimension. When a pairing appears between a concept and a congruent descriptor, mapping them to the same response key is considerably easier than if the pairing is incongruent. An example of which would be a quicker response time for pairing a picture of a young person into the category ‘young/good’ rather than ‘young/bad’. It is also important to note that there are equal numbers of congruent and incongruent pairings in each of the trail blocks. 21 Fig 1: A screenshot of the congruent condition IAT Figure 1 shows a screen shot taken from the IAT which illustrates the way in which participants would see the categorisation options. The stimulus (picture of a young or old face or a positive/good or negative/bad word) is presented in the centre of the screen and the categories are static in the top left and right corners of the screen. The example above is that of an old face presented in a congruent condition where young and good are categorised together. The response latency difference measures the extent to which positive and negative evaluations are attributed to the target concepts. This is calculated by summing the response times for the congruent and incongruent pairings and then taking the time for the congruent pairings away from the incongruent condition. The larger the time difference, the more negative the attitude is towards the target concept. The IAT has been widely accepted in social psychology as a measure of implicit attitudes. It is not however a tool used only within this field; it is a measure that has been used in a variety of disciplines. It has been used in cognitive psychology where 22 the underlying constructs measured in implicit cognition are identified (Fazio & Olsen, 2003). The IAT has been used in developmental psychology where implicit pro-White/anti-Black bias was evident even in the youngest group, with self reported attitudes revealing bias in the same direction. In 10-year-olds and adults, the same magnitude of implicit race bias was observed, although self reported race attitudes became substantially less biased in older children and vanished entirely in adults, who self reported equally favourable attitudes toward Whites and Blacks (Baron & Banaji, 2006). Further it has been used in clinical psychology where the IAT was identified as an accurate tool for measuring fear-related automatic associations (Teachman, Gregg & Woody, 2001). Neuroscientists have also used the IAT where it was established that even though amygdala activation to Black versus White faces is correlated with performance on indirect measures of race bias, the amygdala is not critical for normal performance on the IAT and as such expression of implicit attitudes (Phelps et al, 2000). Finally it has been used extensively in health psychology where strong implicit anti-fat attitudes and stereotypes were demonstrated using the Implicit Association Test, despite no explicit anti-fat bias (Teachman et al, 2003). It has also shown its use in practical applications outside the spheres of psychology, most prominently in the market research domain where IAT test results have been used to shape marketing campaigns based upon the positive or negative associations by consumers (Maison, Greenwald & Bruin, 2001). These studies have illustrated that the IAT assesses internal constructs that are often distinct from the corresponding constructs measured using explicit self report measures. It is this distinction that has proven to be the IAT’s strength as it measures the implicit cognition associated with those unidentified or unreported implicit 23 attitudes (Fazio & Olsen, 2003; Teachman et al, 2003; Baron & Banaji, 2006). It is however, worth noting that the IAT does not set out to, or propose that it does, measure the beliefs held by an individual. The IAT simply measures the associations involved in certain beliefs and it is from these associations that indirect evidence is collated for the presence of certain beliefs and attitudes (De Houwer, 2002). Equally, it has been said that as the IAT measures ‘only’ associations, these measurements can be reflections of societal exposure to stimuli and even if attitudes are reflected, it is the societal attitude reflected, not that of the individual. De Jong et al. (2000) found that the scores reported on an IAT for spiders were equally low for those people who were fearful and for those people who were not. From this they concluded that the negative association was a societal norm and not reflective of the attitude held by the individual. What de Jong et al. failed to consider was that the level of fear was self reported so could have been flawed, as well as the possible desire for some of the participants to appear unafraid of spiders as this was what they believed to be acceptable. Most results suggest that the IAT measures more than just societal views. Banse et al. (2001) examined attitudes towards homosexuality in heterosexual and homosexual men and women. Despite the report that there was a negative general attitude towards homosexuality in 2001, they found that IAT scores of the homosexual participants demonstrated a significantly more positive attitude than those heterosexual participants. This, they concluded, demonstrates that the IAT measures at least in part the attitudes of the individual rather than the societal norm. The design of the IAT itself requires careful selection of the category labels that define the concepts for measurement as well as of the stimuli materials. To ensure that the test measures what it sets out to do, Nosek et al. (2007) outline the criteria that 24 must be addressed. The categories for each stimulus must be clear. If the stimuli used exhibit characteristics for both of the categories then the participant might not categorise the stimuli using the correct characteristic and as such the test would not measure what it set out to. The stimuli thus need to be distinctive. Each stimulus used must have a single distinctive characteristic for categorisation and not allow for cross-categorisation using different characteristics. As such it must be difficult to distinguish the two categories of a single nominal dimension (e.g. men or women) using any other characteristic except the nominal feature (gender). Therefore, controls are put in place for attractiveness, gender, race, expression, age etc when using faces, depending on what the measurement of the IAT is. The IAT has shown to be a reliable test of implicit cognition with split half internal consistency being reported where r = .69 (Bosson et al, 2000) and internal consistency estimates consistently ranging from .7 to .9 (Greenwald & Nosek 2001). As already noted, these figures are more impressive when taken in the context of alternative latency-based measures where Fazio and Olson (2003) report r = .06 and Nosek and Banaji (2001) report r = .20 for the Go/No-go test. Further to this, Schmukle and Egloff (2004) conducted a thorough test of internal consistency and test-retest reliability. They concluded that the IAT demonstrated satisfactory test-retest results (r = .56) and showed evidence of both trait and occasion specific variation. In a recent review of existing literature, Perugini (2005) has illustrated that the IAT has shown consistently high internal consistency scores (α = 0.80). In addition to this he also 25 highlighted the fact the IAT has consistently given reasonable test-retest values (R = 0.60). The IAT has also been shown not to be susceptible to several possible confounding extraneous influences which have been criticisms of other implicit measures. Greenwald et al. (1998) highlight the fact that whether a category is assigned to either the left or right response key, the results show very little difference. This is also supported by Greenwald and Nosek (2001) who identified that it made no difference to response times and subsequent results whether the participant was either left or right handed. It is however of note that best practice adopts the procedure of counterbalancing as a failsafe to prevent this as a variable for any individual study. The IAT is often delivered in tandem with an explicit measure either for comparison or for a combined result. One possible influence is the delivery order of measures. In a meta-analysis of IAT studies, Hofmann et al. (2005) found that there were no order effects observed. There are three variables which have been proved to influence the first version of the IAT. These variables are: cognitive fluency, participant age and IAT experience. Greenwald et al. (2003) developed the improved algorithm (as detailed in the thesis methodology) called the D-measure which overcame each of these variables or at least reduced their effects. Cognitive fluency is a phenomenon where those participants who perform the task more slowly overall tend to show a higher IAT effect (either positive or negative). Those slower responders have higher raw scores on all of the measures (congruent and incongruent), resulting in larger overall scores which can exaggerate effects. By using block means and standard deviation 26 calculations Greenwald et al. (2003) demonstrated that this effect can be minimised. Similarly, older participants tend to show larger IAT response times which can again be reduced and the results made more comparable when using the D Score calculation (Greenwald et al, 2003). Greenwald and Nosek (2001) identified the decline in effect magnitude with repeat administration of the IAT. When using the D Score this effect also decreases, however this is one extraneous variable that should be considered if using multiple IATs in a single session or repeated measures IAT over a short period of time. Nonetheless, the IAT is a robust measure of attitude. For example, it is difficult to fake, in part because automatic responses are a key part of the participant response and these are hard to control. For example, Kim (2003) tested the controllability of the IAT in two studies using racism as the measure and flowers and insects as a control. He found that in both cases the results from the IAT could not be controlled / faked even if under instruction to do so. Kim demonstrated that the only way to control answers was to be told how to do so (responding slowly to a subset of the stimuli). He also identified that even after multiple trials, participants did not spontaneously discover the strategy for controlling their responses. From this he concluded that the IAT was a robust measure, and is clearly more so than explicit measures which have been shown to be susceptible to self presentational bias as well as capturing more subtle biases that may be undetectable using self report measures (Dasgupta et al., 2000). These findings have been supported by Banse et al. (2001) who identified that the IAT could only be reasonably ‘controlled’ by participants upon instruction on how to do so and with multiple exposures to the IAT format. It can further be argued that as the IAT often reveals associations not explicitly endorsed by participants that it is 27 resistant to deliberate alteration and manipulation. This point is demonstrated by Nosek (2002) who demonstrated that many white participants showed a consistent implicit preference for black relative to white despite the explicit desire not to do so. To cement this point, the same study also showed that many of the black participants do not show implicit preference for black relative to white despite the explicit desire to do so. Nosek et al (2007) further posit that as IAT measures are often so weakly correlated to explicit attitude measures it is likely that deliberate faking is not a substantial issue under typical study conditions. Predicting Behaviour The IAT has been shown to be a valid and reliable measure of associations and from that an indicator of relative implicit attitudes held. This in itself is a useful tool for measuring and assessing the attitudes held by individuals, groups and the wider society. However, how predictive of behaviour can IAT results be? Steffens, Schulze and Konig (2006) used IATs to measure the ‘Big Five’ personality traits and tested the IAT’s ability to predict spontaneous behaviour. They tested participants using both implicit and explicit measures and found that IATs were predictive of spontaneous behaviour where explicit measures were not. They also identified explicit measures as being related to self ratings or behaviour where IATs were not. These results can be explained when the processes behind the behaviour are examined further. Automatic processes are based on an associative network that operates in a fast, effortless and unintentional manner where controlled processes are in contrast based on higher order mental processes of reasoning that influence behaviour in a slower more intentional way (Evans, 2008). The opportunity and motivation to 28 control behaviour can mediate the amount of automatic and controlled process for any behaviour. As controlled processes are time and resource intensive they will only guide behaviour if the opportunity is given and if the individual is sufficiently motivated to do so. If there is the absence of either opportunity or motivation, automated processes will have a greater impact. As implicit measures assess automatic processes, they should be successful in predicting behaviour to the extent that automatic processes drive behaviour. As controlled processes are able to override automatic processing, the predictive validity of implicit measures will decrease as motivation and opportunity increase. Linked to the cognitively demanding nature of controlled processes, those individuals with a higher working memory capacity (WMC) should be better at overriding their automatic processes even in high demand situations. Thus, the predictive validity of the IAT would be lower in these circumstances. Barrett et al. (2004) similarly found that those with higher WMC were more successful in enacting controlled goal-directed processing than individuals low in WMC. It can therefore be suggested that those with higher WMC are able to moderate the predictive ability of the IAT and other implicit measures but not the attitude/associations that they measure. However, this said, the moderation can still only occur when there is the motivation, opportunity and sufficient WMC, otherwise the automatic processes return and the associated behaviour can be observed. Another personality trait that can moderate the predictive ability of the IAT is control over non-verbal behaviour. Controlling non-verbal behaviour is more difficult than controlling verbal behaviour as it is an automatic response not requiring conscious processes. However, control over these processes is not impossible (DePaulo, 1992). Indeed, whereas most people are unaware of their body language, there are those who 29 are more aware and have become practised at controlling these cues (Dasgupta & Rivera, 2006). From this it can also be implied that in addition to an individual’s WMC, the more aware and higher the person’s ability to control their non-verbal behaviours, the less predictive of non-verbal behaviour the IAT becomes. Further to these individual traits, the following situational moderators also affect the predictive ability of the IAT - cognitive capacity, processing time and alcohol consumption. Cognitive capacity refers to the finite amount of processing capacity. If there is increased cognitive load from multiple drains on the capacity, the influence of controlled processes on behaviour will decrease and consequently the automatic processes which are less susceptible to capacity constraints will increase. This has been demonstrated succinctly by Friese, Hofmann and Wanke (2008) who conducted a study using self report measures and IATs for chocolate and fruit. Half of the participants were instructed to remember a one digit number (retaining their cognitive capacity) and the other half were instructed to remember an eight digit number (reduced cognitive capacity). Following the tests, participants chose either 5 pieces of fruit or chocolate as their reward for participation. The IAT as expected predicted the choice behaviour well for those people whose cognitive capacity had been diminished and the explicit measure was predictive for those who retained their cognitive capacity. Processing time refers to the time available in which to process the information, make a decision and act upon it. Similar to cognitive capacity, the shorter the time available the increased cognitive demand and the more reliance on time and memory saving strategies. Schemas, stereotypes and more easily available cues are used to make 30 decisions on behaviour than if under unconstrained conditions (Dijker & Koomen, 1996). When the available time is decreased the opportunity to engage in controlled processing also decreases and as such it would be expected that the IAT would be a better predictor of behaviour when the participant is under time constrained conditions and reliant on automated processes rather than when no time pressure is applied. This has been demonstrated when Friese, Wanke and Plessner (2006) found that when placed under time constrained conditions, more than 60% of participants followed their implicit preference as shown using an IAT. Conversely when no time pressure was applied they found nearly all of the participants behaved as expected from their explicit measure. Finally, alcohol consumption also affects the predictive ability of the IAT. Alcohol impairs functioning across many domains including working memory, attention and self regulation (Giancola, 2000). That said, it has been shown that despite affecting controlled processing, it leaves automatic processing largely unchanged (Fillmore et al, 1999). From this it can be hypothesised that the IAT would be a better predictor of behaviour for those individuals who have consumed alcohol when compared to those who have not. Hofmann and Friese (2008) measured eating restraint using both implicit and explicit measures using both participants who had consumed alcohol and those who had not. They found that those who had consumed alcohol followed the pattern of eating behaviour predicted by the IAT whereas those sober participants were more likely to follow the predictions from the restraint questionnaire. Thus, empirical evidence suggests that opportunity, motivation, situational and individual factors all play a role in the predictive validity of the IAT. Each of these 31 factors can be linked to the central executive and how its impairment of function leads to greater predictability using the IAT. The central executive is responsible for the distribution of cognitive resources and information processing (Baddeley, 1996) and to fulfil these functions must harness controlled processing. Time constraints, alcohol consumption, low capacity etc are drains on the central executive and as such impair controlled processes where automatic processes remain unaffected (Baddeley, 1996; Fillmore et al, 1999; Hull & Sloane, 2004). Implicit measures will primarily predict behaviour under conditions of low opportunity or motivation to control cognitive processes (Friese, Hofmann & Schmitt, 2009). It is the lack of conscious control that underpins spontaneous, unplanned behaviour that gives the IAT high predictive validity in this area. It is in the area of planned behaviour when the individual is able to and motivated enough to control their processes that explicit measures provide a better predictor of behaviour. The reason why the IAT is more successful than other implicit measures at predicting this spontaneous behaviour is the dissonance caused by switching between congruent and incongruent conditions and the increased time lag associated with the latter due to increased cognitive load. When compared to its closest ‘rival’ (Extrinsic Affective Simon Task – EAST), De Houwer and De Bruycker, (2007) concluded that the IAT is a better measure of inter-individual differences in attitude than the EAST. This is an especially pertinent point when considering that De Houwer was the psychologist responsible for the construction of the EAST. This they concluded when analysing consistency, reliability and predictability of future behaviours. 32 However, it is also important to note the main limitations of the IAT. Further to the fact the IAT predicts only spontaneous rather than planned behaviour, the IAT is also malleable in that it can be influenced by indirect means. Foroni and Mayr (2005) demonstrated that by using a priming story, the IAT effect could be shifted and the results changed. By this, what is meant is that participants’ attitudes could be changed on the IAT if they were first presented with positive or negative priming stories for the attitude object. This they found to be more effective than explicitly telling the participant deliberately to alter their results. This should not cause problems in controlled conditions but where additional stimulus material is presented even in the form of an explicit tool with positive and negative statements, measures should be in place to prevent this becoming an extraneous variable. To this end, the studies in this thesis will present the explicit measure after the completion of the IAT so that the implicit measure cannot be coloured. In summary, the usefulness of the IAT is due to a number of factors: resistance to self presentational bias (Egloff & Schmuckle, 2002); its adaptability to numerous forms of concept measurement (Greenwald & Nosek, 2001); and its lack of dependence on introspective access to the association strengths being measured (Greenwald et al., 2002). Because of these, the IAT can be used as a flexible yet powerful measure of implicit attitude. This flexibility gives researchers the opportunity to access implicit attitudes and stereotypes over numerous categories, overcoming some of the limitations presented by using self report methods. 33 Chapter Three Prejudice So far our focus has been on attitudes. However, although attitudes are central to the formation of prejudicial thoughts and ultimately treatment, it is arguably the stereotypes derived from attitudes that are the key factor in shaping behaviour. Eagly and Diekman (2005) highlight role incongruity as the basis for prejudicial behaviour. They propose that prejudice itself derives from the dissonance between beliefs about the stereotyped attributes associated with a group and the beliefs about the attributes that allow success in valued social roles. Prejudice is as the name suggests the process of pre-judging a particular object (person, place or thing) by one or more of its characteristics. It implies the arrival at a judgement before sufficient evidence has been gathered. Nowicki (2008) presents prejudice in a similar manner to that of an attitude in that it can be broken into component parts; cognitive, affective and conative. Cognitive prejudice refers to the belief that an individual holds, that a certain opinion or attitude is correct and true. Affective prejudice describes what the individual likes or dislikes. Conative prejudice is similar to the conative component of attitude, in that it refers to the propensity of the individual to act on their prejudice. It is the inclination or predisposition and direction of action that it is aimed to measure when assessing the attitudes held by an individual. Psychologically, these components can be drawn together succinctly to define prejudice as: “…the holding of derogatory social attitudes or cognitive beliefs, the expression of negative affect, or the display of 34 hostile or discriminatory behaviour towards members of a group on account of their membership to that group” (Brown, 2001, p.8) This highlights the fact that prejudice is the social orientation towards either a whole group of people or towards individuals due to their belonging to a certain group. Indeed, when judgements and assertions are made about certain social groups, the person making the assertion is using one or more social categorisations as a starting point to infer attributes towards the people in question. It is this process of social categorisation that is so central to the operation of prejudice. Indeed Allport (1954) argued it is the condition without which prejudice could not exist. Although central to prejudice, categorisation is a process that occurs not only in unusual circumstances but as Bruner (1957) posited, is an integral part of human existence. Social categorisation is a useful phenomenon as it allows for the grouping of similar characteristics enabling the world to be processed in a less arduous and more efficient way. We do not have the processing capabilities to assess each characteristic belonging to each person and object that we come across in our daily lives. As such we rely on these grouping and categories to ease cognitive load. Put succinctly, “…categories are nouns that cut slices through our environment” (Allport, 1954, p.174). In order for categorisation to work, however, there must be clear distinctions in the characteristics held by each of the groups. This is also the basic premise behind the formation of prejudice. As the categories help us to order and simplify groups of 35 stimuli/people, they also aid in the discrimination between those who do and do not belong. Tajfel (1959) defined the formation of prejudice through categorisation, creating two hypotheses regarding the consequences of this categorisation. First, he stated that when a category is created which incorporates a set of stimuli (be they objects or people etc.) which cause some of the stimuli to fall into either one group or another, this will enhance any pre-existing differences between the two categories. Second, as an extension to the first hypothesis, he stated that members of different groups will be seen as more different from each other than they may truly be, with members of the same group appearing more similar and therefore exaggerating differences. Both facets of this were illustrated by Eiser (1971) who asked participants to judge the permissiveness of a series of attitude statements concerning recreational drug use. In the experimental condition, half of the statements were attributed to one newspaper and half to another, whereas in the control group only one source was stated. The perceived difference between the permissive and restrictive statements was significantly greater in the experimental condition. This finding clearly illustrates the importance of the relatively arbitrary assignment of categories. McGarty and Penny (1988) used a similar design and repeated the findings for both category differentiation and assimilation within groups. More recently, in a meta-analysis of categorisation literature, Bigler and Patterson (2007) highlight the importance of in-group bias as well as implicit attributions in the development of stereotypes and prejudices towards and for salient social groupings. Social categorisation and intergroup discrimination were demonstrated in a striking way by Tajfel, Flament, Billig and Bundy (1971). The aim was to establish whether simply belonging to a group might be enough to initiate behavioural prejudice 36 (establishment of in-group and out-group behavioural differences). It was during these experiments that the researchers established the Minimal Group Paradigm where the only difference known to the participant was that of the arbitrary grouping with no categorical contexts. They found that over 70% of participants made choices that favoured their own group as well as identifying the adoption of the ‘maximising difference’ strategy (Turner, 1983) where intergroup differences are maximised often to the detriment of the out-group. Similar results were reported by Sturmer, Snyder, Kropp and Siem (2006) when looking at in-group empathic concern. They found support for ‘in-group helping’ whereby helping behaviour was initiated for the members of a perceived in-group and not the out-group members. Moreover, the level of in-group empathy was directly linked to the level and salience of perceived similarities of in-group members. These findings are important as they clearly demonstrate the cognitive processes behind prejudice formation and the apparent normalisation of this process. It is important to note, however, that personality perspectives of prejudice formation (conative and affective) are more useful in explaining the extremes forms of prejudice, a point covered later. Addressed above are the results of categorisation along one domain (e.g. Old/Young, Male/Female, British/Non-British). From a theoretical perspective what happens when the activation of two of these domains is present and people span categories making them partly in-group and partly out-group dependant on the dimension observed? Doise (1976) argues that when two categories cut across each other (e.g. age and gender) any difference in initial categories will be reduced because of simultaneous between- and within-group effects on both dimensions. Essentially, the processes of assimilation and differentiation should cancel one another out, reducing 37 or removing bias in terms of either age or gender. This theoretical finding has been explored and supported on numerous occasions (Brown & Turner, 1979; Diehl, 1990; Vanbeselaere, 1991; Pickett, Silver & Brewer, 2002). These results, however interesting in the laboratory, could potentially provide significant inroads to reducing prejudice in the real world. They seem to suggest that if situations can be arranged so that two or more dimensions cross one another, the likelihood of prejudice between groups being persistent is reduced. But is this the case when tested outside the laboratory? When tested in a real world environment, this model does not seem to work as well as with basic laboratory-based concepts. The salience of one of the crossed categories tends to take precedence over the other so that an equilibrium can be reached over the incongruence caused by no clear cut in-group/out-group category. In essence this means that one of the crossed categories has higher salience and to resolve any disequilibrium, it is this category through which discrimination and group categorisation occurs. Hewstone, Islam and Judd (1993) demonstrated this where the crossing groups were religion (Muslim/Hindu) and nationality (Bangladeshi/Indian). In this as with other similar studies (Brewer, Ho, Lee & Miller, 1987), if people were seen to be sharing the same religion, they were always evaluated in a more positive light. If the person did not share the same religion, regardless of nationality or language spoken, they were always rated significantly lower. Similar findings were also reported by Kang Fu (2007) who identified cultural salience as the overarching mediating factor in inter-race/cultural/religion/class marriages in the USA. These studies demonstrate that unlike in the laboratory, category salience is dependant on 38 the locale and the cross category encountered which in turn influence the evaluation of in-group and out-group members. Two fundamental aspects of the categorisation process are the exaggeration of the intergroup differences and the enhancement of intragroup similarities. When looking at everyday situations such as sporting clashes it is surprisingly the out-group homogeneity that is emphasised rather than that of the in-group. For example, Hamilton and Bishop (1976) observed the categorisation of ethnicity in these terms. When speaking to residents of communities in America into which new families moved, they found that six months after the new family moved in, only 11% of the community knew the surname of the new family if they were black compared to 60% if they were white. The remaining 89% referred to the new family as “the blacks” with no distinguishing characteristics, whereas the white family were perceived more individually. Similarly, Hutchinson, Jetten, Christian and Haycraft (2006) found that higher in-group identifiers presented with a homogeneous in-group perceived more in-group homogeneity when their group was under threat than did low identifiers. This has also been found in the field of gerontology where, when questioned, both residents of an older persons’ residential home and members of a younger student group each rated the other’s age group as more homogenous than their own (Linville, Fischer & Salovey, 1989). The process of out-group homogeneity is, however, not universal. Brown (2001) emphasised that out-group homogeneity is observed when the in-group is not just larger but is not a minority group. When minority groups were observed, in-group homogeneity was significantly more salient and reported as such by members. This is not to suggest that group size is the factor at play (Bartsch & Judd, 1993) but instead that those in minority groups identify more strongly with their 39 in-groups than do members of less salient larger groups. An example of this would be the BME communities within larger cities where their cultural background as a minority is more salient than the sense of ‘Britishness’ is for the larger general outgroup population. The reason for this is that when in the minority, people strive to make themselves more similar to their conception of when stereotypical in-group member should be (Turner, Hogg, Oaks, Reicher & Wethrell, 1987). In sum, it is fundamental for people to categorise due to the size and complexity of the daily information processed. One outcome of this categorisation process is the accentuation of intergroup differences and the reduction of intragroup differences, both of which affect both evaluation of the out-group and intergroup perceptions, attitudes and behaviour. When two or more of these categories cross, in a real world environment, the more salient category is dominant. Once categorisation has occurred, intergroup differences are accentuated with homogeneity dependent on the minority status of the in-group. It is the salience attributed to the in-group membership and often stereotyped differences between categories that determine the basis for prejudice. Stereotypes Stereotypes are the belief that members of the same group indeed also share a certain attribute. This assumption arises directly from the categorisation process through the assimilation of in-group differences and as such the promotion of out-group homogeneity. A cultural example of this can be seen in football supporters where followers of a particular team will assume a group identity and norms differentiating themselves from supporters of rival teams. Further to this they will also attribute a 40 different set of characteristics to the out-group supporters to further differentiate themselves. Allport (1954) identified some key ways in which stereotypes were formed simply through the way in which we are raised within our culture and environment. He identified family socialisation, exposure to images in books, television and newspapers as key contributing sources for potential prejudicial stereotypes. Obviously in today’s world, the Internet and seemingly barrier-less technology all feed into the process of stereotype formation (Kvieskaite, 2007). Devine and Sherman (1992) propose an alternative to this view in that attitudes are formed to serve an ideological function. It is in this function that they justify the way in which certain groups of people are treated and endorse the dominant group’s right to its privileged position. Both of these viewpoints suggest that stereotypes are not only rooted in the cognitive processes of the individual but moreover are integral to social constructs. Further to this, it has also been identified that stereotypes can be formed from a cognitive bias, resulting in a perception-based correlation between minority groups and infrequently occurring attributes (Stroessner, Hamilton & Mackie, 1992; Rupp et al., 2005). The reason for this being adopted as a stereotypical attribute of the minority group is due to both a categorical distinctiveness process (increasing the in-group/out-group difference) and the distinctiveness of the attribute occurring. An example of this would the perception of increased crime rates attributed to members of BME communities in otherwise Caucasian majority areas. So when do stereotypes become activated and when do they influence our behaviour? Darley and Gross (1983) concluded that we use stereotypes not indiscriminately, but rather we use them to create a platform from which we then seek out further information. It is in the absence of additional information that we apply these 41 stereotypes, however hesitantly we may do this. It has been shown that the use of stereotypes (especially gender) do affect people’s judgements even when additional information is presented about the individual character of the person being judged (Glick, Zion & Nelson, 1988). It has further been shown that both of these hypotheses can be correct in that people tend to use the stereotype as a platform on which to base their assumptions of a person. However, rather than seeking information generally about the person, information is sought to confirm the stereotype rather than to cast doubt on it (Stangor & Ford, 1992). There is still the ability, however, for individuals to search for information to contradict the stereotype (Macrae, Millne & Bodenhausen, 1994). As discussed previously the use of stereotypes, as with other heuristic techniques, allow for increased processing capacity of other information presented simultaneously. Linked to this, it has also to be considered that stereotype use will also increase if people are cognitively or emotionally preoccupied with other concerns. The reason for this is the inverse to that presented by Macrae et al. (1994) in that these distractions create a higher cognitive load thus making it more efficient to employ labour-saving stereotypes (Brown 2001). Further to this, Huntsinger, Sinclair, Dunn and Clore (2010) identify not only emotional preoccupation but positive mood state as a greater predictor of stereotype activation. When observing stereotypes rather than influencing them, they can be used to make sense of both in-group and out-group behaviour. Ross (1977) proposed the notion of ‘fundamental attribution error’ which states that people assume internal causes for others behaviour but external causes for their own. Pettigrew (1979) expanded this theory including group phenomenon to the ‘ultimate attribution error’. This posits that negative behaviour by out-group members will be attributed to internal causes 42 whereas those behaviours by in-group members will be rationalised by external influences. Similarly, when the out-group expresses positive behavioural traits it will be seen as the exception that proves the rule instead of in-group members where it is seen as an in-group characteristic. Ultimate attribution error has been shown in a research setting by Beal, Ruscher and Schnake (2001) whereby they demonstrated that subsequently presented mitigating explanations for negative acts did not temper impressions of out-group members, and subsequently presented crediting explanations for positive acts did not enhance impressions of out-group members. Brown (2001) highlights from this that the more abstract a general stereotype construct is, the more resistant to change in the light of new information it may be, whereas concrete representations are more easily disconfirmed by one or two contrary instances. He further explains that positive in-group and negative out-group stereotypes lean towards abstraction whilst negative in-group and positive out-group images are usually more concrete. From this it has to be of concern how negative stereotypes can be addressed and changed. Stereotypes can be changed through the presentation of contradictory information, but how that information is presented (concentrated examples or sporadic) and the affective nature (positive or negative) of the stereotype undergoing change are integral factors to the extent and level of success of the modification. A growing body of research (Allport, 1954; Cook, 1962, 1978; Pettigrew, 1979; Stephan & Stephan, 1984; Paolini, Hewstone, Cairns & Voci, 2004; King, Winter & Webster, 2009) has shown that contact between groups can alter stereotypes and reduce prejudice provided that it takes place under certain conditions. 43 The first of these conditions states that there should be a framework of social and institutional support for the interventions designed to facilitate contact (Allport, 1954). By this it is meant that those in authority (actual or perceived) should demonstrate unwavering support for the goals of the intervention. In doing this, people are encouraged to act in a non-discriminatory way that should eventually lead to them internalising these behaviours into their own attitudes. Festinger (1957) posits this is because most people have a need to bring into line both their behaviour and their beliefs so as to alleviate any possible dissonance. The second of these conditions refers to the acquiescence potential. For contact to be successful, it needs to be of sufficient frequency, duration and closeness to facilitate meaningful relationships to develop between members of the groups concerned. As Cook (1978) highlighted, this can be in stark contrast to the short burst of casual contact in many intergroup contact situations. Third, it is necessary for the two groups to meet where both are considered to have equal status. Many of the stereotypes of out-groups are held as they are considered lesser or inferior in a multitude of different ways. If, when meeting, this unequal status is maintained then the likelihood is that existing negative stereotypes for both groups will be strengthened rather than weakened (Blanchard, Weigel & Cook, 1975). The final condition identified by Allport (1954) was that of co-operation. This follows on from realistic group conflict theory (Sherif, 1954). Members of different groups are dependent on one another for the achievement of a jointly desired goal and as such must co-operate towards this from a unified platform. Ultimately, any solution to 44 reducing prejudice must be through minimising the destructive potential of division maintenance whilst still allowing each of the groups to maintain their distinctive identities. Recent research (Tam, Hewstone, Harwood, Voci & Kenworthy, 2006; Prestwhich, Kenworthy, Wilson & Kwan-Tat, 2008) has shown that this is not such a straightforward process. Both studies have found that direct contact does indeed affect the attitudes held. However, they have established that the perceived quality of the contact affects the explicit attitude held where the quantity of contact is that factor affecting the implicit attitudes expressed. Some research on prejudice indicates a change from blatant to more subtle forms of prejudice from the fear of reprisal as social norms change towards what might be loosely termed ‘political correctness’ (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986; McConahay 1986). Specifically, with little conscious awareness or intent on action, individuals’ negative associations that are consciously renounced can be rekindled and used as a benchmark for responding to members of a stereotyped group (Bargh, Chen & Burrows, 1996; Devine, 1989; Fazio, Jackson, Dunton & Williams, 1995; Greenwald, McGhee & Schwartz, 1998; Legault, Green-Demers & Eadie, 2009; Mendoza, Gollwitzer & Amodio, 2010). Now that these negative attitudes have been internalised and become implicit, the attitude holder may indeed no longer be immediately aware that they hold these attitudes. It is with the lack of conscious awareness that the negative attitude now becomes the basis for unplanned responses to the attitude target. This phenomenon can be observed with ageism in that the explicit bias is not expressed, however, behaviour and language used precipitates the negative stereotypes 45 assimilated. This has been highlighted as a more dangerous form of prejudice as even those outwardly expressing the best of intentions have difficulty trying to avoid negative responses that are generated by implicit processes (Bargh, 1997). Since the advent of the IAT, it has been possible to accurately measure the implicit attitudes held by individuals. It is this technique that has led to the belief that racial prejudice is in fact no less prevalent than it was in the 1940’s even if it is not explicitly expressed (McConahay, 1986, Devine & Monteith, 1999). Monteith, Voils & Ashburn-Nardo (2001) conducted a study with 79 participants using both implicit (IAT) and explicit (Modern Racism Scale and a 32 point discrepancy questionnaire) measures. The majority of their sample exhibited negative implicit racial attitudes that were only moderately linked to their explicit measures. They have suggested that this may not only show that the participants have negative implicit attitudes but also that they either control their explicit attitudes to appear socially acceptable or (as the discrepancy measure indicates) that the majority are unaware that they harbour negative attitudes. The gulf between implicit and explicit measures has also been highlighted by Fazio & Olsen (2003), who concluded that implicit and explicit measures each report aspects of attitude that are unique and interact as a predictor of behaviour in any given setting. By this they meant that both measures of implicit and explicit attitudes are valid in so much as they measure distinctly different concepts. It is only when combining data on both subsets of attitude (implicit and explicit) that a holistic picture of attitudes can be obtained. This is a troubling finding when it is recalled that implicit attitudes have been shown to be accurate predictors of spontaneous behaviour and the explicit measure only as a predictor of conscious deliberate behaviour (Perugini, 2005). Both points keenly 46 demonstrate the need to address not only one aspect of attitudes held but for researchers to be mindful of both before conducting research and disseminating findings. 47 Chapter Four Ageism In order to examine how realistic stereotypes of ageing are, we need to investigate and identify what ageing constitutes. Sonnenschein and Brody (2005) predicted that by 2050 almost 50% of the population will live past 85, whereas in 1900 only 25% could expect to reach 65. The picture illustrated suggests that the average life expectancy has and will continue to increase albeit at a diminishing rate. Despite prejudice often being a largely irrational, with regard to older people some of the prejudice can be based in biological and observable declines. There are both physical and psychological losses associated with ageing, a fact that is universally accepted. From a psychological point of view the key losses are associated with memory and brain function. The subsets of dementia affect many older people but are not necessarily associated specifically with ageing or indeed with affecting all older people and as such will not be covered. One of the main areas of decline both studied and associated with older people is that of intelligence. Intelligence is generally seen as declining in later life. What truth is there in this? It should be stated at the outset, that on most measures there is evidence for a decline, but the key question is the size of this decline. ‘We are ... in the unpleasant and illogical condition of extolling maturity and depreciating age’ Dewey’s paradox of ageing (Dewey, 1939, pp.4) 48 The problem with ageing is that societally it is seen as being simultaneously a time of wisdom and a time of stupidity. Looked at more closely, what we are saying is that people believe older adults are wiser and more knowledgeable, but that they are slower and less efficient at dealing with the new and/or when they have to think on their feet. Horn and Cattell (1967) identified an age related decline in fluid intelligence (problem solving) whereas crystallised intelligence (facts) remained stable. It is worth noting, however, despite the fact younger people do significantly better than older people at fluid intelligence tests, fluid tests are generally against the clock whereas crystallised tests have no time limit so people can take as long as they like to produce their answers. In addition to this, it is the lack of practice of mental skills causes the neural systems to deteriorate through lack of use rather than specifically the ageing process (Stuart-Hamilton 2006). When referring back to Dewey’s Paradox it seems that there is some truth in the commonplace observation that old age brings wisdom at the expense of a loss of wit, but the size of this change is open to debate. But the above points are still academic in nature because should it or does it matter if in older age you are unable to analyse problems in the same way as younger people? Should it or does it matter if you are no longer able to remember what a Lammergeier is? To most people these things are not important and to purpose that they are because they are important to ourselves takes a narrow view of society. Klaczynski and Robinson (2000) demonstrated that older people shift more to heuristics rather than addressing each problem as novel as a way of coping with everyday tasks. It is in solving problems in everyday life that predict 49 an older person’s everyday functioning better than any traditionally used psychometric test (Allaire and Marsiske, (2002). In addition to cognitive declines, there are also agreed physical declines associated with ageing. Vision is something often taken for granted as we go through life but about 33% of people aged 65+ have one or more diseases relating to their eyes and vision (Quillan, 1999). The most common decline is the inability to focus at different distances due to the lens in the eye losing some of its elasticity. However, despite observing the marked acuity decline associated with age, Corso (1981) noted that the problem could be alleviated to that of a younger adult if visual displays had high contrast in luminance (black writing on white display). Similar to that of visual deterioration, auditory decline has been linked to ageing throughout the adult lifespan too with people as young as 50 being impaired in at least some circumstances (Bromley, 1988). Research has identified that of those people aged 70-80, 32% would have serious hearing impairment but further, of those aged over 80, 50% would have severe auditory impairment (Herbst, 1982). It is clear that both physical and psychological changes occur as one enters later life. What is generally believed and what forms the basis of stereotypes and societal norms might at times be at variance with this. Ageism appears to have a base in physiological and psychological fact, however, little or no account is taken of the compensation method adopted by older people to minimise the effects of age related loss. Similarly little importance is placed on the positive aspects of ageing which are similarly integral to an older person but counter the existing accepted heuristics and as 50 such are often overlooked or cast aside. From these disparate viewpoints, a legitimate question arises in whether the objective views of ageing (which are generally negative) are therefore likely to cue negative attitudes in students who take ageingrelated courses. This is a distinctly under-researched area but one of great importance. If attitudes become worse the more educated a person becomes in the field of ageing, what does this say for the state of care for older people? What has to also be reflected on is whether these negative views of ageing are necessarily ‘wrong’ since the wealth of objective evidence is generally negative. With old age inevitably leading to some degree of cognitive and physical decline, is it not warranted to view old age in a negative light especially when compared to youth? This thesis examines in detail both the effects of specific and general higher level education on attitudes towards older people. Amassing a research base for training and policy decisions in this area is crucial in the delivery of key objectives surrounding dignity in care in later life. The term ageism is one originally coined by Butler (1969) to describe the observed discrimination and prejudice directed towards older people specifically because of their age. Palmore (1999) furthered this concept by explaining that ageism involved both cognitive (stereotypes and attitudes) and affective (prejudice and discrimination) processes. Like other forms of prejudice (e.g. racism and sexism), ageism is a way of pigeon-holing people into heuristic categories that ignore individuality and apply accepted groups’ norms to all group members. Cummings et al. (2000) have identified that common forms of modern ageism include devaluing the contributions made by older people and viewing the pathologic processes sometimes associated with later life as normal components of the ageing process. Angus and Reeve (2006) have further identified that this socially ingrained ageism actively promotes stereotypes of 51 social isolation, physical and cognitive decline, lack of physical activity and economic burden. Gerontophobia is a narrower band of ageism that specifically refers to a phobia (irrational / unreasonable fear) of older people. Lynch (2000) identified ageing anxiety as a major component of gerontophobia and more widely ageism. Ageing anxiety was explained as the “combination of people’s concerns or fears about getting older” (Lynch, 2000. p. 533). These fears were based on concerns over loss of social contact, reduction in cognitive abilities, changes in physical appearance, declines in overall health and financial hardships that are themselves stereotyped characteristics of the ageing process. In addition to these somewhat irrational fears, it is the knowledge that simply by living life we will become a member of this out-group, a process and transition whose path cannot be altered or avoided. The inevitability of the transition itself is stressful and causes anxiety amongst those who hold these negative stereotypes. Cummings et al. (2000) found that people with a greater knowledge of the ageing process actually held fewer of the stereotyped opinions of the ageing process and as such exhibited significantly lower levels of ageing anxiety and worries about later life. The prevalence of all forms of ageism has increased past those of sexism and racism (Banaji, 1999; Kite & Wagner, 2002) although it is typically harder to measure due to the implicit ways it is conducted (Levy & Banaji, 2002). This has increased to such a degree due to the change in societal ethos regarding prejudice. It is now much less socially acceptable to be sexist, racist or homophobic, however, to be ageist is not seen in the same way. Ageism is prominent in advertising, media, comedy and in the way in which older people are generally viewed. It is due to this prominence that acceptance surrounding ageism has occurred. It is often seen as humorous and based 52 in some degree of fact, this negating any negative effects or outcomes. Due to the ‘humorous’ nature of ageism, the aforementioned negative effects on the older person (self-esteem, disablement, self-isolation) are generally ignored by the wider public. A problem arises, however, when trying to measure the ‘colloquially’ clear prevalence. As previously mentioned, on explicit measures, people will present themselves in what they see as the most socially acceptable light. Because of this self presentational bias, the explicit measure of ageism (unless very subtle) is not truly capturing the full picture of ageism. Spontaneous behaviour and that not consciously thought about is influenced by implicit attitudes. Due to the difficulty of accessing the implicit constructs of attitudes, these are often overlooked. However, as discussed, they form an integral part of the overall attitude. Implicit tests often require computer programmes (IAT, EAST, GNAT) to measure accurate response latencies and as such it is prohibitive to conduct them in public spaces. So, why should ageism be singled out as an important area for research? Ageism is different from all other prejudices in one key way. A racist will never change skin colour, a misogynist will never change sex, but an ageist person will become that which they hate should they live long enough. As such, every person should be conscious of the fact that if discrimination against older people is tolerated, one day it could be directed towards them. This causes issues around self-esteem and wellbeing in older adults, as well as resulting in costs to the wider society as well as the individual. There are negative consequences both personal and societal associated with prominent ageist attitudes. In the workplace discrimination due to age is increasing (McCann & Giles, 2002) which affects the company and the victim alike. The practice of discrimination has been shown to cause lowered self-efficacy, 53 decreased productivity and cardiovascular stress (Levy, Ashman & Dror, 2000). This is costly to the individual and to the company and due to the symptoms described (decreased productivity), can lead to the perpetuation of ageist attitudes. For the companies involved there can be costly lawsuits for age discrimination, ranging from a few thousand dollars up to $58.8 million (McCann & Giles, 2002) as well as having the associated costs of decreased morale, segregated workforces and decreased per capita productivity. Most of the early research conducted into ageism was conducted using scales which measure commonly held opinions or a person’s knowledge about ageing. These range from the infrequently used Old People Questionnaire (Tuckman & Lorge, 1953) to the still often used Facts on Ageing Quiz, or FAQ (Palmore, 1977). The FAQ consists of 25 true or false items which measure a person’s knowledge of the ageing process. As it measures knowledge rather than individual opinion it cannot be classed as a pure attitude measure, however, it is useful in gauging the actual level of knowledge and the level of misinformation upon which attitudes can be based. More frequently used as an explicit attitude measure is the Fraboni Scale of Ageism – FSA (Fraboni et al., 1990). The FSA was developed to fill the hole Fraboni et al. (1990) believed existed in that the previous scales only measured the cognitive components of ageism. The FSA aimed to be a more complete measure of ageism including items to measure antagonistic, discriminatory attitudes and the tendency toward avoidance. The scale includes three factors for a complete measure: Antilocution (antagonism and apathy fuelled my misconceptions and misinformation), Avoidance (withdrawal from social 54 contact with older people) and Discrimination (discriminatory opinions regarding the political rights, segregation and activities of older people) (Rupp et al., 2005). There had been sparse empirical evidence to support the validity and reliability of the FSA further than the establishing theory. However, Rupp et al. (2005) conducted an indepth analysis of the scale and concluded that the scale was a valid and reliable multidimensional measure of ageism. They conducted a confirmatory factor analysis that supported the multidimensional nature of FSA scores that emphasised both cognitive and affective facets of attitude measurement. Finally they also identified that younger individuals and men had significantly higher ageism scores on the FSA than older individuals and women. These were similar to the findings by Kalavar (2001) who had previously identified that male college students displayed more ageist attitudes than female college students. In a practical context, Mueller-Johnson et al. (2007) provided 94 undergraduates with the same testimony given by a 79-year-old male or female witness, and they were then asked to complete the FSA and the Ageing Semantic Differential. Participants who evidenced stronger ageing stereotypes on these measures rated the witness less favourably than did participants who were less prejudiced. This experiment demonstrated a clear link between perceived credibility of older adults and ageist attitudes. Discussed above are two established ageism measures. This study, as with the majority of other current studies, has used the Fraboni Scale of Ageism. The reasons for this are succinctly demonstrated by Stuart-Hamilton and Mahoney (2003) when they collected data from two hundred British participants in an age awareness workshop. The participants were given the Palmore Ageing Quiz (PAQ) and the Fraboni Scale of Ageism (FSA) immediately prior to the workshop and one month 55 after the event. Post-event testing demonstrated a significant improvement in PAQ scores, reflecting retained knowledge acquired at the workshop. Scores on the FSA were unaltered, though there was a significant lowering in scores on the Antilocution section of the test. In combination, these results indicate an increased awareness of ageing issues may alter factual knowledge and increase awareness of politically correct language, but attitudes toward older people and ageing remain essentially unaltered. This and similar studies (Gething et al., 2004; Rupp et al., 2005) question whether the PAQ is a valid choice of tool for measuring changes in attitudes to ageing, since PAQ performance can improve independent of measures of attitudinal change. Despite there being apparently equal amounts of positive and negative stereotypes pertaining to older people (Hummert et al., 1994), the pervasive attitudes present in research suggest higher negative attitudes than positive ones (Kite & Johnson 1988). This finding is also repeated in measures of implicit attitudes (Perdue & Gurtman, 1990). Isaacs and Bearison (1986) have shown that children as young as six exhibit ageist prejudices present in their cultures. Unlike other prejudices mentioned, ageist attitudes are still openly prevalent in society. In television comedies, elderly people are depicted, defined by stereotyped negativities regarding physical decline and both physical and mental incompetencies (Zebrowitz & Montepare, 2000). When the age stereotypes have been acquired, they will be easily activated by the presence of an elderly person (Banaji & Hardin, 1996; Perdue & Gurtman, 1990). This will result in the generalisation of elderly people to the stereotyped schema held. Once acquired, these attitudes are maintained and strengthened when encountering elderly people even if they do not exhibit characteristics associated to the stereotypes held (Levy et 56 al., 2000; Murphy, Monahan & Zajonc, 1995). Challenging these negative attitudes also proves harder for ageism than other forms of prejudice. Hill et al. (1990) demonstrated that even when encountering contradictory evidence, attitudes towards older people were resistant to change and in most cases did not alter. As with most negative associations, if younger people can avoid spending time with older people and encountering evidence that may either support or challenge their beliefs about them, they will do. Purdue and Gurtman (1990) observed that young people are actively seeking ways to refrain from engaging in social meetings with elderly people. This avoidance only reinforces the implicitly held beliefs as it prevents the individual from having ‘meaningful’ encounters with elderly adults that may in fact cause inconsistencies in schema to be noticed and re-evaluations of attitudes to take place. Much of the ageing literature on attitudes relates to the explicit attitudes held (Cummings, Kropf & DeWeaver, 2000; Catterall & Maclaran, 2001; Depaola, Griffin, Young & Neimeyer, 2003). As already discussed, this is acceptable when measuring attitudes to predict behaviour in planned situations but in novel situations or with prolonged exposure, what attitudes or behaviours would be expressed? Levy and Banaji (2002) conducted a review on implicit ageism illustrating a pervasive and wide reaching proliferation of negative ageist attitudes. They found that as well as older examples of ageism such as within fairytales like Hansel and Gretel where older people are portrayed as sinister and evil, more modern equivalents exist. They highlighted the fact that in modern media, older adults appear in comical roles or where stereotypes of ageing such as physical decline or incompetence are used (Zebrowitz & Montepare, 2000). This highlights the fact that ageism has deep roots in the social unconscious and from an early age is instilled into the thoughts of younger 57 children. The continual reinforcement of these implicit stereotypes is not questioned by children as they see it having no relevance to their emerging selves as they are not referring to their in-group (Levy & Banaji, 2002). Due to the lengthy process of ageing, children’s concept of older people is more abstract and when little contradictory evidence is presented, the negative characteristics are assumed. It is then continued exposure to negative stereotypes throughout the lifespan that causes this preponderance of ageism. Repeated exposure to the primes/negative associations increase the strength of the negative implicit attitude whilst often leaving the explicit attitude unaffected (Levy et al., 2000). Further to the review by Levy and Banaji (2002), research has continually demonstrated that implicit attitudes towards older people demonstrate a wide reaching ageist bias. In the same year, Jelenec and Steffens (2002) found that in using both community and university samples, implicit ageism was recorded at a significantly high level. They further analysed the data produced during the IAT and demonstrated that not only were younger target stimuli judged more positively but also that those data pertaining to older people were judged more negatively. This trend has continued to become more evident over the years, most recently demonstrated by Turner and Crisp (2010). They found that the pervasive implicit attitude towards older people was negative. In addition to this, they found that both with actual and perceived contact with older people, the implicit attitude improved. This is in support of Tam, Hewstone, Harwood, Voci and Kenworthy (2006) who also found contact to improve implicit attitudes towards older people. 58 These findings were not only in the young. Negative implicit ageist attitudes are held by older people themselves (Levy & Banaji, 2002). The reasoning given for this is that elderly people have acquired the same implicit prejudices throughout their lives and have not had sufficient time or opportunity to develop the mechanisms to defend against this. There are negative consequences for elderly people as a result of the ageist attitudes held as they are not only subjected to ageist prejudices from others but also internalise these implicit biases (Nemmers, 2005). As people progress through the life span their age schema become more elaborate as more information both about others and themselves becomes incorporated. As they age, the number of traits, categories and subcategories they have within the schema grows, however, core elements are still retained. Research does support this developmental approach, finding that despite having a more complex picture of ageing, older people do not necessarily hold more positive views. Hummert et al. (1994) found that older people did indeed hold more stereotypes about older people but they had more negative ones as well as having more positive ones. Some studies further report that older people do judge their age category more favourably than younger people do, however, that is only more positively as a comparison but both groups have generally negative attitudes towards older age (Kite et al., 1991). Levy (1996) found that elderly people who exhibited higher negative implicit attitudes also performed significantly worse on memory tasks. She found that the perceptions of older adults could also be affected by implicit self stereotyping. She concluded that implicit age stereotypes can influence the views of older adults both towards others as well as upon themselves. 59 Levy’s (1996) research supports previous research findings that when elderly people adopt these societal stereotypes, they see decline as inevitable and that becoming a less active member of society as the only option (Rodin & Langer, 1980). Similarly, Butler (1987) found that when adopted, these stereotypes became a self-fulfilling prophecy, reinforcing stereotypes through the inaction and deficits resulting from their initial belief and internalisation. Although some physical decline through arthritis, heart disease, etc, is related to ageing, much of the widely held belief about age changes in health are over-pessimistic (Rowe & Kahn, 1987). Ageing should not be viewed in simple terms of losses and gains but more within the Selection, Optimisation, Compensation (SOC) model proposed by Baltes and Baltes (1990). In this model it is accepted that losses are associated with ageing but that successful ageing is less to do with these and more reflected by the way in which the older person adapts. The model suggests that first, salient goals are selected, strategies and techniques for achieving these goals are optimised and those losses associated with ageing that may hinder goal attainment are compensated for. In a review of literature, Rowe and Kahn (1987) also found that psychological wellbeing and social interactions affected the physical wellbeing of the elderly. They showed that with decreased social support often associated with old age, mortality and morbidity rates increased as did the adherence to previously well maintained health-promoting regimes. One of the most common prejudices against the elderly comes from the perceived cognitive decline associated with ageing. The most important concept concerning cognitive ability is that of perceived control. Older people see physical and mental deterioration as something they cannot control; it is this perception that results in the reduction in active coping mechanisms (Rodin, 1986). He also demonstrated that this perceived helplessness is directly associated with the decrease 60 in motivation and self esteem and eventually with the increase in illness, mortality rates and memory problems. Similarly, Rodin (1986) showed that the opposite is true; with the provision of activity to increase perceived self control a marked improvement in memory, alertness, activity and physical health was observed. This point has been echoed by Coleman and O’Hanlon (2008) who highlight that successful ageing / optimal ageing is achieved not only through acknowledgement of the associated losses (as with other stages of the lifespan) but also the successful adaptation and coping with the stresses and changes in life. It is taking control of those challenges and adapting through acquired life skills that is fundamental to mental, psychological and emotional health at all stages in the lifespan. With an increasingly ageing population this pervasive negative attitude clearly has negative effects on a growing proportion of UK residents. Over 65’s account for a substantial proportion of the hospital admissions in England and Wales. In the year April 2008 – March 2009 22.8% of the total admittance to NHS A&E departments in England was for those people aged 60 and over (Hospital Episode Statistics, 2010). With there being a pervasive negative attitude towards older people, it is possible that the care of these individuals may indeed be less than that given to a younger person. Gatz and Pearson (1988) believed that despite negative attitudes towards ageing not being global within the healthcare profession, there were specific biases that affected the way in which older patients received care. Duerson, Thomas, Chang and Stevens (1992) indicated that the attitudes held by staff can affect the treatment received and the way in which elderly people are treated. This was succinctly demonstrated where older people were not receiving the same diagnosis based on the same symptoms as younger people where the only differentiating feature was that of the patient’s age. 61 Not only that, but they also refer to findings that health care workers in general have been shown to hold negative attitudes towards older people. They cite Coccaro and Miles, (1984) who conducted a series of explicit measures demonstrating that ageism was a widespread issue even in the early 1980’s. James and Haley (1995) reviewed ageism in the German healthcare system and found several cases where clinicians considered psychotherapy with depressed elderly patients to be ineffective with no assessment other than initial categorisation by age. Similarly, Filipp and Schmitt (1995) found that medical professionals were refraining from treating patients with mental impairments because due to their age the conditions were considered irreversible. It is apparent that the unfavourable and stereotypic view of older people held by some clinicians is the crucial factor behind the inadequate treatment often being received. This is an enduring phenomenon and continues a long tradition which is by no means new in origin. Freud was reluctant and often refused to treat older patients because he didn’t believe that they had the remaining life span in which to experience the benefits (Woodward, 1991). As with the preponderance of research in this area, Duerson et al. (1992) conducted a series of attitude tests to medical students using explicit measures (Palmore’s Facts on Ageing Questionnaire (FAQ i&ii)). Primarily this questionnaire is employed to assess the knowledge of ageing but it is also accepted as an explicit measure of ageist attitudes. Duerson et al. (1992) found that the scores were no higher than those reported in the general population and no real increase was measured from pre to post educational training. They concluded that this was a reflection on the lack of specific geriatric training. Duerson suggested that in order to improve this knowledge and sensitise them to the growing needs of the elderly, medical students required more 62 specific geriatric training not simply training which focussed on the losses associated with ageing. She also highlighted that direct contact with older people and patients would help student clinicians improve their perceptions. This is a point also highlighted by Tam et al. (2006) who identified direct contact with older people as a factor improving implicit attitudes towards older people. Further they established that quality of contact can be the precursor to changing the explicit attitudes held. They identified that simply encountering and being exposed to older people could improve the implicit attitudes towards older people. This was under the condition that during this exposure, no overtly stereotypical experiences occurred such as a frail older person falling over. It was only when quality time was spent with an older person such as in conversation or a prearranged activity that an individual’s explicit attitudes were improved. This is a revision from Gatz and Pearson (1988) who identified that simple exposure to elderly people and ageing issues had been shown to reduce ageism. This point was reiterated by Grant (1996) who suggested that ageing texts often referred only to the problems rather than the successes with ageing and describing elderly people as suffering from multiple handicaps. She draws on the fact that this specific education should be given as a matter of course in institutions offering health care courses to increase the level of understanding and care given to elderly patients. The question remains, if the attitudes presented are clear within society and specifically within the health care system, can they be altered? It is difficult but not impossible to change attitudes and there are several ways in which attitudes can be changed or modified which fit into different categories. 63 Weakly held and less salient attitudes are easier to change than strongly held attitudes and as such, stronger attitudes are developed in areas which an individual (or in-group to which they belong) considers to be of higher salience. These strongly held attitudes can be either positive or negative but are usually clearly polar. In areas of limited or questionable importance, attitudes tend to be weakly held, ambivalent or neutral which means that they are more susceptible to change. There are six basic categories of attitude change strategies: • Changing the basic motivational function. These strategies are based on the theory that attitudes serve four basic functions: utilitarian, ego-defensive, value-expressive and knowledge. By changing the basic motivational function, the attitude towards the product can be changed (Sarnoff & Katz, 1954). • Associating the attitude object with a special group, event or cause. Attitudes can be altered by indicating the attitude object’s relationship to particular groups, events or causes. Concern for the environment has been one cause used recently (Petty, Wegener & Fabrigar, 1997). • Relating to conflicting attitudes. Generally people do not like dissonance and strive to reconcile this. If they can be shown that their attitude towards an attitude object is in conflict with another attitude, they may be induced to change one of the attitudes (Petty, Wegener & Fabrigar, 1997). 64 • Altering components in the multi-attribute model. These strategies attempt to change the evaluation of attributes by upgrading or downgrading significant attributes; change beliefs by introducing new information; and by adding an attribute, or by changing the salience ratings (Mitchell & Olson, 1981). • The elaboration likelihood model. This model suggests that attitudes can be changed by either central or peripheral routes to persuasion. In the central route, attitude change occurs because an individual seeks and evaluates additional information about the attitude object. In this case, motivation levels are high and the individual is willing to invest the time and effort to gather and evaluate the information, indicating a high level of involvement. Multi-attribute models are based on the central route to persuasion as attitudes are believed to be formed on the basis of important attributes/features and beliefs. In the peripheral route, individuals are either unwilling or unable to seek additional information. Involvement is low, so they must be offered secondary inducements in an attempt to influence attitude change. (Jones et al., 2003) It is of note that all of these strategies take the traditional view that attitude precedes behaviour, and use the relationship between attitude and behaviour to effect attitude change. Also each of these strategies can be used in an educational setting to impart knowledge and cause disequilibrium in the current schema held to force a reassessment of existing attributes and evaluations to modify the existing ageist attitude. This has been shown by Kite et al. (2005) who demonstrated that upon providing information about a person that countered existing negative ageist stereotypes, the negative attitudes can be diminished. An example of this would be 65 providing participants with pictures or video clips of older people participating in competitive sport. These images would be contrary to those stereotypes held and cause a re-evaluation to occur. More recently in an educational context, Westmoreland et al. (2009) demonstrated that through a well structured educational based on the psychological principles above, attitudes towards older people can be changed. They demonstrated, using a longitudinal study, that attitude change was possible and that reflexive accounts also illustrated a conscious awareness of this on the part of the students involved. It is in the utilisation of these strategies in the training of nurses and people involved in the care of older people that the pervasive negative attitudes can be challenged. In challenging these stereotypes and commonly held misconceptions the inequalities in care can also be addressed. The first step in this process is to assess the current standing and efficacy of existing gerontological awareness/training. From here, future studies can identify the gaps in training and trial interventions can be developed, tested and implemented. In summary, previous research has found that there are pervasive and ingrained negative implicit attitudes towards older people (Levy & Banaji, 2002). It has further demonstrated that those negative attitudes are also recorded on explicit measures of ageism (Depaola, Griffin, Young & Meimeyer, 2003). Exposure to older people can improve attitudes, however it occurs in differing ways. Contact in and of itself can improve implicit attitudes whereas only quality contact time can improve a person’s explicit attitude towards older people (Tam, Hewstone, Harwood, Voci & Kenworthy, 2006). These attitudes are not solely confined to the general populace but are also present in the medical profession where caring for older people is integral to their 66 daily routines (Duerson et al., 1992). Filipp and Schmitt (1995) also highlighted that medical professionals were refraining from treating older patients with mental impairments because due to their age the conditions were considered irreversible. It has been shown that these attitudes (implicit and explicit) can be altered in the favour of older people. Westmoreland et al. (2009) demonstrated that through a well structured educational based on the psychological principles above, attitudes towards older people can be changed. Despite there being research on attitudes towards older people, the quantity is representatively small and restricted in nature. Most of the studies use either implicit or explicit measures and as such only collect data on one facet of attitudes towards older people. The studies that have been done have generally used students and have used this group as a representative sample of the wider population without making due consideration for the effects of education on individuals’ implicit and explicit attitudes. Considering that there are clear behavioural implications in the manifestations of attitudes, there is surprisingly little research on care professionals providing care for younger and older people on a daily basis. Studies one and three will assess the attitudes of nurses in training and qualified nurses working in hospital environments to try and build a knowledge base in this gap. The studies will assess the effects of nurse training on attitudes towards older people as well as looking at the progression from this stage, post-training, when nurses are in situ caring for the general populous and those specifically caring for older people. This should aid the understanding of the training required to ensure that attitudes towards older people are more positive and that the care older people receive is of the same standard expected for other patient groups. Following this, two educational classroom based courses will 67 be assessed, looking at the effects of education about older people and ageing (Study Five) and of younger people (Study Two). This will assess the current psychological course content on two specific student groups enabling conclusions to be drawn about the current levels of educational interventions and the direction that future interventions may need to take. This is an important step because although there have been successful intervention measures in other countries, none have been conducted in the UK and none have to date influenced the content of training received by healthcare professionals in the UK. Further, Study Four will provide information on the effects of general higher level education on the implicit and explicit attitudes towards older people. As previously mentioned studies are often conducted with student samples and then generalise findings to the general populous. This is a potentially problematic procedure because as yet there is little known about the impact of general higher level education on attitudes towards older people. Finally as each of the studies has used the same data collection instruments and procedures, a meta-analysis will be conducted to assess the impacts of each of these variables on both implicit and explicit measures of ageism. This is a unique angle as to date no study has looked at both implicit and explicit measures of ageism with homogeneous and heterogeneous populations to provide a comprehensive picture of ageism. This thesis will present the following studies in order to address the gaps in existing research presented above: 1. A longitudinal study assessing the impact of a three year undergraduate nurse training degree on implicit and explicit attitudes towards older people. 68 2. A cross-sectional study investigating the impact of an educational intervention promoting younger people on implicit and explicit measures of ageism 3. A cross-sectional study investigating the impact of contact with older people in a nursing context on implicit and explicit measures of ageism. 4. A cross-sectional study assessing the impact of general higher level education on implicit and explicit attitudes towards older people 5. A longitudinal study assessing the effects of a Psychology of Ageing module on implicit and explicit attitudes towards older people 6. A cross-sectional study to assess the levels of implicit and explicit ageism of older people 7. A meta-analysis of all sample subsets For each of the above studies the same implicit and explicit measures were used so that there was parity between the scores returned in each of the samples. As the same tests were used, a meta-analysis could be conducted to give a holistic picture of attitudes towards older people. Further to this, it also allowed for a test-retest analysis to be conducted on the longitudinal studies, thus enabling reliability and consistency measures to be taken. It was important for there to be comparable scores so that the differences that occurred between groups could be more easily compared and the effects of any interventions more accurately reflected. Details of the procedure are provided in the next section. 69 Chapter Five General Methodology All studies followed the same procedure, testing materials and controls. To this end, a single comprehensive methodology section will be included to save repetition in the description of each investigation. However, for each study, separate participant information will be provided plus additional information regarding populations or variations in the test manipulations. Materials and Procedure Two measures were used: an explicit measure, the Fraboni Scale of Ageism (Fraboni, Saltstone & Hughes, 1990) and an implicit measure, the Implicit Association Test (Greenwald, McGhee & Schwartz, 1998). Explicit Measure: Aspects of the FSA are described here for the sake of clarification, but a full consideration of the relative merits is presented in Chapter Four. The Fraboni Scale of Ageism (Fraboni, Saltstone & Hughes, 1990) was chosen as a well established and well accepted explicit measure of ageism (Rupp, Vodanovich & Credé 2005). The Fraboni Scale of Ageism (FSA) was developed in 1989 as a way of building on past ageism scales which focused on age stereotypes and myths. The FSA is a 29-item Likert scale which uses a specific operational definition of ageism based on three of Allport's (1958) five levels of prejudice: "...antilocution (mere antipathetic 70 talk); avoidance (avoiding members of the disliked group); and discrimination (excluding members from the disliked group). The FSA requires participants to respond to either negative or positive age-related statements using a 4-point Likert scale (1=Strongly Disagree to 4=Strongly Agree). The total scores that can be recorded on this scale range from 29 – 116 with higher scores illustrating higher levels of ageism. Any score over 58 is illustrative of explicit ageism. To avoid habitual response errors, some items are reverse coded. For example, item 26 reads: "Most older people should not be allowed to renew their drivers licence." This was reverse coded so that a response of 1 (strong agreement) became 4, to reflect a more ageist response. The Fraboni Scale of Ageism (FSA) has been shown to have good levels of internal consistency with a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of .86. (Fraboni et al., 1990). An exploratory factor analysis supported the antilocution, avoidance, and discrimination factors, these accounting for 23.3%, 7.2%, and 7.0% of the variance, respectively (Fraboni et al., 1990). Reliability has been shown on each of the factors mentioned with the following Cronbach’s coefficient alpha reliabilities: Antilocution (.76), Avoidance (.77), and Discrimination (.65). A copy of the scale presented to participants is in Appendix 1. The scale is an untimed paper and pencil measure and the items are always presented in the same order. This is in accordance with standardised instructions. 71 Implicit Measure: Aspects of the IAT are described here for the sake of clarification, but a full consideration of the relative merits of different implicit measures is presented in Chapter Two. Several implicit measures have been developed, the principal of which are: the Affective Priming Task (Fazio, Jackson, Daunton & Williams, 1995), the Implicit Association Test (IAT – Greenwald & Banaji, 1995) and the Extrinsic Affective Simon Task (De Houwer & Eelen, 1998). The IAT has scored well against other measures of reliability. In a recent review, De Houwer & De Bruycker, (2007) demonstrated that the IAT has shown consistently high internal consistency scores of .83. In addition to this Schultz et al. (2004) also highlighted the fact the IAT produced stable test/re-test correlations of 0.46 over immediate, 1 week and 4 week periods. As already noted in Chapter Two, the r values for the IAT are significantly better than those returned on other comparable tests. The IAT has also shown to be more of a robust measure of attitude (Dasgupta, 2000; Kim, 2003, Nosek et al., 2007). Its supporters argue that it is difficult to fake, in part because automatic responses are a key part of the participant response and these are hard to control. As has already been noted, Kim (2003) tested the controllability of the IAT in two studies using racism as the measure, and flowers and insects as a control. He found that in both cases the results from the IAT could not be controlled / faked even if under instruction to do so. Kim demonstrated that the only way to control 72 answers was to be told how to do so (responding slowly to a subset of the stimuli). He also identified that even after multiple trials, participants did not spontaneously discover the strategy for controlling their responses. From this he concluded that the IAT was a robust measure, and is clearly more so than explicit measures that have been shown to be susceptible to self presentational bias as well as capturing more subtle biases that may be undetectable using self report measures (Dasgupta et al., 2000). The usefulness of the IAT is due to a number of factors: resistance to self presentational bias (Egloff & Schmuckle, 2002); its adaptability to numerous forms of concept measurement (Greenwald & Nosek, 2001); and its lack of dependence on introspective access to the association strengths being measured (Greenwald et al., 2002). Because of these factors allowing the IAT to be used as a flexible yet powerful measure of implicit attitude, it was chosen as the implicit measure for this investigation. The IAT requires participants to categorise target concepts with descriptors representing positive and negative poles of an attribution dimension. It is argued that when a pairing appears between a concept and a congruent descriptor, mapping them to the same response key is considerably easier than if the pairing is incongruent. In other words, if the participant believes that the image and the description match, they will respond faster than when there is a mismatch. For example, for most people there would be a quicker response time for pairing a picture of Adolf Hitler into the category man/bad rather than man/good. The response latency difference measures the extent to which positive and negative evaluations are attributed to the target 73 concepts. This is calculated by summing the response times for the congruent and incongruent pairings and then taking the time for the congruent pairings away from the incongruent condition. The greater the time difference, it is argued, the more negative the attitude is towards the target concept (Greenwald et al., 2003). In essence, when translated into real term scores produced from the IAT, positive scores equate to negative implicit attitudes towards older people and the higher the number, the more negative the attitude expressed. The IAT used in these studies was presented using proprietary software (E-prime created by Psychology Software Inc). The images and words used for the IAT were supplied and previously tested for suitability and effectiveness by Banaji and her research team. Their contribution and assistance is gratefully acknowledged. The IAT consisted of both practice and test datasets. Initially the participants were presented with simple pleasant/unpleasant categories and similarly old/young categories: this was to familiarise both with the concepts being introduced and also with the responses required from them. After the familiarisation, pairings were made of either good/ young or good/old. For the purpose of internal counterbalancing, the categories were then reversed and presented to participants with both practice and test blocks. Each test had identical words and images; these were presented in a random order of the programmes generation. Again as previously, a test set was presented first to familiarise the participants with the concept of response pairings before the measurement sets were presented. Table 2 is a representation of the order of presentations used in the IAT programme (the order is reversed in the counterbalanced trials). 74 Table 2: The order of presentation for the IAT Block No. of Trials Items Assigned to Left Key Items Assigned to Right Key B1 12 Old Faces Young Faces B2 16 Good Words Bad Words B3 28 Old Faces + Good Words Young Faces + Bad Words B4 28 Old Faces + Good Words Young Faces + Bad Words B5 12 Young Faces Old Faces B6 28 Young Faces + Good Words Old Faces + Bad Words B7 28 Young Faces + Good Words Old Faces + Bad Words A copy of the IAT is available upon request. To produce scores for the implicit test, the D Score algorithm has been used. This divides the differences between congruent and incongruent pairings by the individual standard deviations to produce a normed implicit score. Full details of the algorithm can be found in Appendix 4 on page 236. Participants completed the implicit and explicit tasks either singly or as part of a group with each participant working in a separate and isolated cubicle or booth. The order of presentation of items was counterbalanced. The participants provided their responses anonymously, but were given anonymised identification numbers so that: (1) potential order of presentation effects could be measured; and 75 (2) it was possible to match participant responses in the longitudinal study component of the research. Ethical Considerations Prior to the commencement of any research, the programme of study was submitted to and approved by the University of Glamorgan Ethics Committee. It was considered that there would be no psychological harm or distress caused to participants so no special measures were observed. However, the following considerations were implemented. 1) Confidentiality: Participant records were anonymised. All information collected will be held in a secure location for a five-year period after publication of results. This will then be destroyed in accordance with accepted University procedures. 2) Right to withdraw: Participants were fully informed of their right to withdraw at any stage, without penalty. They were also informed of their right to refuse to answer any questions. 3) Informed consent: Participants were informed prior to commencing the study about what they would be asked to do. They were not fully informed about the hypotheses of the study in advance, because of concerns that this might bias explicit responses. All groups were capable of providing their own informed consent. No deception occurred at any point and all participants were debriefed at the cessation of testing. 4) Full debrief: Participants were debriefed after completing both the implicit and explicit measures to share with them the objectives of the research and to 76 allow discussion of their experiences. These are discussed within Chapter Seven. 5) Potential distress: There was little chance of the study creating distress. Participants were asked to complete accepted IAT measures - no overtly unpleasant or threatening imagery was involved. However, participants were permitted to withdraw at any stage, as already stated. 6) Co-operation with participating institutions: The experimenter agreed to comply with any further checks or other requirements imposed by participating institutions (e.g. hospital, medical surgery and residential care homes). Procedure Upon entering the computer laboratory, participants were spaced one computer apart from each other so that they each had their own work space and maintained the individual test area integrity. Each participant sat in front of a terminal with the testing software pre-loaded and the IAT instruction screen visible. The participants were initially given verbal instructions advising them how they should complete the test, stressing that the computerised implicit section be completed first. This instruction was included as it was considered that completing the explicit measure first may indeed affect the participants due to the content of some statements. Further on screen instructions were provided detailing the manner in which completion of the IAT should occur. Participants were advised that if they were unclear of the procedure after reading this additional information, clarification should be sought from the experimenter in the room. 77 When the participants had completed the IAT, another information screen was presented advising them to continue to the explicit paper measure. As part of the initial procedural statement, participants were informed that the paper questionnaire was to be completed by circling the most relevant answer category for each of the statement items. This was again completed in silence. When each of the participants had completed the two tests, they remained at their stations until all participants had completed the test. All participants received a verbal debrief giving contact details of experiment staff, supervisors and support services. In addition to this the relevant certification (see appendix two) and debrief (see appendix three) were also given to participants for their reference. In those longitudinal studies, participants were also all reminded that they would be required to undertake a similar experiment at the end of their academic programme and that the contact details they had provided to enable contact at this stage would remain secure. Scoring Explicit Measure: The Fraboni Scale of Ageism (FSA) requires participants to respond to either negative or positive age related statements using a 4-point Likert-scale (1 representing strong agreement with the statement and 4 representing strong disagreement). In order for higher scores to reflect more ageism some items were reverse coded. The scores are then summed for each participant to enable mean group population scores to be calculated. Any score over 58 was a score denoting explicit ageism. 78 Implicit Measure: Scoring for the IAT has been taken from the improved scoring algorithm devised by Greenwald, Nosek and Banaji (2003). The IAT programme collects the response latencies for all of the congruent and incongruent pairings made by each participant. Each participant’s scores are then collated and calculated separately prior to the group aggregation. Test scores/latencies were used from Blocks B3, B4, B6 and B7 where previously only data collected in B4 and B7 were used. This change was made to double the overall amount of data collected and after testing it was established that including these blocks which were previously used only as practice blocks did not adversely affect the integrity of the data. Initially each of the congruent and incongruent pairing response latencies were grouped so that mean scores could be calculated. In each of these groups, any categorisation errors were removed with then an additional 600ms added to the latency recorded when the correct category pairing was given. This error adjustment yielded more significant results and removed the often quicker responses recorded when a participant simply hit the keys to record a quick response without observing the categories. Any participant who returned a latency of over 10,000ms had that trial disregarded as Greenwald et al. (2003) identified this group to have simply responded too slowly either through distraction or lack of concentration and their response was no longer reflective of their attitude. 79 When these data adjustments had been made, the mean congruent and incongruent response latencies were calculated for each participant. The congruent latencies were then taken from the incongruent scores and the higher the resulting number, the more preference was shown towards younger people/stronger the ageist attitudes. Normalisation of each of the participant’s scores was then undertaken whereby the score was divided by the standard deviation of each individual’s responses. This was done because magnitudes of differences between experimental treatment means are often correlated with variability of the data from which the means are computed (Greenwald, Nosek & Banaji, 2003). Using the standard deviation as a divisor adjusts differences between means for this effect of underlying variability. This produces the D score which has been shown to protect against cognitive skill confounds in participants (Cai, Sriram & Greenwald, 2004) which was previously highlighted as an issue with the IAT (McFarland & Crouch, 2002). Any positive score indicates a preference for younger over older people and a negative score reflects a preference for older over younger people. The computational scoring algorithm can be found in Appendix Four on pp. 236 80 Chapter Six Empirical Studies Prior to the advent of the IAT (Greenwald, McGhee & Schwartz, 1998) the overwhelming majority of ageism (and other attitude) research centred on self report explicit measures (Cummings, Kropf & DeWeaver, 2000; Catterall & Maclaran, 2001; Depaola, Griffin, Young & Neimeyer, 2003). These provided a picture of a person’s or group’s expressed attitude but did not access their implicitly held opinions. Since their advent, implicit measures, with the IAT being the forerunner, have enabled researchers to access this previously inaccessible attitudinal construct. This new ability was quickly adopted into many fields of research including: racism (Baron & Banaji, 2006); clinical psychology (Teachman, Gregg & Woody, 2001); neuroscience (Phelps et al., 2000); health psychology (Teachman et al., 2003) and even in a more practical setting with customer preferences in market research (Maison, Greenwald & Bruin, 2001). Despite ageing and ageism receiving a lot of interest in research, policy, practice and in the population as a whole, the majority of the research evidence base as shown in this review is rooted in explicit measure research (Kalavar, 2001; Mueller-Johnson et al., 2007). Even when implicit measures have been used, Levy & Banaji (2002) have shown these tend to be with single groups or comparing homogeneous groups. By homogenous groups, it is meant those groups with similar characteristics (i.e. age, gender, education etc). Studies that compare only these groups are generally controlling for certain factors but in doing so are not looking at factors such as age differences. By not looking at heterogeneous groups 81 (where key characteristics between groups are different) there are central variables not being assessed thus leaving a wide research gap. This research is not only addressing the lack of implicit evidence base in the field of ageism but is also widening the scope of the research done. From a review of literature, six main studies have been identified and conducted using eight different participant populations. These populations are used in comparison for specific investigation into the effects of direct contact and specific education on attitudes held. In addition to this, a meta-analysis has been conducted to compare each of the groups to ascertain where any group differences may lie. Further to this, as the testing included the administration of the Fraboni Scale of Ageism, these comparisons between groups can be repeated for explicit measures while clearly demonstrating the dichotomy between the implicit and explicit scores. More unique to this study, however, is the collection of data over a period of time with the same cohort. The vast majority of studies, even when they have used implicit measures have relied solely on the cross-sectional data available through single testing. This study includes two longitudinal samples, one investigating the impact of university based nurse training compared to other higher level education and the other investigating a specific geropsychology module. The longitudinal sample is important to accurately reflect any changes in both individual and cohort attitudes (implicit and explicit) due to interventions and situational variables. Each set of results will be presented with their literature rationale and a description of the sample, stating any deviations from the standard testing detailed in the methodology section. In brief, the studies will look firstly at the effects of three year 82 undergraduate nursing and psychology degree programmes on attitudes towards older people. Second, the effects of a youth-centric education course will be assessed, followed by the effects of direct contact with older people from working in different nursing environments. The fourth study will look more generally at the effects of higher level education on attitudes towards older people, whereas the fifth will specifically look at the effects on attitudes of a Psychology of Ageing course. The penultimate study will look at the attitudes of older people before a meta-analysis is presented. The order of this has been chosen to present a coherent argument about how attitudes are affected primarily by education. First, a longitudinal study enables a picture to emerge on the effects of a non-specific higher education and a nursing course on attitudes towards older people. From this it is of interest to understand the directional nature of the IAT and the effects of undertaking education specifically on the stimuli diametrically opposed to that which is being measured (youth-centric course for ageism measure). Building on the literature review and the findings from the nursing students, the direct contact hypothesis was tested in a nursing environment which also allows for implications to be drawn about care of older people. With knowledge gained specifically about two university courses, it is of interest to ascertain more widely the impact of higher level education with participants who have not undertaken higher or further education. Building on this, the effects of specific age related education are assessed relative to the general adult population prior to looking at the effects of ageing on attitudes towards older people when effects of education have been controlled for. Finally a meta-analysis is presented to retrospectively compare each of the groups, something that has not previously been done and something that allows different factors to be assessed. 83 Study One – A longitudinal cohort study with Nursing and Psychology undergraduate students Introduction The effects of education on self presentational bias in explicit measures have long been established (Goffman, 1959); the more educated a person is, the more aware they become of the subjective and societal norms against which they will measure themselves and in turn be measured. How this affects the implicit attitudes held by a person is not as well established, with no longitudinal data existing that follow cohorts looking at this particular factor. Cross-sectional studies have been conducted that suggest particular student cohorts have different attitudes towards older people (Jelenec and Steffens, 2002). However, a key criticism of these studies could indeed be that they are cross-sectional. As data have only been collected at one point, there is no way of knowing for example, whether the results are due to individual differences between the types of people attracted to these courses or whether the course itself has influenced the attitudes. Similarly cross-sectional data have been collected illustrating the differences between implicit and explicit attitudes (Betsch, Plessner & Schallies, 2004) but again few give any picture of changes over time. Ageism in nursing care has been extensively researched, specifically with regard to Nursing students (Duerson et. al., 1992; Gatz & Pearson, 1988; Grant, 1996). Research findings indicate that generally negative attitudes towards older people are held and expressed. Higgins, Van der Riet, Slater and Peek (2007) found this pervasive negative attitude to be clearly apparent in Nursing students and expressed through marginalisation, segregation and “Chinese whispers” style stereotyping. 84 Despite the less than rosy outlook, Cheong, Wong and Koh (2009) have identified that through successful education, medical students in Singapore are reported to hold positive attitudes towards older people. This is, however, a test with explicit measures and a finding that could be culturally specific. More recently, Gonzales, MorrowHowell and Gilbert (2010) established that this is a tide that can be turned. By implementing a bespoke training package, they have illustrated how the attitudes of medical students can indeed be made more positive through a mixture of education and contact with older people. Each of these studies has been conducted using explicit measures as the key measure and with the exception of Gonzales, Morrow-Howell & Gilbert, has employed a cross-sectional methodology. The measurement of Nursing and Medical students’ attitudes towards older people is of high importance as Duerson, Thomas, Chang and Stevens (1992) indicated that the attitudes held by staff can affect the treatment received and the way in which elderly people are treated. With increasing numbers of older people requiring hospitalisation or medical treatment due to an ageing population, the attitudes and associated behaviours of medical professionals will be of increasing importance. Previous studies have indicated that the attitudes held by medical students are predominantly negative, however, these have been single groups. It is also of importance to be able to compare these groups to other students not undergoing medical training to ascertain whether it is the medical training per se that is resulting in the negative attitudes flourishing. The current study seeks to build upon the research base looking at the attitudes held by Nursing students. This study sought to follow two cohorts of students, one undertaking a higher education undergraduate degree level programme in psychology and the other undertaking a higher education undergraduate degree level programme 85 in nursing. These two cohorts were chosen primarily to assess the efficacy of nurse training in reducing ageism. Psychology students were selected as a base measure as there is no specific gerontological education received during the programme, only minimal content included in a developmental context. In addition to this, the gender balance on both courses is very similar. Both implicit and explicit measures of attitudes towards older people will be taken to track any changes caused by the education programme being undertaken. The hypotheses to be tested are: (H1) There will be a significant difference between the implicit and explicit scores returned by each of the groups at each time point. (H2) Explicit attitudes will not be significantly changed by the delivery of either Nursing or Psychology undergraduate programmes. (H3) There will be a significant change in the implicit attitudes of the Nursing students between commencement and completion of their course. Participants The initial cohort comprised 74 students. Of this sample, 40 were Psychology students (Mean age: 24.0, S.D.: 8.0) and 34 were Nursing students (Mean age: 22.6, S.D.: 7.6). At mid-point testing, the cohort comprised of 65 students. Of these, 34 were Psychology students (Mean age: 25.3, S.D.: 7.7) and 31 were Nursing students (Mean age: 23.4, S.D.: 7.4). However, by the end of the course of study, due to course attrition, withdrawal and non-attendance the sample for the full longitudinal analysis was reduced to 52 students. Of these, 26 were Psychology students (Mean age: 28.0, S.D.: 8.1) and were 26 Nursing students (Mean age: 25.2, S.D.: 7.9). Of the Psychology students, 4 were male and 22 were female with an age range of 20-47 86 years. Of the Nursing students, 3 were male and 23 were female with an age range of 20-46 years. Results will be reported using all participants where appropriate and when correlation analysis is being conducted only those students completing all three testing phases will be included2. The initial sample was an opportunity sample of two targeted undergraduate course programmes at the University of Glamorgan. In addition to standard course content, none of the participants had received any additional gerontological interventions or training prior to testing. Methodology The materials used and overall methodology were similar to those presented in the General Methodology section (see pp. 70). The sole difference was that this study was a longitudinal cohort study taking measures of implicit and explicit ageism at three time points rather than on a single occasion. Measures were taken at the start of the respective degree programmes, again in the second year to obtain midpoint data, and finally again at the end of the courses. This method was chosen to gain a base measure of attitudes and then to ascertain any effects caused by either the degree or specific nurse training. Contact details (name, email, telephone number) for each of the participants were taken at the start of the project and stored in a secure filing cabinet in a locked storage room for the duration of the project so that participants could be contacted again and linked to their participant numbers to allow for cohort comparisons. The contact details and participant numbers were stored separately so a theft or accidental release of a document would not lead to the identification of participants. 2 Those Psychology students who undertook the psych-gerontological education module were not included in this sample. 87 Results The descriptive results for both groups at each time point are presented prior to ANOVA tests being conducted. Further to this both correlations between implicit and explicit measures and correlations over time are presented. Table 3: Table of longitudinal means Group Psychology Students Nursing Students D-Score D-Score FSA Score FSA (mean) S.D. (mean) S.D. 1 .4526 .428 54.4 10.13 2 .7588 .759 53.8 7.56 3 .7091 .710 54.2 9.36 1 .2631 .474 49.5 9.28 2 .7028 .610 47.3 9.07 3 .6206 .671 47.8 9.57 Year The descriptive results above illustrate the mean scores received by each of the groups (Nursing and Psychology students) at each measurement time in both implicit (DScore) and explicit (FSA) measures. The explicit results are expressed in graphical form in Fig 2 88 Fig. 2: Graph illustrating explicit longitudinal data At time point one there was no significant correlation between the implicit and explicit measures (r= -0.094, p=0.495). Similarly at time two there was no significant correlation (r= 0.149, p=0.247). Finally this finding was repeated at time three (r= 0.069, p=0.628). Data were analysed using a mixed ANOVA on the PASW statistical package. This showed a significant group difference in explicit scores (F(1,183)=19.157; p<0.0001). The within participants explicit measure was, however, not significant (F(2,183)=0.406; N.S.). Further to this the interaction was also not significant (F(2,183)=0.160; N.S.). Post hoc analysis using the LSD test of pairwise comparisons found that there were no significant differences between any of the years of study. 89 The implicit scores are shown graphically in Fig. 3 Fig 3: Graph illustrating implicit longitudinal data The implicit data were analysed using a mixed ANOVA which found there was no significant group difference (F(1,183)=1.338; N.S.). There was, however, a significant within subjects difference for the implicit measure (F(2,183)=6.321; p<0.01). There was also no significant interaction effect (F(2,183)=0.194; N.S.). Post hoc analysis using the LSD test of pairwise comparisons found that there was a significant difference in scores at time one and time two (p<.001) and times one and three (p<0.05) but not between time two and three. Overall model analysis using the Wilks’ Lambda indicates that there are both significant within (F(6,362)=2.41; p<0.05) and between (F(3,181)=10.76; p<0.0001) 90 participant effects. However, no significant interaction was found (F(6,362)=0.694; N.S.). Due to the longitudinal nature of this study, the data were also analysed for correlations between individuals’ scores over time using PASW. These are summarised in Table 4. Table 4: Table of Nursing students’ explicit measure correlations Time 1 Time 2 Time 3 Time 1 - .639** .568** Time 2 - - .707** **. Correlation significant at the 0.01 level The Nursing students’ explicit results correlated significantly at each time point. Similar results have been recorded for the Psychology undergraduate cohort. Table 5: Table of Psychology students’ explicit measure correlations Time 1 Time 2 Time 3 Time 1 - .611** .714** Time 2 - - .646** **. Correlation significant at the 0.01 level The Psychology students’ explicit results correlated significantly at each time point. 91 Of further interest are the correlations for both groups in terms of their implicit scores which do not reflect the consistency demonstrated by the explicit measures. Table 6: Table of Nursing students’ implicit measure correlations Time 1 Time 2 Time 3 Time 1 - .405* .367 Time 2 - - .204 *. Correlation significant at the 0.05 level Table 7: Table of Psychology students’ implicit measure correlations Time 1 Time 2 Time 3 Time 1 - .169 .314 Time 2 - - .329 Neither set of implicit correlations show the same level of consistency as the explicit measure, a point that will be discussed later. As illustrated, the explicit measures correlate at each time point for both Psychology and Nursing students. However, there are no time point correlations for Psychology students on the implicit measure and only between time one and two for the Nursing students. Discussion As is evident from the descriptive statistics, there is a clear difference between the implicit measures of ageism for both of the groups. As an overview, it is clear from each time point that there is no correlation between implicit and explicit measures, therefore, the results support the supposition that implicit and explicit attitudes are 92 different constructs. It is further evident that explicit results stay the same over time but there is a significant difference between groups such that Psychology students have higher levels of expressed explicit bias. These differences are also stable over time. On the other hand, there are no correlations between the implicit scores recorded except at one time measure. The implicit measures start by showing an embedded preference for young over old with attitudes getting worse in the second year and staying high in the third. There are no significant differences between groups on the implicit measures. The above allow for the first hypothesis (there will be a significant difference between the implicit and explicit scores returned by each of the groups at each time point) to be accepted. From the commencement of the undergraduate university courses there is a significant shift in attitudes. This shift was not in the expected direction where it may be assumed attitudes towards older people would become better, instead the attitudes towards them became worse. This worsening of attitudes is something that is common for both Psychology and Nursing undergraduates. Despite the course having a larger overall effect on the Nursing students, the attitudes expressed by them still remain less ageist than those expressed by the Psychology students. With regard to findings on explicit measures, it would appear that participants are subject to self presentational bias (Goffman, 1959) whereby the individual is expressing attitudes under conscious control to place themselves in the best possible socially acceptable light. Subjective norms are adopted through expectation of this acceptability and despite interventions, educational or otherwise, the understanding of what is socially acceptable or expected is ingrained and as such expressed when explicitly questioned. Unlike the implicit scores, the explicit scores are stable over the 93 three data collection phases, which supports the second hypothesis (explicit attitudes will not be significantly changed by the delivery of either Nursing or Psychology undergraduate programmes) to be accepted. This is likely to be due to the acceptance of what socially acceptable responses are and despite any internal changes, participants are aware that social norm responses will hold them in better regard. This point will be readdressed when analysing data from the adult population in Study Four. This self presentational bias is also a possible explanation for the strong correlations, something that will be discussed further later in the discussion. Implicit attitudes are not, however, subject to the same self presentational bias and as such, any interventions (including educational) that could change these would be measurable through testing. As there is no significant difference in the implicit scores between the groups and in fact they appear to change in a relatively consistent way, it can be argued that the education received by Nursing students has no more of a positive or negative effect than does the completion of a psychology undergraduate course. The fact that in both courses the implicit test results demonstrate a worsening attitude could seem to indicate one of two things. Either: higher level education in general affects the implicit attitudes towards older people in a negative way; or, that within higher education, the environment and lack of contact with older people causes an increased preference for younger people rather than an implicit disliking for older people per se. What is clear is that unless the intervention being used has been specifically tailored to reduce negative attitudes towards older people (Gonzales, Morrow-Howell & Gilbert, 2010), it does not appear to significantly influence the implicit attitudes held. 94 Of consideration is the progression of implicit attitudes over time. Unlike the explicit attitudes which stayed relatively stable, there was a statistically significant increase in the D-Measure score between time one and time two and between time one and time three. This was expected for the Nursing students, allowing for the third hypothesis (there will be a significant change in the implicit attitudes of the Nursing students between commencement and completion of their course) to be accepted. In addition to the predicted outcome, the same was also true with the Psychology undergraduate students, something that will be discussed later. This increase in D-Score is illustrative of an increase in implicit ageist attitude strength. The reasons for this marked increase are discussed in detail below but in brief, could either be due to the course content of the first year influencing the implicitly held beliefs or from the immersion into student life through the first year of study. The reason for the plateau from time two to three can again be explained through either of these theories. First, it could be that the course content, despite still including age specific information, has less of an impact in the final year than it did in the first as individuals had little previous ageing knowledge base. Due to this, the effect on increasing or decreasing implicit ageism during the final year is negligible (and not significant) as the information which affects the attitude has in the main already been assimilated. Second, the attitude may have stabilised in the final year due to the stability of the living environment. There is a big change when moving into a university environment with the sheer number of young people and the sheltered environment that this affords. By the final year, the participants will have experienced this big change and consequential shift in their attitude and any subsequent shift would be marginal. 95 The first possibility outlined above would seem to suggest that higher level education makes people more implicitly ageist. Although only a small part of the course, ageing is covered in both courses from either a medical perspective or a developmental context. It has been previously acknowledged that there was a minor element of ageing content in the psychology course; it has to be further considered that this may have affected the results. In psychology the approach is designed to give a reflective account of the ageing process but will invariably always have more information on the negative factors associated with ageing including the losses (memory, mobility etc) and increased incidence of illness (Whitborne & Hulicka, 1990). It can be argued that the weighting on negative ageing information causes the assimilation and aggregation with pre-existing information/schema creating a more negative impression than that which previously existed. The inclusion of this additional information from the first year of study into existing schema could then strengthen the pre-existing negative implicit bias and cause the marked increase in reaction times differences evident from the data displayed. Returning to the argument that the attitude measured does not in fact demonstrate a worsening implicit ageist bias, but instead an increasing implicit preference for younger people. At the commencement of this study, there were eight mature students (over 25) in the Nursing sample and four in the Psychology sample. The rest of the participants had come to university straight from A-levels (or equivalent) and of those, the majority were in student accommodation. It is a safe assumption that the majority of students living in university halls of residence will be under the age of 25 and as such would be for the purposes of the IAT, classified in the younger section. These students will spend much if not all of their time with people of their own age, 96 creating new friendships and generally experiencing undergraduate life. This increased exposure to younger people would increase the strength of the attitudes held. Previous research by Auty and Lewis (2004) found increasing attitude strength in which ever direction attitudes were previously held (unless experiences changed the valence) which from time one data would suggest implicitly ageist. To resolve this issue, future studies could include a longitudinal investigation but vary the students and courses assessed. If the sample of students included both residential and non-residential as well as ‘mature’ and school-leaver age students, there would be control over the type of environment that the students were exposed to in addition to the course content. The second factor to be controlled would be the courses being undertaken. The current samples were chosen due to the sample homogeneity (age, gender etc) which despite being a strength, has resulted in two courses being assessed where age related content has been included. It would be of interest when teamed with the improved sampling, to include undergraduate courses that do not include age related information (e.g. computing, maths). By including these courses, the effect of general higher level education could be seen as well as any mediating effects of living with peers and being surrounded 24/7 only by people of a youth demographic. As an extension to the study it may be of interest to assess the attitudes of those who undertake a distance learning course (e.g. Open University) to assess the impact solely of the traditional university environment on the implicit and explicit attitudes held. The correlational data are of specific interest as it is clear that over time, the scores on the explicit measures for each participant correlate significantly. This means that when looking at the individual, their specific score at each time point correlates and 97 this is true for both Psychology and Nursing students. When analysing the implicit scores, however, it is clear that that scores returned by each individual participant do not consistently correlate with other implicit scores they recorded at the other time points. This disparity has two core possible explanations, concerning either the nature of the constructs or the nature of the tests. The explicit measure correlates significantly at each time period with each group and although returning at times what are relatively low “r” values, these are nonetheless significant and show that there is consistency in the way in which participants are responding. This should, however, be taken in context. Kline (2000) states that correlations should ideally be over .9 but should not fall below .7 for them to demonstrate good internal reliability. The effect size (as measured by the size of the correlation) is not particularly high (at least as required by Kline, 2000) but that nonetheless, there is a consistently significant relationship between scores. As previously explained, the reason for score consistency is likely to be due to the nature of self presentational bias. The participants are aware of the socially acceptable answer and as such at each time point they answer each of the statements on the FSA in a way that would reflect these subjective norms. This answering strategy is possible in most explicit measures and the FSA is no exception. The individual items are quite clear in their intent so from this, a socially acceptable answer is easily provided. As the measure is a straightforward one, despite the participants being unlikely to remember the questions between data collection points, similar responses would be easily provided thus explaining not only the strength and direction of the attitudes but also their stability and correlation. 98 As has shown to be the case throughout, the implicit measure provides a different picture. There are no significant correlations within the groups between any of the time points (except between time one and time two for nurses) and the correlations returned are very weak. This suggests that the scores returned on an individual basis fluctuate between time periods and as such are not a stable indicator of implicit attitudes. It would be expected that each time point (similarly to the explicit measures) would be correlated significantly to the other implicit data returned on an individual level. An initial supposition from this could be that although the IAT performs well as a group measure, this study does not support the IAT as a consistent measure of individual implicit attitudes. The results would hold that individual fluctuations are such over time that few predictions can indeed be made on an individual level about future attitudes or behaviours. If true, this could be worth serious consideration prior to using the IAT as an individual measure both in terms of academic research and theory development but also in practical terms for marketing and screening tools. If the fluctuations are such that only basic direction rather than strength of an attitude can be accurately predicted, the use of the IAT for screening personality types or attitudes held on an individual basis should be severely scrutinised. There is, however, an alternative explanation for the differentiation of scores between time points. The focus of this study was to investigate the effects of higher level education and specifically undergraduate nurse training on attitudes to older people. As such there is an educational course present that has been discussed throughout this section. The education is not solely dedicated at any stage to the promotion of innovative ageing or reducing ageism and as such there has not been an observable flux in the explicit measurement scores. The implicit test is attempting to measure 99 attitudes outside of the conscious control of the participant and as such the effects of any intervention or education could have a greater impact. The impact of education on any one person is not a linear process and as such different methods and content can affect different people in different ways. Due to the differing impact of the education being received at each of the time points during the undergraduate programme, it stands to reason that an individual’s score may not correlate as the scores fluctuate (within the same valence). If this were indeed the case, the IAT itself could be considered a stable measure all things being equal without the presence of an intervention measure. The IAT has been shown to be a stable measure over time (Perugini, 2005) even on an individual basis. However, in the meta-analysis conducted by Perugini, none of the testing was with groups undergoing any form of intervention. Similarly to the argument surrounding exposure and environment detailed above, the nature of the university setting may also have positively affected the preferences for younger persons as opposed to negatively affecting the attitudes towards older people. So, as previously suggested, it would be prudent to conduct the longitudinal data collection controlling for sample demographics and surrounding environment to ascertain in this case whether the fluctuations in individual IAT scores (lack of correlation over time) was due to the educational courses or environment. In addition to those retests, a longitudinal control group should be employed to test the correlation of time point data in a group where no manipulation/intervention is present. If the results from this still suggest that there are no statistically significant correlations at the different time points with all other things being equal then this would support the initial supposition that the IAT should be used with caution on an individual basis. There is, however another plausible explanation. Responses on the IAT are in effect reaction time measures. Longitudinal studies (Lovden, Li, Shing & 100 Lindenberger, 2007) have reported quite low inter-session correlations (.2 to .3 levels). Therefore, it is not surprising that the IAT shows low correlations between test sessions. What matters is that the negative attitudes (which measure relative differences between positive and negative responses) remain consistent. Thus, caution must be used with the IAT as the absolute size of the score is almost certain to fluctuate. What is important to note, however, is if this is the case, it does not invalidate findings that look at relative differences between groups as they are comparing a single time point. Either way, more investigation is needed to conclusively support whether specific or general higher level education have an impact on the implicit attitudes held about older people. Conclusions It can be concluded from these results that Psychology students have more negative implicit and explicit attitudes towards older people than do Nursing students. However, neither Nursing students nor Psychology students expressed explicitly negative ageist attitudes. This explicit trend was stable throughout the testing period which can be explained through higher level awareness and self presentational bias. Implicitly, both groups expressed negative attitudes from the outset with a marked increase in ageist scores following the completion of the first and second halves of their degree programmes. This could be due to either situational or intervention factors so further studies have been recommended to clarify this point. Individual IAT test score correlations over time are weak and not significant, again possibly caused by the intervention or situational factors so similarly additional tests have been recommended for clarity purposes. Further to the latter tests, it is possible that the use of the IAT for individual level attitude and preference assessment could be called into 101 question, therefore, longitudinal retest recommendations should be undertaken post haste. 102 Study Two – A cross-sectional study using Early Years students to ascertain directional preference in the IAT Introduction In Study One, it was noted that the increasingly negative implicit scores might be due to increased negativity towards older adults, or conversely, could be explained by increasingly positive attitudes towards younger people. The IAT measures attitude strength and valence through paired associations using opposing words and images, with the results illustrating an implicit preference. When measuring a cross-sectional sample there are few issues with this. When assessing the impact of an intervention measure or programme of education in a longitudinal setting, however, it becomes apparent that the direction of the effect from the intervention is an important consideration. Steinman and Karpinski (2008) have shown that through priming that preference of one category can be increased to the detriment of the opposing category. Of interest was that the rating per se of the second category did not decrease, simply that it appeared to do so with the promotion of the first category. The previous study illustrated this point. However, does the increase in apparent implicit ageism come from the educational programmes’ content promoting youth development thus increasing youth preference or from the ageing content reaffirming existing negative ageist stereotypes? Alternatively, as was raised in the discussion of the previous study, the university environment might increase youth preference, thus only coincidentally appearing to indicate an increase in ageist attitudes. 103 The current study addresses a key component of this question. If the educational environment can have an effect, then is it possible for an educational programme to negatively affect implicit ageist attitudes when no ageing research literature of any kind is covered in the course? To address this question, the following study was devised. A group of students were chosen with as similar a demographic background as possible to the Nursing and Psychology students used in the previous study, but who did not study ageing as any part of their course. The group in question all took the Early Years degree at the same university. Educational and geographical background was similar, and many of the lecturers on the Psychology course also taught on the Early Years course. Thus, the environment was as close as was practical without including an ageing component. Although the students were very similar in many respects, a failsafe procedure was built into the analysis of the results. The DMeasure/Score algorithm is known to control for cognitive differences, which otherwise might act as a confounding variable (Cai, Sriram & Greenwald, 2004). The premise of this study is that by undertaking a programme of education based only on early years/child development, a stronger implicit age bias can be developed. It is supposed that through this course of study, preference for younger people will increase due to the nature of the course but this will be reflected through the IAT as an ageist attitude. This leads to the generation of the hypothesis: (H1) That undertaking an undergraduate degree programme based on child development will increase preferences for youth stimuli, resulting in a more implicitly ageist IAT result. 104 Explicit measures will also be taken as a broad measure of accepted explicit ageism as a comparison. Participants Participants for this study were recruited via opportunity sampling from the Early Years undergraduate degree programme at the University of Glamorgan. This course was specifically selected due to the course content excluding material on older people and specifically educating the participants in development of younger people. The sample consisted of 16 students (1 male, 15 females) with an age range from 20-25 (Mean: 22.4, S.D.: 1.8) years. Methodology No amendments have been made to the standard methodology previously detailed on pp. 70. The measures were taken at the end of the students’ programme of study as a cross-sectional indicator for comparison with the scores returned by participants from other undergraduate programmes. Results The descriptive results for all three comparative groups are presented prior to ANOVA tests being conducted. Explicit and implicit inferential tests are presented separately to illustrate group and test differences. Further to this both correlations between implicit and explicit measures and correlations over time are presented. 105 Table 8: Table of comparable means for Psychology and Nursing Students D-Score D-Score FSA Score FSA (Mean) S.D. (Mean) S.D. Psychology Students .7091 .9539 54.2 9.36 Nursing Students .6206 .6714 47.8 9.57 Early Years Students 2.4644 .8608 53.1 11.62 Groups Results from the early years cohort indicate no significant correlation between implicit and explicit measures (r=0.183, p=0.497). The results were analysed using a one-way between-subjects ANOVA. In the case of the explicit scores there was no significant main effect (F(2,65)=2.923; N.S). Post-hoc LSD pairwise comparisons found the only significant difference in these groups to be between the Psychology and Nursing students as reported in Study One. In the case of the implicit scores, between-subjects ANOVA results indicated a significant main effect (F(2,65)=28.604; p<.0001). Post-hoc LSD pairwise comparisons found that there were significant differences between Early Years students and both Psychology students (p<.0001) and Nursing students (p<.0001). Discussion As an overview, it is evident that the results from the Early Years cohort supports the central argument that there is no correlation between implicit and explicit measures and as such they measure different subsets of an attitude. It is also clear that there is a significant difference between the implicit scores of the three groups with Early Years students being significantly more ageist than either of the other student groups. The 106 explicit scores recorded are similarly non-ageist for each group with no significant difference between Early Years students and either Psychology or Nursing students. These results allow the hypothesis (that undertaking an undergraduate degree programme based on child development will increase preferences for youth stimuli, resulting in a more implicitly ageist IAT result) to be accepted. As with the Psychology and Nursing students before, the Early Years students also show a significant (p<0.001) difference in the implicit and explicit scores recorded. This significant difference adds support to the understanding that implicit and explicit attitudes are separate subsets of the same attitudinal construct. Two main reasons could be presented to account for this; either the students are keenly aware of the correct social response and respond in such a way to present themselves in line with societal expectations or that they are truly not ageist and the strong IAT score is as a result of their increased preference for younger people, however, not at the expense of the older person. The latter point will be further discussed with suggestions for clarification. Because of the nature of this cohort and the course that they have undertaken, the Early Years sample will not be directly compared to another cohort to assess the impacts of general higher level education. The course they have undertaken was measured to assess if there was an impact of directed education (youth-centric) on attitudes towards older people. It has been demonstrated that there has been an effect above and beyond that of general higher level education per se, thus comparing them as if they were representative of higher education would be incorrect. 107 The results clearly indicate the expected direction, supporting the supposition that an ageist IAT result can be influenced through interventions concentrating on the opposite IAT category. At no point in the Early Years course was ageing in terms of older people broached. Therefore, as similar environmental situations were experienced to those of the Psychology and Nursing students, it can be argued that the main effect is due to the difference in educational course content. This is an important finding as it questions whether the ageism IAT is in this case actually measuring the attitudes held towards older people or more accurately measuring an implicit preference towards younger people. To illustrate this further, the following analogy might be useful. It is perfectly possible for someone to prefer a person with particular characteristics as a sexual partner, but that does not mean that they hate people who do not possess these characteristics. Thus, a heterosexual man might have a strong preference for blonde haired women, but that does not mean that he hates men with dark hair. However, an IAT measure of such people with such groups set in comparison with each other might well indicate a strong bias in favour of blonde women. That cannot and should not be interpreted as indicating an anti-dark haired men prejudice. There are problems with this study that will need to be addressed if any firm conclusions are to be made about what indeed is being measured by the IAT. First, the tests need to be conducted prior to the commencement of the study and upon completion in a similar manner to Study One. This is something that was omitted in the current study but should be central to future intervention studies. In conducting a longitudinal study it is possible to map the progression of the attitudes from the initial 108 base measure to ascertain whether there has been a change or whether the group itself was more implicitly bias to start with. When viewed from this approach, the results obtained in the current study support a case for further investigation. They do not, however, stand alone to conclusively say whether increasing the preference for younger people would in turn affect the implicitly expressed attitudes towards older people. To improve the study in a similar vein to the previous longitudinal study, it may be of interest to extend the sample to those partaking in a remote access course whereby the university environment can be controlled for. As with the Study One, this would allow for judgements to be made about the influence of interventions and environment on implicitly held attitudes towards older people. Second and of high importance is to develop a way of assessing which direction an attitude is being influenced. It is distinctly possible to have a preference for younger over older people but not to be ageist in your attitude or behaviour. To this end a way of measuring the attitudes directly towards old and young categories rather than as a preference for one over the other should be implemented. One such method of doing so is the Single Category Implicit Association Test (SC-IAT) developed by Karpinski & Steinman (2006). Where the standard IAT relies on categorisation using pairings for both old and young simultaneously, the SC-IAT uses only one of those categories at a time to elicit the underlying attitudes held for a specific target. This could be used in conjunction with the existing IAT measure to ascertain base line and post intervention measures. This would allow for accurate reporting not only of the preferential attitude but also to demonstrate which aspect of the attitudes held have been affected (positive liking or negative disliking). The SC-IAT has been shown to be effective in eliciting implicit single concept attitudes (Bohner, Siebler, Gonzalez, 109 Haye & Schmidt, 2008; Steinman & Karpinski, 2008) and as such would be ideal when paired with the current IAT to clarify the current outstanding questions. Conclusions Explicit attitudes in this group, as with any other, are subject to self presentational bias should the individual be aware of what is being measured and/or the socially accepted response. Alternatively, explicit attitudes could be a reflection of how an individual truly thinks and as such are reflective of their consciously held beliefs. Either way, the explicit results need to be taken within context and under due consideration. This warning is also true when interpreting data from a standard IAT. The IAT provides a score of preference between two opposing concepts and as such provides an attitude along that dyadic continuum. The SC-IAT or similar measure could be used to tease out whether the attitudes expressed on the IAT are indeed a measure of negativity or of preference. These results indicate that there is a significant effect of category preference due to the nature of the intervention undertaken. However, before conclusive evidentiary support can be gained, a longitudinal study will need to be conducted to ascertain a base measure and post intervention measure for both IAT and SC-IAT measures. In addition to this, the study should be expanded to include sample controls as outlined in Study One to minimise the confounding variables. 110 Study Three – A cross-sectional study assessing attitudes held by hospital nurses working in A&E and Geriatric medicine Introduction A considerable body of research literature identifies Nursing students as holding negative attitudes towards older people (Duerson et. al., 1992; Gatz & Pearson, 1988; Grant, 1996; Higgins, Van der Riet, Slater & Peek, 2007). The initial longitudinal study in this investigation has demonstrated that, albeit no worse than Psychology undergraduates, Nursing students do exhibit negative implicit attitudes towards older people. Steffens & Schulze Konig (2006) conducted a review of literature suggesting that implicit biases are expressed as actions when the stimulus presented to the agent is spontaneous and unexpected. They also conducted an experiment to support their review, finding that automatic aspects of behaviour are predicted better by IAT results than explicit measures whilst the inverse is true for controlled behaviour involving self-presentation. When this is placed in the sphere of ageing, it is of interest to see what effect educational interventions can have on implicit attitudes (as shown in the previous two studies). However, more serious concerns can be raised in the context of caring for older people. It has been shown that Nursing students have a negative implicit attitude towards older people and also that these implicit attitudes in turn govern behaviour in spontaneous unplanned environments. Emergency and acute medical treatment such as that provided in a hospital environment would in the vast number of cases fall into the category of unplanned behaviour due to the reactionary nature of the job. It can, therefore, be reasonably supposed that if these negative 111 implicit attitudes continue through from their training, the nursing staff caring for older people might indeed provide care that is substandard for older people. This type of care ranges from withholding appropriate treatment as it would benefit a younger person more, to simply not providing the level of care expected. If there are pervasive negative attitudes towards older people then it can be argued that some form of intervention such as those trialled successfully in Singapore (Cheong, Wong & Koh, 2009) and Australia (Gonzales, Morrow-Howell & Gilbert, 2010) should be implemented in the UK. Successful interventions such as those trialled in Singapore and Australia have demonstrated that they need to be tailored for the specific purpose of reducing ageism, include both classroom and practical components and engage students/participants with older people. This current study takes a sample of qualified nurses working within a single hospital in a South Wales (UK) NHS Trust. An opportunity sample of nurses working in an Accident and Emergency (A&E) and Geriatric Medicine setting were recruited. These two samples are important as the A&E nurses would encounter caring for older people who are acutely ill and admitted quickly often in an emergency. The decisions that are made by the nurses in this setting would be spontaneous, relying on implicitly held attitudes and could result in life and death calls on treatment. In this environment older people would only make up a proportion of the total number of patients cared for so it is possible that discrimination between young and old could occur. Further to this, in an A&E environment, nurses only see patients who are too ill to be cared for in any other setting so would generally only see people (both young and old) while very ill, before being transferred to other departments to fully recover. Exposure to only the most ill patients could influence the implicitly held stereotypes associated 112 with older people if this is the only contact they have with them. However, it can be reasonably assumed that younger nurses (in the sample) would come into daily contact both in work and outside of work with fit and healthy younger people, thus balancing their negative stereotype assimilations. Nurses working in dedicated geriatric medicine wards of the hospital were selected as they have contact only with older people in their daily working lives. Zajonc’s (1968) theory of mere exposure effect suggests that just from coming into contact with something on a regular basis will make that object appear more favourable. A wealth of research has supported this and the direct contact hypothesis in that contact between groups where negative stereotypes have previously been held can itself help reduce the negativity of the stereotype and associated attitude (Allport, 1954; Cook, 1962, 1978; Pettigrew, 1979; Stephan & Stephan, 1984; Paolini, Hewstone, Cairns & Voci, 2004; King, Winter & Webster, 2009). From this it could be suggested that those nurses working in a ward specifically for care of older people would hold less negative (if not positive) attitudes towards older people than do those working in and A&E environment. As a result, the proposed hypotheses are: (H1) Those working in geriatric wards will have implicit attitudes less ageist than those who are working in A&E departments (H2) Nurses generally will hold negative implicit attitudes towards older people (H3) Neither group of nurses will express explicitly ageist attitudes 113 Participants Participants were either: Nurses in an A&E medical environment and nurses who work specifically on wards for care of older people. Each of the nurses for both subsets was recruited from the same South Wales NHS trust. Nurses were informed of the study and volunteers from each of the appropriate departments were tested. The measures were taken from nurses with a minimum of 3 years post qualification so all participants had experience of working in a ‘real world’ medical environment. The sample consisted of 32 (30 female, 2 male) nurses specialising in emergency care (Mean age: 35.1, S.D.: 7.4) medicine and 17 (16 female, 1 male) nurses specialising in care of older people (Mean age: 34.0, S.D.: 5.4). Methodology No amendments have been made from the procedure described in the General Methodology section (pp. 70). Results The descriptive results for all three comparative groups are presented prior to ANOVA tests being conducted. Explicit and implicit inferential tests are presented separately to illustrate group and test differences. Further to this both correlations between implicit and explicit measures and correlations over time are presented. 114 Table 9: Table of comparable means for Qualified Nurses and Nursing Students D-Score D-Score FSA Score FSA (Mean) S.D. (Mean) S.D. Geriatric Nurses 1.011 .8131 51.1 7.39 A&E Nurses 0.973 .8502 52.3 9.12 Nursing Students .621 .6714 47.8 9.57 Group When combining the scores from both A&E and Geriatric nurses it is clear there is no significant correlation between the implicit and explicit measures (r=-0.33, p=0.824). The explicit results were analysed using a one-way between-subjects ANOVA. This produced no significant main effect (F(1,47)=0.226; N.S) demonstrating that there was no significant difference in explicit scores between the three groups. All LSD post-hoc pairwise comparisons found no significant individual group differences. Results from the between-subjects ANOVA using the implicit data also showed no significant difference in main effect (F(1,47)=0.024; N.S), again demonstrating no statistically significant difference between groups. The Table overleaf shows the aggregated means of the two qualified nurses compared again to the nursing students. This is to show the difference between student and qualified nurses. 115 Table 10: Table of aggregated comparable means for Qualified Nurses and Nursing Students D-Score D-Score FSA Score FSA (Mean) S.D. (Mean) S.D. Qualified Nurses 0.987 .8221 51.9 8.02 Nursing Students 0.621 .6714 47.8 9.57 Group The results were analysed using a one-way between-subjects ANOVA. In the case of the explicit scores there was no significant main effect (F(1,73)=0.243; N.S). Results from the between-subjects ANOVA using the implicit data also showed no significant difference in main effect (F(1,73)=0.029; N.S). Discussion As an overview, all groups recorded scores on the implicit measure suggesting high levels of implicit ageism, however scores for the explicit measure suggest outwardly non-ageist attitudes. There were no significant differences reported between the two groups of nurses. In addition, when combining the two groups, results support the assertion that implicit and explicit tests measure different subsets of the same attitude as there is no significant correlation between measures. The data recorded mean that the first hypothesis (Those working in geriatric wards will have implicit attitudes less ageist than those who are working in A&E departments) has to be rejected as there was no significant difference between the two groups. The second hypothesis (that Nurses generally will hold negative implicit 116 attitudes towards older people but no explicit ageism will be expressed) was accepted. The third hypothesis (Neither group of nurses will express explicitly ageist attitudes) was also accepted as all scores fell below 58, which is the point where explicit ageism is recorded. Despite the arguments presented in the rationale, there was no significant difference in the implicit (or explicit) attitudes held between the two groups of qualified nurses. It would have been expected that due to the larger amount of contact, those nurses working in a dedicated ward for the care of older people would have implicit attitudes less ageist than those who are working in A&E departments. However, research has also indicated that even after taking selection and social desirability processes into account, all types of exposure affect attitudes in a favourable direction (Lee, Farrell & Link, 2004). When taking this into account and acknowledging that both types of qualified nurse are likely to have contact with older people on a daily basis, it is possible that the level of exposure to older patients in non-geriatric nursing might in itself be sufficient to trigger the observed effects. Nurses working in a ward for the care of older people have to deal with older patients on a day in day out basis, encountering much that is supposed to be stereotypically ‘annoying’ about older adults, whereas the A&E nurses see them only briefly, where their capacity to be ‘trying’ is far less. What this implies is that geriatric nurses’ level of negative implicit feelings is surprisingly low. It is further probable that the exposure that both groups experience accounts for the lack of significant difference between the two groups. However, if exposure to older people made attitudes better then the question remains as to why both groups held implicitly negative attitudes and indeed why they were more so than the nursing students. This can be answered at least in part by looking at 117 the type of contact and experience held. Although it can and has been argued in this review that any exposure will make attitudes better, continual exposure to negative information and experience of older people can negatively affect the schema held regarding older people (Grant, 1996). It is a distinct possibility that those younger nursing staff only come into contact with older people when they are at work and seeing them in varying degrees of illness and/or infirmity. If this is the case then those nursing staff will have only additional negative information to assimilate into their schema for older people that will in turn affect their beliefs and attitudes about older people. Auty and Lewis (2004) found similar results in that exposure to older people changed the strength of the attitude held but in the direction that it currently existed, except in the case of extreme experiences where the valence too was altered. As there is a daily contact in both settings, it can be argued that the existing negative implicit attitudes (gained as a societal norm and nurse training) are only strengthened from working in that environment and with the absence of any extreme experience to alter the valence of the attitude, it continues to become more negative. Negative attitudes could be increased by day-to-day exposure to some of the traits associated with older patients in care. These can include; wandering, forgetfulness, incontinence, accusing nurses of stealing, etc. It is possible that this daily exposure and continual reinforcement of negative associations would add to any preconceptions about older adults that students and nurses might have. The explicit scores again are expressing no such age bias as they are potentially subject to self presentational bias (Goffman, 1959) whereby the individual is expressing attitudes under conscious control to place themselves in the best possible / socially acceptable light. Subjective norms are adopted through expectation of this 118 acceptability and despite any contact or intervention, the understanding of what is socially acceptable or expected is ingrained and as such expressed when explicitly questioned. Expression of explicitly accepting attitudes is likely to be due to the acceptance of what socially acceptable responses are. Despite any internal changes, participants are aware that social norm responses will hold them in better regard. As stated, implicit attitudes are not, however, subject to the same self presentational bias and as such, any intervention and/or contact that could change these would be measurable through testing. What is important, however, is that the apparently pervasive negative implicit attitude held by society generally (Levy and Banaji, 2002) is also present to a similar level within the nursing community, including in those nurses specifically caring for older people. This is of concern as it is these people who are charged with meeting the needs of older people when they are at their most vulnerable and it is these people who will have to make spontaneous treatment decisions, activating implicitly held stereotypes and attitudes. Conclusions Nurses working in both A&E and geriatric medicine settings within a hospital environment hold similar implicit and explicit attitudes towards older people. Neither group expresses an explicit ageist bias, however, their implicit scores indicate a relatively high ageist bias. This disparity is not uncommon in that it has been the trend for each of the groups tested, however the negative implicit ageist bias is higher than those nursing students sampled. Although the difference between nursing students’ and qualified nurses’ implicit bias is not quite significant, there is a clear increase. Of 119 future interest would be to monitor the career pathway of nurses through either a series of cross-sectional studies or with a longitudinal cohort to assess whether this is a trend that continues throughout their careers. The nature of the implicit attitudes held by both the nursing students and the qualified nursing staff would seem to indicate that there is a need for systemic change. Training interventions have been shown to be successful in reducing age bias in both students and qualified nurses (Cheong, Wong & Koh, 2009; Gonzales, Morrow-Howell & Gilbert, 2010) and should be adapted for a UK care context. As the population ages it is imperative that a successful method of combating this issue is found so as to minimise / eliminate ageism as a factor in care choices made for older people. 120 Study Four – A cross-sectional study assessing the effects of higher level education on implicit and explicit attitudes towards older people Introduction The previous studies in this investigation have identified that educational content can affect a person’s implicit attitudes. This is specifically in the case of Early Years students and more generally, for those Psychology and Nursing students. They have also revealed that qualified nurses’ implicit attitudes are no better and are in fact worse than their comparable student counterparts (albeit not quite at a significant level). Each of these groups has undertaken post A-level (or equivalent) education. However, with 44% of school leavers currently continuing into higher level education (Department for Education & Skills, 2008), that still leaves the majority of people in the UK who do not attend higher level education institutions. The figure for those who have not been through higher education was higher in the past, so that amongst 40 yr olds, the proportion without a degree is circa 79%. When taken in the broader sense, in 1980, only 21% of school leavers went to higher level education institutions (Department for Education & Skills, 2008). When looking UK wide, this indicates that the vast majority of those adults living in the UK have not undertaken higher level education. Preston and Feinstein (2004) found that the level of education received does affect the attitudes held by participants. They established that those who underwent higher level education were more likely to be open-minded and hold more accepting attitudes than those who did not. With this in mind and considering the results from previous studies in this thesis, the question remains, what effect does general higher level education 121 have on specific attitudes to ageing? The literature would seem to suggest that the attitudes of those who have not undertaken higher level education would be significantly more negative than those who have (Hogan & Mallott, 2005). The current study will measure the attitudes of a sample of adults who have not undertaken higher level education and then compare them to those adult qualified nurses who have undergone higher level education and those students currently undertaking that education. The hypotheses being tested are: (H1) Those adults who have not undertaken higher education will express more ageist implicit attitudes than either those students undertaking, or those adults who have undertaken, higher level education. (H2) Those adults who have not undertaken higher education will express more ageist explicit attitudes than either those students undertaking, or those adults who have undertaken, higher level education. Participants The sample for this study consisted of 20 participants (4 males, 16 females) with an age range of 20-48 years (Mean: 35.1, S.D.: 8.0). The sample was recruited through the Human Resources department at the University of Glamorgan. Those staff who were not university graduates but who had completed A-Levels were identified by line managers across several departments and directly approached to participate in the study. Staff were not made aware of the reasons pertaining to their selection, only the key aims of the study in general so as not to cause angst or upset. Staff worked in a mixture of environments in the university, some of which were front of house and 122 dealing with students and others were more support/administration. Comparisons have been made between this group, Nursing students and the General Adult sample. This has been done to minimise the cohort effects regarding age and education. The General Adult sample has been compared to the Qualified Nursing sample as they are of a more comparable age than the other sample groups. Nursing Students have then been used as a comparison group as they have undergone a higher education course where a full longitudinal data set is available and being Nursing Students are comparable to the Qualified Nurse sample. As such, the effects of higher level education on attitudes towards older people can be assessed through the comparison of these two groups with other sample comparisons provided in the meta-analysis. Comparisons have not been made at this stage with Psychology Students as they are similar to those Nursing Students and as such do not add anything to the argument at this stage. Further to this comparisons have not been made with the Psychology of Ageing cohort or the Early Years group. These groups have both undertaken specific education (on ageing or youth) and as such are not representative of the effects of higher education in general. As with the other cohorts, these will be included in the meta-analysis. However, they do not add to the understanding of general higher level education on attitudes towards older people. Methodology No amendments made, please see General Methodology section (pp. 70). Results The descriptive results for all three comparative groups are presented prior to ANOVA tests being conducted. Explicit and implicit inferential tests are presented 123 separately to illustrate group and test differences. Further to this both correlations between implicit and explicit measures and correlations over time are presented. Table 11: Table of means comparing the effects of education D-Score D-Score FSA Score FSA (Mean) S.D. (Mean) S.D. Qualified Nurses 0.987 .8221 51.9 8.02 Nursing Students .621 .6714 47.8 9.57 General Adult Sample 1.093 .942 57.2 7.71 Group Results from the General Adult population indicate no significant correlation between the implicit and explicit measures of ageism (r=0.018, p=0.94). The explicit data clearly shows a difference between the general adult population and both qualified and student nurses. Nurses have been chosen as a comparison group as they are of a similar age to the adult population and as such are the most directly comparable group to ascertain the effects of education. The student nurses have further been chosen to show the progression from training through to the work place. Psychology students have not been included in the comparison at this stage (but are in the meta-analysis on pp. 152) as the effects of higher education are reflected in the undergraduate nursing course and the scores of these students are comparable. This demonstrates not only the effects of education in the immediate context but also shows the effects of working in a caring profession/hospital environment. The scores in all cases are below that of explicit ageism (58 on the FSA), however, the general adult sample comes close to the threshold. 124 These data illustrate that there is no appreciable difference between the general adult population and qualified nurses, where both express comparably high levels of implicit ageism. Unlike the qualified nurses, however, the student nurses have a substantially different implicit score to the general adult population. When comparing the students to the general adult population using a between-subjects ANOVA there was a significant main effect returned for the explicit data (F(1,44)=12.84; p≤0.001) and the implicit data was also approaching significance (F(1,44)=3.948; p=.053). Further between-subjects ANOVAs revealed that there were no significant main effect differences between Geriatric nurses and the general adult sample on the implicit measure (F(1,35)=0.78; N.S) but there was on the explicit measure (F(1,35)=6.058; p<.05). When comparing those nurses who worked in the A&E department to the general adult population using the same ANOVA technique there was similarly no main effect on the implicit measure (F(1,50)=.225; N.S.) and the explicit measure was approaching significance (F(1,50)=4.016; p=.051). Discussion An overview of the results shows that there is no significant correlation between the implicit and explicit measures of ageism. This supports the assertion that implicit and explicit attitudes are different subsets of the overall attitude and are measured in different ways with potentially different valence shown. Data also show those who have not received higher level education are similarly as implicitly ageist as those who have undertaken higher level education. Those adults who have not undertaken higher level education, however, exhibit significantly worse explicit attitudes towards older people. Of note is that despite being significantly worse, no group expresses 125 attitudes on the explicit measure that when scored would suggest ageist attitudes. There were also no significant differences in the explicit scores between the qualified nurses and the adult population but there were significant differences in the explicit scores. This difference showed nurses (both qualified and students) to have a less bias explicit attitude than those who had not been through higher level education. Of note, however, is that despite being significantly more explicitly ageist, the general adult population similarly did not exhibit ageist attitudes (Scores above 58 on the FSA). The results mean that the first hypothesis (Those adults who have not undertaken higher education will express more ageist implicit attitudes than either those students undertaking, or those adults who have undertaken, higher level education) is rejected. The second hypothesis (Those adults who have not undertaken higher education will express more ageist explicit attitudes than either those students undertaking, or those adults who have undertaken, higher level education) has, however, been supported by the analysis. In terms of the implicit attitudes, there is a noticeable difference when the descriptive statistics are observed. However, this difference is not significant for any of the compared groups. Due to these differences falling short of the level of significance required, the first hypothesis cannot be supported. There are possible methodological reasons why the results just fell short of the required level of significance. The general adult population sample was all taken from those who worked in the university. Despite the fact that the majority of the adults were not working with students on a daily basis (kitchen, clerical, grounds maintenance etc) they would still have been in a general environment where they would have come into daily contact with them. As 126 research has shown, (Lee, Farrell & Link, 2004) when in day to day contact, attitudes towards the target group (in this case the students/young people) will become more favourable. If this is the case then the implicit attitudes of those adults working at the university would be more favourable regarding young people and not necessarily worse with regards to older people. This means that it is not solely the effect from lack of higher level education that is being observed. This leaves the same question as was generated in Study One. Are the results which demonstrate an increased implicit prejudice due to an increased preference for younger people or an increased prejudice against older people? In addition to this is whether the same trends will be found in the general adult population outside of the university environment or are the sample that were selected influenced by their place of work? To address the issues that have arisen with implicit testing, the following improvements to methodology will need to be made: The sample will have to be increased to allow for a wider distribution of responses; the sample will have to include or comprise solely those people who have not received higher level education and do not work in environments with young or older people (e.g. office workers) and finally the use of a Single Category IAT (SC-IAT) in addition to the standard IAT would allow for direction of preference to be shown too. This is not, however, the full picture. The explicit results do show a significant difference between the general adult sample and both students and qualified nurses. Despite not quite demonstrating an explicit level of ageism, is can be said that they are significantly explicitly more ageist than either of the other groups. When using the general adult population as a comparison group, it becomes evident that the qualified 127 nurses do not hold any more or less negative implicit attitudes than do those of a similar age who have not undertaken higher level education. The main difference between the nurses and the general adult sample was regarding their explicit scores where those in the general population recorded significantly more ageist scores. This in itself is quite an interesting finding as it can be hypothesised that those who have not received higher level education are either more explicitly ageist or that they do not have the same level of ability to monitor their expressed opinions. The latter explanation would infer that self presentational bias increases as the level of education increases. This supports early research by Goffman (1959) who stated the more educated a person is, the more aware they become of the subjective and societal norms against which they will measure themselves and in turn be measured. As this is an explicit measure and it is the expression of the explicit attitude that is on interest, the fact the adult sample work in a university environment should be of little consequence. Unlike the implicit measure, if the explicit tests are repeated with similar results in a wider sample, it will add support to an already solid finding rather than clarifying the data already obtained. If this is the case and increased education leads to increased self presentational bias, any study using only explicit measures should consider this as a confounding variable. Conclusions It would appear that those adults who have not undergone higher level education are more implicitly and explicitly ageist than their counterparts who have undertaken or are currently undertaking higher level education. This result is in support of previous research (Hogan & Mallott, 2005) in that increased education levels would appear to decrease ageism. This would suggest that there is benefit (additional to that of 128 increased knowledge) in embarking on further education as any of the courses would seem to improve attitudes more so than if higher level education was absent. Despite the implicit measure not producing significant results, the descriptive results illustrate an observable difference that warrants further investigation. The improvements of the investigation would include a larger sample inclusive of adults who do not work with either young or older people and the addition of the SC-IAT for directional clarity. It would also be of worth to include a range of student comparables rather than from a single course. If it is simply higher level education that decreases implicit and explicit age bias, the course content should be irrelevant and thus a span of courses needs to be included for analysis. Results from the explicit measure indicate that those without higher level education express a more ageist attitude. However, taken in isolation it is apparent that they do not show an explicitly ageist attitude, just more of a leaning towards it than those who have received additional education. Further to this it may also suggest that those who do not receive higher level education are less able to monitor the way in which they present themselves and the attitudes they express. In addition to the implicit improvements suggested, further explicit data should be collated to ensure this is a robust finding and additional self monitoring questions should be included to assess whether education level affects an individuals’ ability to present themselves in the most socially acceptable way. 129 Study Five – A longitudinal study assessing the effects of specific age education on attitudes towards older people Introduction The previous study has illustrated that higher level education has a significant effect on explicit attitudes towards older people. Broader studies have been suggested to strengthen the research findings. The current study seeks to specifically address this and assess the effects of a Psychology of Ageing elective third year module that would seek to give a rounded perspective of ageing. Research suggesting that negative attitudes towards older people can be made worse by existing educational interventions has been present for some time. Duerson et al. (1992) concluded that to increase knowledge about ageing and to decrease ageist attitudes, modern approaches to education need to be taken. They suggested that formal classroom sessions should be teamed with planned experiences with older people, both addressing the positive associations of ageing as well as the more widely accredited losses. This research supports the analysis conducted by Whitborne and Hulicka (1990) who reviewed the content of 139 psychology textbooks. They found that little space was given over to ageing even in developmental texts where the weighting was very much on development in early years. They found that where ageing was mentioned, the focus was on problems rather than successes, describing older people as suffering from multiple handicaps attributed solely to the ageing process. The texts did not mention the difference between healthy ageing and that of disease or how individuals can compensate for losses associated with ageing. It is this 130 education practice which is widespread even today that exposes students to a narrow and permanently fixed view of the ageing process that increases the implicit age bias. Recent innovative training programmes have demonstrated that through an integrated approach, attitudes towards older people can be improved. Cheong, Wong & Koh (2009) identified that through successful education, medical students in Singapore are expressing increasingly positive attitudes towards older people. This programme included a balanced approach to ageing, presenting models of successful healthy ageing as well as the ways older people compensate for those losses associated with ageing thus maintaining their quality of life. In addition to the standard classroom based education, Gonzales, Morrow-Howell & Gilbert (2010) demonstrated that through adding contact with older people through activities outside of a classroom, the attitudes of medical students improved significantly. Both of these interventions show how psychological knowledge can be used to structure interventions to effectively decrease negative attitudes towards older people. The current study is longitudinal, assessing the effectiveness of an elective third year module entitled “the Psychology of Ageing” in reducing implicit and explicit ageism. This is a module developed to provide a rounded view of the ageing process which may be overlooked in standard developmental psychology programmes where the emphasis is on early lifespan development. Although there is no contact with older people provided in the course, it is thought that the Psychology of Ageing module will follow the successes in Singapore and reduce the age bias in those students electing to complete the module. 131 The hypotheses being proposed are: (H1) Implicit attitudes following the completion of the Psychology of Ageing module will be significantly less ageist than those recorded prior to commencement of the course. (H2) Explicit attitudes following the completion of the Psychology of Ageing module will be significantly less ageist than those recorded prior to commencement of the course. Participants This study sought to follow a complete cohort of participants undergoing a year of age specific education (Psychology of Ageing module – undergraduate level). The initial full cohort comprised of 23 students (5 male, 18 female) with an age range of 20-44 years (Mean: 25.9, S.D.: 5.7). However, by the end of the year due to course attrition, withdrawal and non-attendance the sample for longitudinal analysis was reduced to 12 participants (4 male, 8 female) with an age rage of 20-34 years (Mean: 26.6, S.D.: 4.3). The initial sample was an exhaustive sample of a final year elective module on an undergraduate psychology programme. In addition to the module undertaken, none of the participants had received any additional gerontological interventions or training. Methodology The materials used and overall methodology is the same as that presented in the General Methodology section (pp. 70). The sole difference being that this study was a small cohort study specifically identifying those students undertaking a module on the Psychology of Ageing. Measures were taken in the first and last weeks of the course 132 to gain a base measure of attitudes and then to ascertain any immediate effects of the year course. Contact details (name, email, telephone number) for each of the participants were taken at the start of the project and stored in a secure filing cabinet in a locked storage room for the duration so that participants could be contacted again and linked to their participant numbers to allow for cohort comparisons. The contact details and participant numbers were stored separately so a theft or accidental release of a document would not lead to the identification of participants. Results Descriptive results are displayed for the two time points of the Psychology of Ageing course and a comparison is drawn to the General Adult sample. ANOVA results are presented for the explicit and implicit scores as well as post hoc analysis prior to giving correlational information on the implicit and explicit measures at both time points. Table 12: Table of means comparing the Psychology of Ageing course and General Adult data sets Group Psychology of Ageing General Adult Sample Time D-Score D-Score FSA Score FSA Point (Mean) S.D. (Mean) S.D. 1 0.710 .688 58.3 10.1 2 1.011 .651 55.8 8.95 1 1.093 .942 57.2 7.71 133 Data collected at time point one indicate that there is no significant correlation between implicit and explicit measures (r=0.23, p=0.41). Similarly at time point two there is no significant correlation (r=0.006, P=0.985). Data for each time point were analysed using a mixed ANOVA on the PASW statistical package. This showed that there was no significant difference between time one and time two in either the implicit (F(1,25)=0.61; N.S.) or explicit scores (F(1,25)=0.428; N.S.). Post hoc analysis using the LSD test of pairwise comparisons found that there were no significant differences in implicit or explicit scores between those who had undergone the Psychology of Ageing course and those in the general adult sample. When comparing the implicit scores at time one (prior to course but after two years of higher level education) there is a distinct difference in descriptive statistics between the students and general adult samples. Despite the noticeable difference, this is not significant (p=0.193). Similar to the longitudinal data set in Study One, it is of interest to ascertain whether there is a correlation between the scores for each student at time one and time two. The implicit measure did not correlate highly between time one and time two (r=.260, N.S.), however, the explicit measure did (r=.750, p=0.01). 134 The explicit results correlate significantly at both time points indicating that provides a stable prediction of the explicit attitudes held by those students. In addition to that, the r value is strong enough to suggest that the explicit measure itself is robust. Correlations on the implicit measure are neither strong nor significant. Discussion As an overview of the results, data at both time point one and time point two demonstrate that there is no significant difference between implicit and explicit measures. This supported the central theme of the thesis that implicit and explicit attitudes are measured in different ways with sometimes different results as they measure different subsets of the same attitude. Data also show that those participants who underwent the Psychology of Ageing course started with a negative implicit attitude towards older people and a non-ageist explicit attitude. Following the course, explicit attitudes towards older people improved but implicit attitudes became worse. However, neither of these changes was statistically significant. Further to this, correlational analysis of the implicit and explicit measures indicated that the explicit measure was both stable and reliable where the implicit measure was not. The results mean that the hypotheses (Implicit attitudes following the completion of the Psychology of Ageing module will be significantly less ageist than those recorded prior to commencement of the course and Explicit attitudes following the completion of the Psychology of Ageing module will be significantly less ageist than those recorded prior to commencement of the course) have not been supported. Rather than the expected direction, the attitudes towards older people (implicit) have become 135 markedly worse despite this change not being statistically significant. This finding goes against the expected result and supporting literature, however, there are some reasons for this and improvements that could be made to the methodology to accurately reflect the effects of a rounded intervention measure. Prior to the commencement of the study, the content of the course was not assessed. Upon doing so post completion of testing, it became evident that there was considerable time spent on the losses associated with ageing. The course comprised the following topics of ageing: Demographics, biology, intelligence, memory, mental health, personality and lifestyle and linguistics. Each of the 17 weeks of teaching centred on the losses associated with ageing, reflecting the predominance of this in the literature. This had the effect of maximising the differences between older and younger people, potentially perpetuating the myths surrounding intergroup characteristics. This in turn could have increased the strength of negative stereotypes held and the associated negative attitudes. The course had attempted to reflect the way in which older people can compensate for losses associated with ageing, however, as an undergraduate third year course rather than a training course, much of the additional information regarding the gains associated with ageing should have been gathered by the students in wider reading outside of the classroom context. As the measure for an intervention, all of the learning material should be presented to the students so that they have all of the information to assimilate and there is not the reliance on individual motivation to improve knowledge. As there was no measure of the additional work that students had completed outside of the weekly lecture, it is not known how rounded the information taken by the students was. 136 Another reason why the course may not have been successful is the method of course delivery. Firstly, although classroom education alone has been shown to be effective in Singapore (Cheong, Wong & Koh, 2009), the addition of direct contact/activities with older people to this has shown to be more successful (Gonzales, Morrow-Howell & Gilbert, 2010). It is the direct contact with older people that could in turn cause the information being imparted to take on a real world context and more deeply affect the students’ implicit attitudes. Second, the course consisted of one weekly lecture. It is possible that a more intensive course, where the students would have less time between exposures in which to forget the content, would be more successful in promoting attitudinal shifts. In conducting this study again, any intervention measure would firstly have to be critiqued. By this it is meant that the intervention would have to present a positive account of ageing reflecting the gains and achievements in ageing, something that would be assured prior to the commencement of intervention testing. Further to this, the recommendations about the course structure should be implemented in the development of a new intervention measure for testing. The intervention should be more intensive, either over a lesser period of time or more frequent contact time and should include contact time with older people. In adopting these improvements to the intervention measure and methodology, a more accurate assessment of an intervention measure could be trialled. If the intervention was shown to be successful, this could then be tested in a wider setting with a larger sample prior to it being recommended as a general intervention to reduce implicit age bias. This would be an important development with regards to the health care settings 137 as it could be included for mandatory continual professional development and tests conducted to assess whether it improves attitudes and subsequent behaviours. To assess the longevity of any changes in attitude, a year follow-up should be conducted to assess whether any intervention is successful in long term attitudinal shifts. These results do however indicate that if an education programme is not fully rounded and inclusive of the positive aspects associated with growing old, it may have the opposite effect to that which was intended. In presenting a course that was not balanced with the positive aspects as well as the negative aspects of ageing, a course was delivered that worsened the learners’ attitudes towards older people. This is mirroring the warnings arising from the review of psychology texts by Whitborne and Hulicka (1990). Further, the findings support the findings of Gonzales, MorrowHowell and Gilbert (2010) in that an intervention has to be specifically tailored to reduce ageism in order for it to be successful. Explicit data from time one to time two demonstrates a decrease in ageism despite remaining relatively high when compared to the other student groups. Although it is not statistically significant, it does give more support for further investigation to be conducted looking into the effects of higher level education on self presentational bias. In each of the studies where base measures have been taken, there has been a clearly observable if not statistically significant decrease between start and end points. This disparity between implicit and explicit scores which is more marked in those who have received higher level education is additional support for the supposition that self presentational bias increases with level of education. Of interest with the Psychology of Ageing students is that they had higher explicit scores (more negative 138 attitudes) at the start point than did either the third point Nursing or Psychology students or the General Adult sample. They had by this point undertaken two years of higher level education and as such it would be expected that they would be expressing more positive explicit attitudes towards older people. The fact that this finding is not in line with those conclusions from the previous study, however, can be explained. For timetabling reasons, some Psychology of Ageing students were taking the module as no others were available to them with their other module choices. This meant that those on the course, rather than selecting the module due to an interest in ageing, were indeed taking it by default. From this it can be supposed that those ‘forced’ to take the module might have a generally negative attitude towards the module per se and as such would not be receptive to the course content. It could further be supposed that they would (initially) extend their negative attitudes towards the content of the course, i.e. older people, which was then reflected in the explicit scores. The trend of the explicit scores, however, is in line with previous studies in that they decreased (became less ageist) from time point one to time point two. This supports the central argument that higher level education does result in decreasing scores on explicit measures of ageism. As suggested in the previous study, this is something that should be investigated further with self-monitoring scales being added as items in the explicit measures given to participants. Of potential interest is the comparison between the implicit scores for the general adults and those of the time one Psychology of Ageing students. The results presented show that despite not reaching significance, there is a distinct difference between the two. This difference could be illustrative of the effects of general higher level education as at point one there had been no specific Psychology of Ageing measure 139 trialled and the students had received two full years of university education. When coupled with the data collected in the previous study, this would highlight the need for further investigation of the effects of both specific and general higher level education. The last results to be displayed were those of the longitudinal data correlations for both the implicit and explicit measures. These are of specific interest as it is clear that over time, the scores on the explicit measures for each participant correlate significantly. This means that when looking at the individual, their specific score at each time point correlates, providing a stable measure. The “r” value falls below that of .9 recommended by Kline (2000) but does fall above the .7 which he asserts to be the minimum level. When analysing the implicit scores, however, it is clear that that scores returned by each individual participant do not correlate with the other implicit scores recorded at the second time point. This disparity has two core possible explanations, either the nature of the constructs or the nature of the tests. Explanations for these disparities are explained in the discussion for the first experiment and as such will not be repeated in detail here. However, they will be summarised. In the explicit measure, participants are aware of the socially acceptable responses and as such respond in such a manner at each point which increases the correlation between time points. The scores on the implicit measure could be caused by one of two reasons, the IAT being an inaccurate measure for individual attitudes over time; or that the course, as with other educational interventions, has had differing effects on participants and as such would have affected the implicit scores at time two differently. 140 Conclusions Similar to other educational interventions already investigated in previous studies, the explicit measure of ageism in this study decreases post course delivery. This decrease supports the notion that as education level increases, so does the individuals’ self presentational bias. As previously suggested this is important ground to be covered in future studies to ascertain whether explicit measures can be accurately used to measure the explicit attitudes of those people who have undertaken higher level education. The current course has not shown to be an effective way of reducing implicit ageist bias due to a number of factors outlined in the above discussion. Additional studies need to be conducted to develop a more intensive and effective intervention measure including both a positive curriculum and contact with older people. It is important to develop and test this intervention further as it has been established that interventions can affect the implicit attitudes held both positively and negatively and in the case of nurses and medical professionals it is clearly a concept of central importance. Individual IAT test score correlations over time are weak and not significant possibly caused by the course or situational factors so the same tests recommended in Study One should be undertaken for clarity purposes. Further to the latter tests, it is possible that the use of the IAT for individual level attitude and preference assessment could be called into question, therefore, longitudinal retest recommendations should also be undertaken. 141 Study Six – A cross-sectional study assessing the levels of implicit and explicit ageism held by older people Introduction The previous studies have investigated specifically the effects of higher level education, specific education and direct contact on implicit and explicit attitudes towards older people. In doing so a comparison group of adults who had not received higher level education was sampled. This group was used as a control to see the effects of each condition on the aforementioned attitudes. It was apparent, although not always significantly, that those attitudes held by the general population sample were the most explicitly ageist and except for those attitudes expressed by early years students, the most implicitly ageist too. From this it can be rationalised that there is a generally pervasive ageist attitude in the wider population. Research has supported the initial conclusions made from these studies that there is a generally negative attitude towards older people (Cummings, Kropf & DeWeaver, 2000; Catterall & Maclaran, 2001; Depaola, Griffin, Young & Neimeyer, 2003). The cited research specifically looks at the explicit attitudes held by a cross sample of people in the general population and conclude that this is a far reaching and deeply instilled bias. Levy and Banaji (2002) conducted a review on implicit ageism which also illustrated a pervasive and wide reaching proliferation of negative ageist attitudes. This is something that has been supported continually since the introduction of the IAT with more recent studies (Turner & Crisp, 2010) not only identifying the same trend but also trying to address it. What was of concern, however, is that unlike in other explicit studies, these findings are not only in the young. They found that 142 negative implicit ageist attitudes are held by the elderly themselves (Levy & Banaji, 2002). The reasoning given for this is that elderly people have acquired the same implicit prejudices throughout their lives and have not had sufficient time or opportunity to develop the mechanisms to defend against this. There are negative consequences for elderly people as a result of the ageist attitudes held as they are not only subjected to ageist prejudices from others but also internalise these implicit biases. As people progress through the life span their age schema become more elaborate as more information both about others and themselves becomes incorporated. As they age, the number of traits, categories and subcategories they have within the schema grows, however, core elements are still retained. Research does support this developmental approach, finding that despite having a more complex picture of ageing, older people do not necessarily hold more positive views. Hummert et al. (1994) found that older people did indeed hold more stereotypes about older people but they had more negative ones as well as having more positive ones. Some studies further report that older people do judge their age category more favourably than younger people do, however, that is only more positively as a comparison but both groups have generally negative attitudes towards older age (Kite et al., 1991). Coleman and O’Hanlon (2008) highlight that successful ageing/optimal ageing is achieved not only through acknowledgement of the associated losses (as with other stages of the lifespan) but also the successful adaptation and coping with the stresses and changes in life. It is taking control of those challenges and adapting through acquired life skills that is fundamental to mental, psychological and emotional health 143 at all stages in the lifespan. They also posit that holding these negative attitudes can indeed curtail adaptive thinking and coping strategies. With an increasingly ageing population this pervasive negative attitude clearly has negative effects on a growing proportion of UK residents. Over 65’s account for a substantial proportion of the hospital admissions in England and Wales. In the year April 2008 – March 2009 22.8% of the total admittance to NHS A&E departments in England was for those people aged 60 and over (Hospital Episode Statistics, 2010). With this negative attitude being held in the social conscious, it is possible that the care of these individuals may indeed be less than may be given to a younger person. This study is seeking to build upon previous research and ascertain whether older people do indeed hold similar ageist attitudes to those expressed by the general adult population. Research would suggest that older people are similarly affected by societal opinions and stereotypes thus their implicit attitudes would reflect this. To test the effects of pervasive societal ageism on older people’s attitudes, the following hypotheses have been presented: (H1) Older people will express implicit attitudes that are significantly ageist and that these will be similar to those expressed by the general adult sample. (H2) Older people will return scores that are not explicitly ageist and that their scores will be less than those recorded by the general adult sample. 144 Participants For this study there were 21 older people (aged 65+) who completed both the questionnaire explicit measure and IAT implicit measure. These people were recruited from local activity groups creating an opportunity sample and ranged from 65-84 years of age (Mean: 73.4, S.D. 5.7). Of the sample, 18 participants were female and 3 were male. This gender imbalance is reflective of the recruitment strategy and membership of the selected activity groups. Further to this, none of the participants had received any formal higher or further education past that of mandatory schooling. This fact is of note so as to eliminate education as a factor affecting the outcome results. None of the participants when asked reported having any pre-existing conditions that may impair their ability to complete either section of the test. Methodology No substantial amendments were made to the methodology of this study. The only variation was that rather than completing the study in a computer laboratory, the testing was completed using two laptop computers. This method was used as it was inappropriate to ask participants to travel to the campus when many did not drive and no remuneration was being made. Each of the testing sessions was conducted in either quiet areas or separate rooms at the venues for the meetings of the social groups. This deviation in the methodology should have no discernible effect on the responses given by participants. 145 Results Comparative descriptive statistics are presented for the Older People sample and for the General Adult sample. ANOVA results are then presented to investigate the significance of any differences between the two groups. Table 13: Table of means comparing the effects of ageing on attitudes towards older people D-Score D-Score FSA Score FSA Score (Mean) S.D. (Mean) S.D. Older People 1.0108 .759 51.5 9.58 General Adult Sample 1.093 .942 57.2 7.71 Group Results from the Older Person cohort similarly illustrate no significant correlation between implicit and explicit measures (r=-0.283, p=0.213). When the descriptive data for the older person sample are taken in isolation, it is clear that the explicit measure illustrates an attitude that is not ageist where the implicit measure illustrates a distinctly ageist attitude. The data comparing the general adult sample and the older persons sample were analysed using a between-subjects ANOVA on the PASW statistical package. This found that there was no significant group difference on the implicit measure (F(1,39)=.096; N.S.). There was, however, a significant between subjects difference for the explicit measure (F(1,39)=4.339; p<0.05). 146 Discussion In short, the results from the current study illustrate a similar picture for older people as to that of the general population. Results from implicit and explicit measures do not correlate and as such support the central tenet that implicit and explicit attitudes are separate constructs, subsets of the larger attitude concept. Older people also hold similarly negative implicit attitudes towards older people at a level that is not significantly different from the general adult population. The explicit attitude expressed by older people is, however, significantly more positive than those expressed by the general adult sample. From the data collected, the first hypothesis (older people will express implicit attitudes that are significantly ageist and that these will be similar to those expressed by the general adult sample) can be accepted. The results clearly indicate that there is an implicit ageist bias in the sample of older people. Also as there is no significant difference between that sample and the general adult population, it can be concluded that the attitudes held implicitly are relatively similar. The second hypothesis (older people will return scores that are not explicitly ageist and that their scores will be less than those recorded by the general adult sample) can also be accepted. This hypothesis is supported as there is a clear and significant difference between the two samples on the explicit measure. The implicit scores as a stand alone measure support existing research suggesting that older people also internalise the negative societal stereotypes of ageing. In adopting these negative stereotypes, older people often ignore the way they actually feel and replace these thoughts and behaviours with those they would expect from their 147 negative stereotypes. Levy (1996) found that elderly people who exhibited higher negative implicit attitudes also performed significantly worse on memory tasks. She found that the perceptions of older adults could also be affected by implicit self stereotyping. She concluded that implicit age stereotypes can influence the views of older adults both towards others as well as towards themselves. This supports previous research findings that when elderly people adopt these societal stereotypes, they see decline as inevitable and that becoming a less active member of society is the only option (Rodin & Langer, 1980). Similarly, Butler (1987) found that when adopted, these stereotypes became a self-fulfilling prophecy, reinforcing stereotypes through the inaction and deficits resulting from their initial belief and internalisation. This finding supports the argument that additional intervention measures need to be developed not only for the medical community but also for a wider societal change starting during compulsory schooling. Psychological theory can be used to design interventions that can be used in an educational setting. These can be designed to impart knowledge and cause disequilibrium in the current schema held to force a reassessment of existing attributes and evaluations to modify the existing ageist attitude. This has been shown by Kite et al. (2005) who demonstrated that upon providing information about a person that countered existing negative ageist stereotypes, the negative attitudes can be diminished. As a comparative measure, the implicit scores being so similar indicate that the negative attitude expressed is not confined to a single group of people but is more widely present within society. This is despite there being an explicit expression in the opposite direction due to socially acceptable responses. The reasons why there is such a pervasive negative implicit bias are many in number, however, daily contact with 148 media is seen as a major reinforcing factor. In television comedies for example, elderly people are depicted, defined by stereotyped negativities regarding physical decline and both physical and mental incompetencies (Zebrowitz & Montepare, 2000). The subtle reinforcement of the negative bias through socially acceptable media only strengthens the ingrained attitudes and stereotypes. In addition to this, once acquired, these attitudes are maintained and strengthened when encountering elderly people even if they do not exhibit characteristics associated to the stereotypes held (Levy et al., 2000; Murphy, Monahan & Zajonc, 1995). As such it is important that these be addressed not only on an individual level but also that rather than pushing the negative stereotypes of ageing within the wider media, more reflective and counter (existing) stereotypical images and characters should be portrayed. Scores on the explicit measure show a clear and significant difference between the group of older people and the sample from the general adult study. This as predicted could be due to the nature of the explicit measure. The items on the scale are varied but generally centre on the desire to spend time with older people and their rights. For the younger cohorts, it is possible to dissociate themselves from the category of older person. They are able to create an in-group/out-group categorisation which in turn enables them to explicitly express the implicit bias that they hold. The reason for doing this would be to create a wider gap between the ‘them’ and ‘us’ categories. This is done so that first, they do not see themselves as having those traits and characteristics attributable to the out-group and second, that they do not see themselves as an ageing/older person. It is a lifestyle threat to see oneself in a transitional role from an in-group where one is comfortable to another lesser perceived group which is currently an out-group. The reason why this is more 149 stressful with regards to ageing is that it is a transition that cannot be prevented and the perceived disadvantages associated with being an older person far outweigh the benefits. The older sample on the other hand would see themselves as having the same rights as they have always had (e.g. the right to hold a driving licence over the age of 65) and as such this would be reflected in the explicit measure. They do not have the same desire or at least ability to dissociate themselves from the older person category and as such exert the least ageist opinions in order to increase the perceived worth of their in-group. Conclusions Older people hold similarly negative implicit attitudes towards their own in-group to those held by the general adult population. This is important as older people who exhibited higher negative implicit attitudes also perform significantly worse on memory tasks as well as measures of self-esteem and physical abilities (Levy, 1996). This demonstrates that a pervasive negative attitude towards older people exists within the wider population and that even when becoming part of that group, the attitude remains due to the reinforcement of that negativity throughout the lifespan. Explicit measures identify that as people grow older they become more outwardly positive about older people. This is likely to be due to the desire to promote the status of the in-group to which they belong. Both findings support the fact that measures need to be taken on an individual level and on a wider societal platform. The individual level would be to educate younger people accurately on the ageing process and the positive associations with ageing. More widely a change is required in mainstream media to curtail the barrage of reinforcing negative stereotypes of older people. Neither task is simple but both are essential to improve the implicit attitudes 150 held by the general population, if not for the benefit of others, for the benefit of themselves when they too become an older person. 151 Study Seven – A meta-analysis of study one-six data sets Introduction Prior to this investigation, most of the ageism literature had centred on explicit measures (Cummings, Kropf & DeWeaver, 2000; Catterall & Maclaran, 2001; Depaola, Griffin, Young & Neimeyer, 2003) with only one substantial review of the existing implicit data (Levy & Banaji, 2002). This thesis has shown the effects of specific educational interventions, direct contact, nurse training and general higher level education on implicit and explicit attitudes towards older people. In addition to this it has supported the understanding that implicit and explicit measures assess distinct constructs of an attitudinal concept. Further still, it has added weight to both arguments that there is a societal wide negative implicit bias against older people and that older people also internalise this bias and associated stereotypes which in turn can lead to a decline in their physical and cognitive wellbeing. To date, there has not been a single study whereby a series of investigations has been conducted either longitudinally or cross-sectionally that compares such a diverse set of antecedents and their effects on attitudes. This affords this investigation a unique opportunity to conduct a meta-analysis comparing the results from these interventions to each other to provide the broadest picture of the attitudes held by each group and the overall effects of different interventions. In addition, correlational analyses have been conducted on the order of presentation for congruent and incongruent test blocks. This has been done to assess equivalence of different forms of the IAT and possible priming effects. This analysis is only 152 meaningful when comparing the whole sample of participants, rather than sub-groups as this allowed a reasonable sample size as well as presenting a broader cross-section of the population. Similarly, the comparison between implicit and explicit measures is also presented with data from the whole sample as this affords greater weight to the findings already presented. Participants Data from all 203 participants were collated and cross-tabulated. No additional participants were sampled for this meta-analysis and where longitudinal data existed for any sample, this was aggregated to create a single score for each group. There are eight groups of participants whose data has been collated and compared: ï‚· Older people ï‚· Psychology of Ageing course ï‚· Student nurses ï‚· Psychology students ï‚· Early years students ï‚· A&E nurses ï‚· Gerontology nurses ï‚· General adult sample Methodology The data from all of the previous studies have been collated and a MANOVA test has been conducted to find the mean results and to test overall differences between implicit and explicit scorings. Further to this, LSD post-hoc pairwise comparisons are displayed. 153 Results Table 14: Table of means for each sample population Group D-Score D-Score FSA Score FSA (Mean) S.D. (Mean) S.D. Older People (1) 1.0108 .759 51.5 9.58 Psychology of Ageing 1.0108 .651 55.8 8.95 Psychology Students (3) 0.7091 .954 54.2 9.36 Nursing Students (4) 0.6206 .671 47.8 9.57 Early Years Students (5) 2.4644 .861 53.1 11.62 A&E Nurses (6) 0.9732 .85 52.3 9.12 Geriatric Medicine 1.0118 .813 51.1 7.39 1.0931 .942 57.2 7.71 Course (2) Nurses (7) General Adult (8) Table 14 shows the implicit and explicit scores for each of the study samples included in this investigation. These groups have been numbered to allow for ease of display whilst comparing scores. The Table clearly indicates that each of the samples included in the analysis possess a significant implicitly ageist attitude. The Table also shows a large span of explicit scores with nursing students again being the least ageist and the general adult sample returning the most explicitly ageist scores. The main point of note, however, is that despite there being a difference between the scores, none of the groups (when data are pooled) have shown an 154 explicitly ageist attitude. This demonstrates the core difference between implicit and explicit attitudes which is central to the arguments of this thesis. Table 15: Table of significance values between samples for the implicit D-Score Measure Group Numbers Group Numbers 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 1 - 1.000 .216 .110 <.001*** .872 .997 .751 2 - - .298 .179 <.001*** .894 .997 .786 3 - - - .701 <.001*** .229 .243 .121 4 - - - - <.001*** .109 .132 .057† 5 - - - - - <.001*** <.001*** <.001*** 6 - - - - - - .877 .612 7 - - - .- - - - .766 8 - - - - - - - - † . Correlation approaching significance ***. Correlation significant at the 0.001 level The Table above shows a cross-tabulation of the p values for each of the groups implicit D-Scores. This shows where there is a significant difference between any of the groups. 155 Table 16: Table of significance values between samples for the FSA Explicit Measure Group Numbers Group Numbers † 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 1 - .199 .326 .172 .602 .771 .877 .050* 2 - - .611 .014* .443 .257 .172 .686 3 - - - .014* .716 .434 .278 .275 4 - - - - .072† .068† .261 .001*** 5 - - - - - .766 .521 .190 6 - - - - - - .660 .063† 7 - - - - - - - 0.45* 8 - - - - - - - - . Correlation approaching significance *. Correlation significant at the 0.05 level ***. Correlation significant at the 0.001 level Table 16 shows a cross-tabulation of the p values for each of the groups’ explicit FSA scores. This shows where there is any significant difference between any of the groups. Unlike the implicit measures, there are several samples that show significantly different scores on the explicit measure. In addition to the individual groups, between subjects ANOVA reported significant overall differences for both the implicit (F(7,162)=8.352, p<0.001) and explicit (F(7.162, p<0.05) measures. This indicates that there are significant differences between the groups in each of the two measures as an aggregate of the data presented in Tables 15 and 16. 156 When calculating the implicit scores for each of the studies presented, the D-Score algorithm has been used. This is the improved scoring algorithm as presented by Greenwald, Nosek and Banaji (2003) whereby as the final calculation, the scores for each person are divided by the standard deviation of their own data. This is a fundamental change from the original scoring method, which omitted this final step. This has shown to be an important change because magnitudes of differences between experimental treatment means are often correlated with variability of the data from which the means are computed. Using the standard deviation as a divisor adjusts differences between means for this effect of underlying variability. Overleaf, Table 17 shows the results of the implicit tests should the SD calculation not be made. 157 Table 17: Table of significance values between samples for the non-normed implicit scores Group Numbers Group Numbers † 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 1 - <.001*** <.001*** <.001*** <.001*** <.001*** .005** .005** 2 - - .615 .931 .686 .068† .060† .039* 3 - - - .600 .299 .003** .005** .002** 4 - - - - .561 .014* .018* .009** 5 - - - - - .128 .111 .074† 6 - - - - - - .763 .635 7 - - - - - - - .891 8 - - - - - - - - . Correlation approaching significance *. Correlation significant at the 0.05 level **. Correlation significant at the 0.01 level ***. Correlation significant at the 0.001 level The above Table has been produced as an illustrative depiction of what the result would have looked like prior to the D-Score algorithm and before variability had been accounted for. The first thing to notice is that there are a lot more significant differences especially with regards to older people who, it would appear are significantly more implicitly ageist than any of the other groups tested. Prior to the DScore algorithm, similar calculations had been recommended for use in cognitive ageing studies, in which treatment effects on latencies are often greater for elderly subjects, who show both higher means and greater variability of latencies than young subjects (Ratcliffe, Spieler & McKoon, 2000). 158 Cross-study comparisons of order effects The analysis was conducted with 170 participants across the studies to give a large data set for comparison purposes. Due to the fact data were taken across the studies, the age range was 20-84years with approx. 80% of the sample being female (an artefact of the sub-samples chosen as discussed in the individual studies). Using this full cross-study sample, the ordering of the implicit test stimulus presentation (congruent or incongruent first) was assessed. In each of the studies half of the sample completed the IAT with the congruent condition first and half of the sample with the incongruent pairings. As each of the studies was internally counterbalanced, if cognitive priming does occur then there is a lesser, if any, consequential effect due to the equal number of ordered presentations. In addition to this, the IAT was always completed by participants first so that no additional priming or context could be derived from the completion of the explicit measure. An ANOVA was conducted to compare the mean latencies and D-Score measures produced for both congruent and incongruent lead testing. Those IATs completed where a congruent condition was presented first returned a mean response of 404ms (D=.834) and those with incongruent initial presentations had a mean latency of 446ms (D=.788). When compared in an ANOVA, there was no significant difference between scores returned based on congruency of the initial presentation (p=.519). From this it can be taken that there was no significant order effect and any order effect on an individual level would be counterbalanced through the precautions taken in each of the individual studies in addition to the general measure whereby the IAT was completed first. 159 The structure of the IAT is repeated in each of the studies in the same way; as such, the counterbalancing result is important in methodological justification in each of the individual studies. The reason data were used from each of the studies was to demonstrate that this is a result that is relevant to each of the studies and generalisable to each of the sample populations used. In the literature review it has been clearly shown that implicit and explicit tests measure different subsets of the same attitude (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995). This has been an important concept to justify the use of the two tests in measuring the construct of ageism. It has further been shown that the two distinct subsets of attitude (implicit and explicit) influence different types of behaviour (spontaneous and planned, respectively). It is core to each of the studies detailed in this thesis that the measures used for each of the samples similarly reflect this difference. Each of the studies has independently shown that implicit and explicit tests measure different components of the same overall attitude. As each study has shown this result, a metaanalysis was conducted using every participant’s data for each test. This meta-analysis was important to illustrate the core theme that implicit and explicit attitudes are indeed different and are measured in distinctly different ways due to their inherent differences. Implicit vs. Explicit measures The implicit and explicit scores were compared using the same full study sample detailed for the order effects analysis. A Pearson’s test shows that there is only a weak positive correlation between the implicit and explicit scores with a co-efficient of 0.107. As the relationship between the two measures is so weak it can be taken that no 160 discernable correlation exists between them. This result is also not significant (p=.167) which would suggest that there is a large amount of variance/incidence of chance in any correlation shown. Both of these results are important as they support that supposition and basis of the testing that both measures are indeed testing two separate constructs that are not controlled in the same way (conscious and unconscious processes). If there was a stronger correlation which demonstrated a level of significance then it could be argued that the two subsets of attitude being measured are indeed linked and measure different aspects of the same construct. The results support the rationale of these studies and support previous research which suggests that there is no correlation between implicit and explicit measures (Hofmann, Gawronski, Gschwendner, Le & Schmitt, 2005). With both conditions satisfied, the data from each of the studies can be assumed to be both internally valid and not subject to order effects. Discussion The meta-analysis has further clarified some of the existing findings and also generated some others which had not been made apparent from analysing each of the studies as stand alone data sets. First, it is important to note that the overall effect was significant in that there are significant differences between the implicit and explicit scores as well as significant differences between the groups in both the implicit and explicit measures. The implicit and explicit difference is important, as stated prior to the commencement of the first study as it stands central to the understanding of implicit and explicit attitude measurement. The difference shows that the two scores are measuring distinct subsets 161 of attitude that combine to create the overall attitude held. This supports recent research by Greenwald, Poehlman, Uhlmann and Banaji (2009) who identified that for socially sensitive topics such as ageism, the predictive validity of self report measures was remarkably low where the IAT scored consistently highly. They posited that the reasons for this (especially in the more educated and socially aware participants) were that first, people were more aware of the socially acceptable answer even if this differed drastically from their own; and second, that participants wished to express attitudes that presented themselves in the most socially acceptable light. The results presented above illustrate that in this socially sensitive area, the two measures are significantly different with the explicit results showing the more socially desirable response. This not only shows that the two measures are probing different components of attitude but also that those self report measures are clearly more susceptible to self presentational bias. Not only are the two measures significantly different, but they are also opposite in the attitudes that have been expressed. It is evident from the descriptive statistics that the implicit results show a clear and distinct negative attitude towards older people in each of the eight participant groups sampled. This is not something that is characteristic of only one group, moreover it is clearly a societal norm that is exacerbated by other individual factors discussed later. The explicit measure on the other hand reports scores of below 58 for each of the sample groups. Scores of this nature (as detailed in the FSA methodology on pp. 70) indicate explicit attitudes that are not ageist and are indeed reflective of the expressed social norm. Although there is a significant difference between some of the groups which will be discussed later, the difference is in the levels of expressed attitude and not in the overall direction. 162 Looking at the implicit results separately it becomes clear that each of the participant groups is ageist. The only significant difference is with the Early Years students who express significantly more ageist implicit attitudes than do any of the other groups. No one group (including those where the Psychology of Ageing course had been implemented) show a significantly better implicit attitude than any other. This is an important result as it demonstrates that the trailed courses have not significantly improved the level of implicit bias in relation to other samples. Further to this, because the implicit scores are that much worse for the Early Years students compared to any of the other groups, certain questions are raised. Those courses where content looked at the psychology or biology of ageing did not affect the expressed implicit attitudes towards older people in any significant way to differentiate them from any other group of participants. The only differentiation came when participants were not shown an account of ageing (biased or not) but were instead educated on the development of children. This course centred on the gains associated with the formative years development as well as the development and acquisition of skills and socialisation. This course did not portray a negative account of ageing but simply a positive account of youth. As the IAT measures congruent and incongruent association response latencies, this youth-centric educational course would reasonably strengthen the congruent (youth/good) categorisations. In strengthening the associations with young and good, the response latencies for this pairing would likely decrease in the same way that increasing the negative associations with older people would increase the incongruent (old/good) latencies. The net effect of both of these scenarios is the same; a greater difference between the 163 two types of categorisation and the faster responses being associated with the congruent condition. In the case of the Early Years students, the IAT results demonstrate a significant preference for young people when compared to older people, this is not in question. The question generated is whether this preference for younger people is also reflective of a negative association for older people. Put colloquially, all ageist people prefer younger people over older people, but are all of those who prefer younger people over older people ageist? To resolve this, a way of measuring the attitudes directly towards old and young categories rather than as a preference for one over the other should be implemented. One such method of doing so is the Single Category Implicit Association Test (SCIAT) developed by Karpinski & Steinman (2006). Where the standard IAT relies on categorisation using pairings for both old and young simultaneously, the SC-IAT uses only one of those categories at a time to elicit the underlying attitudes held for a specific target. This could be used in conjunction with the existing IAT measure to ascertain base line and post intervention measures. This would allow for accurate reporting not only of the preferential attitude but also to demonstrate which aspect of the attitudes held have been affected (positive liking or negative disliking). The SCIAT has been shown to be effective in eliciting implicit single concept attitudes (Bohner, Siebler, Gonzalez, Haye & Schmidt, 2008; Steinman & Karpinski, 2008) and as such would be ideal when paired with the current IAT to clarify the issue. Other than this, this IAT has clearly illustrated an implicit ageist bias present throughout each of the societal samples taken which it can be argued are then more widely pervasive. The SC-IAT was not used in this study mainly because at the time the studies were conceived it did not exist. Even to date there is still a comparatively 164 small literature on the SC-IAT when compared to the IAT. With there being unanswered questions about the SC-IAT such as Richetin and Perugini (2008) who demonstrated that the SC-IAT did not show robust predictive validity whereas the IAT had an incremental validity for self reported behaviours, it was decided the IAT was a better instrument to use at that stage. The explicit scores, as previously mentioned, show the opposite valence to those indicated in the implicit tests. Each of the groups express explicitly accepting attitudes, however, the differences between some of these groups are illustrative of two core concepts; higher level education increasing self presentational bias and older people explicitly promoting the status of their in-group. The significant differences between the General Adult population and the Nursing students, Geriatric nurses and the approaching significance of the difference with A&E nurses indicate an effect on explicit attitudes by nurse training. Throughout their training nurses will be instructed on their duties of care in addition to the health requirements and needs of different demographics of patients. This explicitly gained information will inform the nurses of appropriate ways to act and that each patient should be treated as an individual rather than as a stereotype. This education would act only to reinforce the societal norms that prejudice is wrong and as such, expressions of prejudice are not acceptable. As nurses will have undergone this specific education as well having gone through higher education, they are not only generally educated to a higher level and as such aware of presenting themselves in a socially acceptable light, but also specifically in they would be aware that expression of prejudice towards any one group of people would be even more unacceptable in 165 their profession. Although not affecting their implicit bias, the specific education that they have received reduces the expressed explicit bias due to the heightened awareness of social acceptability. As previously mentioned it is evident from the descriptive statistics that the explicit scores of each of those sample groups who have undertaken higher level education are lower than those who have not. This point illustrates that the more educated people become, the higher the level of self presentational bias due to the increased awareness of social acceptability. When this level of education is not present, it appears that participants are unable to monitor to the same degree the way in which their expressed opinions will reflect on them. This follows the prediction based on the theory of self presentational bias (Goffman, 1959) where it is stated the more educated a person is, the more aware they become of the subjective and societal norms against which they will measure themselves and in turn be measured. So in short, both general higher level education and more significantly higher level nurse training increase an individual’s level of self presentational bias and in turn decreases the levels of ageism expressed on explicit measures where conscious mental representations can be made. The significant difference in explicit scores between older people and those in the General Adult population can be explained by older people explicitly promoting the status of their in-group. The items on the scale are varied but generally centre on the desire to spend time with older people and their rights. For the younger group, it is possible to dissociate themselves from the category of older person. They are able to create an in-group/out-group categorisation which in turn enables them to explicitly express the implicit bias that they hold. The reason for doing this would be to create a wider gap between the ‘them’ and ‘us’ categories. This is done so that first, they do 166 not see themselves as having those traits and characteristics attributable to the outgroup and second, that they do not see themselves as an ageing/older person. It is a lifestyle threat to see oneself in a transitional role from an in-group where one is comfortable to another lesser perceived group which is currently an out-group. The reason why this is more stressful with regards to ageing is that it is a transition that cannot be prevented and the perceived disadvantages associated with being an older person far outweigh the perceived benefits. The older sample on the other hand would see themselves as having the same rights as they have always had (e.g. the right to hold a driving licence over the age of 65) and as such this would be reflected in the explicit measure. They do not have the same desire or at least ability to dissociate themselves from the older person category and as such exert the least ageist opinions in order to increase the perceived worth of their in-group. The reasons for presenting the results in Table 17 were to illustrate the differences in the scores that are made through simply dividing by the standard deviation. The DScore algorithm was an appreciable development in scoring methods and the results clearly illustrate this. Reaction times and variance have clearly made a huge difference in which samples differ significantly with regards to the implicit age bias. The fact that the results significantly alter, even between groups which on the surface would appear to be similar/homogeneous, when this adjustment for response time is made, support the change to the D-Score algorithm. There are three fundamental reasons that the D-Score will have made such a difference even in apparently homogeneous groups. First are simple levels of IQ. Less able groups have a lower level of fluid intelligence and as such this will affect their response speed. This could previously have meant that those less academically able groups appear more 167 prejudiced whereas in actuality they simply have longer response times. This result from the current analysis supports Cai, Sriram and Greenwald (2004) who conclude that the D-Score eliminated cognitive skill confounds as a mediating factor within the IAT. Second are the effects of task recurrence. Both using a computer keyboard and being versed in categorisation responses using push buttons will affect the response times of participants. Undergraduate students are constantly barraged with pleas from psychology students to participate in tests which often require keyboard responses. If people are practiced in this type of procedure then responding to the categorisation stimuli in the IAT would be easier than for those to whom this procedure was alien. Without suitable weighting, this difference could again make those people who are procedurally unfamiliar appear more prejudiced. Dutilh, Vandekerckhove, Tuerlinckx and Wagenmakers (2009) reinforced this as a distinct possibility, highlighting that being familiar with or repeating a set cognitive task would undoubtedly result in decreasing response times. By dividing the score by the standard deviation in a similar way to the Cohen’s d, the effect of familiarity can be eliminated. Third is the effect of generally slow responses. As has shown to be the case for older people, some groups are simply slower to make responses both in judgements and in actual time taken to physically respond. As with the other conditions, the slower the response, the more prejudice the raw IAT score would suggest. Through using the D-Score IAT measure, this problem can be removed as a consideration. Conclusions The meta-analysis has led to several conclusions being made that would not have otherwise come to light. The IAT demonstrated no significant order effect and any order effect on an individual level would be counterbalanced through the precautions 168 taken in each of the individual studies. There is also a clear implicitly negative attitude towards older people pervasive across all groups. The expression of this implicitly negative attitude by older people as well as the other groups would suggest that this is a pervasive societal negative attitude. The explicit scores indicate that there is a tacit knowledge of social acceptability in response as no group is openly explicitly ageist. As well as demonstrating a clear level of implicit ageism, the dissonance between the two measures supports the fact that implicit and explicit tests measure two distinct constructs. Trialled age education courses had no discernible effect of implicit ageism. However, by promoting children’s development through education, implicit attitudes towards older people became significantly worse than any other group. This suggests the need for SC-IAT’s or similar measures to be implemented when looking at the direction of an attitude rather than at the preference of one category over another. Higher level education and specifically nurse training reduce the explicit measure of ageism through increasing knowledge on social acceptability and self presentational bias. Further to this, the explicit scores for older people are also lower than those of other groups (excluding education as a factor increasing self presentational bias) due to the explicit promotion of their in-group. Finally the metaanalysis has also provided overwhelming support for the use of the D-Score algorithm over the previous conventional scoring methods. 169 Chapter Seven General Discussion All of the results displayed and discussed throughout the investigation and in turn in this discussion have been based upon the D-Measure algorithm (Greenwald, Nosek & Banaji, 2003). For comparison purposes, results were also displayed in the metaanalysis that were scored using the previous conventional algorithm. The fact that the results altered significantly when the D-Measure was applied indicates a clear need to adjust for the differences in response speed even in groups that superficially appear to be homogeneous. There are three fundamental reasons that the D-Score will have made such a difference even in apparently homogeneous groups. First are simple levels of IQ. Less able groups have a lower level of fluid intelligence and as such this will affect their response speed. Second are the effects of task recurrence. Both using a computer keyboard and being versed in categorisation responses using push buttons will affect the response times of participants. Third, is the effect of generally slow responses where slower responses to all categories lead to larger proportional differences and as such larger effect sizes. The improved scoring algorithm has shown to account for these confounds as mediating factors and to strengthen the reliability and applicability of the IAT (Cai, Sriram & Greenwald, 2004; Dutilh, Vandekerckhove, Tuerlinckx & Wagenmakers, 2009). There are no outspoken criticisms of using the D-Measure in calculating the IAT effect and these results support the use of the D-Measure in every IAT study and not just those where heterogeneous groups are being compared. A synopsis of the D-Measure scoring process can be found in Appendix Four on page 236. 170 As an overall point, each of the studies has demonstrated significant differences between the implicit and explicit scores. When added to the analysis presented at the start of the results and again in each separate study whereby no significant correlation was found between the two measures, it is clear that two distinct concepts are being measured. These data support previous research which suggests that there is no correlation between implicit and explicit measures (Hofmann, Gawronski, Gschwendner, Le & Schmitt, 2005). This thesis does not suggest that explicit measures are incorrect, nor that the implicit measures are innately better. The research emphasises that the ability now exists to collect data on both forms of attitude and as such, allows collection of the complete attitude as opposed to one or either subset. The difference between the implicit and explicit measures is no more clear than when the valence of these attitudes is considered. In each of the studies in this investigation, the sample populations have always exhibited negative implicit attitudes towards older people and positive explicit attitudes. This is a stable finding and is still evident post exposure to any of the courses tested. The difference between the implicit and explicit measures was also evident during the debriefing procedure. All participants were informed of the overall aim of the research and the difference between implicit and explicit attitudes. Further to this they were also informed that no answers given were incorrect nor would any individual’s scores be highlighted or separated from the aggregate totals. During the discussion following the formal debrief there were some interesting trends emerging. Inline with previous research (Monteith, Voils & Ashburn-Nardo, 2001) those participants who 171 responded slowest to incongruent pairings (good-old, young-bad) on the IAT were often aware that this was the case. They voiced their opinions that this may be the case, however, they did not report that they were concerned this was the case. All participants who exhibited this degree of reflexivity said that the task caused them to think more about the incongruent condition and they believed that it would be reflected in their scores. After exploration and explanation of the procedure, participants were happy that the scores were not easy to predict and that individual scores would not be highlighted. At this point all participants were reminded if they were unhappy they could remove their data from the experiment. No participant withdrew at this point. In contrast to the implicit measure, all participants reflected positively on the Fraboni Scale of Ageism. Even those participants who returned higher scores did not seem concerned about portraying themselves as ageist. When this was explored following the formal debriefing, the general consensus was that it was clear what each of the statements were asking and that they ‘knew what the answers were’. This indicates that the participants may very well have a preconceived idea about what was expected / socially acceptable and that they responded in such as way to comply with expectation. This further supports the supposition that the explicit measure was subject to self presentational bias, something that was not evident with the implicit measure. General implicit ageism The data indicate that all groups are implicitly ageist. But why is this so? As this result is common to each of the groups irrespective of their backgrounds, gender, occupation or level of education, it can be posited that the implicit ageist bias is something that is more firmly ingrained into the wider society. Levy and Banaji (2002) conducted a review on implicit ageism illustrating a pervasive and wide 172 reaching proliferation of negative ageist attitudes. The understanding that society generally holds negative attitudes towards older people is long since established. Kite and Johnson (1988) found that pervasive attitudes present in research suggest higher negative attitudes than positive ones. This supported the startling findings by Isaacs and Bearison (1986) that showed children as young as six exhibit ageist prejudices present in their cultures. Unlike other prejudices, ageist attitudes are still openly prevalent in society due to their wider implicit acceptance and the commonly held misconception that these assertions are harmless. In television comedies, elderly people are depicted, defined by stereotyped negativities regarding physical decline and both physical and mental incompetencies (Zebrowitz & Montepare, 2000). When these age stereotypes have been acquired, they are easily activated by the presence of an elderly person (Banaji & Hardin, 1996; Perdue & Gurtman, 1990). This will result in the generalisation of elderly people to the stereotyped schema held and as is the case with implicit attitudes, will define the way in which older people are treated when the consequence is outside of the actor’s conscious thoughts. Once acquired, these attitudes are maintained and strengthened when encountering elderly people even if they do not exhibit characteristics associated with the stereotypes held (Levy et al., 2000; Murphy, Monahan & Zajonc, 1995). Challenging these negative attitudes also proves harder for ageism than other forms of prejudice. Hill et al. (1990) demonstrated that even when encountering contradictory evidence, attitudes towards older people were resistant to change and in most cases did not alter. This thankfully has been shown more recently to be surmountable with improved training programmes designed to actively combat negative ageing stereotypes (Cheong, Wong & Koh, 2009). As with most negative associations, 173 however, if encountering contact can be avoided then generally it is. Purdue and Gurtman (1990) observed that young people are actively seeking ways to refrain from engaging in social meetings with elderly people. This avoidance only reinforces the implicitly held beliefs as it prevents the individual from having ‘meaningful’ encounters with elderly adults which may in fact cause inconsistencies in schema to be noticed and re-evaluations of attitudes to take place. These negative implicit attitudinal findings are not only characteristic of younger generations. Negative implicit ageist attitudes are held by the elderly themselves (Levy & Banaji, 2002). The reasoning given for this is that elderly people have acquired the same implicit prejudices throughout their lives and have not had sufficient time or opportunity to develop the mechanisms to defend against this. The results from this investigation support the fact that older people hold these negative implicit attitudes in the same way that other groups do too. Butler (1987) found that when adopted, these stereotypes became a self-fulfilling prophecy, reinforcing stereotypes through the inaction and deficits resulting from their initial belief and internalisation. This finding in and of itself adds weight to the argument that additional intervention measures need to be developed not only for the medical community but also for a wider societal change starting during compulsory schooling. Psychological theory can be used to design interventions that can be used in an educational setting to impart knowledge and cause disequilibrium in the current schema held to force a re-assessment of existing attributes and evaluations to modify the existing ageist attitude. This has been shown by Kite et al. (2005) who demonstrated that upon providing information about a person that countered existing negative ageist stereotypes, the negative attitudes can be diminished. As the valence 174 of the results on the implicit tests are the same for all groups including older people, support can be taken for the wide reaching nature of the negative implicit attitude. The different groups in the investigation demonstrated varying degrees of implicit ageism due to differing characteristics and courses tested. These will be reflected on individually. However, the overall picture is one of a pervasive societal negative attitude towards older people. General explicit ageism In opposition to the implicit attitudes held and again in support of the measurement of two distinct attitudinal concepts, the scores on all of the explicit tests denoted attitudes that were accepting of older people/not ageist. As they are so polar opposite in each of the studies, the question that presents itself is why? Explicit measures are subject to self presentational bias (Goffman, 1959) whereby the individual is expressing attitudes under conscious control to place themselves in the best possible / socially acceptable light. Subjective norms are adopted through expectation of this acceptability and despite interventions (educational or otherwise) the understanding of what is socially acceptable or expected is ingrained and as such expressed when explicitly questioned. The results of the explicit tests demonstrate an acceptance of what socially acceptable responses are and despite any internal changes as shown by implicit scores, these will still be outwardly expressed. As Goffman further posited, however, level of education can also be a mediating factor in the level of explicit bias shown. This is something that will be considered further later. Also of note are the results on the explicit scores for the older people. As with the other groups, they demonstrate a non-ageist attitude but they are also one of the lower 175 scores recorded. This suggests that comparably, older people hold less ageist explicit attitudes than most of the other groups. When higher level education has been taken into account as a mediating factor (a point discussed later) the scores for older people on the explicit measure are not just lower but significantly so. When comparing older people’s explicit scores to those of the general adult population there is a significant difference with older people exhibiting a significantly lower level of prejudice (though both are still not high enough to be classed as ageist on the measure). The items on the explicit scale are varied but generally centre on the desire to spend time with older people and their rights. For the younger cohorts, it is possible to dissociate themselves from the category of older person. They are able to create an in-group/outgroup categorisation which in turn enables them to explicitly express the implicit bias that they hold. The reason for doing this would be to create a wider gap between the ‘them’ and ‘us’ categories. This is done so that first, they do not see themselves as having those traits and characteristics attributable to the out-group; and second, that they do not see themselves as an ageing/older person. It is a lifestyle threat to see oneself in a transitional role from an in-group where one is comfortable to another lesser perceived group which is currently an out-group (Brandtstadter & Greve, 1994). The reason why this is more stressful with regards to ageing is that it is a transition that cannot be prevented and the perceived disadvantages associated with being an older person far outweigh the benefits. The older sample on the other hand would see themselves as having the same rights as they have always and as such this would be reflected in the explicit measure. They do not have the same desire or at least ability to dissociate themselves from the older person category and as such exert the least ageist opinions in order to increase the perceived worth of their in-group. 176 Effects of education on attitudes towards older people Four of the six studies in the investigation were looking at educational courses to assess whether they had an impact on attitudes towards older people. These studies were looking at the effect of higher level education, specific nurse training and two different age-related educational courses. Each of these studies provided additional evidence that informs about the ways in which education affects attitudes towards older people. Higher level education per se was studied by individually looking at Psychology students vs. Nursing students and by looking at the groupings of those people who have been in education post A-Level to those in the general adult sample who have not. As with the other samples, each illustrated a solid negative implicit attitude to older people and scores on the explicit measure that were not considered ageist. Despite not quite demonstrating an explicit level of ageism, it is evident that those not educated to a higher level are significantly explicitly more prejudiced than either of the other groups. It is also evident from the descriptive statistics that the explicit scores of each of those sample groups who have undertaken higher level education are lower than those who have not. This point illustrates that the more educated people become, the higher the level of self presentational bias due to the increased awareness of social acceptability. When this level of education is not present, it appears that participants are unable to monitor to the same degree the way in which their expressed opinions will reflect on them. This follows the prediction based on the theory of self presentational bias (Goffman, 1959) where it is stated the more educated a person is, the more aware they become of the subjective and societal norms against which they will measure themselves and in turn be measured. This is a strong finding with 177 regards to the effects of education on the explicit attitudes towards older people and an individual’s self presentational bias. Further to this it may also suggest that those who do not receive higher level education are less able to monitor the way in which they present themselves and the attitudes they express. These results, however, only look at the effects of general higher level education from amassed student data. When broken down to specific types of education, other results become apparent. The significant differences in explicit scores between the General Adult population and the Nursing students, Geriatric nurses and the approaching significance of the difference with A&E nurses indicate an effect on explicit attitudes by nurse training. Throughout their training nurses will be instructed on their duties of care in addition to the health requirements and needs of different demographics of patients. This explicitly gained information will inform the nurses of appropriate ways to act and that each patient should be treated as an individual rather than as a stereotype. This education would act only to reinforce the societal norms that prejudice is wrong and as such, expressions of prejudice are not acceptable. As nurses will have undergone this specific education as well having gone through higher education, they are not only generally educated to a high level and as such aware of presenting themselves in a socially acceptable light, but also specifically in that they would be aware that expression of prejudice towards any one group of people would be even more unacceptable in their profession. Although not affecting their implicit bias, the specific education that they have received reduces the expressed explicit bias due to the heightened awareness of social acceptability. Along with the effects of general higher level education, the effects of nurse training need to be taken into consideration 178 when looking at explicit attitudinal scores especially those where the attitude being measured is easily noticeable. Educational interventions to reduce implicit and explicit ageism have in recent years shown to be effective. Cheong, Wong & Koh (2009) identified that through successful education, medical students in Singapore are expressing increasingly positive attitudes towards older people. This programme included a balanced approach to ageing, presenting models of successful healthy ageing as well as the ways older people compensate for those losses associated with ageing, thus maintaining their quality of life. In addition to the standard classroom based education, Gonzales, Morrow-Howell & Gilbert (2010) demonstrated that through adding contact with older people through activities outside of a classroom, the attitudes of medical students improved significantly. Both of these interventions show how psychological knowledge can be used to structure interventions to effectively decrease negative attitudes towards older people. The current investigation looked at a psychology course as an intervention measure and also the effects of a course not looking at ageing but instead, specifically child development. The Psychology of Ageing elective third year module was assessed to ascertain whether in presenting a rounded perspective of ageing, attitudes towards older people could be made more positive. Despite not producing any significant results, the descriptive results did show an alarming trend. Rather than reducing any implicit ageism that may have been present, the implicit attitudes towards older people became markedly worse. There are a few possibilities as to why this may have been the case when the course is measured against those previously mentioned successful 179 interventions. The course had attempted to reflect the way in which older people can compensate for losses associated with ageing, however, as an undergraduate third year course rather than a training course, much of the additional information regarding the gains associated with ageing should have been gathered by the students in wider reading outside of the classroom context. As the measure for an intervention, all of the learning material should be presented to the students so that they have all of the information to assimilate and there is not the reliance on individual motivation to improve knowledge. As there was no measure of the additional work that students had completed outside of the weekly lecture, it is not known how rounded the information taken by the students was. Another reason why the course may not have been successful is the method of course delivery. Firstly although classroom education alone has been shown to be effective in Singapore (Cheong, Wong & Koh, 2009), the addition of direct contact/activities with older people to this has shown to be more successful (Gonzales, Morrow-Howell & Gilbert, 2010). It is the direct contact with older people that could in turn cause the information being imparted to take on a real world context and more deeply affect the student’s implicit attitudes. Second, the course consisted of one weekly lecture. It is possible that a more intensive course, where the students would have less time between exposures in which to forget the content, would be more successful in promoting attitudinal shifts. The other educational course that was measured was the Early Years course at the University of Glamorgan. This is a course that does not look at older people in any context and does not look at the losses or gains associated with ageing so should not affect the attitudes held towards older people. This course centred on the gains associated with the formative years development as well as the development and 180 acquisition of skills and socialisation. This course did not portray a negative account of ageing but simply a positive account of youth. Results from the implicit tests clearly illustrate that the implicit attitudes held by the early years students was significantly more prejudiced than either of the two other student populations (Psychology or Nursing). This is a worrying finding as these are the only students who have not undertaken any formal education that should directly affect their implicit attitudes towards older people. The IAT measures attitude strength and valence through paired associations using opposing words and images. The IAT results illustrate an implicit preference and when measuring a cross-sectional sample there are few issues with this. The results would suggest that it is possible for an intervention/educational programme to negatively affect implicit ageist attitudes when no ageing literature is covered in the course. This suggests that through educating about youth and improving attitudes to them, the opposing IAT categorisation and associated negative attitude will also increase. However, it is distinctly possible to have a preference for younger over older people but not to be ageist in one’s attitude or behaviour. The argument follows that if a preference for youth is cultivated through exposure, group membership or education, the relative evaluation of the opposing category will decrease. Bohner, Siebler, Gonzalez, Haye and Schmidt (2008) have shown this precise argument in an IAT/SCIAT measure of country preference. They found that when using the IAT measure, Turkey had a more favourable evaluation than Germany, however, when using the SC-IAT there was no significant difference in appraisal of the two countries. This they argue shows that a negative attitude can be expressed towards an attitude object where only a relative negative attitude exists. When only a single category is assessed 181 at any one time then both the preference for one category over the other can be reduced/removed and the non-relative attitude can be assessed. To this end a way of measuring the attitudes directly towards old and young categories rather than as a preference for one over the other should be implemented. As previously discussed, one such method of doing so is the Single Category Implicit Association Test (SCIAT) developed by Karpinski & Steinman (2006). Where the standard IAT relies on categorisation using pairings for both old and young simultaneously, the SC-IAT uses only one of those categories at a time to elicit the underlying attitudes held for a specific target. This could be used in conjunction with the existing IAT measure to ascertain base line and post intervention measures. This would allow for accurate reporting not only of the preferential attitude but also to demonstrate which aspect of the attitudes held have been affected (positive liking or negative disliking). The SCIAT has been shown to be effective in eliciting implicit single concept attitudes (Bohner, Siebler, Gonzalez, Haye & Schmidt, 2008; Steinman & Karpinski, 2008) and as such would be ideal when paired with the current IAT to clarify the direction of expressed attitudes. It is important to reiterate an earlier point that the SC-IAT was not used throughout this thesis mainly because at the time the methodologies were formulated, agreed and approved, the SC-IAT did not exist. To date there are still many unanswered questions surrounding the applicability of the SC-IAT (Richetin & Perugini, 2008) and as such its appropriateness is not as widely accepted as that of the IAT. Because of these factors and the widely accepted nature of the IAT it was decided that the IAT would be used throughout with recommendations to include the SC-IAT in future studies when further extensive testing has been conducted and more about its validity is known. 182 Effects of direct contact on attitudes towards older people Zajonc’s (1968) theory of mere exposure effect suggests that just from coming into contact with something on a regular basis will make that object appear more favourable. A wealth of research has supported this and the direct contact hypothesis in that contact between groups where negative stereotypes have previously been held can itself help reduce the negativity of the stereotype and associated attitude (Allport, 1954; Cook, 1962, 1978; Pettigrew, 1979; Stephan & Stephan, 1984; Paolini, Hewstone, Cairns & Voci, 2004; King, Winter & Webster, 2009). From this it could be suggested that those nurses working in a ward specifically for care of older people would hold less negative (if not positive) attitudes towards older people than do those people who do not have daily contact. Results from the meta-analysis indicate that there was no significant difference in the implicit (or explicit) attitudes held between the two groups of qualified nurses. It would have been expected that due to the larger amount of contact, those nurses working in a dedicated ward for the care of older people would have less negative implicit attitudes than those working in A&E. However, research has also indicated that even after taking selection and social desirability processes into account, all types of exposure affect attitudes in a favourable direction (Lee, Farrell & Link, 2004). When taking this into account and acknowledging that both types of qualified nurse are likely to have contact with older people on a daily basis, it is probable that the exposure that both experience accounts for the lack of significant difference between the two groups. What is of note, however, is when comparing those nurses who have specific daily contact with older people to those general adults, a significant difference is apparent. Previous comparisons of contact could very well have been 183 mediated by the effects of higher level education on self presentational bias, however this would not be the case for those adults who have not undertaken this level of education. Despite there being a significant difference between these two groups there are points of caution. No specific effect of contact was found between the two types of nurses although this can be explained by undoubted contact with older people in the A&E department. Also there is no significant difference between nurses working in a geriatric ward and other groups of students who do not have this contact, although this can be explained due to increased self presentational abilities from higher education. The difference between the Geriatric nurses and the Adult sample could be due to educational level alone, however, if this were the case there would be significant differences for each of the groups with higher level education. The results do not categorically support the contact hypothesis, however, they do support further investigation to look at the mediating factors of education and direct contact on the explicit attitudes held towards older people. Test stability over time Two of the studies in the investigation were longitudinal in nature in that they had a base measure taken, an intervention introduced and a measure taken post intervention with the same cohorts. Results of these tests have already been presented in the actual results that they show, however, this type of test also allows for the consistency of the measures used to be analysed. In the first longitudinal study the explicit measure correlated significantly at each time period with each group and although returning at times what are relatively low “r” values, these are significant and show that there is consistency in the way in which 184 participants are responding. An explanation for this is the nature of self presentational bias. The participants are aware of the socially acceptable answer and as such at each time point they answer each of the statements on the FSA in a way that would reflect these subjective norms. This answering strategy is possible in most explicit measures and the FSA is no exception. The individual items are quite clear in their intent so from this, a socially acceptable answer is easily provided. As the measure is a straight forward one, despite the participants being unlikely to remember the questions between data collection points, similar responses would be easily provided thus explaining not only the strength and direction of the attitudes but also their stability and correlation. Either way, the explicit measure is a stable predictor of the attitudes held that would be expressed through behaviours in planned situations. The implicit measure, however, provides a different picture. There are no significant correlations within the groups between any of the time points and the correlations returned are themselves very weak. This suggests that the scores returned on an individual basis fluctuate between time periods and as such are not a stable indicator of implicit attitudes. It would be expected that each time point (similarly to the explicit measures) would be correlated significantly with the other implicit data returned on an individual level. An initial supposition from this would be that although the IAT performs well as a group measure, this study does not support the IAT as a consistent measure of individual implicit attitudes. The results would hold that individual fluctuations are such over time that few predictions can indeed be made on an individual level about future attitudes or behaviours. If true, this could be worth serious consideration prior to using the IAT as an individual measure both in terms of academic research and theory development but also in practical terms for 185 marketing and screening tools. If the fluctuations are such that only basic direction rather than strength of an attitude can be accurately predicted, the use of the IAT for screening personality types or attitudes held on an individual basis should be severely scrutinised. What also needs to be considered, however, is that previous longitudinal research (Lovden, Li, Shing & Lindenberger, 2007) has demonstrated low test correlations over time. They hypothesised the important distinction in longitudinal studies is that the negative attitudes (which measure relative differences between positive and negative responses) remain consistent. They advised that caution must be used with the IAT as the absolute size of the score is almost certain to fluctuate. There is, however, an alternative hypothesis to the differentiation of scores between time points. The focus of the longitudinal studies was to investigate the effects of higher level education and a Psychology of Ageing course on attitudes to older people. The general higher level education is not solely dedicated at any stage to the promotion of innovative ageing or reducing ageism and as such there has not been an observable flux in the explicit measurement scores. The psychology course is specifically looking at the effects of ageing, however, this is in the context of undertaking other modules when ageing content is not present. The implicit test is attempting to measure attitudes outside of the conscious control of the participant and as such the effects of any intervention or education could have a greater impact. The impact of education on any one person is not a linear process and as such different methods and content can affect different people in different ways. Due to the differing impact of the education being received at each of the time points during the undergraduate programme, it stands to reason that an individual’s score may not correlate as the scores fluctuate (within the same valence). If this were indeed the 186 case, the IAT itself could be considered a stable measure all things being equal without the presence of an intervention measure. The IAT has been shown to be a stable measure over time (Perugini, 2005) even on an individual basis, however, in the meta analysis conducted by Perugini, none of the testing was with groups undergoing any form of intervention. The results from this investigation are not conclusively supportive of either possibility regarding the effectiveness of the IAT, however, they do support further investigation as to the reliability of the IAT for predicting behavioural intent on an individual rather than group level. Despite some of the issues raised through analysis of data, this study has still significantly furthered the understanding of attitudes towards older people. It has furthered understanding of the prevailing implicit and explicit attitudes held by different groups of people and through meta-analysis the wider society. In doing so it has identified societal wide negative implicit attitudes towards older people and conversely explicitly accepting attitudes. It has also supported the use of both implicit and explicit measures of attitude measurement in obtaining a complete measure of an attitude. Further to this it has looked in depth at the educational and situational factors that affect attitudes towards older people and the implications for different subgroups. Moreover it has identified ways in which existing courses have not been successful in modifying attitudes as well as highlighting potential pitfalls in interpreting data from the IAT. To build on the research conducted in this study, further advancing the knowledge of implicit attitude research and ageing research in general, future research and directions have been suggested. 187 Future research directions Each of the studies conducted in this investigation have highlighted possible methodological improvements or directions that could be undertaken to further the knowledge gained. Presented are a series of studies that would lend further support to the current research and provide theoretical and practical advancement both on an academic platform and in a practical context. To gain a better picture of the effects of general higher level education, several changes could be made to the methodology of the initial study presented. The study should again be longitudinal in nature taking measures throughout the course programme but it should vary the students and courses assessed. The current samples were chosen due to the sample homogeneity (age, gender etc) which despite being a strength, has resulted in two courses being assessed where age related content has been included. It would be of interest, when teamed with the improved sampling, to include undergraduate courses that do not include age related information (e.g. computing, business). By including these courses, the effect of general higher level education could be more accurately extracted. Further to the course content, sampling issues and environmental contexts should be controlled to provide a more structured framework for analysing mediating factors. If the sample of students included both residential and non-residential as well as ‘mature’ and school-leaver age students, there would be control over the type of environment that the students were exposed to in addition to the course content. An extension of this would be to also assess the attitudes of those who undertake a distance learning course (e.g. Open University) to assess the impact solely of the university environment on the implicit and explicit attitudes held. This would allow for any mediating effects of living with peers and 188 being surrounded 24/7 only by people of a youth demographic to be factored into the analysis. In addition to those retests, a longitudinal control group should be employed to test the correlation of time point data in a group where no manipulation/intervention is present. If the results from this still suggest that there are no statistically significant correlations at the different time points with all other things being equal then this would support the initial supposition that the IAT should be used with caution on an individual basis. Either way, more investigation is needed to conclusively support whether specific or general higher level education have an impact on the implicit attitudes held about older people. A study of the general adult population would also further the understanding of the effects of higher level education so as a study to run in conjunction with that outlined above modifications to the current adult sample should be undertaken. For practical reasons it was not possible to examine more sections of the population. Therefore future research should expand to test other samples, stratified by education, socioeconomic status, etc. However, the fact that relatively few differences have been observed in the samples tested indicates that this is unlikely to yield anything that has not already been established here, only strengthen it. In addition to this, the general adult population sample was all taken from those who worked in the university. Despite the fact that the majority of the adults were not working with students on a daily basis (kitchen, clerical, grounds maintenance, etc) they would still have been in a general environment where they would have come into daily contact with them. As research has shown, (Lee, Farrell & Link, 2004) when in day-to-day contact, attitudes towards the target group (in this case the students/young people) will be affected. This means that in the current study it is not solely the effect from lack of higher level 189 education that is being observed. The sample for future studies will have to include or comprise solely those people who have not received higher level education and do not work in environments with young or older people (e.g. office workers) and finally the use of a Single Category IAT (SC-IAT) in addition to the standard IAT would allow for direction of preference to be shown too. The measurement of attitudes held by the early years students highlighted some other methodological issues with the IAT that warrant additional investigation. It is important to be able to assess the direction of the attitudes expressed rather than seeing it as a preference of one category over another. Especially with regard to measuring the effectiveness of intervention measures, it is important to be able to assess the direction an attitude is being influenced. It is distinctly possible to have a preference for younger over older people but not to be ageist in your attitude or behaviour. To this end a way of measuring the attitudes directly towards old and young categories rather than as a preference for one over the other should be implemented. One such method of doing so is the SC-IAT developed by Karpinski & Steinman (2006). This could be used in conjunction with the existing IAT measure to ascertain base line and post intervention measures. This would allow for accurate reporting not only of the preferential attitude but also to demonstrate which aspect of the attitudes held have been affected (positive liking or negative disliking). When conducted with a control group and using a longitudinal design, the true effects of any intervention measure should be visible rather than making assumptions as is currently the case with the IAT where only dyadic preference can be noted. 190 The implicit attitudes of nurses both in training and those who are qualified have been shown to be significantly negative. This is of concern as it is reflective of the behaviour expressed when in spontaneous situations such as emergency or acute care of elderly patients. It is apparent that interventions need to be developed that follow the directions shown by other successful programmes (Cheong, Wong & Koh, 2009; Gonzales, Morrow-Howell & Gilbert, 2010). As the population ages it is imperative that a successful method of combating this issue is found so as to minimise / eliminate ageism as a factor in care choices made for older people. Once developed for a UK care context, the same longitudinal technique as outlined above should be used to assess its effectiveness. Of interest would be to monitor the career pathway of nurses through either a series of cross-sectional studies or with a longitudinal cohort to assess whether this is a trend that continues throughout their careers. In addition, post intervention testing would demonstrate the longevity of any attitudinal change that does occur. Each of the future research directions outlined above would further the field of psycho-gerontology and specifically the understanding of implicit and explicit attitudes towards older people. The reason for conducting this research does have to be placed in a practical context as this research is not just to increase theoretical understanding. The population of the UK is ageing. Over the last 25 years the percentage of the population aged 65 and over increased from 15% in 1983 to 16 per cent in 2008, an increase of 1.5 million people in this age group. Over the same period, the percentage of the population aged 16 and under decreased from 21% to 19%. This trend is projected to continue. By 2033, 23 per cent of the population will be aged 65 and over compared to 18% aged 16 or younger. The fastest population 191 increase has been in the number of those aged 85 and over, the ’oldest old‘. Since 1983 when there were just over 600,000 people aged 85+ in the UK, the numbers have more than doubled reaching 1.3 million in 2008. By 2033 the number of people aged 85 and over is projected to more than double again to reach 3.2 million, and to account for 5% of the total population (Office for National Statistics, 2009). In a health care context, if attitudes towards older people remain as they currently are the incidence of neglect and substandard care is likely to increase. The training of medical and nursing students to reduce or eradicate their negative attitudes must be the primary focus for the benefit of the wider society. Teamed with this, any intervention should be trialled in the wider society along with the promotion of a paradigm shift in the way in which advertising and the wider media portray older people. Despite these being ambitious and outside of the influence of any one person or piece of research, through the production of conclusive research and amassing a robust evidence base, research has the power, when made known, to affect policy and effect societal change. This is of central importance for several reasons. It is only through education of all ages that the societal change will occur. It is possible to change the attitudes of a few smaller groups like those of medical professionals; however this will not affect the internalised negative societal stereotypes held by older people themselves. These stereotypes have been shown to have a negative impact on cognition, mobility and activities of daily living. If these are addressed then the quality of life for older people will increase and future generations of older people will not be limited by the stereotypes imposed on them through a lifetime of being subject to implicit reinforcement through norms and media portrayals. 192 References Allaire, J. & Marsiske, M. (2002). Well- and ill-defined measures of everyday cognition: Relationship to older adults intellectual ability and functional status. Psychology & Ageing. 17, 101-115 Allport, G. (1954). The Nature of Prejudice. Reading, Mass. Addison-Wesley Allport, G. (1958). The Nature of Prejudice. Garden City, NY: Doubleday Anderson, N. (1971). Integration theory and attitude change. Psychological Review, 78, 171-206 Angus, J. & Reeve, P. (2006). Ageism: A threat to “aging well” in the 21st century. Journal of Applied Gerontology. 25, 137-152. DOI:10.1177/0733464805285745 Arking, R. (2006). The biology of aging: Observations and principles. Oxford: Oxford University Press Austad, S. (1997). Why We Age: What Science Is Discovering about the Body's Journey through Life. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Auty, S. & Lewis, C. (2004). Exploring children's choice: The reminder effect of product placement. Psychology and Marketing. 21 (9), 697-713 193 Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 5-28 Baltes, P. & Baltes, M. (1990). Psychological perspectives on successful aging: The model of selective optimization with compensation. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioural sciences (pp. 1–34). New York: Cambridge University Press. Banaji, M. (1999). Unconscious isms: Examples from racism, sexism and ageism. Paper presented at the Way Women Learn Conference, New Haven, CT. Banaji, M. & Hardin, C. (1996). Automatic Stereotyping. Psychological Science. 7 (3), 136-141 Banse, R., Seise, J. & Zerbes, N. (2001). Implicit attitudes towards homosexuality: Reliability, validity and controllability of the IAT. Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 145-160 Bargh, J. (1997). The Automacity of Everyday Life. Mahwah, New Jersey. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers Bargh, J., Chen, M. & Burrows, L. (1996). Automacity of human behaviour: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on behaviour. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 71 (2), 230-244 194 Baron, A. & Banaji, M. (2006). The development of implicit attitudes: Evidence of race evaluations from ages 6 to 10 and adulthood. Psychological Science, 17 (1), 5358 Barrett, L., Tugade, M., & Engle, R. (2004). Individual differences in working memory capacity and dual process theories of mind. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 553-573 Bartke, A. (2003). Can growth hormone (GH) accelerate aging? Evidence from GHtransgenic mice. Neuroendocrinology, 78 (4), 210-216. Bartsch, R. & Judd, C. (1993). Majority-minority status and perceived in-group variability revisited. European Journal of Social Psychology. 23, 471-483 Beal, D., Ruscher, J. & Schnake, S. (2001). No benefit of the doubt: intergroup bias in understanding causal explanation. The British Journal of Social Psychology. 40 (4), 531-544 Betsch, T., Kaufmann, M., Lindow, F., Plessner, H. & Hoffman, K. (2006). Different principles of information integration in implicit and explicit attitude formation. European Journal of Psychology, 36, 887-905 Betsch, T., Plessner, H. & Schallies, E. (2004). The value-account model of attitude formation. In G.R. Maio & G. Haddock (Eds), Contemporary perspectives on the psychology of attitudes (pp. 251-273). Hove: Psychology Press 195 Bigler, R. & Patterson, M. (2007). When and why social categorization produces inequality (and vice versa). Human Development. 50 (6), 328-333 Birren, J., Butler, R., Greenhouse, S., Solokoff, L. & Yarrow, M. (1963). Human Ageing. Washington Public Health Service Publication No. 986 Birren, J., Woods, A., & Williams, M. (1980). Behavioural slowing with age: Causes, organisation and consequences. In L.W. Poon (ed.). Ageing in the 1980’s. Psychological Issues. Washington DC. American Psychological Association Blanchard, P., Weigel, R. & Cook, S. (1975). The effects of relative competence of group members upon interpersonal attraction in cooperating interracial groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 32, 519-530 Bohner, G., Siebler, F., Gonzalez, R., Haye, A. & Schmidt, E. (2008). Situational flexibility of in-group-related attitudes: A single category IAT study of people with dual national identity. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 11 (3), 301-317 Bosson, J., Swann, W. & Pennebaker, J. (2000). Stalking the perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind man and the elephant re-visited. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 79, 637-643 Botwinick, J. (1977). Intellectual abilities. In J.E. Birren & K.W. Schaie (eds), Handbook of the Psychology of Ageing. New York. Van Nostrand Reinhold 196 Botwinick, J. & Storandt, M. (1974). Vocabulary ability in later life. Journal of Genetic Psychology. 125, 303-308 Brandtstadter, J. & Greve, W. (1994). The ageing self: stabilizing and protective processes. Developmental Review. 14, 52-80 Brattburg, G., Parker, M. & Thorslund, M. (1996). The prevalence of pain amongst older adults in Sweden. Pain. 67, 29-34 Bremner, J. & Narayan, M. (1998). The effects of stress on memory and the hippocampus throughout the life cycle: Implications for childhood development and ageing. Development & Psychopathology. 10, 871-885 Brewer, M., Ho, K., Lee, J. & Miller, N. (1987). Social identity and social distance among Hong Kong schoolchildren. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 13, 156-165 Bromley, D. (1988). Human Ageing. An Introduction to Gerontology. 3rd Edition. Harmondsworth. Penguin Brown, R. (2001). Prejudice: Its Social Psychology. Oxford. Blackwell Publishers Brown, R. & Turner, J. (1979). The criss-cross categorization effect in intergroup discrimination. British Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology. 18, 371-383 197 Bruner, J. (1957). On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review. 64, 123-151 Butler, R. (1969). Age-ism: Another form of bigotry. Gerontologist, 9, 243-246 Butler, R. (1987). Ageism. In Maddox, G. (Eds). The Encyclopaedia of Ageing. New York: Springer Cai, H., Sriram, N. & Greenwald, A. (2004). The implicit association tests D measure can minimise a cognitive skill confound: Comment on McFarland and Crouch (2002). Social Cognition. 22 (6), 673-684 Catterall, M. and Maclaran, P. (2001) "Body Talk: Questioning the Assumptions in Cognitive Age. Psychology and Marketing. 18 (10), 1117-1133. Cheong, S., Wong, T., & Koh, G. (2009). Attitudes towards the elderly among Singapore medical students. ANNALS Academy of Medicine Singapore. 38 (10) 857861 Christensen, H., Mackinnon, A., Korten, A., Jorm, A., Henderson, A., Jacomb, P. & Rogers, B. (1999). An analysis f diversity in the cognitive performance of elderly community dwellers. Individual differences in change scores as a function of age. Psychology of Ageing. 14, 365-379 198 Coleman, P. & O’Hanlon, A. (2008). In Clare, L. & Woods, R. (Eds) Handbook of the Clinical Psychology of Ageing. (pp.17-32). London. Wiley Collins, M. & Abeles, N. (1996). Subjective Memory complaints and depression in the able elderly. Clinical Gerontologist. 37, 355-364 Comfort, A. (1964). Ageing: The Biology of Senescence. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Cong, Y., Wright, W. & Shay, J. (2002). Human telomerase and its regulation. Microbiology & Molecular Biology Review. 66, 407-425 Cook, S. (1962). The systematic analysis of socially significant events. Journal of Social Issues, 18, 66-84 Cook, S. (1978). Interpersonal and attitude outcomes in cooperating interracial groups. Journal of Research and Development in Education. 12, 97-113 Coccaro, E. & Miles, A. (1984). The attitudinal impact of training in gerontology & geriatrics in medical school. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 32, 762–768. Corso, J. (1981). Ageing Sensory Systems and Perception. New York. Praeger Cummings, S., Kropf, N.& DeWeaver, K.. Knowledge of and attitudes toward aging among non-elders: Gender and race differences (2000). Faculty Publications. Paper 8. 199 http://digitalarchive.gsu.edu/ssw_facpub/8 Darley, J. & Gross, P. (1983). A hypothesis-confirming bias in labelling effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 20-33 Dasgupta, N., McGhee, D., Greenwald, A. & Banaji, M. (2000). Automatic preference for Whit Americans: Ruling out the familiarity effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 316-328 Dasgupta, N. & Rivera, L. (2006). From automatic antigay prejudice to behaviour: The moderating role of conscious beliefs about gender and behavioural control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 268-280 de Magalhaes, J. (2003). Is mammalian aging genetically controlled? Biogerontology, 4(2), 119-120. Deary, I., Whalley, L., Lemmon, H., Crawford, J. & Starr, J. (2000). The stability of individual differences in mental ability from childhood to old age: Follow-up from the 1932 Scottish Mental Survey. Intelligence. 28, 49-55 De Houwer, J. (2002). The Implicit Association Test as a tool for studying dysfunctional associations in psychopathology: strengths and limitations. Journal of Behaviour Therapy 33, 115-133 200 De Houwer, J. & De Bruker, E. (2007). The implicit association test outperforms the extrinsic affective Simon task as an implicit measure of inter-individual differences in attitudes. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 401-421 De Houwer, J. & Eelen, P. (1998). An affective variant of the Simon paradigm. Cognition & Emotion, 12, 45-61 De Houwer, J. & Moors, A. (2007). How to define and examine the implicitness of implicit measures. In B. Wittenbrink & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit measures of attitudes: Procedures and controversies (pp.179-194). New York: Guilford Press De Jong, J., Van den Hout, M. & Rietbrork, H. (2000). Dissociations between implicit and explicit affect in response to phobic stimuli, submitted for publication. DePaola, S. J., Griffin, M., Young, J. R., & Neimeyer, R. A. (2003). Death anxiety and attitudes toward the elderly among older adults: The role of gender and ethnicity. Death Studies, 27, 335–354. DePaulo, B. (1992). Non-verbal behaviour and self-presentation. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 203-243 Devine, P. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 56, 5-18 201 Devine, P. & Monteith, M. (1999). Automaticity and control in stereotyping. Dualprocess theories in social psychology. In Chaiken, Shelly (Ed); Trope, Yaacov (Ed). (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. (pp. 339-360). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press Devine, P. & Sherman, S. (1992). Intuitive versus rational judgement and the role of stereotyping in the human condition: Kirk or Spock? Psychological Inquiry. 3, 153159 Dewey, J. (1939). Introduction. In E.V. Cowdrey, Problems Of Ageing. Baltimore, MD. Williams and Wilkins DFES (2008). Five year strategy. Department For Education and Skills [online]. Available: http://www.dfes.gov.uk/publications/5yearstrategy/chap8.shtml Diehl, M. (1990). The minimal group paradigm: Theoretical explanations and empirical findings. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (eds), European Review of Social Psychology 1, Chichester. John Wiley Dijker, A. & Koomen, W. (1996). Stereotyping and attitudinal effects under time pressure. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 61-74 Doise, W. (1976). Groups and Individuals: Explanations in Social Psychology. Cambridge. Cambridge University Press (1978) 202 Dovidio, J. & Fazio, R. (1992). New technologies for the direct and indirect assessment of attitudes. In J. Tanur (Ed.), Questions about questions: Meaning, memory, expression and social interaction in surveys (pp. 204-237). New York: Sage. Duerson, M., Thomas, J., Chang, J. & Stevens, B. (1992). Medical Students’ knowledge and misconceptions about ageing: Responses to Palmore’s Facts on Ageing Quizzes. The Gerontologist. 32 (2), 171-174 Dutilh, G., Vandekerckhove, J., Tuerlinckx, F. & Wagenmakers, E. (2009). A diffusion model decomposition of the practice effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 16, 1026-1036 Eagly, A. & Diekman, A. (2005). What is the Problem? Prejudice as an Attitude-incontext. On the Nature of Prejudice (pp. 19-35). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing Eiser, J. (1971). Enhancement of contrast in absolute judgement of attitude statements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 17, 1-10 Egloff, B. & Schmukle, S. (2002). Predictive validity of an Implicit Association Test for assessing anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1441-1455 Emery, C., Pedersen, N., Svartengren, M. & McClearn, G. (1998). Longitudinal and genetic effects in the relationship between pulmonary function and cognitive performance. Journal of Gerontology B: Psychological Sciences & Social Sciences. 53, 311-317 203 Evans, J. (2008). Dual-Processing accounts of reasoning, judgement and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255-278 Fazio, R., Jackson, J., Dunton, B., & Williams, C. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 69 (6), 1013-1027 Fazio, R. & Olsen, M. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297-327 Festinger, L. (1957). A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Evanston, Ill. Row Peterson Filipp, S. & Schmitt, K. (1995). Mittleres und hoheres Erwachsenenalter (pp. 439486) in Entwicklungspsychologie, (Ed.) L. Montada. Weinheim: Psychologie Verlags Union Fillmore, M., Vogel-Sprott, M. & Gavrilescu, D. (1999). Alcohol effects on intentional behaviour: Dissociating control and automatic influences. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 7, 372-378 Finch, C. E. (1990). Longevity, Senescence, and the Genome. Chicago and London: The University of Chicago Press. 204 Fishbein, M. & Ajzen, I. (1974). Attitudes towards objects as predictors of single and multiple behavioural criteria. Psychological Review. 81, 59-74 Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Foroni, F. & Mayr, U. (2005). The power of a story: New, automatic associations from a single reading of a short scenario. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 12 (1), 139-144 Fraboni, M., Saltstone, R., & Hughes, S. (1990). The Fraboni Scale of Ageism (FSA): An attempt at a more precise measure of ageism. Canadian Journal on Aging. 9, 56– 66. Fries, J. (2000). Compression of morbidity in the elderly. Vaccine. 18, 1584-1589 Friese, M., Hofmann, W. & Schmitt, M. (2009). When and why do implicit measures predict behaviour? Empirical evidence for the moderating role of opportunity, motivation and process reliance. European Review of Social Psychology. 19 (1), 285338 Friese, M., Hofmann, W. & Wanke, M. (2008). When impulses take over: Moderated predictive validity of explicit and implicit attitude measures in predicting food choice and consumption behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology. 47, 397-419 205 Friese, M., Wanke, M. & Plessner, H. (2006). Implicit consumer preferences and their influence on product choice. Psychology and Marketing. 23, 727-740 Gaertner, S. & Dovidio, J. (1986). The aversive form of racism. In J.F. Dovidio and S. L. Gaertner (eds), Prejudice, Discrimination and Racism. New York. Academic Press Gatz, M. & Pearson, C. (1988). Ageism revised and the provision of psychological services. American Psychologist. 11, 184-188 Gawronski, B., Lebel, E. & Peters, K. (2007). What do implicit measures tell us? Scrutinising the validity of three common assumptions. Perspective on Psychological Science. 2 (2) 181-193 Gething, L., Fethney, J., McKee, K., Persson, L., Goff, M., Churchward, M., Matthews, S., Halversson M. & Johannsson, I. (2004). Validation of the reactions to ageing questionnaire: Assessing similarities across several countries. Journal of Gerontological Nursing. 30 (9) Giancola, P. (2000). Executive functioning: A conceptual framework for alcohol related aggression. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 8, 576-597 Glick, P., Zion, C. & Nelson, C. (1988).What mediated sex discrimination in hiring decisions? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 55, 178-186 206 Erving Goffman, E. (1959). The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life. New York: Doubleday Gonzales, E., Morrow-Howell, N. & Gilbert, P. (2010). Changing medical students’ attitudes towards older adults. Gerontology & Geriatrics Education. 31 (3), 220-234 Gosden, R. (1996). Cheating Time. New York: W. H. Freeman & Company. Grant, L. (1996). Effects of ageism on individual and health care providers’ responses to healthy ageing. Health & Social Work. 21 (1), 9-17 Kim, D. (2003). Voluntary controllability of the implicit association test (IAT). Social Psychology Quarterly. 66 (1), 83-97 Greenwald, A. & Banaji, M. (1995). Implicit Social Cognition: Attitudes, Self Esteem and Stereotypes. Psychological Review. 102 (1), 4-27 Greenwald, A., Banaji, M., Rudman, L., Farnham, S, Nosek, B. & Mellot, D. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self concept. Psychological Review. 109, 3-25 Greenwald, A., McGhee, D. & Schwartz, J. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 74 (6), 1464-1480 207 Greenwald, A. & Nosek, B. (2001). Health of the implicit association test as age 3. Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle Psychologie. 48, 85-93 Greenwald, A., Nosek, B. & Banaji, M. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: An improved Scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 85, 197-216 Greenwald, A, Poehlman, T., Uhlmann, E. & Banaji, M. (2009). Understanding and using the implicit association test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 97 (1), 17-41 Hamilton, D. & Bishop, G. (1976). Attitudinal and behavioural effects of initial integration of white suburban neighbourhoods. Journal of Social Issues. 32, 47-67 Hamilton, W. (1966). The moulding of senescence by natural selection. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 12 (1), 12-45. Hammerman, M. (1987). Insulin-like growth factors and aging. Endocrinol Metab Clin North Am. 16 (4), 995-1011 Hayflick, L. (1994). How and Why We Age. New York: Ballantine Books. Hayflick, L. (1997). Mortality and immortality at the cellular level. A review. Biochemestry. 62, 1180-1190 208 Herbst, K. (1982). Social attitudes to hearing loss in the elderly. In F.Glendenning (ed). Acquired Hearing Loss and Elderly People. Keele. Beth Johnson Foundation Publications Hewstone, M., Islam, M. & Judd, C. (1993). Models of crossed categorisation and intergroup relations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 64, 779-793 Higgins, I., Van der Riet, P., Slater, L & Peek, C. (2007). The negative attitudes of nurses towards older patients in the acute hospital setting: A qualitative descriptive study. Contemporary Nurse. 26 (2) 225-237 Hofmann, W. & Friese, M. (2008). Impulses got the better of me: Alcohol moderates the influence of implicit attitudes towards food cues on eating behaviour. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 117, 420-427 Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H. & Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the implicit association test and explicit self report measures. Personality and Social Psychology. 31 (10) 1369-1385 Hoffman, W., Nosek, B. & Schmitt, M. (2005). What moderates implicit-explicit consistency?. European Review of Social Psychology. 16, 335-390 Hogan, D. & Mallott, M. (2005). Changing racial prejudice through diversity education. Journal of College Student Development. 46 (2), 115-125 209 Horn, J. & Catell, R. (1967). Age differences in fluid and crystallised intelligence. Acta Psychologia. 26, 107-129 Hospital Episode Statistics (2010). Accident and emergency attendance in England (experimental statistics) 2008-2009. NHS Information Centre for Health and Social Care. P.12 http://www.hesonline.nhs.uk/Ease/servlet/ContentServer?siteID=1937&categoryID=1 271 Hull, J. & Slone, L. (2004). Alcohol and self regulation. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory and applications (pp. 466491). New York: Guildford Press Hummert, M., Garstka, T., Shaner, J. & Strahm, S (1994). Stereotypes of the elderly held by young, middle aged and elderly adults. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Science. 49, 240-249 Huntsinger, J., Sinclair, S., Dunn, E. & Clore, G. (2010). Affective regulation of stereotype activation: Its the (accessible) thought that counts. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 36 (4), 564 Hutchinson, P., Jetten, J., Christian, J. & Haycraft, E. (2006). Protecting threatened identity: Sticking with the group by emphasising in-group heterogeneity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 32 (12), 1620 210 Isaacs, L. & Bearison, D. (1986). The development of children's prejudice against the aged. International Journal of Ageing and Human Development. 23 (3), 175-194 James, J. & Haley, W. (1995). Age and health bias in practicing clinical psychologists. Psychology and Ageing. 10, 610-616 Jelenec, P. & Steffens, M. (2002). Implicit attitudes towards elderly women and men. Current Research in Social Psychology. 7 (16), 275-291 Johnson, F., Marciniak, R. & Guarente, L. (1998). Telomeres, the nucleolus and ageing. Current Opinions in Cell Biology. 10, 332-338 Jones, E. & Pittman, T. (1982). Toward a General Theory of Strategic Self presentation in J.M. Suls (ed.). Psychological Perspectives of the Self. pp. 231–62. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Jones, L., Sinclair, R. & Courneya, K. (2003). The Effects of Source Credibility and Message Framing on Exercise Intentions, Behaviors, and Attitudes: An Integration of the Elaboration Likelihood Model and Prospect Theory. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 33 (1), 179-196 Kalavar, J. (2001). Examining ageism: Do male and female college students differ? Educational Gerontology. 27 (6), 507-513 211 Kang Fu, V. (2007). How many melting pots? Intermarriage, pan ethnicity, and the black/non-black divide in the United States. Journal of Comparative Family Studies. 38 (2), 215-241 Karpinski, A., & Steinman, R. (2006). The single category implicit association test (SC-IAT) as a measure of implicit social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 91, 16-32 Kermis, M. (1983). The Psychology of Human Ageing: Theory, Research and Practice. Boston, MA. Allyn and Bacon Kim, D. (2003). Voluntary controllability of the implicit association test (IAT). Social Psychology Quarterly. 66,1, 83-97 King, M., Winter, S., & Webster, B. (2009). Contact reduces transprejudice: A study on attitudes towards transgenderism and transgender civil rights in Hong Kong. International Journal of Sexual Health. 21 (1), 17 Kirkwood, T. (1977). Evolution of ageing. Nature. 270 (5635), 301-304. Kirkwood, T. & Austad, S.(2000). Why do we age? Nature. 408 (6809), 233-238. Kite, M., Deaux, K. & Miele,M. (1991). Stereotypes of old and young: does age outweigh gender? Psychology and Ageing. 6, 19-27 212 Kite, M. & Johnson, B. (1988). Attitudes toward older and younger adults: A metaanalysis. Psychology and Ageing. 3, 233-244 Kite, M., Stockdale, G., Whitley, B. & Johnson, B. (2005). Attitudes towards younger and older adults: An updated meta-analytic review. Journal of Social Issues, 61, 241266 Kite, M. & Wagner, L. (2002). Attitudes toward older and younger adults. In T. Nelson (Ed)., Ageism: Stereotyping and prejudice against older persons pp.129-161. Boston: MIT Press. Klaczynski, P. & Robinson, B. (2000). Personal theories, intellectual ability and epistemological beliefs: Adult age differences in everyday reasoning biases. Psychology & Ageing. 15, 400-416 Kline, P. (2000). Handbook of Psychological Testing. (pp.13). Routledge: London. Krosnick, J. (1988). Attitude importance and attitude change. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 24 (3), 240-255 Kvieskaite, E. (2007) Stereotypes in Civic Journalism, in Ross, A. (ed) Citizenship Education in Society. London: CiCe, pp 657-662. Lee, B., Farrell, C. & Link, B. (2004). Revising the contact hypothesis. The case of public exposure to homelessness. American Sociological Review. 69, 40-63 213 Legault, L., Green-Demers, I., & Eadie, A. (2009). When internalization leads to automatization: The role of self-determination in automatic stereotype suppression and implicit prejudice regulation. Motivation and Emotion. 33 (1), 10 Levy, B. (1996). Improving memory without awareness: Implicit self-stereotyping in old age. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 71, 1092-1107. Levy, B., Ashman, O. & Dror, I. (2000). To be or not to be: The effects of ageing stereotypes on the will to live. Omega – Journal of Death and Dying. 40 (3), 409-420 Levy, B. & Banaji, M. (2002). Implicit Ageism. In T.D. Nelson (Ed.), Ageism: Stereotyping and Prejudice against Older Persons. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press Li, S., Lindenberger, U. & Sikström, S. (2001). Aging cognition: from neuromodulation to representation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 11 (5), 479-486 Linville, P., Fischer, F. & Salovey, P. (1989). Perceived distributions of characteristics of in-group and out-group members: empirical evidence and a computer simulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 42, 193-211 Lovden, M., Li, S., Shing, Y. & Lindenberger, U. (2007). Within-person trial-to-trial variability precedes and predicts cognitive decline in old and very old age: Longitudinal data from the Berlin Ageing Study. Neuropsychologia. 45, 2827-2838 214 Lynch, S. M. (2000). Measurement and prediction of aging anxiety. Research on Aging. 22(5), 553-558. Macrae, C., Milne, A. & Bodenhausen, G. (1994). Stereotypes as energy-saving devices: A peek inside the cognitive toolbox. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 66, 37-47 Maio, G., Esses, V. & Bell, D. (2000). Examining conflict between components of attitude: Ambivalence and inconsistency are distinct constructs. Canadian Journal of Social Science. 32 (1), 58-70 Maison, D., Greenwald, A. & Bruin, R. (2001). The implicit association test as a measure of implicit consumer attitudes. Polish Psychological Bulletin. 32 (1) 61-69 McCann, R. & Giles, H. (2002). Ageism in the workplace. A communication perspective. In T. D. Nelson (ed.), Ageism: Stereotyping and prejudice against older persons (pp. 163-199). Cambridge, Ma. MIT Press McConahay, J. (1986). Modern racism, ambivalence and the modern racism scale. In J.F. Dovidio and S. L. Gaertner (eds), Prejudice, Discrimination and Racism. New York. Academic Press McDonald, G. & Nail, P. (2005). Attitude change and the public-private attitude distinction. British Journal of Social Psychology. 44, 15-28 215 McFarland, S. & Crouch, Z. (2002). A cognitive skill confound on the implicit association test. Social Cognition. 20 (6), 483-510 McGarty, C. & Penny, R. (1988). Categorisation, accentuation and social judgement. British Journal of Social Psychology. 27, 147-157 Mendoza, S., Gollwitzer, P., & Amodio, D. (2010). Reducing the expression of implicit stereotypes: Reflexive control through implementation intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 36 (4), 512 Mitchell, A. & Olsen, J. (1981). Are product attribute beliefs the only mediator of advertising effects on brand attitude? Journal of Marketing Research. 18, 318-332 Monteith, M. & Mark, A. (2005). Changing one’s prejudices ways: Awareness, affect, and self-regulation. European Review of Social Psychology. 16, 113-154 Monteith, M., Voils, C. & Ashburn-Nardo, L. (2001). Taking a look underground: Detecting, interpreting, and reacting to racial biases. Social Cognition. 19 (4), 395418 Moscovitch, M. (1982). A neuropsychological approach to memory and perception in normal and pathological aging. In F.I.M. Craik & S. Trehub (eds), Ageing and Cognitive Processes. New York, Plenum 216 Mueller-Johnson, K., Toglia, M., Sweeney, C. & Ceci, S. (2007). The perceived credibility of older adults as witnesses and its relation to ageism. Behavioural Sciences & The Law. 25 (3), 355-375 Murphy, D., Craik, F., Li, K. & Schneider, B. (2000). Comparing the effects of ageing and background noise of short-term memory performance. Psychology and Ageing. 15, 323-334 Murphy, S., Monahan, J., & Zajonc, R. (1995). Additivity of nonconscious affect: Combined effects of priming and exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 69, 589–602 Nemmers, T. (2005). The influence of ageism and ageist stereotypes on the elderly. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Geriatrics. 22 (4), 11-20 Nosek, B., & Banaji, M. (2001). The go/no-go association task. Social Cognition. 19, 625-666 Nosek, B., Greenwald, A. & Banaji, M. (2007) The Implicit Association Test at Age 7: A Methodological and Conceptual Review, in Automatic Processes in Social Thinking and Behavior, John A. Bargh, Ed.: Psychology Press. Nowicki, E. (2008). The interaction of attitudes toward racial membership and learning ability in school-age children. Educational Psychology. 28 (3), 229 217 OECD (1988). Ageing Populations: The Social Policy Implications. Paris. OECD Office for National Statistics (2009). Mid-year population estimates, Office for National Statistics, General Register Office for Scotland, Northern Ireland Statistics and Research Agency. Newport. ONS Olovnikov, A. (1996). Telomeres, telomerase and ageing: Origin of the theory. Experimental Gerontology. 31, 443-448 Olsen, M. & Fazio, R. (2003). Relations between implicit measures of prejudice: What are we measuring? Psychological Science. 14 (6), 636-639 Palmore, E. (1977). Facts on Ageing: A short quiz. Gerontologist. 17, 315-320 Palmore, E. (1999). Ageism: Negative and positive. New York. Springer Paolini, S., Hewstone, M., Cairns, E. & Voci, A. (2004). Effects of direct and indirect cross-group friendships on judgements of Catholics and Protestants in Northern Ireland: The mediating role of an anxiety-reduction mechanism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 30 (6), 770 Perdue, C. & Gurtman, M. (1990). Evidence for the automacity of ageism. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 26, 199-216 218 Perugini, M. (2005). Predictive models of implicit and explicit attitudes. The British Journal of Social Psychology. 44 (1), 29-46 Pettigrew, T. (1979). The ultimate attribution error: extending Allport’s cognitive analysis of prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 5, 461-476 Petty, R., Wegener, D. & Fabrigar, L. (1997). Attitudes and attitude change. Annual Review of Psychology. 48, 609-647 Phelps, E., O’Conner, K., Cunningham, W., Funayama, E., Gatenby, J., Gore, J. & Banaji, M. (2000). Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 12 (5), 729-738 Pickett, C., Silver, M. & Brewer, M. (2002). The impact of assimilation and differentiation needs on perceived group importance and judgements of in-group size. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 28 (4), 546 Preston, J. & Feinstein, L. (2004). Adult education and attitude change. Centre for Research on the Wider Benefits of Learning [online]. Available: http://www.learningbenefits.net/Publications/ResReps/ResRep11.pdf Prestwich, A., Kenworthy, J., Wilson, M. & Kwan-Tat, N. (2008). Differential relations between two types of contact and implicit and explicit racial attitudes. British Journal of Social Psychology. 47, 575-588 219 Quillen, D. (1999). Common causes of vision loss in elderly patients. American Family Physician. 60, 99-108 Rabbitt, P. (1984). Memory impairment in the elderly. In P.E. Bebbington & R. Jacoby (eds). Psychiatric Disorders in the Elderly. London. Mental Health Foundation, 101-119 Ratcliffe, R., Spieler, D, & McKoon, G. (2000). Explicitly modelling the effects of ageing on response time. Psychnomic Bulletin and Review. 7, 1-25 Reber, A. (1995). Dictionary of Psychology. London: Penguin Reference Rebok, G. (1987). Life-Span Cognitive Development. New York. Holt, Rinehart & Winston Richetin, J. & Perugini, M. (2008). When temporal continuity matters: A moderator of the predictive validity of implicit measures. European Journal of Psychological Assessment. 24 (4), 246-253 Rodin, J. (1986). Ageing and health: effects of the sense of control. Science. 233 (4770), 1271-1278 Rodin, J. & Langer, E. (1980). Aging Labels: The Decline of Control and the Fall of Self-Esteem. Journal of Social Issues. 36 (2), 12-29 220 Rose, M. (1991). Evolutionary Biology of Aging. New York: Oxford University Press. Rosenzweig, M. (1996). Aspects of the search for neural mechanisms of memory. Annual Review of Psychology. 47,1-32 Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: distortions in the attribution process. In L. Berkowitz (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 10. New York Academic Press Rowe, J. & Kahn, R. (1987). Human Ageing: Usual and Successful. Science. 237 (4811), 143-149 Rudman, L., Greenwald, A., Mellot, D. & Schwartz, J. (1999). Measuring the automatic components of prejudice: Flexibility and generality of the Implicit Association Test. Social Cognition. 17 (4), 437-466 Ruys, K. & Stapel, D. (2009). Learning to like or dislike by association: No need for contingency awareness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 45 (6), 12771280 Rupp, D., Vodanovich, S., & Credé, M. (2005). The Multidimensional Nature of Ageism: Construct Validity and Group Differences. The Journal of Social Psychology, 145 (3), 335–362 221 Sarnoff, I. & Katz, D. (1954). The motivational bases of attitude change. The Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology. 49 (1), 115-124 Schaie, K. (1983). The Seattle Longitudinal Study: A 21 year exploration of psychometric intelligence in adulthood. K.W. Schaie (ed.). Longitudinal Studies of Adult Psychological Development. New York. Guilford Press, pp. 64-135 Schaie, K. (1994). The course of adult intellectual development. American Psychologist. 49, 304-313 Schmukle, S. & Egloff, B. (2004). Does the Implicit Association Test for assessing anxiety measure trait and state variance? European Journal of Personality. 18, 483494 Schnabel, K., Banse, R. & Asendorpf, J. (2006). Assessment of implicit personality self-concept using the implicit association test (IAT): Concurrent assessment of anxiousness and angriness. British Journal of Social Psychology. 45, 373-396 Sherif, M., Harvey, O., White, B., Hood, W. & Shrif, C. (1954). Experimental study of positive and negative intergroup attitudes between experimentally produced groups: Robbers cave experiment. Norman, Ok. University of Oklahoma Press. Shultz, W., Shriver, C., Tabinico, J. & Khazian, A. (2004). Implicit connections with nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology. 24, 31-42 222 Sonnenschein, E. & Brody, J (2005). Effect of population ageing n proportionate mortality from heart disease and cancer, U.S. 2000-2050. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 60, 110-112 Stangor, C. & Ford, T. (1992). Accuracy and expectancy-confirming processing orientations and the development of stereotypes and prejudice, pp.57-89 in W. Stroebe and M. Hewstone (eds). European Review of Social Psychology 3, Chichester. John Wiley Steffens, M. & Schulze Konig, S. (2006). Predicting spontaneous big five behaviour with Implicit Association Tests. European Journal of Psychological Assessment. 22 (1) Steinman, R., & Karpinski, A. (2008). The single category implicit association test (SC-IAT) as a measure of implicit consumer attitudes. European Journal of Social Sciences. 7 (1), 32-42 Stephan, W. & Stephan, C. (1984). The role of ignorance in intergroup relations. In N. Miller and M.B. Brewer (eds). Groups in Contact: The Psychology of Desegregation. New York. Academic Press Storandt, M. (1977). Age, ability level and scoring the WAIS. Journal of Gerontology. 32, 175-178 223 Strehler, B. (1999). Time, Cells, and Aging. Larnaca: Demetriades Brothers. Stroessner, S., Hamilton, D. & Mackie, D. (1992). Affect and stereotyping: the effect of induced mood on distinctiveness-based illusory correlations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 62, 564-576 Stuart-Hamilton, I. (2006). The Psychology of Ageing. An Introduction (4th Ed.). London. JKP Stuart-Hamilton, I. & Mahoney, B. (2003). The effect of ageing awareness training on knowledge of, and attitudes towards, older adults. Educational Gerontology. 29 (3), 251-260 Sturmer, S., Snyder, M., Kropp, A. & Siem, B. (2006). Empathy-motivated helping: The moderating role of group membership. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 32 (7), 943 Tajfel, H. (1959). The anchoring effects of value in scale of judgements. British Journal of Psychology. 50, 294-304 Tajfel, H., Flament, C., Billig, M. & Bundy, R. (1971). Social categorisation and intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology. 1, 149-178 Tam, T., Hewstone, M., Harwodd, J., Voci, A. & Kenworthy, J. (2006). Intergroup contact and grandparent-grandchild communication: The effects of self-disclosure on 224 implicit and explicit biases against older people. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 9 (3), 413-429 Taylor, H. (2005). Assessing the Nursing and Care Needs of Older Adults. Oxford: Radcliffe Publishing Teachman, B., Gregg, A. & Woody, S. (2001). Implicit associations for fear-relevant stimuli among individuals with snake & spider fears. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 110 (2), 226-235 Teachman, B., Gapinski, K., Brownell, K., Rawlins, M. & Jeyaram, S. (2003). Demonstrations of anti-fat bias: The impact of providing causal information and evoking empathy. Health Psychology. 22 (1), 68-78 Teige, S., Schnabel, K., Banse, R. & Asendorpf, J (2004). Assessment of multiple implicit self-concept dimensions using the Extrinsic Affective Simon Task (EAST). Unpublished Manuscript Thompson, D. (1997). Contributions to the history of psychology: CVIII. On ageing and intelligence: History teaches a different lesson. Perception & Motor Skills. 85, 28-30 Tuckman, J. & Lorge, I. (1953). Attitudes towards old people. The Journal of Social Psychology. 37, 249-260 225 Turner, J. (1983). Some comments on ‘The measurement of social orientations in the minimal group paradigm’. European Journal of Social Psychology. 13, 351-367 Turner, J., Brown, R. & Tajfel, H. (2006). Social comparison and group interest in ingroup favouritism. European Journal of Social Psychology. 9 (2), 187-204 Turner, J., Hogg, M., Oaks, P., Reicher, S. & Wetherell, M. (1987). Rediscovering the Social Group: A Self-Categorisation Theory. Oxford. Basil Blackwell Turner, R., & Crisp, R. (2010). Imagining intergroup contact reduced implicit prejudice. British Journal of Social Psychology. 49 (1), 129-142 Vanbeselaere, N. (1991). The different effects of simple and crossed categorisations: a result of the category differentiation process or differential category silence, pp. 247278 in W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (eds) European Review of Social Psychology 2, Chichester. John Wiley Walsh, D. (1982). The development of visual information processes in adulthood and old age. In F.I.M. Craik & S. Trehub (eds), Ageing and Cognitive Processes. New York. Plenum Wang, J., Michelitsch, T., Wunderlin, A. & Mahadeva1, R. (2009). Aging as a consequence of misrepair: A novel theory of ageing. Nature Preceedings: hdl:10101/npre.2009.2988.3 : Posted 15 Jun 2009 226 West, R., Murphy, K., Armilio, M., Craik, F. & Stuss, D. (2002). Effects of time of day on age differences in working memory. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences & Social Sciences. 57, 3-10 Westmoreland, G., Counsell, S., Sennour, Y., Schubert, C., Frank, K., Wu, J., Frankel, R., Litzelman, D., Bogdeic, S. & Inui, T. (2009). Improving medical student’s attitudes towards older patients through a council of elders and reflective writing experience. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 57, 315-320 Whitborne, S. & Hulicka, I. (1990). Ageism in undergraduate psychology texts. American Psychologist. 11, 1127-136 Wilkins, R. & Adams, O. (1983). Health expectancy in Canada, late 1970’s: demographic, regional and social dimensions. American Journal of Public Health. 73, 1073-80 Wilson, T., Lindey, S. & Schooler, T. (2000). A Model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review. 107 (1), 101-126 Woodward, K.M. (1991). Aging and its Discontents: Freud and Other Fictions. Bloomington, Indiana: Indiana University Press World Health Organisation (2004). World Health Report 2000. Geneva. World Health Organisation 227 Zagefka, H., Brown, R., Broquard, M. & Martin, S. (2007). Predictors and consequences of negative attitudes towards immigrants in Belgium & Turkey: The role of acculturation preferences and economic competition. British Journal of Social Psychology. 46, 153-169 Zajonc, R. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 9, 1-27 Zebrowitz, L. & Montpare, J. (2000). Too young, too old: Stigmatizing adolescents and elders. In Heatherton, T., Kleck, R., Hebl, M. & Hull, J. (Eds.). The Psychology of Stigma. (pp.334-373). London: Guildford Press. 228 Appendices Page Appendix One: Fraboni Scale of Ageism …………………………………….. 230 Appendix Two: Student nurse participation certificate …………………….. 234 Appendix Three: Psychology student debrief form …………………………. 235 Appendix Four: IAT D-Scoring algorithm …………………………………... 236 229 Appendix One: Fraboni Scale of Ageism Part Two Please can you complete the below questionnaire by clearly circling the response to each question that is most appropriate for you This questionnaire must be completed in silence and without conferring 1. Many old people are stingy and hoard their money and possessions 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 2. Many old people are not interested in making new friends, preferring instead the circle of friends they have had for years 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 3. Many old people just live in the past 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 4. Most old people should not be trusted to take care of infants 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 5. Many old people are the happiest when they are with people their own age 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 6. Most old people would be considered to have poor personal hygiene 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 230 4 Strongly Agree 7. Most old people can be annoying because they tell the same stories over and over again 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 8. Old people complain more than other people do 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 9. I would prefer not to go to an open house at a seniors club if invited 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 10. Teenage suicide is more tragic than suicide amongst the elderly 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 11. I sometimes avoid eye contact with old people when I see them 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 12. I don’t like it when old people try and make conversation with me 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 13. Complex and interesting conversation cannot be expected from most old people 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 14. Feeling depressed when around old people is probably a common feeling 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 231 4 Strongly Agree 15. Old people should find friends their own age 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 16. Old people should feel welcome at the social gatherings of young people 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 17. Old people don’t really need to use our community sports facilities 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 18. It is best that old people live where they wont bother anyone 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 19. The company of most old people is quite enjoyable 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 20. It is sad to hear about the plight of the old in our society these days 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 21. Old people should be encouraged to speak out politically 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 22. Most old people are interesting individualistic people 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 232 4 Strongly Agree 23. I personally would not want to spend much time with an old person 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 24. There should be special clubs set aside within sports facilities so that old people can compete at their own level 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 25. Old people deserve the same rights and freedoms as do other members of our society 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 26. Most old people should not be allowed to renew their drivers licence 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 27. Old people can be very creative 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 28. I would prefer not to live with an old person 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree 29. Old people do not need much money to meet their friends 1 Strongly Disagree 2 Disagree 3 Agree 4 Strongly Agree Thank you for completing the questionnaire. Please indicate to the researcher that you have completed the questionnaire so that this can be collected 233 Appendix Two: Student nurse participation certificate Faculty of Humanities and Social Science Cyfadran Dyniaethau a Gwyddorau Cymdetihasol Certification of Participation This certificate does show that ………………………………. has taken part in a social psychological study for the Humanities and Social Science Department at the University of Glamorgan. The study involved multi-modal testing to gauge a measure of both implicit and explicit ageism attitudes. The participant named above has given 30 minutes of their time and successfully completed both sections of the test. Researcher: Paul Nash Supervisor: Signed: Prof. Ian Stuart-Hamilton 234 Appendix Three: Psychology student debrief form Faculty of Humanities and Social Science Cyfadran Dyniaethau a Gwyddorau Cymdetihasol Research Participation Sheet Introduction/Background: Attitudes are formed from exposure to attitude / target objects. This forms one of two types of attitude, either explicit or implicit. Explicit attitudes are those which are openly expressed and are good predictors of planned behaviour. Implicit attitudes are internalised attitudes which give a good prediction of spontaneous behaviour. Current research is interested in measuring and comparing both attitudes and assessing the effectiveness of attitude change measures Methodology: A paper based explicit attitudinal measure has been used (Fraboni Ageism Scale) to be compared with the implicit measure. The implicit measure used is a computer based Implicit Association Test (IAT) which has been created especially for this study. The IAT measures response time latencies between concordant and non-concordant category pairings of words and pictures. Predictions: There will be a significant difference between the scores on the implicit and explicit measures of ageism. References: Kim, D. (2003). Voluntary controllability of the implicit association test (IAT). Social Psychology Quarterly, 66 (1) pp.83-97 Monteith, M., Voils, C. & Ashburn-Nardo, L. (2001). Taking a look underground: Detecting, interpreting, and reacting to racial biases. Social Cognition, 19 (4) pp.395-418 Perugini, M. (2005). Predictive models of implicit and explicit attitudes. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 44 (1) pp.29-46 Signed: Researcher: (Print): Supervisor: Paul Nash 235 (Print): Ian Stuart-Hamilton Appendix Four: IAT D-Scoring algorithm Group Trial Blocks Upper Tail Treatment Error Penalty Collate Score Calculate Direction Standardise Block 3 & Block 4 Block 6 & Block 7 Delete if latency exceeds 10,000 ms Replace errors: mean + 600ms Mean of blocks 3 & 4 Mean of blocks 6 & 7 Congruent blocks – Incongruent blocks Divide score by individual standard deviation (SD) 236