Can Biases About the Future Push Consumers to Act? Effects of Affective Forecasting on Consumer Procrastination Short Abstract This paper introduces affective forecasting as a means of manipulating forward-looking behavior in an attempt to mitigate procrastination. Three studies show that consumers who make affective forecasts overestimate negative emotions associated with procrastination and attempt to avoid forecasted negative states by initiating and completing tasks earlier than non-forecasters. Can Biases About the Future Push Consumers to Act? Effects of Affective Forecasting on Consumer Procrastination Extended Abstract Behavioral economics classifies procrastination as present-biased preference. This perspective implies that people are insufficiently forward-looking and that procrastination occurs as a consequence of failing to appreciate that repeated decisions to postpone today’s tasks will result in a lack of progress in the long term (Akerlof 1991). A logical extension of this perspective suggests that manipulations that encourage forward-looking behavior may de-bias present-bias preference. The proposed research introduces affective forecasting as a means of manipulating forward-looking behavior in an attempt to mitigate procrastination. Procrastination applies to a wide range of consumer contexts with important consequences, such as completing tasks on time to obtain financial rewards or avoid penalties (e.g., tax returns, financial investments, etc.), and meeting purchasing deadlines for gift-giving occasions (e.g., birthdays, Christmas, Valentine’s day, etc.). Research on procrastination suggests that people are likely to procrastinate due to factors such as task aversiveness and perceived effort required for a given task (Anderson 2003). Numerous individual difference variables can also contribute to procrastiantion (Steel 2007). Research in this domain, however, is largely silent on mediating variables that may motivate action. For example, research has yet to examine whether limitations in people’s self-knowledge (e.g., inability to accurately predict one’s future emotional states) can influence procrastination. Affective forecasting refers to predictions people make about their future emotional states. Research in this domain has consistently demonstrated an effect termed the “impact bias” whereby “forecasters” tend to overestimate the intensity and duration of their reactions to future events compared to “experiencers” who experience the same events but do not engage in affective forecasts. (Gilbert et al. 1998). Importantly, the impact bias has been shown to be stronger for negative emotions (e.g. regret) than for positive emotions (e.g. happiness) (Wilson and Gilbert 2003). While the majority of research in affective forecasting has focused on the identification of these biases, little empirical work has looked at how these biases might influence people’s actions. That is, most work has focused on the forecasts themselves and research has yet to examine whether the motivation to avoid such negative states in the future might encourage individuals to take action to change their environment. Therefore, the main motivation behind this research is to examine the behavioral consequences of affective forecasting by looking at its influences on procrastination. The current research examines the effects of affective forecasts on people’s progress in three consumer contexts. Three studies track changes in consumers’ progress, stress, and other emotional responses over time to identify underlying mechanisms that influence procrastination. We observe that consumers who make affective forecasts overestimate the negative emotions associated with procrastination. This impact bias motivates consumers to avoid forecasted negative states by initiating and completing tasks earlier than non-forecasters. Further, we find that issuing implicit and explicit warnings about the negative consequences of procrastination are largely ineffective at motivating action. Findings suggest that the underlying process that takes place during affective forecasts is unique and does more than serve a mere warning function. Experiment 1 examined the relationship between affective forecasting and consumers’ progress on holiday shopping. Longitudinal data was collected over a one-month period to assess changes in participants’ affective forecasts and progress on their holiday shopping lists. One month prior to the holiday gift exchange season, 44 undergraduates were randomly assigned to either the “forecaster” or “experiencer” condition. Following the lab session, participants were contacted periodically and asked to complete six additional online diaries. As predicted, forecasters were found to significantly overestimate the intensity of negative emotions (stress, unhappiness, and perceived difficulty) they would feel in the future. Results also showed that forecasters initiated and completed tasks earlier than experiencers and made greater progress over time. Experiment 2 examined the relationship between affective forecasting and consumers’ progress toward Valentine’s Day shopping. The complexity of the task was decreased in this study because participants only needed to shop for one individual. Additionally, a shorter twoweek time frame was examined in order to test if similar effects would be observed over a shorter period of time. In essence, we were curious to know whether the effects observed in Experiment 1 would manifest themselves in a less complex task with potentially higher consequences over a shorter period of time. The overall design and basic methodology were similar to Experiment 1. Fifty-six undergraduates in romantic relationships were randomly assigned to either the “forecasters” or “experiencers” condition. Following the lab session, participants were contacted asked to complete 3 additional online diaries. As in Experiment 1, the forecasters overestimated intensity of their future negative emotions, initiated and completed tasks earlier than experiencers, and made greater progress over time. The purpose of Experiment 3 was to examine whether affective forecasting essentially operates as a mere warning function. Specifically, we aimed to investigate whether the process that occurs when making affective forecasts differs from being warned about how one will feel in the future if they procrastinate. To test this,169 undergraduates participated in a consumerthemed scavenger hunt modeled after the TV reality show The Amazing Race. Participants were given two weeks to complete a series of five tasks which required them to take a photograph of various retail stores in the local area and upload the photographs to a website that tracked each participant’s progress over time. Participants were randomly assigned to the “forecasters”, “experiencers”, “implicit warning”, or “explicit warning” condition. The manipulations for the forecasters and experiencers conditions were similar to the first two studies. Those in the implicit warning condition were warned that they would feel negative emotions if they were to procrastinate, and were told to act accordingly. Those in the explicit warning condition received the same warning and were explicitly told that such negative emotions could be avoided by not procrastinating and starting the task right away. Forecasters again exhibited an impact bias and a higher proportion of forecasters completed the race by the deadline. Issuing implicit and explicit warnings about the negative consequences of procrastination were largely ineffective at motivating action, suggesting that the underlying process that takes place during affective forecasting does more than serve a mere warning function. References Akerlof, George (1991), “Procrastination and Obedience,” American Economic Review, 81 (2), 1-19. Anderson, Christopher J. (2003), “The Psychology of Doing Nothing: Forms of Decision Avoidance Result From Reason and Emotion,” Psychological Bulletin, 129, 139-67. Ariely, Dan and Klaus Wertenbroch (2002), “Procrastination, Deadlines, and Performance: SelfControl by Precommitment,” Psychological Science, 13 (3), 219-24. Baron, Rueben M. and David A. Kenny (1986), “The Moderator-Mediator Variable Distinction in Social Psychological Research: Conceptual, Strategic and Statistical Considerations,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51 (6), 1173-1182. Blunt, Allan K. and Timothy Pychyl (2000), “Task Aversiveness and Procrastination: A MultiDimensional Approach to Task Aversiveness Across Stages of Personal Projects,” Personality and Individual Differences, 28 (1), 153-67. Dunn, Elizabeth W., Timothy D. Wilson, and Daniel T. Gilbert (2003), “Location, Location, Location: The Misprediction of Satisfaction in Housing Lotteries,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29 (11), 1421-32. Finkenauer, Catrin, Marcello Gallucci, Wilco W. van Dijk, and Monique Pollmann (2007), “Investigating the Role of Time in Affective Forecasting: Temporal Influences on Forecasting Accuracy,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33 (8) 1152-66. Gilbert, Daniel T., Elizabeth C. Pinel, Timothy D. Wilson, Stephen J. Blumberg, and Thalia P. Wheatley (1998), “Immune Neglect: A Source of Durability Bias in Affective Forecasting,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75 (3), 617-38. Greenleaf, Eric A. and Donald R. Lehmann (1995), “Reasons for Substantial Delay in Consumer Decision Making,” Journal of Consumer Research, 22 (2), 186-99. Liberman, Nira, Yaacov Trope, Sean M. McCrea, and Steven J. Sherman (2007), “The Effect of Level of Construal on the Temporal Distance of Activity Enactment,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43 (1), 143-49. MacInnis, Deborah J. and Vaness M. Patrick (2006), “Spotlight on Affect: Affect and Affective Forecasting in Impulse Control,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16 (3) 224-31. Morewedge, Carey K., Daniel T. Gilbert, and Timothy D. Wilson (2005), “The Least Likely of Times: How Remembering the Past Biases Forecasts of the Future,” Psychological Science, 16 (8), 626-30. Steel, Piers (2007), “The Nature of Procrastination: A Meta-Analytic and Theoretical Review of Quintessential Self-Regulatory Failure,” Psychological Bulletin, 133 (1), 65-94. Tice, Dianne M. and Roy F. Baumeister (1997), “Longitudinal Study of Procrastination, Performance, Stress, and Health: The Costs and Benefits of Dawdling,” Psychological Science, 8 (6), 454-58. Tice, Dianne M., Ellen Bratslavsky, and Roy F. Baumeister (2001), “Emotional Distress Regulation Takes Precedence Over Impulse Control: If You Feel Bad, Do It!,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80 (1), 53-67 Vohs, Kathleen D., Roy F. Baumeister, Brandon J. Schmeichel, Jean M. Twenge, Noelle M. Nelson, and Dianne M. Tice (2008), “Making Choices Impairs Subsequent Self-Control: A Limited-Resource Account of Decision Making, Self-Regulation, and Active Initiative,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94 (5), 883-98. Wilson, Timothy D. and Daniel T. Gilbert (2003), “Affective Forecasting,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 35, ed. Mark Zanna, Elsevier, New York, 345-411.