Supplemental Materials The Longer-Term Effects of a Brief Hazard Perception Training Intervention in Older Drivers by M. S. Horswill et al., 2015, Psychology and Aging http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038671 Table A1 Descriptive Statistics of the Sample by Condition Trained no booster 71.05 (6.88) Placebo Age (years) Trained with booster 72.32 (4.87) Years with open driving licence 52.23 (4.32) 51.05 (7.66) 54.33 (6.85) Kilometres per year 11763.64 (6886.74) 10892.73 (7300.02) 9213.04 (6939.36) Sex 45.5% female 36.4% female 37.5% female Modified Mini Mental State Examination (out of 100) Own a computer 94.45 (4.57) 94.9 (4.22) 94.91 (3.19) 95.50% 95.50% 83.30% Prior experience with computer Simple spatial reaction time (s) 95.50% 0.82 (0.16) 100% 0.89 (0.24) 87.50% 0.82 (0.11) 73.46 (6.04) Table A2 Means and Standard Deviations of Hazard Perception Test Response Times (s), With ANOVA Results, After Matching Placebo Group Participants to Individuals in the Training Condition by Pre-Intervention Hazard Perception Response Times Training Conditions M (SD) Placebo Condition M (SD) Time × Group interaction Time main effect Group main effect (N = 24) (N = 24) Pre-intervention hazard perception test 5.01 (0.63) 4.84 (0.80) Immediately following intervention 4.32 (0.94) 4.72 (0.74) F(1,46) = 9.47, MSE = 0.21, p < 0.01, 2 = 0.17 F(1,46) = 19.29, MSE = 0.21, p < .001, 2 = 0.30 F(1,46) = 0.30, MSE = 1.04, p = .587, 2 = 0.01 One month following intervention 4.44 (0.81) 4.75 (0.71) F(1,46) = 4.42, MSE = 0.30, p = .043, 2 = 0.09 F(1,46) = 9.17, MSE = 0.30, p = .004, 2 = 0.17 F(1,46) = 0.13, MSE = 0.80, p = .729, 2 < 0.01 Three months following intervention 4.46 (0.89) 4.72 (0.58) F(1,46) = 5.09, MSE = 0.23, p = .034, 2 = 0.10 F(1,46) = 11.4, MSE = 0.23, p = .002, 2 = 0.20 F(1,46) = 0.07, MSE = 0.86, p = .801, 2 < 0.01 Note: Due to the difference in pre-training hazard perception response times between the training and placebo groups in the present study, we matched 24 participants from the combined training groups (i.e., both training with booster and training without booster) with the 24 participants in the placebo group, based on pre-intervention hazard perception response times. The matching was done using the following rules: (1) Each member of the control group in turn was matched with the member of the combined training groups who had the closest pre-intervention hazard perception test response time (excluding training group participants who did not complete one of the hazard perception tests). (2) We allocated matches working from the participant with the fastest preintervention hazard perception test response time in the control group to the slowest (but note that matching slowest to fastest did not change the outcomes of these analyses). (3) If the closest match for any participant had already been allocated to another individual in the control group, then we instead used the next closest match (whether faster or slower). (4) If the next closest match had also already been allocated, then we chose the next closest match (repeating until an unallocated participant was identified). It was found that the training effect (time × group interaction) was significant for all three post-intervention tests, consistent with the analyses from the full sample. Table A3 Means (and Standard Deviations) of Hazard Perception Response Times (s) for Training and Placebo Groups Across the Four Hazard Perception Tests Used in the Study (Full Sample) Trained Placebo 6.01 (1.64) 4.89 (1.54) 4.23 (1.83) 4.66 (1.42) 1 month after intervention 4.55 (1.53) 4.72 (1.36) 3 months after intervention 4.92 (1.85) 4.66 (1.1) Pre-intervention hazard perception response time Immediate post-intervention hazard perception response time Table A4 The Effect of Training on Hazard Perception Test Hit Rates. Means (and Standard Deviations) of Hazard Perception Hit Rates for Training and Placebo Groups Across the Four Hazard Perception Tests Used in the Study Training Conditions M (SD) Placebo Condition M (SD) Time x Group interaction Time main effect Group main effect (N = 51) (N = 24) Pre-intervention hazard perception test 74% (11%) 80% (11%) Immediately following intervention 86% (12%) 81% (9%) F(1,73) = 20.16, MSE = 0.01, p < .001, 2 = 0.22 F(1,73) = 26.22, MSE = 0.01, p < .001, 2 = 0.26 F(1,73) = 0.13, MSE = 0.02, p = .715, 2 < 0.01 One month following intervention 82% (10%) 83% (16%) F(1,73) = 2.66, MSE = 0.01, p = .112, 2 = 0.04 F(1,73) = 13.94, MSE = 0.01, p = .001, 2 = 0.16 F(1,73) = 2.54, MSE = 0.02, p = .117, 2 = 0.03 Three months following intervention 80% (13%) 82% (9%) F(1,73) = 4.33, MSE = 0.01, p = .053, 2 = 0.06 F(1,73) = 8.51, MSE = 0.01, p = .005, 2 = 0.10 F(1,73) = 2.26, MSE = 0.02, p = .149, 2 = 0.03 Note: The hazard perception tests used in this study were not designed to yield hit rates as a performance measure. Traffic conflicts were chosen to optimize response time as the outcome, which meant only including events to which most participants would respond eventually. As a result, hit rates were not predicted to be sensitive to any interventions, consistent with our previous work. However, we still calculated hit rates (proportion of traffic conflicts responded to) as a secondary outcome measure in case any effects emerged that might have consequences for the interpretation of the response time findings. As can be seen in Table A4, training condition participants responded to a higher proportion of items than their baseline, in comparison with placebo, immediately post-intervention. However, the effect of training on hit rates was not significant at 1 month and 3 months. Figure A1. Change in hazard perception hit rates following the interventions (postintervention minus pre-intervention), where a positive value indicates that hit rate has increased following the interventions. Error bars are standard errors of the mean.