Running heading: DYAD CRITICAL SUMMARY Dyad Critical Summary of Motivation Theory Long Island University: C.W. Post Campus Marni Gavritsas and Raphael Morey DYAD CRITICAL SUMMARY For the purpose of our study, we refer to Graber and Brooks-Gunn's 1996, definition of adolescence. There is great significance to their understanding of this monumental change in the human lifespan as " the transition between pubertal onset and parental independence where puberty refers to changes in reproductive maturation and adolescence refers to the transition from dependence on parents to relative independence" (Casey, Dohoux, & Cohan, 2010). In the anchor study, the ideas presented in "Adolescence: What do transmission, transition, and translation have to do with it?" we are forced to recognize that humans and animals alike go through a developmental process that impacts the brains decision making abilities. This translational process explains the behaviors or what we define as the "typical teenager" and in effect defines the affect of what at times appears externally to be poor decision making. In addition to the work of Casey, Duhoux, and Cohan, we explored the Triadic Model presented by Ernst, Pine, and Hardin (2006). The Triadic Model studies the motivated behaviors in adolescence. The motivational approach, like Maslow's theory of separation and individuation, focuses on three actions 1) approach; 2) avoidance; and 3) regulation. Essentially, every decision that is made is a result of consideration to reward, harm avoidance and responses to punishment. Unlike adults, adolescence have not yet strengthened connections and weighed impulsivity results. As adults, we generally have a "top down" management system that prevents acting upon risky behaviors that outweigh benefits. "Variability in the degree of risk-taking, generic changes in decision-making during adolescence have been acknowledged throughout human history (Hall, 1904) and across species (Spear, 2000), and are recognized as primary sources of morbidity and mortality in adolescents" (Dahl, 2004). 2 DYAD CRITICAL SUMMARY In the adolescent years, the brain is developing at a pace that is not necessarily in sync with external factors such as changes in the environment, social pressures, parental expectations and intrinsic motivations. Within the brain, The ventral striatum circuits, particularly the nucleus accumbens, support reward processes and approach behavior (Wise et al. 1992; Di Chiara, 2002). The amygdala circuits have been de- scribed as the ‘behavioral brake’ to protect organisms from potential harm (Amaral, 2002; Zald, 2003), and are a key mediator of avoidant behavior (LeDoux, 2000). Finally, circuits of the prefrontal cortex, owing to their widely accepted role in cognitive control (Miller, 1999, 2000), help to orchestrate the relative contribution of the approach and avoidant behavioral systems, thus providing a supervisory or modulatory control of behavior. (Ernst, Pine, & Hardin, 2006) The resulting behavior is that adolescents show relatively higher impact of stimuli signaling reward on striatal activation and lesser impact of stimuli signaling punishment on amygdala activation compared to adults. This pattern would support predominant approach and riskseeking behavior. In essence, what we see in our society are teenagers often making choices that match the environment that they are experiencing but not the socially acceptable expectations we have placed upon them. Our theoretical framework is the theory of Human Motivation, first postulated by Abraham Maslow in 1943 in his paper A Theory of Human Motivation. Abraham Maslow is the founder of humanistic psychology, a psychology that bridges the twin peaks of development; homeostatic development and cognitive development. Maslow describes the value of humanistic psychology in that, “When we ask what man wants of life, we deal with his very essence” (p. 18). Our theoretical framework then is not merely an analysis of the development of brain 3 DYAD CRITICAL SUMMARY chemistry and physiology through the life cycle but rather how this development subsequently relates to the crafting of character and shaping of personality and how one applies these to their environment and relationships. Maslow (1943) hypothesizes that humans are driven by a hierarchy of five “basic needs” (p. 4), these needs being the physiological need, the safety needs, the love needs, the esteem needs, and the need for self-actualization. Towards the end of his life Maslow is noted as also considering the addition of a sixth need dubbed “self-transcendence” (Koltko-Rivera, 2006). Maslow describes human development as the satisfaction of basic needs leading to subsequent higher needs. Children desire physiological need satisfaction first which is satisfied by attaining consistent food supplies. After food comes safety and once their physical safety is satisfied comes the need for love. Once these three homeostatic needs are satisfied, cognitive needs become central to development specifically the self-esteem need, self-actualization need and the described self-transcendence need. While Maslow considers it not necessary to fully satisfy one need before reaching the next tier on the hierarchy, he does acknowledge that, “A healthy man is primarily motivated by his needs to develop and actualize his fullest potentialities and capacities. If a man has any other basic needs in an active, chronic sense, then he is simply an unhealthy man” (p. 18). Normal development, then, for adolescents and young adults focuses on selfesteem and self-actualization development rather than the homeostatic satisfactions. The growth of brain matter, strengthening of neuroconnections and changes in behavior during the adolescence phase of development can be interpreted as physiological changes manifesting themselves in behaviors seeking identity development and crafting through self-esteem building and self-actualization. Additionally, there exist cognitive needs, i.e., the desires to know, to understand and to find truth that can sometimes supersede the more basic homeostatic needs. 4 DYAD CRITICAL SUMMARY This points to the concept of self-transcendence and perhaps the fulfillment of this need shows a change in development from adolescent/young adult identity building and relationship forging to adulthood tendencies to contribute to the world and to the desire to give back. A study to supports Maslow’s motivation theory was conducted by Yeung and McInerney (2005). Their research examined students’ school motivation and aspirations over high school years. Their study questioned the reasons behind the universal observation of parents and teachers that adolescents demonstrate dramatic change, “Many student experienced a deterioration in perceptions of self, affect, motivation, and performance during early adolescence and in particular, when they moved to middle level schools...it is only during adolescence that formal operational thought merges and abstract processing with reference to both self and others occurs. It is during this period of development that the adolescent starts asking questions about ego, status, goals and ambitions” (p. 539). This demonstration of change, particularly with regard to self-perception and critical observation of their environment points to motivation theory’s fourth tier of need satisfaction, the need for self-esteem. Adolescents experience physiological change causing their behavior to shift towards reward driven actions. This is coupled with a degree of recklessness and heightened value of personal experience over guidance and advice. These actions speak to developing one’s sense of self-worth, value and understanding of identity. As the prefrontal cortex gains executive power over the brain during development, identity and self-esteem grow too until young adulthood occurs and the typical person moves onto the fifth tier, need for self-actualization. A study that relates to adolescent development and particularly the fifth tier of Maslow’s hierarchy of needs is by Duckworth, (2011) entitled The Significance of Self-Control. In his report, Duckworth cites multiple reports that display a strong correlation between self-control 5 DYAD CRITICAL SUMMARY and favorable attributes later in life. He specifically describes why Self-Control Matters, “Selfcontrol... during the first decade of life predicts income, savings behavior, financial security, occupational prestige, physical and mental health, substance use, and (lack of) criminal convictions, among other outcomes, in adulthood...the predictive power of self-control is comparable to that of either general intelligence or family socioeconomic status” (p. 2639). Studies support that concept that conscious or unconscious self-control can mitigate neurological and chemical changes in adolescence that could lead to negative behavior and identity impact. This early developmental dominance can elevate some adolescence above their peers, giving them a competitive advantage academically and perhaps, later on, professionally. Self-control can aid adolescence, whose brains are encouraging them to focus on full satisfaction of lower level needs, to perhaps move onto the higher level ones like self-actualization, desire for knowledge, etc. This suggests that the hierarchy of needs does not necessarily follow a rigid trajectory (self-esteem onset during adolescence, self-actualization during young adulthood) but rather can take place at an individual’s own pace. Duckworth’s study suggests that perhaps people who develop their identity and who receive strong self-esteem need satisfaction from an early age will more readily adapt self-actualization upon entering adolescence allowing them to develop characteristics like self-control which will aid them in achieving their goals from an early age. Class Discussion A lively discussion ensued both during and after the presentation. With our newly developed formula that E=MC (Environment=Motivation Control), we were able to present a simplified understanding that teenagers are greatly impacted by the environment that they experience and as a result their control or lack their of impacts their motivation to do or not to 6 DYAD CRITICAL SUMMARY do. Questions posed included 1) Is there value to be found in primary and secondary education classrooms that are focused primarily on intrinsic motivation for learning outcomes? 2) Could Governor Cuomo’s education section of his budget (APPR and Teacher Excellence Fund) serve as a negative motivator for teachers and prompt short term goal reward seeking behavior at the expense of long term gains in student in achievement? 3) Are self-control and motivation malleable traits or biologically predetermined? 4) There is currently no link between IQ and selfcontrol, yet both are indicators of success, which do you believe has a stronger correlation to motivation? 5) If we agree with motivation theory (Nurture Shock & Drive), do you believe that we are truly preparing our students to succeed in a 21st century workforce? In addition to these discussion questions, we presented the class with an activity that asked them to reflect upon satisfaction levels in past employment positions and then rank 10 different qualities of that position in terms of experience. The 10 different qualities represented a continuum of feelings that range from extrinsic motivation (high salary, bonuses, praise of supervisors) to intrinsic motivation (intrinsic interest, curiosity, challenging work). The data accumulated from this activity as well as the subsequent discussion was in line with the work of Deci and Ryan (2008) discussion of intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation on one’s need satisfaction. Deci and Ryan (2008) suggest that the more one feelings intrinsically motivated towards a task the greater need satisfaction they will receive leading to greater happiness, less stress and greater ability to learn. Extrinsic motivation alternatively leads to greater stress, greater burnout and less feeling of being in control of one’s actions. Our class discussed how their ranking of employment positions followed this pattern, specifically with all doctoral students ranking their current positions as highly intrinsically motivating and jobs in the past that were taken solely for convenience sake or out of monetary necessity as extrinsically motivating. 7 DYAD CRITICAL SUMMARY References Bronson, P. & Merryman, A. (2009) Nurtureshock: New thinking about children. New York. Twelve. Casey, B.J., Duhoux, S., & Cohan, M. (2010). Adolescence: What do transmission, transition, and translation have to do with it? Neuron, 67 (5), 749-760. Duckworth, A. (2011). Self-control and why it matters. PNAS, 108(7), 2639-2640. Deci, E.L., Ryan, R.M. (2008). Facilitating Optimal Motivation and Psychological Well-Being Across Life’s Domains. Canadian Psychology, 49(1), 14-23. Ernst, M., Pine, D., & Hardin, M. (2005). Triadic model of the neurobiology of motivated behvaior in adolescence. Psychological Medicine, 36, 299-312. Retrieved from doi:10.1017/S0033291705005891 Giedd, J. (2008). The teen brain: Insights from neuroimaging. Journal of Adolescent Health, 42, 335-343. Maslow, A. (1970). Motivation and personality. New York: Harper & Row. (pp. 35-58, “A theory of human motivation”). Moffitt, T., Arseneault, L., Belsky, B., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, L., et al. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 108 (7), 2693-2698 Pink, D. (2011). Drive: The surprising truth about what motivates us. New York: Riverhead Books. 8 DYAD CRITICAL SUMMARY Yeung, A.S., McINerney, D.M. (2005). Students’ School Motivation and Aspiration Over High School Years. Educational Psychology, 25(5), 537-554. 9