Perspective Taking, Religious Symbols and Jury Biases|1 Running Head: PERSPECTIVE TAKING, RELIGIOUS SYMBOLS, AND JURY BIASES Balancing Judgment: The Role of Perspective Taking in Reducing Juror Biases and the Effects of Religious Symbols Eve F. E. Turow Submitted to the Department of Psychology of Amherst College in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of the Arts with honors. Friday, April 24, 2009. Faculty Advisor: Allen J. Hart Perspective Taking, Religious Symbols and Jury Biases|2 Thank you to my friends and family who supported me throughout this process. Thank you for keeping me sane, entertaining me and motivating me. Thank you to Professors Foels and Schulkind for your statistical help and to the rest of the psychology professors I had here at Amherst. Thank you to Dean Hart for your constant encouragement and guidance. I had a great time completing this project with you and learned a great deal throughout. Perspective Taking, Religious Symbols and Jury Biases|3 Table of Contents Abstract……………………………………………………………………………………6 Introduction………………………………………………………………………………..7 Religion in the United States……………………………………………………...8 The History of Religion in the United States..………………………….....9 Religion and Adjudication……………………………………………………….11 Conclusion……………………………………………………………………….13 Chapter One: The Psychological Effects of Religion on Decision-Making……………..15 The Complexities of Religion.…………………………………………………...15 Morality………………………………………………………………….16 Prejudice…………………………………………………………………18 Effects of Religion on Rational and Experiential Thinking……………………...21 Cognitive Experiential Self-Theory (CEST)………….…………………22 Heuristic System Model (HSM)…………………………...…………….23 Schemas in Social Perception: Ingroup/Outgroup Attitudes…………………….28 Outgroup Punitiveness…………………………………………………...29 Similarity-Leniency Hypothesis…………………………………………32 Black Sheep Effect……………………………………………………….35 The Intricacies of Religious Groups………….……………………………….....39 Measuring Religious Prejudice…………………………….…………………….42 Extrinsic versus Intrinsic Orientation……………………………………43 Quest (Q) scale…………………………………………………………...45 Fundamentalism………………………………………………………….46 Chapter Summary………………………………………………………………..51 Chapter Two: The Role of Perspective Taking in Reducing Juror Biases and the Effects of Religious Symbols…………………………………………………………………….53 Methods…...……………………………………………………………………...54 Participants………………………………………………………………54 Materials…………………………………………………………………54 Procedure………………………………………………………………...60 Hypotheses……………………………………………………………………….60 Perspective Taking……………………………………………………….60 Religious Conditions…………………………………………………......61 Results……………………………………………………………………………62 General…………………………………………………………………...62 Comprehension Check…………………………………………………...63 Effects of Perspective Taking……………………………………………64 Effects of Religious Conditions………………………………………….74 Gender……………………………………………………………………81 Discussion………………………………………………………………………..82 Perspective Taking, Religious Symbols and Jury Biases|4 Perspective Taking: Conclusions………………………………………...84 Religious Conditions: Conclusions………………………………………86 Limitations……………………………………………………………………….89 Suggestions for Future Research………………………………………………...91 Implications for the Future…………………………………………………….....91 Chapter Three: Regulating Religion & Implications…………………………………….94 Durkheim: Emphasizing Citizenship…………………………………………….96 Conclusion……………………………………………………………………...100 Further Conclusions…………………………………………………….103 Appendices Appendix A: Trial Summary…………………………………………………………...105 Appendix B: Dependant Measures Survey……………………………………………..107 Appendix C: Vignettes………………………………………………………………….109 Appendix D: Correlations Chart………………………………………………………..110 Appendix E: Correlations without Atheist Participants………………………………...111 Appendix F: Regulating Religion……………………………………………………....112 Religion Clauses………………………………………………………………..112 Faith-Based Rehabilitation……………………………………………………..118 Innate Ties between Religion and Law………………………………….……...121 References………………………………………………………………………………126 Perspective Taking, Religious Symbols and Jury Biases|5 List of Illustrations Figures Figure 1: Recommended Sentences with & without Perspective Taking instructions for all participants.…………………………………………………………….………..66 Figure 2: Recommended Sentences from Christian participants with & without Perspective Taking instructions………………………………………………………..68 Figure 3: Religious Identification of all participants with & without Perspective Taking instructions……………………………………………………………………..73 Figure 4: Devotionalism of all participants with & without Perspective Taking instructions……………………………………………………………………………...73 Figure 5a. Reported Religious Identification of all participants…..………………...77 Figure 5b. Reported Religious Identification of Christian participants...……….….78 Figure 6a. Reported Devotionalism of all participants……………..………………...80 Figure 6b. Reported Devotionalism of Christian participants…………….………...80 Tables Table 1. Sum of Perspective Taking with & without Perspective Taking instructions……………………………………………………………………………...64 Table 2. Sum of Perspective Taking with & without Perspective Taking instructions for all Christian participants…………...……………………….……………………..64 Table 3: Percent of guilty verdicts for aggravated sexual abuse in conditions with & without Perspective Taking…………………………………………………………….66 Table 4: Percent of guilty verdicts for aggravated sexual abuse in conditions with & without Perspective Taking; Only Christian participants…………………...............67 Table 5a: CEST Vignettes, Heuristic Means data of only Christian participants….70 Table 5b. CEST Vignettes, Logical Means data of only Christian participants……70 Table 6: Gender Data…………………………………………………………………..82 Perspective Taking, Religious Symbols, and Jury Biases |6 Abstract Through a review of literature relevant to the issues of perspective taking, religion, and jury biases, we provide an overview of decision-making processes, ingroup and outgroup biases as well as religious biases and their role in courtrooms today. A study investigated the role of perspective taking and religious symbols on jury biases and decision-making by manipulating perspective taking instructions and religious content in a 3x2 factorial design. By including or excluding perspective taking instructions as well as changing the religion of a defendant within a case summary (control, Christian or Sikh), we were able to observe changes in participants’ judgments and thought processes. Measures of perspective taking as well as experiential and rational thinking were administered, utilizing Davis’ (1980/1983) Interpersonal Reactivity Index along with Epstein’s (1996) Rational-Experiential Inventory and Cognitive-Experiential Self Theory vignettes, originally theorized by Cacioppo and Petty (1982). It was hypothesized that perspective taking instructions would reduce the expression of juror biases and increase rational thinking while the presence of religious symbols was expected to increase levels of juror bias and experiential thinking. Evidence supported the first of these hypotheses, with lowered sentences and higher rationality reported with perspective taking instructions, while mixed results were collected regarding the effect of religious symbols. Correlations between religious symbols, experiential thinking, Devotionalism, as well as Religious Identification shifted between the Christian and Sikh conditions. Findings led to the conclusion that perspective taking acts to inhibit the expression of juror biases and increase rationality while the inclusion of religious symbols dulls the effect of perspective taking by altering the ways in which participants view themselves as well as the defendant. In the end, it is concluded that perspective taking instructions be added to Judge’s instructions and that religious symbols be expelled from the courts. Perspective Taking, Religious Symbols, and Jury Biases |7 Introduction The arenas of law and religion are often mixed in American culture. With the country’s underpinnings set in Judeo-Christian thought, many see it as inevitable for religion to work into the seams of United States policy and traditions. From the use of a holy book for swearing in witnesses, to the formidable placement of the Judge upon the pulpit, and jurors referring to the Bible to help reach their verdicts,1 religious undercurrents are felt in courtrooms throughout the country. And while many accept these undercurrents as inescapable and often an assumed part of the legal realm, several studies have begun to show the biasing effects of religious salience within the courts (e.g., Howard & Redfering, 1983; Egland, 2004; Herek, 1987; Jackson & Hunsberger, 1999; Kerr et al., 1995; Miller, 2008). These studies beg one to question the biases present within jurors around the country and the role of religious content in further instigating the expression of religious biases. Further, one must question how religious references affect a juror’s decisionmaking processes and adjudication. By investigating the influence of perspective taking on juror biases and the effects of religion on rational and experiential thinking, I aim to show that: 1) jurors hold inherent biases 2) the use of perspective taking (asking one to take the perspective of another) can reduce expression of inherent biases 3) the presence of religious symbols creates religious salience and instigates the expression of ingroup/outgroup biases, effecting the ways in which we view ourselves and others, 4) that the salience of religion also hinders one’s ability to think rationally, causing one to rely on personal feelings over judge’s instructions and evidence provided within the case, 1 See, e.g., People v. Harlan (2003). Perspective Taking, Religious Symbols, and Jury Biases |8 and finally 5) perspective taking reduces these tendencies. Through this investigation, I hope to demonstrate the significant biases present within jurors and the effects of religion in the United States court system, and furthermore the importance of perspective taking instructions in reducing juror biases and the need to extract any and all religious references from the courtroom environment. Religion in the United States For many U.S. citizens, religion acts as an additional set of laws, instructing believers in ethics and supplying a moral code for framing their lives. “All men desire to know,” wrote Aristotle, 2 and religion lets us do just that; it opens the door to the intrigues of life and death, the meaning of life,3 and the rules to follow for everyday laments. 4 Furthermore, religion plays an integral part in self-definition and positive self-evaluations, often depicting one’s upbringing and daily actions (Kentworthy, 2003). 5 People say that today, few topics are taken more seriously than religion (Spilka et al., 2003). We have created a society in which religion is constantly invoked, whether through celebrations of marriage or birth or for ceremonies for those we have lost. Particularly in the United States, religion is omnipresent, and consistently influences our political system, laws, and education.6 As cited in McKeon, 1941, p. 689. Clark (1958) claims that “religion more than any other human function satisfies the need for meaning in life.” 4 Argyle (1959) claims that “a major mechanism behind religious beliefs is purely cognitive desire to understand.” 5 E.g., Blaine & Crocker (1995). 6 In the 2008 Primary Presidential debates, held in New Hampshire on September 26, all Democratic nominees were asked “What is your favorite Bible verse?”; Fallows, J (2008, 11). Rhetorical questions. Atlantic, 34-52; Further discussion of religion’s role in U.S. politics and law can be found in Appendix F. 2 3 Perspective Taking, Religious Symbols, and Jury Biases |9 Many fail to recognize the overwhelming number of religious Americans. A 2007 Gallup Poll found that 56 percent of Americans say religion is “very important” in their own lives and 61 percent of Americans are members of a church or synagogue.7 For the past 60 years, the percentage of individuals reporting that they believe in God has exceeded 90 percent (Miller & Bornstein, 2005; Kentworthy, 2003); according to the 2001 Census, 79.8% of Americans label themselves Christians, 1.4% Jewish, and 0.6% Muslim. These statistics explain why Judeo-Christian traditions are an integral part of our society, indicating that most Americans believe in the Old Testament as well as the concept of one God, and nearly 80% believe in the teachings of the New Testament. The history of religion in the United States. It is essential to understand the difficulty in addressing religion and the long-lasting role religion has played in the formation of the United States. Thomas Jefferson stated that the First Amendment clearly indicates a separation of church and state.8 Even so, the lines between the two continue to be blurred. Whereas several laws have been put into place to limit the government’s role in religious matters, splitting these two issues has proven to be nearly impossible. From confusion regarding the Religion Clauses, to the blatant presence of religion within American culture, religion has maneuvered its way into our courtrooms.9 Furthermore, Judeo-Christian beliefs influenced the founding of the United States, and thus played an inevitable role in the creation of our laws as well as traditions. Gallup Poll, Religion, 2007. In a 1802 letter he wrote, “Believing with you that religion is a matter which lies solely between Man & his God, that he owes account to none other for his faith or his worship, that the legitimate powers of government reach actions only, & not opinions, I contemplate with sovereign reverence that act of the whole American people which declared that their legislature should ‘make no law respecting an establishment of religion, or prohibiting the free exercise thereof,’ thus building a wall of separation between Church & State.”; Jefferson, T. (1802). Jefferson's Letter to the Danbury Baptists. U.S. Library of Congress, from http://www.loc.gov/loc/lcib/9806/danpre.html. Retrieved 2/11/09. 9 More information on this topic can be found in Appendix F. 7 8 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 10 Examples of Christian roots are present in several common-place American traditions. 10 In fact, the precursors to the current Maryland Sunday closing laws are undeniably religious. The first Maryland statute regarding Sunday activities, enacted in 1649, was titled “An Act concerning Religion.” It made it illegal to “profane the Sabbath or Lords day called Sunday by frequent swearing, drunkenness or by any uncivil or disorderly recreation, or by working on that day.”11 A later statute in 1962, entitled “An Act for the Service of Almighty God and the Establishment of the Protestant Religion within this province,” emphasized the importance of keeping the Sabbath and later called for a Sunday labor prohibition. By 1973, the Sabbath-breaking section of the Maryland statutes assumed its present form by omitting the specific prohibition against swearing and the religiously significant title. 12 In recent years the religious motivations of Sunday closings have been de-emphasized, with the focus shifted to a common wellbeing rather than religious expectations, but Sunday closings remain today nonetheless. This is just one example of Christian-based laws, plentiful in the legal history of the United States, acting as the founding for laws in place today. While religion acts as an assumed and natural part of American society, few recognize its warping effects on one’s rationality, making the issue of religion’s role in the legal world necessary to address. Because of the moral and emotional significance of religion, its presence acts to inhibit the proper functioning of our judicial system. Religion initiates experiential thinking, biases, and often reliance on one’s religious Laws regarding religion date back to 1448 when Henry VI banned the selling of goods on Sundays in England, which continued to be enforced through statutes such as, “Sabbath Breaking” and “Lord’s Day” in Maryland and the “Bill for Punishing…Sabbath Breakers” in Virginia; McGowan v. Maryland (1961, p. 438 & 445). 11 Art. 27, §§ 492-534C; 1 Archives of Maryland 244-247. 12 McGowan v. Maryland (1961, p. 446). 10 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 11 morals above the law, and in this manner prejudices juries and judges alike (Miller & Bornstein, 2006; Miller & Bornstein, 2005). Whereas the ties between religion and the law are difficult to address, the effects of religious beliefs on one’s psychological state are enormous and therefore must be further researched. Religion and Adjudication It is unmistakable that jurors of all faiths feel pressure to adhere to their beliefs while in court, depending upon religious teachings to guide them in adjudications. In the past, jurors have met to “pray for divine assistance” during deliberations and have openly admitted to researching Biblical passages in search of guidance (Howard & Redfering, 1983; Egland, 2004).13 Even political figures such as George W. Bush have openly relied on religious morals when making legal judgments. While considering the death penalty case against Karla Faye Tucker, then Governor Bush admitted that he had “sought guidance through prayer” and decided to place the responsibility of judging another on a “higher authority” instead of himself (Egland, 2004). Situations such as this highlight the importance of religion to many jurors throughout the U.S. And not only does religion provide an alternative book of guidance, but it influences the decision-making process. Religious salience can affect the decision-making process in several ways, one of which is eliciting experiential thinking. The topic of religion often evokes juror’s emotions, instigating a purely experiential decision rather than a rational one, following the Cognitive Experiential Self-Theory (CEST). During experiential processing, jurors are more likely to rely on religious teachings and biases to help them reach a verdict. 13 See, e.g., People v. Harlan (2003). P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 12 Experiential processing is oriented around feelings of pleasure and pain and encourages people to make decisions based on what feels best to them at the given moment, in a very immediate manner. Therefore, many prosecutors and attorneys exploit the religious beliefs of citizens by incorporating Biblical statements into their arguments, attempting to sway the jury for or against a defendant.14 By doing so, they encourage jurors to make emotional evaluations. As such, the topic of religion greatly jeopardizes one’s ability to think rationally about a case before them. Additionally, as was the case with former President Bush, religious beliefs often act as mental shortcuts that relieve the evaluator of responsibility in accordance with the Heuristic Systems Model (HSM). Heuristics allow an individual to process information in a cursory manner, often resulting in experiential decisions, reflecting any prejudice or biases a person may hold. Additionally, by attributing one’s decision to his/her religious teachings, he/she defers the responsibility of the decision from himself/herself to a higher authority. As shown in several studies (e.g., Miller & Bornstein, 2005; Miller & Bornstein, 2006; Giner-Sorolla et al., 2002; Kirkpatrick et al., 1992), the topic of religion often elicits heuristic thinking due to its emotional and personal relevance. Heuristic thinking greatly hinders one’s ability to make a rational and effortful decision, compromising the validity and honesty of a potential juror. Furthermore, beyond the mental processes involved in decision-making, ingroup/outgroup biases are also a factor when it comes judging others, especially when the religion of the defendant is known. Religion connects people; it socializes behavior See e.g., Sandoval v. Calderon (2000); Andrea Yates’s prosecutor told the jury, “[it] was wrong in the eyes of God and it was wrong in the eyes of the law” (Yates Found Guilty of Murdering Her Children, CNN.com, (March 13, 2002), at http://archives.cnn.com/2002/LAW/03/13/yates.trial/index.html.) 14 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 13 and creates a common community. As stated by Lumsden and Wilson (1983), religion is a “powerful device by which people are absorbed into a tribe and psychically strengthened.”15 Emile Durkheim considers religion to be a collective action that engages and fuses people together, legitimizing and reinforcing society’s values and creating groups with common goals and idealization. According to Durkheim, one finds collective effervescence within religious groups, unifying communities around a common belief. 16 Due to the strong bonds between those of a similar religious group, ingroup and outgroup biases are often triggered when the religious affiliation of the defendant is known. Such biases manipulate thought processes through the similarity-leniency hypothesis as well as the black sheep effect, both of which will be discussed in further detail in the following chapter. Conclusion Some topics of discussion within this piece will include morality and prejudice, inherent juror biases and the role of perspective taking in controlling for these biases, the various psychological effects of religious material on decision-making and the difficulty of addressing religious beliefs. Additionally, current research and the implications of such research will be discussed. While studies have been conducted to address outgroup religious biases (e.g., Van Pooigen & Lam, 2007; Kerr et al., 1995; Tajfel & Turner, 1986; Jackson & Hunsberger, 1999; Daudistel et al., 1999) and the effects of religious defense and prosecutorial arguments in courts (e.g., Miller & Bornstein 2005; Miller & Bornstein, As cited in Spilka et al. (2003). Collective effervescence is the electrical charge that takes place when people come together and do something in common. It produces community and productivity; Emile Durkheim, The Elementary Forms of the Religious Life, 1961, Collier Books. Discussion of this theory will continue in Chapter 3. 15 16 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 14 2006), no study has looked into the possibility of an innate presence of religion in courtrooms, triggered by the use of a holy book for swearing in and possible religious symbols present on the defendant (such as a necklace or tattoo), and the effects of such religious presence on juror decision-making. Therefore, in an effort to better understand the role of religion in courts today, this study examines the role of perspective taking in reducing levels of bias and altering court verdicts as well as assesses the impact of religious symbols on a juror’s ability to think rationally and the effect it may have upon sentencing decisions. Furthermore, it examines the effects of perspective taking instructions and religious material on evaluations of the self and others. Upon completion of the study, evidence was found to indicate that instructions of perspective taking resulted in higher levels of rational thinking and decreased reliance on religious convictions. Additionally, there was significant evidence that the presence of a religious book and religious symbol within the courtroom effected jurors’ sentencing decisions, reliance on experiential thinking, as well as how jurors viewed themselves. A complete review of the findings will follow in Chapter Two. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 15 Chapter One: The Psychological Effects of Religion on Decision-Making. “The role of religion is paradoxical. It makes prejudice and it unmakes prejudice…Some people say the only cure for prejudice is more religion, some say the only cure is to abolish religion.” –Allport (1954) “The nature of the man himself is the element that determines the juror’s bias for or against his fellowman. Assuming that a juror is not a half-wit, his intellect can always furnish fairly good reasons for following his instincts and emotions. Many irrelevant issues in choosing jurors are not so silly as they seem. Matters that apparently have nothing to do with the discussion of a case often are of the greatest significance.” -Darrow (1940, p.488) The Complexities of Religion Evidence has shown that religion both exacerbates and reduces levels of prejudice (e.g., Allport & Ross, 1967; Donahue, 1985; Herek; 1987; Ponton & Gorsuch, 1988; Perrin, 2000). For some, religion encourages kind behavior and acceptance. For others, religion excuses destructive and discriminatory acts and provides a means by which they develop feelings of supremacy. Religion is tightly bound to both morality and prejudice, feeding both fires with its rhetoric and group cohesion. For this reason, the impact of religious beliefs on one’s behavior is complex and not well understood. Yet, many continue to judge others based on religious affiliation and devotion, often associating one’s level of religiosity with kindness and honesty (e.g., Perrin, 2000; Krause & Ingersoll-Dayton, 2001; Commonwealth v. Daniels, 1994). This, one may argue, is an improper way to judge one’s morality because religious devotion may be an indicator of several contradictory characteristics (e.g., acceptance vs. discrimination). Religion has served as a means to heal as well as destruct, but how and why one uses religion for one purpose or the other is still unclear. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 16 Morality. Many consider religion and morality to be inseparable concepts. Some of the most public leaders and beneficiaries have had strongly religious backgrounds, such as Mother Teresa and Martin Luther King Jr. Churches, synagogues and mosques around the world support social services and recreational activities, expressing the need for acceptance and equality, and as such, many deem religion and morality to be one and the same. As taught in Buddhism, Christianity, Judaism, and many other faiths, religion provides an ethical system that, if applied, could benefit us all. In fact, many religious groups wish to legislate morality, making it a concrete structure in America’s political system.17 Evidently, many believe that integrating religious morality would benefit our government and society today, providing a stronger moral backbone for the nation. Because of the connections between morality and religion, religiousness is often associated with being a good person, with increased levels of honesty, trust and forgiveness. In an effort to legitimize the claim that religion correlates with increased levels of honesty, Perrin (2000) fashioned a study to evaluate honesty and religious values of Christian students at a United States college. The study began with Christian students completing a survey on religiosity, answering questions regarding their frequency of church attendance, belief in the afterlife, and several other similar topics. Afterwards, there was a simple test of honesty: after a weekly quiz, the student participants’ teacher intentionally graded the exams incorrectly, giving each student an additional point. Students were then told that there may have been a grading error and were asked to re-grade their own quizzes. Afterwards, the students were instructed to One such example is the Christian Coalition who desired to impose a Bible-based morality in the U.S. (Birnbaum, 1992). 17 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 17 write either, “I owe you a point,” “Quiz graded correctly,” or “You owe me a point,” at the top of the page. Of the 130 students who participated in the experiment, only 32% honestly admitted to receiving an extra point on the quiz, while 52% claimed the quiz was graded correctly and 16% actually tried to get an extra point in addition to the one incorrectly received in the beginning. But, when crossing the religion score with the level of honesty, it was found that 45% of those who reported weekly church attendance honestly reported the 1-point error, while only 13% of those who attended church once a year honestly reported the error. Perrin took these results to indicate higher levels of honesty in those who attend church more frequently, supporting the claim that morality and religion are correlated. In fact, several other studies have mirrored her claim, correlating religion with lower levels of social deviance (e.g., Tittle & Welch 1983; Welch et al., 1991). In contrast, many argue that religion is not an indicator of one’s morality, and that the two concepts are wholly independent. In fact, Kohlberg argued that religion does not directly contribute to one’s morality in any way.18 Supporting Kohlberg, conflicting evidence has been found regarding the relationship between trust and forgiveness with religiosity. Gorsuch and Hao (1993) demonstrated a positive correlation between religious devotion and the desire to forgive, while McCullough and Worthington (1999) observed weak ties between religion and forgiveness. Schoenfeld (1978) related church attendance to increased trust in people (citing the effect of people’s extended trust in God), but this correlation was not found by Ostow (1990) when looking at fundamentalist, extremely conservative, and Pentecostal groups. Spilka et al. (2003) write that this 18 As cited in Spilka et al. (2003). P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 18 finding is due to these groups’ general belief that people are primarily sinners, hindering their ability to trust. Even so, it directly challenges the assumption that morality is innately tied to religious convictions. By finding evidence to support both sides of the argument, endorsement of religion’s correspondence to morality is nullified. This contradictory evidence makes the link between morality and religion debatable. And because of the confusion surrounding religion and morality, one cannot assume that because one is religious he/she has high morals.19 Prejudice. Adding to the complex connections between morality and religion is the presence of prejudice, a topic in direct opposition to morality, but also often associated with religion. Several studies have connected the strength of one’s religious beliefs to one’s level of prejudice through measures of intrinsic and extrinsic, fundamental, and quest beliefs (e.g., Allport & Ross, 1967; Donahue, 1985; Herek, 1987; Ponton & Gorsuch, 1988). Looking further into predicting discriminatory behavior, McFarland (1989) developed a study to examine religious orientation and several possible prejudices. McFarland (1989) distributed a questionnaire to several students at a United States university, entitled “Religious and Social Attitudes,” that measured religious orientation and attitudes towards African-Americans, women, homosexuals, and communists. After data collection, McFarland (1989) found (in correlations between religious orientations and the discriminatory attitude scales) that all of the orientations tested for within the questionnaire (fundamentalism, extrinsic, intrinsic, and quest religious orientations) indicated a “general tendency to discriminate,” with residual scales This argument stands in the courtroom setting; just because a juror is wearing a religious symbol does not indicate higher morality or innocence. Similarly, lawyers arguing that their defendant is religious should not sway a jury towards their innocence, as religious convictions alone do not reveal a concrete characteristic of an individual. 19 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 19 revealing differing discriminatory attitudes for each group (p.1). This indicates that while these varying religious orientations held differing levels of prejudice affiliated with each racial/gender/political group, all reported significant levels of discrimination. This is just one of many studies that points to a connection between religion and discriminatory beliefs, supporting the conjecture that religion has natural ties to prejudicial viewpoints. While often a predictor for discriminatory attitudes, religion is frequently deemed a catalyst for discrimination. These claims are made with the assumption that religion can provide justification for many prejudicial beliefs (e.g., Whitehead, 1962; Juergensmeyer, 2000). 20 Religious prejudice is apparent around the world, whether it is in the ongoing battle between Jews and Muslims in the Middle East, violence against Christians in Iraq,21 Catholics battling Protestants in Northern Ireland, or the constant fighting between Sikhs, Hindus, and Muslims in India. History has shown over and again the power of religious convictions and the negative attitudes they can create and often condone. One illustration of religious support for discriminatory attitudes can be observed in the issue of homosexuality and the Christian church. Hunsberger et al. (2005) discuss that many Christians point to their religiously-based moral position against homosexuality to justify their negative feelings towards homosexuality and homosexuals, claiming that their feelings are not prejudiced, but religious (e.g., Fulton et al., 1999). Yet, Hunsberger et al. (2005) found that this moral position acts as a predictor of “One might well wonder whether religion does not directly contribute to intolerance, injustice, and even violence,” writes Spilka et al. (2003) in reaction to Jerry Falwell, a fundamentalist preacher’s remarks blaming abortionists, feminists, gays and lesbians for the 9/11 terrorist attacks. 21 The Christian owner of a car repair shop was killed execution-style in Iraq on January 17, 2009; (2009, 1, 18). Christian merchant killed in the north of Iraq. The Boston Globe, Retrieved 1, 22, 2009, from http://www.boston.com/news/world/asia/articles/2009/01/18/christian_merchant_killed_in_the_north_of_ iraq/. 20 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 20 discrimination, legitimizing a believer’s intolerant viewpoint. In this way, prejudicial beliefs are often rationalized through ones religious teachings, reasoning that one’s convictions are supported by the religion. Christians’ attitudes about homosexuality are not the only example of prejudice supported by one’s religious teachings. For example, it has been found that Christianity directly contributes to feelings of anti-Semitism (e.g., Glock and Stark, 1966; Eisinga, Konig, & Sheepers, 1995), 22 today members of AlQaeda’s jihad claim to be following their Muslim faith when they attack innocents in the Middle East and abroad, and many Zionists believe that the Torah gives Jews the right to Israel, often validating their violent treatment of Palestinians. Religious teachings and morals are used to uphold prejudicial attitudes around the world, in several religious groups and nations. Prejudice is ignited in several ways, including ingroup/outgroup prejudice, stereotypes, emotional responses, and symbolic beliefs. In fact, a review by Batson et al. (1993) found that 37 of 47 research findings on this topic conducted between 1940 and 1990 showed a positive relationship between levels of religiousness and prejudice. Furthermore, only two studies showed a negative relationship. Hunsberger et al. (2005) showed that perceived religious competition can increase prejudice, along with levels of fundamentalism and intrinsic versus extrinsic orientations, concurring with the findings in McFarland (1989). Several studies support the assertion that fundamentalism correlates with increased prejudice (e.g., Jackson et al., 1997; Herek, 1987; Griffen et al., 1987; Unger, 2002; Hunsberger et al., 1999; McFarland, 1989), and many others argue 22 As cited in Spilka et al. (2003). P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 21 that religious orientation (extrinsic vs. intrinsic) predicts prejudice. In short, the evidence supporting religious ingroup/outgroup biases is substantial. Religion is often seen as a reflection of one’s moral fiber. Today, testimony regarding a defendant’s religiosity is generally accepted because it is considered evidence of the defendant’s character (Miller & Bornstein, 2005; Miller & Hayward, 2007)23. But, as discussed, religion can be a predictor of one’s moral code as well as one’s prejudicial beliefs. Simply because people are religious does not mean that they are honest and trustworthy, nor does it mean they are discriminatory or prejudiced. Religion is a complex matter that affects individuals in profound and extraordinarily different manners. This conundrum leaves us with several questions to be answered: What do religious beliefs indicate? How do they affect our thinking about ourselves and others? Do most individuals maintain religious biases? These questions will act as a guideline moving forward, challenging the common legal assumption that one’s character can be better understood by one’s level of religiousness and/or religious affiliation (Commonwealth v. Daniels, 1994). 24 Effects of Religion on Rational and Experiential Thinking It has been found that the salience of religious beliefs can lead to instinctual and heuristic decisions (e.g., Miller, 2008; Giner-Sorolla et al., 2002; Miller & Bornstein, 2006). Cognitive Experiential Self-Theory (CEST) and the Heuristic System Model (HSM) are the two leading theories explaining the use of heuristics and experiential There is evidence of judges having allowed “questions about affiliation (e.g., State v. Purcell 2001), service as a missionary (State v. Fuller 2004), strength of religious beliefs (U.S. v. DeJesus 2003), and religious occupation (Highler v. State 2006)” (Miller & Hayward, 2007, p. 8). 24 As cited in Miller & Bornstein (2006). 23 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 22 thinking. According to these models, when emotional content is salient, individuals are often unable to make rational decisions and instead rely on biases and mental shortcuts to make quick, instinctual, and outcome-oriented decisions. In doing so, heuristics often utilize thoughts that lie outside conscious awareness, such as subconscious prejudicial believes. Many claim that the presence of religious material in court sparks such personal biases and heuristics, interfering with rational thought processes necessary when making critical judgments (Miller & Bornstein, 2006); emotional content can jeopardize logical assessments by compromising one’s rationality. Cognitive experiential self-theory (CEST). The threat to logical thinking is best explained by Cognitive Experiential Self-Theory. CEST creates a distinction between two modes of thinking: experiential and rational. Rational thinking is a conscious, effortful system that functions according to a person’s established rules of logic and evidence. While thinking rationally, one employs an analytic and detail-orientated mind frame. Rational thinking requires significant cognitive activity and can be, at times, quite time consuming. Because of the cognitive capacity and time required for rational thinking, experiential thinking provides an alternative way to quickly sift through information. Unlike rational thinking, the experiential system is a preconscious, automatic system that relies on emotional cues for processing information (e.g., Kirkpatrick & Epstein, 1992; Miller & Bornstein, 2005). When using the experiential system, generalizations and heuristics simplify and speed up the decision-making process. While this system of thinking has its evolutionary benefits, experiential thinking can result in illogical and rapid judgments, often based on one’s emotions, making it ill-suited for solving problems P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 23 that require logical analysis (Epstein et al., 1996), highlighting the concern of jury members employing heuristics. As stated earlier, individuals are likely to respond experientially instead of rationally during emotionally significant events (Miller & Bornstein, 2006). This effect was observed in a study by Bright & Goodman-Delahunty (2006), which inspected the effects of jarring images on jurors’ emotional states, demonstrating jurors’ reactions to gruesome photographs. Results indicated that jurors who saw the photographs had significantly higher mean ratings of the weight of inculpatory prosecution evidence than those who did not view the photographs. Furthermore, results indicated that mock jurors’ anger towards the defendant was significantly higher in the condition with gruesome photographs and enhanced the weight of inculpatory evidence. This study showed that emotional reactions can inhibit logical and rational decision processes and impair jurors’ abilities to deliver a verdict based only on the value of evidence in a case. Instead of looking solely at the evidence at hand, mock jurors were emotionally affected by the photographs, developing high levels of anger towards the defendant and weighing inculpatory evidence very strongly. Several other studies have been conducted that have collected similar results (e.g., Oliver & Griffitt, 1976; Douglas, Lyon & Ogloff, 1997; Bright & Goodman-Delahunty, 2004), again emphasizing the distorting effects of emotional content on one’s judgments and thought processes. Heuristic system model (HSM). These emotionally-driven snap judgments are a symptom of the heuristic system. While rational thinking utilizes systematic processing, an in-depth and analytic approach, experiential thinking uses heuristic processing, a structure that relies on “knowledge structures” created by past experiences (Chen et al., P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 24 1999). The role of heuristics is best explained by the Heuristic System Model (HSM). HSM states that people process information in two distinct ways: cursory and effortful (Miller & Bornstein, 2005). While rational-systematic processes require significant cognitive efforts, experiential-heuristic thinking allows for quick, cursory evaluations. Judgments made through heuristic shortcuts often reflect cue information (e.g., memories, biases, past experiences) rather than information particular to the current situation, excluding a thorough analysis of the situation, and instead forcing one to rely on personal biases, subconscious thoughts and prejudiced beliefs (Chen et al., 1999). These heuristics may explain the anger developed by mock jurors in the study by Bright & GoodmanDelahunty (2006). Seeing gruesome photographs could have triggered past memories that biased and influenced the jury members’ decisions. Also, heuristic shortcuts regularly lead to errors in judgment and prevent sufficient comprehension due to its minimal cognitive demands, one more explanation as to why experiential thinking often results in illogical decisions (Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). It is well documented that heuristics and experiential thinking are often activated when religious content is salient (e.g., Miller & Bornstein, 2006; Miller & Bornstein, 2005; Howard & Redfering, 1983; Epstein et al., 1996; Kirkpatrick & Epstein, 1992; Johnson, 1984; Pfeifer, 1999). This evidence is of particular consequence in courtrooms when religious material and/or symbols are present. In an effort to better understand the role of religion in the courtroom, Miller and Bornstein (2006) designed an experiment to evaluate the effects of religious appeals on jurors’ sentencing verdicts and their ability to properly weigh aggravating and mitigating factors. The experimenters had participants read a mock trial that varied in the number of mitigators and aggravators, whether or not P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 25 the prosecutor used a Biblical appeal, and the way the defense incorporated religion (either the defense attorney used a Biblical appeal, the defendant testified that he had always been a Christian, the defendant testified that he recently converted to Christianity, or no religion was mentioned). After reading the scenario, participants issued one of two sentences: life in prison without parole or the death penalty. Miller and Bornstein’s (2006) findings reflect their hypothesis: that the use of religion by the defense would lead to fewer death sentences and interfere in jurors’ abilities to weigh mitigating and aggravating factors. While the prosecution appeals did not affect verdict decisions, it was found that those in the “Always Christian” group could not weigh aggravators and mitigators properly or accurately. Additionally, mock jurors were least punitive toward the defendant who had converted to Christianity, rather than the control group or the defendant who had always been a Christian, revealing juror biases. 25 (The differing treatments of the defendant relate to several ingroup/outgroup biases that will be discussed in further detail later). These biases may have been elicited by heuristic-experiential thinking, implying the significant impact of religious information. These results support the claim that heuristics are used when religious material is present; the experiment suggested that religious content interfered with jurors’ abilities to think rationally and correctly weigh aggravating and mitigating factors, as they relied on biases and acted leniently to the more religious defendant. Similar findings occurred in a study by Johnson (1984) in which religious jurors were more severe in their judgments of the defendant when religion was used as a defense, reflecting personal black sheep biases. This effect was observed in the sentencing trial against Terry Nichols, the Oklahoma City bombing accomplice, who did not received the death penalty because jurors believed his recent conversion to Christianity proved his ability to do positive deeds (Miller & Bornstein, 2006, p. 676). 25 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 26 Evidence from Miller and Bornstein (2006) also displayed the impact of defense appeals on juror members. In fact, Epstein (1994) has suggested that religious messages may be especially influential because they connect well with the experiential system. This theory explains why religious appeals are often effective in swaying juries.26 Kirkpatrick and Epstein (1992) write, “As techniques are used that are designed to bypass people’s need to present themselves as rational or that strongly engage their experiential system, people increasingly behave in a way that conforms to the principles of the experiential system” (p. 543). As discussed, these behaviors include reliance on biases, quick and instinctual decisions, outcome-oriented decisions, and thoughts that lie outside conscious awareness (Epstein, 1990). As such, defense attorneys as well as prosecuting attorneys often utilize the emotional effects of religious appeals. An additional consequence of experiential and heuristic shortcuts is the removal of responsibility from oneself. Miller and Bornstein (2005) write, “Jurors experiencing religious appeals are likely to use the appeal as a mental shortcut because…their highly emotional state leaves them unwilling or unable to accept responsibility” (p. 53). By relying on one’s religious beliefs, an individual defers responsibility for their decision to their religious group or God, avoiding any personal negative feelings associated with the decision. This rejection of responsibility not only hinders the decision-making process, but is illegal if used in a death-penalty trial. If a juror defers responsibility due to a religious appeal or symbol in a death penalty case, that statement or symbol will have violated the Caldwell rule. As cited in Miller and Bornstein (2005), this rule states that it An example can be seen in the case against Susan Smith. See e.g., Burritt, C. (1995). Seeking Meaning in Smith Tragedy: Faith helps Ease Pain but ‘The Mystery is Why God Allows Certain Things to Happen.’ Atlanta J. & Constitution, A12. 26 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 27 is, “constitutionally impermissible to rest a death sentence on a determination made by a sentencer who has been led to believe that the responsibility for determining the appropriateness for the defendant’s death rests elsewhere”(p. 53).27 Therefore, deferring responsibility for one’s opinion violates the law and challenges the logical evaluation abilities necessary to make life and death decisions. Many would argue that those deferring responsibility or making the decision through the use of heuristics is unfit for jury duty (Kirkpatrick & Epstein, 1992). And, as discussed, heuristics are more likely to be used when religion is salient. In short, this implies that religious material should be kept out of courts, avoiding the deference of responsibility and encouraging a complete understanding of the gravity of one’s decisions and verdicts. In brief, the evidence reviewed implies that emotional content, including religious material and/or symbols, can greatly affect the mental processes employed by individuals. Evidence denotes that jurors who feel religious salience are likely to use experiential thinking when delivering a sentence, make less rational decisions, and feel less personal responsibility for their decisions. While studies such as Bright and Goodman-Delahunty (2006) show the experiential effects of emotional content, Miller and Bornstein (2006) illustrate that experiential processing is also activated when jurors hear religious appeals from lawyers in court, inhibiting one’s ability to weigh aggravating and mitigating factors in court. This evidence demonstrates the hampering effects of religious content on the Caldwell v. Mississippi. (1985). 472 U.S. 320. In this case, the prosecutor advised the jury not to view itself as deciding whether or not the petitioner would die, because a death sentence would be reviewed for correctness by the Mississippi Supreme Court. Justice Marshall delivered the opinion of the court stating, “there are several reasons to fear substantial unreliability, as well as bias in favor of death sentences, when there are state-induced suggestions that the sentencing jury may shift its sense of responsibility to an appellate court” (Pp. 472 U. S. 330-334). 27 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 28 proper function of jury decisions and court structure. If religious material, such as defense attorney arguments, effectively sways juries and harms jurors’ abilities to think through trial evidence rationally and logically, then religious material actively compromises the just functioning of the United States jury system. Additionally, a juror's decreased feeling of responsibility for his/her verdict jeopardizes the jury pool and sentencing of the accused. Schemas in Social Perception: Ingroup/Outgroup Attitudes While several biases may be elicited through one’s use of heuristics and experiential processes, a different set of biases are ignited when the religious affiliation of a defendant is known. Studies by Van Pooijen & Lam (2007) as well as Kerr et al. (1995) suggest that social categorizations play a significant role in jury decision-making, stating that if the religion of the defendant is known to jurors, ingroup/outgroup biases may be activated. These biases can alter the way in which jurors judge a defendant’s character and behavior, resulting in harsher or more lenient punishments depending on whether or not the defendant is of the same religious group as the juror. These biases may cause outgroup punitiveness, similarity-leniency, or the black sheep effect. These three theories state that people often punish the outgroup, favor the ingroup, and/or punish the deviant ingroup. These theoretical tendencies are caused by personal prejudices and compromise one’s ability to judge other people and situations impartially. Frequently, these biases are utilized when fashioning a jury. The jury pool selection process, voir dire, provides attorneys with the opportunity to pick lenient jurors for their defendant. In the past, controversies have been ignited due to discrimination of P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 29 jurors based on race, gender, and religious affiliation.28 These discriminations take place due to the power and efficacy of ingroup/outgroup biases and the way in which they shape individual’s attitudes and behaviors towards others. Outgroup punitiveness. Outgroup punitiveness is one of the most common ingroup/outgroup biases perceived. According to the Social Identity Theory, people who identify with a group often show more favorable attitudes towards ingroup than outgroup members. This favoring enhances group members’ self esteem (e.g., Tajfel & Turner, 1986) and maintains a positive group image. But, at the same time, ingroup favoritism acts as an impetus for severe negative outgroup stereotypes, creating a feeling that one’s ingroup is unique and/or superior (Jackson & Hunsberger, 1999; Sidanius, 1993) and leads to outgroup hate and hostility (Brewer, 1999). In fact, both the Realistic Group Conflict Theory (RGCT)29 and Social Identity Theory (SIT)30 claim that members of all groups (e.g., religious, racial, team, etc.) are susceptible to prejudice against outgroup members. Frequently, outgroup hostility is magnified when a group feels threatened, increasing the need to maintain a positive image, often by acting against the outgroup. This can occur in court if a defendant’s actions are somehow perceived as upsetting or disrespectful to a juror’s culture or race. Superiority, fear and distrust of others, and social comparisons all lead to ingroup favoritism and outgroup punitiveness. Currently it is illegal, under the Supreme Court ruling of Batson v. Kentucky, to discriminate against jurors based on gender and race. Yet, whether or not Batson applies to religious discrimination is up to debate (Egland, 2004); See, U.S. Const. amend. XIV, § 1; Batson v. Kentucky, 476 E.S. 79, 89 (1986). In relation, in 2005 it was revealed that John Quatman, a prosecutor from San Jose, California, conspired with a judge to keep Jewish jurors off death penalty cases because he believed that Jews would never vote for the death penalty (CNN.com, 2005, as cited in Miller & Hayward, 2007). It was ruled that excluding jurors based on religion is illegal, and the trial was postponed. 29 The basic thesis of RGCT states that intergroup hostility is produced by competition and reduced by the existence of mutually desired goals, only attainable through intergroup cooperation. (Jackson, 1993). 30 SIT states that the pressures of positive ingroup evaluations leads social groups to differentiate themselves (Tajfel & Turner, 1986). 28 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 30 Additionally, instead of direct hate or distrust towards outgroup members, ingroup favoritism may develop through a lack of admiration and sympathy for outgroup members. Previous research has shown that outgroup members are, “less likely to be helped in certain ambiguous circumstances (Frey & Gaertner, 1986; Gaertner & Dovidio, 1977; Gaertner, Dovidio, & Johnson, 1982), more likely to be seen as provoking aggression (Baron, 1979, Roger & Prentice-Dunn, 1981), [and] less likely to receive the benefit of the doubt in attribution for negative behaviors (Weber, 1994)” (Brewer, 1999, pp. 438). Each of these studies indicate the detrimental effect of being in an outgroup. These disadvantages are particularly important when an outgroup member is in a place of judgment. It is clear that whenever a defendant is of an outgroup to members of the jury panel, they are at a natural and often unavoidable disadvantage.31 Several studies have indicated that outgroup prejudice is ignited by ingroup threat and/or a lack of sympathy for others (e.g., Jackson & Hunsberger, 1999; Taylor & Hosch, 2004; Hunsberger, 1995; Griffen et al., 1987; Kerr et al., 1995). One example can be seen in a study conducted by Daudistel et al. (1999). Daudistel et al. (1999) examined noncapital felony cases in Texas to assess possible relations between ethnicity, conviction 31 Effects of racial outgroup punitiveness and lack of outgroup sympathy are apparent throughout the United States legal history, with racial biases affecting verdicts in several states. One of the most recent is the case of Joe Sullivan, a 13 year old who was sentenced to life without the possibility of parole. A New York Times article states that, “according to court papers and a report from the Equal Justice Initiative, which now represents Mr. Sullivan, there are only eight people in the world who are serving sentences of life without parole for crimes they committed when they were 13…And there are only two people in that group whose crimes did not involve a killing. Both are in Florida, and both are black.” Additionally, no biological evidence was ever presented in court. “When Mr. Sullivan’s new lawyers recently sought to conduct DNA testing on it, they were told that the state had destroyed it in 1993” (Liptak, 2009). Considering the overwhelming lack of evidence presented against the defendant, the extraordinary circumstances of the crime, and evidence that only black criminals have been convicted, the Supreme Court is currently deciding on whether or not this case is of cruel and unusual punishment, due to the likelihood of racial influence on the court’s decision. This case is just one example of outgroup punitiveness in United States court rulings. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 31 rates, and sentences. Regardless of the crime type, Daudistel et al. (1999) found that defendants’ ethnicities were correlated with the length of sentences rendered by juries. Sentences for Anglo defendants were approximately twice as long as Hispanic defendants’ sentences, with the sentences for Anglos rising as more Hispanics were added to the jury. 32 The correlation between rising Anglo sentences and the number of Hispanic jury members points directly to outgroup biases. The Realistic Group Conflict and Social Identity theories apply to religious affiliation as well. Religion often roots individuals to an ingroup, creating group identification, that is, a tendency to make one’s group affiliation part of one’s identity (Deaux, 1996).33 As such, maintaining the belief that one’s religion teaches the absolute truth and represents the ideal morality, religion may contribute to ingroup preference and generate prejudicial beliefs towards the outgroup. In many cases, one may feel as though his/her religious morals are being challenged by a defendant’s poor behavior and react with greater hostility towards the defendant. Or, one may feel a personal bond with the accused and want to support and protect them, as they share the same traditions and faith. The plethora of data on the effects of ingroup/outgroup attitudes directly highlights the dangers in revealing the religious affiliation of a defendant, as it is one more way in which one may affiliate themselves with an ingroup or outgroup. A study by Pfeifer (1999) illustrated this dilemma. By creating a mock trial where the defendant was either allegedly or admittedly participating in a satanic cult or no mention of religion was made, Pfeifer (1999) found that the ratings of guilt were significantly affected by the mention of cult involvement, with convictions increasing 32 33 As cited in Taylor et al. (2004, p. 587). As cited in Jackson & Hunsberger (1999). P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 32 with the mention of the minority religion. 34 Pfeifer (1999) claims these results indicate a prejudicial reaction against minority religions, demonstrating the impact of negative perceptions on jury decision-making. These biases are clearly of issue within courtrooms when a defendant’s religious affiliation is known to the jury, as this information may elicit outgroup biases, resulting in longer sentences and harsher perceptions of the defendant’s behavior and character. Similarity-leniency hypothesis. Biases towards the outgroup are not the only significant prejudices in play. As stated by Brewer (1999), “discrimination can be motivated solely by ingroup preference, in the absence of any negative affect or hostile intent toward outgroups” (p. 431). As outlined by the Social Identity Theory, ingroup biases are extremely influential and powerful. SIT states that individuals wish to be viewed in a positive light. Positive perception can be gained in two ways: by behaving more favorably towards the ingroup or by behaving negatively towards deviant ingroup members. Leniency towards the ingroup is known as the similarity-leniency hypothesis, while negativity towards the ingroup is coined the black sheep effect. In the context of jury trials, this could indicate longer or shorter sentences for ingroup members, altering the way in which the defendant is perceived and judged. The similarity-leniency hypothesis states that individuals favor those who are similar to themselves, or “the more similar a juror and the defendant are, the more lenient the juror is likely to be” (Kerr et al., 1995, p. 546). All research on the similarityleniency hypothesis draws analogous conclusions, whether it is through similarities of language (Stephan & Stephan, 1986), political beliefs (Amato, 1979; Griffitt & Jackson, 34 Regardless of whether the cult involvement was alleged or admitted. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 33 1973), or lifestyle (Griffitt & Jackson 1973).35 In each case, the jurors are less punitive when the defendants are more similar. Brewer (1999) suggests that ingroup favoritism is the result of human groupliving evolution, which she categorizes as the fundamental survival strategy that characterizes humans. The result is the development of obligator interdependence, indicating that we must rely on others for information, help, and resources, and must be willing to share with others (Brewer, 1997; Caporael, 1997). This creates a system of benefits, costs, and mutual cooperation (Brewer, 1999). Creating clear ingroup boundaries provides a mechanism through which one can achieve benefits without substantial costs. By limiting help to those in one’s ingroup, risks of nonreciprocation are greatly reduced. Additionally, it creates communities of mutual trust and altruistic behavior. In short, those within the ingroup expect to be treated well by other ingroup members, eliciting and expecting altruistic behavior. As she states: To the extent that all groups discriminate between intragroup social behavior and intergroup behavior, it is in a sense universally true that ‘we’ are more peaceful, trustworthy, friendly, and honest than ‘they.’ This is reinforced by a general preference for the familiar over the unfamiliar. Social interactions within the ingroup are more predictable and understood than intergroup interactions (p. 435). An expectation for contingent altruistic behavior is what promotes attraction towards one’s ingroup, creating a cycle of ingroup favoritism. 35 As cited in Kerr et al. (1995). P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 34 Due to the altruistic origins of ingroup behaviors, similarity-leniency has been observed in several court cases. Many attorneys utilize this hypothesis by picking jurors similar to the defendant in hopes that they will be more lenient in their sentencing. Gorbert and Jorden (1985) suggest that one should select jurors similar to the defendant because they will have had similar life experiences and are more likely to understand the actions of defendant, possibly excusing their behavior. 36 This same reasoning can be applied to religious affiliation; if the religion of the juror and defendant are the same, and the defendant’s religion is known, similarity-leniency may be used by jurors. In these situations, ingroup members directly benefit from ingroup altruism. In an effort to test the similarity-leniency hypothesis, Kerr et al. (1995) designed a study in which the participant’s religion was crossed with the defendant’s religion as well as the strength of evidence against the defendant. Kerr et al. (1995) hypothesized that juror-defendant similarity would produce greater leniency toward the accused when the evidence was weak, but greater harshness when evidence was strong. This relationship reversal was expected due to the black sheep effect, anticipating harsh treatment of deviant ingroup members. Experimenters recruited 66 all male Christian and Jewish participants, recommended as strongly religious persons by colleagues and peers. Each participant considered four case summaries: two with strong prosecution, two with weak prosecution, two involved Christian defendants, and two involved Jewish defendants, recommending a verdict and ancillary judgments for each case. 37 Results followed a consistent pattern with the similarity-leniency hypothesis: Jewish mock jurors were less As cited in Kerr et al. (1995). Each case summary depicted a child molestation case because the topic was strongly “counternormative” for both religious groups (Kerr et al., 1995, p. 550). 36 37 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 35 likely to convict Jewish defendants than Christian defendants, whereas the opposite was true for Christian participants. This effect was not altered by the strength of evidence against the defendant.38 These results perfectly parallel the similarity-leniency hypothesis, showing favorable verdicts for those of a similar ingroup. Several other studies have mimicked these findings, such as a study done by Jackson & Hunsberger (1999) where religious individuals measured positive attitudes towards religious others and negative attitudes towards non-religious others. Inspection of the mean attitude levels revealed that attitudes towards Christians and believers were well above the scale midpoint, whereas attitudes towards atheists and non-believers were below the midpoint. Each of these studies highlights the positive effects of ingroup favoritism as well as the negative effects of outgroup punitiveness. Kerr et al. (1995) depict the benefits of ingroup altruism and ingroup leniencies, while also showing signs of outgroup punitiveness. Similarly, Jackson & Hunsberger (1999) clearly portray one’s liking of similar others and dislike for those in a religious outgroup. Yet, simply being in one’s ingroup does not ensure lenient evaluations. If an ingroup member is perceived negatively by threatening the ingroup image, they may suffer from the black sheep effect. Black sheep effect. As stated earlier, ingroup favoritism is maintained through a positive group image; individuals identify themselves through their respective communities, and therefore strive to maintain its integrity (Tajfel & Turner, 1986). So what happens if an ingroup member threatens this vision? In situations such as these, the black sheep effect comes into play. First coined by Marques and Yzerbyt (Marques, This goes against the black sheep effect, which suggests that ingroup members punish deviant ingroup members. According to this theory, as the strength of evidence increased, conviction of ingroup members should have increased as well. 38 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 36 Yzerbyt, & Leyens, 1988; Marques & Yzerbyt, 1988; Marques, 1990), the black sheep effect suggests that deviant ingroup members will be punished more harshly than those in the outgroup who commit the same crime.39 If the defendant is of the same religious affiliation, but the juror does not feel as though they are upholding the core values of the religion, the juror is likely to negatively evaluate the defendant (e.g., Biernat, Vescio, and Billings 1999; Wilder, Simon and Faith, 1996).40 Consequently, those in the ingroup may receive harsher punishment. The black sheep effect may be caused by ingroup member’s familiarity as well as one’s awareness of their ingroup affiliation. It is suggested that those who are similar serve as the best self-comparisons, driving the desire to dissociate oneself from the deviant actor (Taylor & Mette, 1971). Additionally, research shows that negative ingroup behavior is more significant to other ingroup members. In fact, research indicates that criminals may be perceived as dishonoring their entire ingroup (Lauderdale et al., 1984; Marques, 1990). This issue may be particularly relevant with religious ingroups/outgroups due to the moral issues at hand in court. As such, deviant ingroup members are often seen as a threat to the betterment of the ingroup’s status and beliefs. In order to further investigate the role of the black sheep effect in the courtroom, Johnson (1984) ran a study to test the relevance and effect of a criminal’s religion as well as religious defense statements. During the study, groups of participants (up to seven people) watched a child abuse trial and were asked to judge the accused as guilty or not guilty. For half of the participants, one line was added to the defense attorney’s argument: Accordingly, Kerr et al. (1995) writes, “an unlikable or deviant ingroup member represents a threat to this positive ingroup image, and should therefore be derogated more than an equally unlikable or deviant person who poses no such threat” (p. 547). 40 As cited in Kenworthy (2003). 39 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 37 “Donne’s minister said that Donne is a fine Christian man who follows the word of God as stated in the Bible in conducting all of his family affairs and, thus, must be a good father” (p. 215). For the remainder of the participants, no mention of religion was made. After the video, participants were given transcripts of the tape and had 10 minutes to review and reach a common verdict, which they reported on a questionnaire. Results indicated that participants were more likely to find the defendant guilty in the religiousdefense condition than in the non-religious defense condition. In parallel, those in the religious-defense condition recommended longer sentences than those in the control condition. The degree of recommended sentence was found to be significantly related to Christian-right orientation, church attendance, and the sex of the participants. Christians Rightists were more severe in their recommended sentences to fellow Christians, but only when religion was used as a defense. Johnson (1984) suggests that this is due to Christians’ expectations and demands that people who claim to be part of their religious community conduct themselves in a respectable way. These results mirror the expectations of the black sheep effect, showing that devoted members of an ingroup will negatively and harshly evaluate deviant members of their ingroup. The black sheep effect can be observed in many other cases as well, including a study by Belli (1963) in which Jewish jurors assigned harsher verdicts to fellow Jews than non-Jews. Additionally, similar studies also show that as strength of evidence increases, so does ingroup punitivenss. One example is the second 1995 study by Kerr et al. where black mock jurors and white mock jurors suggested harsher verdicts for those of the same race when the evidence against the defendant was strong. As such, the presence of strong evidence acted as a catalyst for the black sheep effect, showing that jurors will judge P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 38 ingroup members harshly under certain conditions. Strong evidence increases the need to maintain a positive ingroup image and increases the evidence of deviance within the ingroup, calling for harsher punishment. In summary, ingroup/outgroup biases can often alter how a defendant is judged. Religious biases may interfere when the religious affiliation of the offender is known. These prejudicial attitudes may be expressed in outgroup punitiveness, similarityleniency, and the black sheep effect. Each of these theories and hypotheses correlate with the Realistic Group Conflict Theory and Social Identity Theory, which claim that members of groups are susceptible to prejudice against outgroup members in an effort to uphold positive ingroup perception and self-esteem. Conclusively, the impact of a juror’s religious affiliation on jury perception and decision-making is immense. Simply being aware of one’s religion can greatly alter a juror’s perception of the case, the necessary punishment for the accused, as well as the character of the defendant. These biases can thwart proper decision-making, resulting in prejudiced and personal evaluations, rather than rational and judicial decisions. This concept will be discussed further in the subsequent chapter. Today, evidence of a defendant’s religion is often accepted as evidence of his/her character, and while this is troublesome for several reasons, this evidence may further bias the jury due to their own religious beliefs and preconceived notions regarding religious persons, resulting in improper and partial sentencing (Miller & Bornstein, 2005; Miller & Hayward, 2007). P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 39 The Intricacies of Religious Groups Considering the highly influential effects of ingroup and outgroup biases along with experiential thinking and the use of heuristics, researchers have attempted to decipher what religious groups and individuals are most susceptible to prejudicial beliefs. But, more often than not, researchers have found that religion is far too complex to systematically divide by religion, and that gathering an understanding of what an individual believes by looking at one’s religious teachings is nearly impossible. With consideration given to religious interpretation as well as the influence of one’s religious community and personal relationships within that community, it has been determined that understanding an individual’s beliefs by the label of one’s religion is impossible. Religion is a matter of interpretation for many believers, analyzing and reading into the meanings of holy texts. Interpretation is highly influential in death penalty court cases, as most religious texts address the topics of murder, crime, life and death. As Egland (2004) writes: Although religious texts are not amendable, they are subject to interpretation by any number of ministers, rabbis, and imams, each of whom potentially could advocate a different interpretation. Indeed, 12 Christian jurors could have 12 different interpretations of a particular Biblical passage or rely on any number of different passages to find support for, or opposition to, the death penalty. The same critique applies to the Qur’an (p. 341). Young (1992) concurs, pointing out Biblical evidence that could both support and condemn use of the death penalty. In the legal world, prosecutors often employ quotes P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 40 from the Bible to assert support for retribution (using quotes such as “an eye for an eye,”41 suggesting equal punishment for the strength of the crime, along with a similar quote, “[h]e that smiteth a man, so that he die, shall surely be put to death,”42 in clear espousal of the death penalty) while defense attorneys quote the Bible to promote mercy (often directly contradicting a prosecutor’s use of “an eye for an eye” by quoting, “You have heard that it [has been] said ‘[an] [e]ye for an eye and [a] tooth for [a] tooth.’ But I tell you…[i]f someone strikes you on the right cheek, turn…the other also”43). While both prosecutors and attorneys use the Bible to persuade jurors, both are able to find conflicting opinions within the text. There are conflicting arguments presented in the Bible and several other religious texts. Evidently, one cannot assume that individuals in the same religious group share the same outlook on the death penalty, or any other moral matter. Many religions support both sides of the argument, indicating that one’s outlook on moral issues may be shaped by, but not determined, by one’s religion. Moreover, it has been suggested that one’s religious beliefs are as affected by the religion itself as the environment in which the beliefs are taught. Bjarnason and Welch (2004) tested the strength of Catholic parishioners’ attitudes towards capital punishment on congregants, looking into the formation of moral judgments in a religious congregation. Seeing as the Catholic Church has become a vocal opponent of the death penalty, Bjarnason and Welch (2004) expected to observe greater disdain for the death penalty within congregants who maintain an active and positive relationship with the Exodus 21:24 (NIV); See e.g., Thompson v. State, 581 So. 2d 1216 1243 (Ala. Crim. App. 1991); People v. Wash, 861 P.2d 1107, 1134 n.18 (Ca. 1993); Greene, 469 S.E.2d at 141; Hammond v. State, 452 S.E.2d 745, 753 (Ga. 1995); State v. Shurn, 866 S.W.2d 477, 464 (Mo. 1993). As cited by Miller & Bornstein, 2005, p. 33 42 Exodus 21:12 (King James); See e.g., Wash, 861 P.2d at 1134 n.18; People v. Mahaffey, 651 N.E.2d 1055, 106869 (Ill.1995); Williams, 510 S.E.2d at 642. As cited by Miller & Bornstein, 2005, p. 33. 43 Matthew 5:38-39 (NIV) (quoting Exodus 21:24). As cited by Miller & Bornstein, 2005, p. 37. 41 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 41 parish, regardless of the attitude perceived through the Biblical text, and when the parish has strong feelings against capital punishment. Results of the study indicated that the effect of the moral message of the church depends upon the salience, direction, and content of an individual’s religious beliefs. Results also directly reflected the expected results: it was found that congregant’s support of the death penalty correlated with the personal opposition by the priest, and their personal relationship to the priest. Yet, the correlation is not so simple, as there are a multitude of factors that may affect one’s relationship to a church and priest. Factors listed by the experimenters that may influence this relationship are one’s gender, race, class, the characteristics of the parish, the region in the United States, involvement of the priest with the parish community, the parish priests’ personal opinions, and strength of one’s personal relationship with God. Clearly, the relationship one maintains to one’s religious group is complex and personal, and currently impossible to accurately evaluate or test. For this reason, Bjarnason and Welch (2004) note that their data does not fully account for differences in death penalty attitudes across parishes, citing that seemingly contextual effects (such as a congregation’s geographical location, racial composition, and the involvement and attitude of the priest) can be a consequence of individual differences between parishes. Still, this study helps demonstrate the overwhelming number of factors that contribute to the intricate assemblage of one’s religious, political, and moral attitudes and prejudices. These studies portray the complexities of religious thought, and the multitude of opinions present within each religious group (e.g., Egland, 2004; Young, 1992; Bjarnason & Welch, 2004). Egland (2004) argues that the flexible nature of religious thought and P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 42 “religious legal interpretation makes it unsuitable for application in the modern American justice system,” as it is still unclear how to decipher what individuals believe (Egland, 2004, p.341). Today, many consider it unacceptable to remove persons from jury duty because of their religious affiliation,44 and according to the research discussed in this chapter, that action would clearly be ineffective due to the multitude of beliefs held by members within each religion. Yet, discovering a way to inhibit the expression of prejudicial beliefs and biases in jury pools is still of utmost importance. If possible, outgroup and ingroup biases as well as the use of heuristic and experiential thinking must be discouraged. One additional way in which to address this issue is through measurements of one’s religious orientation: extrinsic, intrinsic, quest, or fundamental. Measuring Religious Prejudice Frustrated by the inability to understand prejudice by religious affiliation, several scales have been designed, including the Intrinsic and Extrinsic Orientation scale and Baton’s Quest scale, in attempts to solve psychologists’ questions regarding what characteristics make one prejudice, and from where the prejudice evolves. Most scales have significant conflicting evidence and have continued to be molded in an effort to create an accurate measurement; while one researcher finds supporting evidence of the scale’s validity and accuracy, another researcher reports conflicting findings. By Several courts and cases have decided that under the laws of Batson and J.E.B, religion-based peremptory challenges are unconstitutional. See Casarez v. State, No. 1114-93, 1994 WL 695868 (Tex. Crim. App. Dec. 14, 1994). See United States v. Greer, 939 F.2d 1076, 1086 n.9 (5th Cir. 1991). Then again, the majority of courts have ruled the religion-based peremptory challenges are indeed legal. See e.g., United States v. Clemmons, 892 F.2d 1153, 1157 (3d Circ. 1989); People v. Malone, 570 N.E.2d 584, 588-89. 44 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 43 examining the history of these religion-prejudice scales, one is able to observe the immense difficulty and complexity in measure one’s beliefs. Extrinsic versus intrinsic orientation. In an attempt to better understand the link between religion and prejudice and formalize the concern surrounding several oversimplified measures of religion and prejudice (example: “how often do you go to church?”), Allport and Ross (1967) created the Intrinsic and Extrinsic Orientation Scales. By distinguishing between two religious orientations, extrinsic (E) as well as intrinsic (I), Allport and Ross (1967) created scales by which to measure these concepts. Extrinsic orientation acts as a self-serving approach that conforms to social conventions, often utilizing “security, comfort, status, or social support.” In opposition, intrinsic orientation uses religion as a framework through which life is understood (Allport & Ross, 1967, p. 441; Herek, 1987); “persons with this orientation find their master motive in religion” (p. Allport & Ross, 1967, 434). Allport and Ross (1967) summarized these concepts by saying, “the extrinsically motivated person uses his religion, whereas the intrinsically motivated lives his religion” (p. 434). Initial findings by Allport and Ross (1967) concluded that more intrinsic persons were less prejudiced than extrinsic persons. They claimed that the lack of prejudice found in intrinsic persons lies in their use of religion in everyday interactions, leaving “no place for rejection, contempt, or condescension” towards others, while extrinsic persons use religion as means for social acceptance and assimilation (p. 441). In short, Allport and Ross (1967) believed that prejudice is part of one’s personality structure, intertwined with P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 44 one’s religious orientation.45 A few years later, a study by Donahue (1985) found similar results (along with studies by Gorsuch & Aleshire, 1974 and Ponton & Gorsuch, 1988), concluding that extrinsic orientation was indeed positively correlated with prejudice and dogmatism, whereas intrinsic orientation was not. Yet, in a similar study by Donahue’s (1985), findings were not nearly as strong as Allport and Ross (1967), and pointed out several internal inconsistencies within the E scale, as well as no correlation between the I scale and prejudice (rather than a negative correlation as suggested by Allport and Ross (1967)). Moreover, several studies directly contradicted the findings of Allport and Ross (1967) with results showing a positive correlation between the I scale and prejudice against gays and lesbians (e.g., Batson and Burris, 1994; Hunsberger, 1995; Herek, 1987) and persons of religious outgroups (e.g., Griffen, Gorsuch, and Davis, 1987; McFarland, 1989). One study that investigated the role of intrinsic and extrinsic beliefs on attitudes towards gays, run by Herek (1987), hypothesized that “attitudes towards outgroups serve different psychological functions for persons with extrinsic and intrinsic orientations” (p. 34). In short, Herek, as well as several others (e.g., Donahue, 1985; Hunsberger, 1995), came to the conclusion that the I and E scales did not sufficiently measure one’s prejudicial attitudes due to the intricacies of prejudice and discrimination. In fact, Altemeyer (1996) stated that the I and E scales, “plainly failed to measure what they were supposed to measure…[and] they plainly failed to show what they were supposed to show” (p. 154). In brief, the question of what characterizes prejudicial beliefs was left unanswered. As cited in Spilka et al. (2003), it may be argued that this is part of the reason religion and prejudice is so difficult to measure. 45 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 45 Quest (Q) scale. In response, attempts have been made to reinterpret the I and E scales. For example, Batson (1993) argued that the element of social desirability may have interfered in the measurements, confusing the relationship between prejudice and IE religiosity. Intrinsic persons may appear to be less prejudiced simply because they are concerned with their social standing (Spilka et al., 2003).46 Upon further investigation, Batson, Naifeh, and Pate (1978) found that when one controls for social desirability, a positive correlation between intrinsic religion and prejudice is found. Moreover, they reported a link between I scores and social desirability.47 In response, Batson (1993) created a three factor model, known as the Quest (Q) Scale, building upon the I-E scales put forth by Allport and Ross (1967). The additional quest scale incorporates an open and flexible approach to religious issues, fulfilling the need to address open-mindedness. Batson has argued that higher scores on the Q scale are negatively correlated to prejudice. This claim was further proven through additional studies by Batson (Batson et al., 1978; Batson et al. 1986). Additionally, research has found an association between the Q scale and increased complexity of thought (Batson & Raynor-Prince, 1983) and openness to different perspectives (McFarland & Warren, 1992). But again, as with Allport and Ross’ I-E scale, conflicting evidence has been found concerning the Q scale. Ponton and Gorsuch (1988) were unable to replicate the findings of a negative correlation with prejudice and Altemeyer (1996) challenged the Hunsberger & Jackson (2005) concurs, writing, “People high in intrinsic religiosity may try…to reject or hide other forms of prejudice…also in order to maintain their self-image as good religious group members” (p. 819). 47 It is important to note that there is substantial evidence contradicting Batson et al. (1978). Hunsberger & Platonow (1986) as well as Spilka, Kojetin, & McIntosh (1985) were unable to find a correlation between I scores and social desirability. Moreover, Duck & Hunsberger (1999) along with Morris, Hood, & Watson (1989) found that controlling for social desirability did not alter its relationship to prejudice. This data further indicates the tenuous and complex relationship between I-E orientation and prejudicial beliefs. 46 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 46 scale on psychometric grounds. Due to the conflicting evidence, several Q scales have been formed, with additional questions tweaked in an effort to gain more accurate results. Even so, a concrete and trustworthy Q scale is yet to be created, with several versions of the measurements being used by researchers today. Certainly, it is easy to see that the measurements of religiosity and prejudice are contradictory and weak. Several of the models created by researchers have been challenged by later studies, and confusion persists regarding what the best characteristics are to focus on. Should one focus on extrinsic and intrinsic orientations or on one’s religious group and religious leader? Unfortunately, these questions remain unanswered. The one scale that has produced convincing and frequently replicated results, is the fundamentalism scale. Fundamentalism. Over the years, several studies have pointed to the connection between fundamentalist beliefs and higher levels of prejudice (e.g., Altemeyer & Hunsberger, 1992; Hunsberger, 1996; Kirkpatrick, 1993; Jackson & Hunsberger, 1999). The term “religious fundamentalism” (RF) has often been used to express an idea of dogmatism, orthodoxy, and a traditional way of thought. However, the definition of fundamentalism is obtuse, and can be defined in several ways. For the specific use of religious prejudicial studies, Altemeyer and Hunsberger (1992) created a definition of RF that is theoretically separate from other aspects of religion, characterizing it as: The belief that there is one set of religious teachings that clearly contains the fundamental, basic, intrinsic, essential, inerrant truth about humanity and deity; that this essential truth is fundamentally opposed by forces of evil which must be P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 47 vigorously fought; that this truth must be followed today according to the fundamental, unchangeable practices of the past; and that those who believe and follow these fundamental teachings have a special relationship with the deity (p. 118). This definition of fundamentalism applies to current research and is applicable to almost all world religions (Spilka et al., 2003). Additionally, Altemeyer and Hunsberger (1992) created a RF scale to measure their concept of fundamentalism, which has been applied in several subsequent studies, providing an individual participant level of fundamental religious beliefs. Many explain fundamentalist prejudice by its correlation to right-wing authoritarianism (RWA) (e.g., Altemeyer & Hunsberger 1992; Hunsberger, 1995; Laythe et al., 2002). This correlation has been found among members of varying religious backgrounds, including Christians, Jews, Muslims, and Hindus (Hunsberger, 1996), as well as several cultures.48 Altemeyer (1996) has defined RWA as a composite of three attitudinal clusters: authoritarian submission, authoritarian aggression, and conventionalism. High scores in RF and RWA indicate several forms of prejudice, including egocentrism and prejudice towards homosexuals, all associated with negative outgroup feelings (e.g., Laythe et al., 2002; Altemeyer, 1996; Hunsberger, 1996). Additionally, subsequent research has revealed a negative correlation between Altemeyer’s (1996) authoritarian conditions and relativism (one’s ability to look at Research in Ghana has found that RWA and RF are associated in Christian and Muslim subsamples (Hunsberger, Owusu, & Duck, 1999; as cited in Spilka et al., 2003). 48 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 48 situations from several points of view).49 As such, it is frequently argued that right-wing authoritarian attitudes promote animosity towards outgroups, indicating an inability to look at a situation from another person’s point of view (Altemeyer, 1996; Altemeyer & Hunsberger, 1992; Hunsberger, 1995). Seen as a personality characteristic, authoritarianism and fundamentalism share an unquestioning nature. It has been suggested that those high in RF cling to existing stereotypes and are unable or unwilling to change their current schemas in accordance with new information (Spilka et al, 2003). Research has indicated that fundamentalists do not have complex thoughts regarding religion, and instead stick to the information they have been taught. Many psychologists assert that the mindset of fundamentalism creates a substantial distinction between ingroups and outgroups. “[Fundamentalist] beliefs make it easier…to divide the world into ‘us’ and ‘them’,” writes Unger (2002, p. 43). The black and white, teachings of fundamentalism foster ingroup tendencies and greater outgroup discrimination. As discussed earlier, outgroup prejudice is also influenced by strong ingroup identification, especially within religious groups. As such, those who score higher on RF scales have stronger ingroup identification, and therefore a higher chance of maintaining negative attitudes towards an outgroup (Hunsberger & Jackson, 2005).50 McHoskey (1996) as cited in Laythe et al., (2002). Research has also been completed indicating that the reduction of fundamentalism decreases prejudice. A study by Billiet (1995) argued that in a sample of Flemish Catholics, “sociocultural Christianity” prevented fundamentalism and ethnocentrism. Sociocultural Christianity was defined as “the values of solidarity, charity, and social justice, which have been emphasized in the legitimations and the collective identity” (p.231). Billiet (1995) proposed that when certain faiths values are taught, in this case sociocultural Christianity, enthnocentrism and RF may be counteracted. The article states that Flemish “Catholic church leaders and prominent Catholics declared openly that they favored this integration of immigrants” (p. 232). 49 50 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 49 As mentioned briefly before, Jackson and Hunsberger (1999) created a study to examine the link between religiosity and attitudes towards religious and non-religious others. The study analyzed prejudicial beliefs held by devout Christians (those who scored high numbers on the RF scale or Christian orthodoxy scale). Participants completed a questionnaire assessing religiosity as well as group identification. Results of the study revealed that religious group identification was significantly correlated with RF and Christian orthodoxy. Furthermore, both RF and Christian orthodoxy were positively correlated with positive attitudes towards Christians and believers, and negatively correlated with atheists and non-believers. This, writes Jackson and Hunsberger (1999) is especially indicative of fundamentalism’s association with outgroup derogation and group identification ties to ingroup favoritism.51 These results mirror the hypothesis that fundamentalism is often associated with prejudicial beliefs due to its strength of ingroup identification, verifying the claim by Unger (2002) that fundamentalism greatly defines ingroups from outgroups, “us” versus “them”.52 The biased attitudes often associated with fundamentalism are ever-present in court decisions today, especially in death penalty trials. Young (1992) and Miller and Hayward (2007) suggest that fundamentalist beliefs are in direct correlation with support 51 Jackson and Hunsberger (1999) also stated that while not all members of a group maintain identical forms of prejudice (as discussed in the previous section), the form of outgroup prejudice is originated in intergroup relations. Consequently, those who identify most strongly with their ingroup are likely to have pervasive and cohesive outgroup discriminations, following the Social Identity Theory discussed earlier. This pattern of ingroup strength and outgroup prejudice can be observed in the strict and deep-seated teachings of religious fundamentalism. 52 These findings also lead one to an even greater question, which is whether or not religious prejudice is tied to religion itself or the way in which one believes. Seeing as fundamentalism is found in almost every religion, and all around the world, one may argue that fundamentalism is set in they ways in which one believes, not the title of one’s religious affiliation or even the teachings of the specific religion. Instead, it is the way in which one interprets and follows religion which defines fundamentalism. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 50 for the death penalty, with fundamentalist individuals experiencing less cognitive dissonance about the appropriateness of such a punishment. “Fundamentalists tend to deny the possibility of moral relativity,” writes Young (1992). “Such absolutism, whether a cause or a consequence of fundamental beliefs, is likely to be associated with the perception of considerable evil in the world, for the morality of human action is not to be judged relative to social context” (p.78). Here, Young addresses the problem of absolutism in fundamental beliefs, the rigid context in which life is often evaluated, and one’s inability to separate the sin from the sinner.53 Sayyid Qutb, the creator of fundamental Islam, preached the traditional reading of the Qur’an, banning any form of interpretation. Islam just is, he says. (Euben, 1999). This closed-minded attitude associated with fundamental beliefs can greatly alter how crimes are perceived. If everything in the world is seen as black and white, good or bad, it seems sensible that fundamentalists would more easily convict others for crimes, and feel less internal conflict about sentencing someone to death. The plethora of research regarding fundamentalist beliefs and thought processes poses an interesting problem to the United States court system and jury selection. According to these studies, individuals who score high on RF have innate ingroup preferences and outgroup discrimination. Moreover, and most importantly, those with high RF are unable to change their opinions and ideas, incorporating new information into pre-set religious schemas instead of accommodating religious doubts or new information (Spilka et al., 2003). This poses a direct threat to the functioning of a fair jury hearing, seeing as fundamentalist jurors would likely be unable to impartially 53 This issue is also discussed in Spilka et al. (2003) in terms of fundamentalist attitudes towards homosexuals. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 51 consider a court case. Another argument to be made is that fundamentalists would likely put their religious morals above the law, seeing as fundamentalism is often viewed as a set of laws in itself. This would inhibit jurors from properly following judge’s instructions and other legal protocol. Chapter Summary To conclude this chapter, evidence indicates that religion is frequently correlated with both morality and prejudice, creating a web of complexity around the issues of religious biases. Furthermore, experiential and heuristic systems are activated by religious content, inhibiting effortful thought processes and logical assessments of information. As seen in Miller and Bornstein (2006), this cursory system jeopardizes rational decision making within courtrooms when religion is salient to jurors, specifically when religious material is used as a defense. Furthermore, knowledge of a defendant’s religion can spark ingroup/outgroup biases including outgroup punitiveness, similarityleniency, as well as the black sheep effect. These biases can directly help or hinder the outcome for the accused, regardless of the evidence presented or arguments made within the trial. Moreover, identifying individual’s religious biases is a complex process. It is impossible to know one’s beliefs based on his/her religious title. Additionally, several religious orientation scales, such as the intrinsic, extrinsic, and quest scales, have failed to create consistent correlation evidence between orientations and prejudicial attitudes. The one scale that has produced consistent evidence is the religious fundamentalism scale, which has repeatedly shown a connection between fundamentalism, right-wing P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 52 authoritarianism, support for the death penalty and prejudice against several racial, political, and religious outgroups. This evidence demonstrates the implications of juror biases and the likely presence of biases within jury pools today. Regulating the presence and expression of personal prejudices is essential to fair and impartial trials. Furthermore, findings discussed in this chapter highlight the possibility of religious salience increasing the expression of religious biases in court. While psychologists have gained a considerable understanding of how religious material affects fundamentalists, the effect on nonfundamentalists remains unclear. Seeing as religious content can elicit emotions and biases, one may argue that it is necessary to rid of religious content from within courtrooms in the United States. In the following study, perspective taking will be utilized in an effort to reduce juror biases as well determine whether bias exists within the jury pool. Additionally, juror reactions to religious symbols are evaluated to see if the presence of religion alters sentencing, the decision-making process, and expression of biases. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 53 Chapter Two: The Role of Perspective Taking in Reducing Juror Biases and the Effects of Religious Symbols “The life of almost any unfortunate, if rightly understood, can be readjusted to some plan of order and system, instead of left to drift on to ruin, the victim of ignorance, hatred and chance”-Darrow (1940, p.494). This chapter will outline a study designed to investigate the effects of religious symbols and perspective taking in courtrooms. This study aims to assess whether religious symbols and instructions to take the perspective of the defendant will alter participant’s levels of rational and experiential processing and in turn, how participants come to their sentencing decisions. Furthermore, this study aims to determine whether perspective taking can inhibit the expression of biases and experiential thoughts. Additionally it investigates individual differences, such as one’s religious identification, religious affiliation and reported devotionalism, which may affect verdicts and interact with rational and experiential thought processes. Specifically this research aims to answer the following questions: 1) Do participants hold inherent religious biases? 2) Are sentences reduced when perspective taking is present? Do directives of perspective taking reduce any religious biases present? 3) Are ingroup/outgroup biases provoked by knowledge of the religion of the defendant, altering verdicts? 4) Does the presence of a holy book and religious symbols increase experiential thinking as seen through REI measures? 5) Do directives to take the perspective of the defendant increase rational thinking in jurors? 6) How does thinking experientially versus rationally affect the verdict and how people come to their decisions? Do jurors depend on instincts over facts when thinking experientially? And finally, 7) do P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 54 the religious conditions affect the way participants view themselves, as seen through measures of Religious Identification and Devotionalism? If so, how does this effect decision-making? Through experimental manipulation, this study will inform the courts about the effects of religious symbols in the courtroom and the role of perspective taking in inhibiting heuristic thinking and biased actions. METHOD Participants 133 participants (50 men and 83 women) were recruited from Amherst College and a local Amherst coffee shop. People participated in the study in exchange for course credit or $5 compensation. Only six participants from the coffee shop were included, and therefore separate tests were not run between the Amherst College and Amherst community participant groups. Materials Participants received one of six packets containing general instructions, a case summary, judge’s instructions, and a brief questionnaire. The questionnaire was constructed to measure the participants’ levels of religious devotion and affiliation to their respective religious group, ingroup and outgroup biases, levels of rational or experiential thinking, and their use or nonuse of perspective taking during the trial. All groups were given the same judge’s instructions and questionnaire. Half of the groups were instructed to take the perspective of the defendant through an additional paragraph, while the other half was not. Additionally, 1/3rd of the participants received a trial P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 55 summary with a Christian defendant, 1/3rd received a Sikh defendant, and the last 1/3rd received a control defendant where no religious affiliation or symbols were mentioned. Trial summary. The written trial summary described the penalty phase of a man convicted on charges of first-degree rape, first-degree sodomy, first-degree sexual abuse and second-degree assault. The summary was approximately 560 words long. It was based loosely on the case against Jose Santos who was sentenced 10 to 20 years in prison on December 20, 1995 for beating and raping his estranged wife.54 The crime was manipulated through the inclusion of alleged spying by the husband, with mention of the husband’s tattoo. It was also altered by the name and religion of the defendant as well as the swearing in procedure. In a third of the packets, the trial highlighted a defendant of Christian beliefs. The trial began with the defendant swearing in on a Bible, reciting, “I swear to tell the truth, the whole truth, and nothing but the truth, so help me God.” Additionally, mention of the defendant’s tattoo of a Cross was present in the case summary. This defendant’s name was Paul Matthews. Another third highlighted a defendant of Sikh beliefs. The trial began with the defendant swearing in on the Guru Granth Sahib reciting, “I swear to tell the truth, the whole truth, and nothing but the truth, so help me God.” Additionally, mention of the defendant’s tattoo of an Ek-Onkar was present in the case summary. The defendant’s name was Talib Rajpal. Lastly, the final third was the control condition. The defendant swore in by putting his hand over his heart, reciting, “I swear to tell the truth, See e.g., (1995, December, 21). Man Gets 10-to-20-Year Term for Raping Wife. The New York Times, from http://query.nytimes.com/gst/fullpage.html?res=9F0DE4D81439F932A15751C1A963958260&n=Top%2FR eference%2FTimes%20Topics%2FSubjects%2FV%2FViolence; James, G. (1995, December, 6). Man Found Guilty of Raping His Wife. The New York Times, from http://query.nytimes.com/gst/fullpage.html?res= 9403E7D81E39F935A35751C1A963958260&sec=&spon=. 54 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 56 the whole truth, and nothing but the truth.” A tattoo was mentioned without specification of its content or image. The defendant’s name was Robert Johnson. In half of these conditions, participants were instructed to take the perspective of the defendant by including a paragraph that read, “Knowing the importance of understanding others, go through the trial as if you were the defendant, walking through the world in his shoes and looking at the world through his eyes. Know that there are two sides to every question; try to look at them both. Try to imagine how you would feel if you were in the defendant’s place.” Instructions and verdict form. Participants were instructed to indicate a verdict of guilty or not guilty for an additional charge of aggravated sexual abuse and to give a recommended sentence for the defendant. The guidelines for the conviction of aggravated sexual abuse were supplied from “Report to Congress: Analysis of Penalties for Federal Rape Cases,” (1993) and was integrated into the judge’s instructions. Participants were informed that if found guilty, the defendant was eligible for up to a maximum of life sentence. Participants were also informed that the crimes the defendant had already been convicted of were eligible for a minimum of 4 ½ to 9 years and a maximum of 12 ½ to 25 years in prison. Participants were then told to determine the appropriate sentence for the defendant, supplying a minimum and maximum sentence along with a recommended sentence for the defendant. Dependant and independant measures survey. The dependent measure survey contained measures of the participant’s sentencing decision, confidence in one’s decision, what influenced the participant’s decision, a participant’s level of religious identification, a devotionalism measure, the Rational-Experiential Inventory, a perspective taking P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 57 measure (Interpersonal Reactivity Index), and CEST measures. For more details please see Appendix B. Interpersonal reactivity index (IRI). Perspective taking was utilized in the following study to curtail stereotypes and biases, in hopes that those who employed perspective taking would report higher levels of rational processing and evaluate the defendant in an unbiased and logical manner. In order to accurately measure perspective taking, this study employed a section of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (Davis, 1980; 1983), a scale that consists of 4 components: the perspective taking scale, the empathetic concern scale, personal distress, and the fantasy scale. Only questions regarding perspective taking were scored. The questions measured a particpant’s tendency to adopt the psychological point of view of others in everyday life, with statements such as, "I sometimes try to understand my friends better by imagining how things look from their perspective." Participants rated each question on a 5-point scale from does not describe me well to describes me very well. This scale was used to measure if and how much the participant tried to take the perspective of the defendant. The use of perspective taking, the active imagination of how an individual feels or is affected by his/her situation, frequently produces an empathetic arousal that leads to altruistic behavior (Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000; Batson, 1991).55 Perspective taking affects the attribution and evaluation of others, as perspective takers evaluate a situation as if they themselves were in it (Jones & Nisbett, 1971). It has been shown that perspective taking leads to the merging of the self and the other and an increased cognitive accessibility of the self-concept (Davis, Conklin, Smith, & Luce, 1996). Just as Davis (1983) found that perspective taking was positively correlated with social competence as well as selfesteem, which may also improve one’s positive and helpful behavior. 55 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 58 the positive self-evaluation is related to the ingroup, the increased self-other overlap caused by perspective taking could lead to more positive evaluations of the outgroup target (e.g., a defendant), diminishing the accessibility and application of stereotypes (Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000). In fact, a study by Galinsky and Moskowitz (2000) found that perspective taking reduced evidence of ingroup bias by increasing evaluations of the outgroup. Galinsky and Moskowitz (2000) suggested perspective taking as an effective strategy for debiasing social thought.56 Rational-experiential inventory (REI). The Rational-Experiential Inventory was administered to investigate the role of thought processes (rational versus experiential) in relation to perspective taking instructions and religious symbols. Based on CognitiveExperiential Self-Theory (CEST), as discussed in the previous chapter, REI was constructed to measure two independent processing modes: analytic-rational and intuitive-experiential. The REI consists of two parts: the Need for Cognition Scale (NFC)57 and Faith in Intuition (FI) Scale,58 which were created to measure personality, adjustment, achievement and interpersonal relations through thinking processes and intuition levels (Epstein et al., 1996).59 A 5-item NFC scale was used in this study, which reports the extent to which individuals enjoy and engage in, or dislike and avoid, cognitive activities (Epstein et al., 1996). Providing the assessment of intuitiveexperiential thinking, Epstein et al. (1996) developed the Faith in Intuition scale, A similar study was also conducted by Vescio et al. (2003), which found that perspective taking promoted improved ingroup attitudes irrespective of stereotypicality. 57 Originally created by Cacioppo & Petty (1982) 58 Jung (1964/1968) proposed that thinking and intuition are among the most basic ways of interpreting information. 59 Cacioppo and Petty (1982) showed that one’s score on the Need for Cognition scale measures analyticrational processing by indicating levels of heuristic thinking, associated with impression formation and people’s receptivity to new information. 56 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 59 measuring engagement and confidence in one’s intuitive abilities. A 5-item FI scale was used in this study, which refers to having confidence in one’s feelings and immediate impressions as a basis for decisions and actions (Epstein et al., 1996).60 Respondents rated all items of the NFC and FI scales on a 5-point scale ranging from completely false to completely true. CEST Vignettes. In addition to the self-report scales, Epstein et al. (1996) created heuristic vignettes to provide an alternate index of individual differences in rational and experiential thinking. These vignettes act as a non-self-report measure from which responses can be scored objectively as heuristic or rational. The vignettes depict situations that require judgments about the degree of distress following arbitrary outcomes or cost-benefit effects. On the basis of CEST, Epstein et al. (1996) correlated heuristic responses with intuitive-experiential processing (as heuristics are the natural mode of the experiential system). In conducting the vignettes, respondents are asked how they themselves would respond (self perspective) and how a completely rational person would respond (logical perspective) in the situations provided.61 These responses allow participants to express their understanding of the rational response, even if they do not believe it reflects common behaviors, and to demonstrate their awareness of the two modes of thinking. If participants are aware of both modes (rational and experiential), it is believed they can alter responses appropriately when asked to make judgments from the different mode of thinking (Epstein et al., 1996), mirroring a judge asking a juror to think logically and impartially. This also allows one to determine whether a participant is Although the original NFC was 45 items then 19 items and FI was 12 items, Epstein et al. (1996) found the shortened REI was sufficiently reliable and independent. 61 This procedure has been used successfully in previous research, demonstrating the differences between rational and experiential thinking (Epstein, 1994; Epstein et al., 1992; Kirkpatrick & Epstein, 1992). 60 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 60 generally experiential or if they are thinking experientially due to the case before them.62 In the following study, participants were scored by their answers regarding how a rational person would respond to the given vignettes, as it indicates a participant’s ability to think rationally and awareness of the rational response. One point was given for each vignette, either to one’s experiential or rational score. Therefore, participants were rated either 3:0, 2:1 rational or 1:2, 0:3 experiential. (For vignettes, see Appendix C). Procedure After participants read and signed an informed consent form, they were reminded that they could terminate the experiment at any time. The experimenter then explained the procedure, informed participants that they were to act as mock jurors at a trial, read through all of the information carefully, and answer each question. After handing in the packet, participants were debriefed, thanked for their participation, and then dismissed. HYPOTHESES Perspective Taking Due to the debiasing effects of perspective taking, it was hypothesized that perspective taking instructions would lower sentences in all three conditions. It was expected that sentences would drop most significantly in the Sikh condition due to decrease in outgroup bias (as no Sikhs are expected to be in the participant pool). It was also hypothesized that those in perspective taking conditions would report higher levels of rational thinking and lower experiential thinking due to a decrease in ingroup/outgroup As Epstein et al. (1996) writes, “[the vignettes] provide an objective index of the ability to distinguish between heuristic and logical thinking” (p. 393). 62 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 61 biases. A such, it was expected that perspective taking would lower the expression of biases in all three conditions, though more substantially in the religious conditions due to increased levels of bias caused by religious symbols. Contrarily, those who did not receive perspective taking instructions were expected to report higher experiential thinking and rely more heavily on personal biases, basing decisions on feelings about the case and defendant as well as their instincts about the case. Furthermore, perspective taking was expected to positively correlate not only with rational thinking, but also how participants came to their respective decisions. Reliance on one’s religious beliefs, instincts, and feelings were considered experiential factors. Reliance on judge’s instructions, logic and reason and evidence provided in the case were viewed as rational factors. Hypothesis 1a. PT will reduce sentences and conviction rates in all three conditions Hypothesis 1b. Sentences will drop most significantly in the Sikh condition due to reduced outgroup biases Hypothesis 2. PT will decrease experiential thinking and increase rational thinking Hypothesis 3a. Rational thinking will lower sentencing and correlate with participants being influenced by judge’s instructions, logic and reason, and evidence provided within the case Hypothesis 3b. Experiential thinking will raise sentences and correlate with participants being influenced by personal feelings, intuition, and religion. Religious Condition Sentences and conviction rates were expected to be higher in the Sikh condition due to ingroup/outgroup biases. Because of ingroup/outgroup biases and the larger number of ingroup members expected in the Christian condition than the Sikh condition, it was hypothesized that the Christian defendant would receive lower sentences than the Sikh or control defendants. This expectation was based on the similarity-leniency P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 62 hypothesis. Additionally, it was expected that lower ingroup sentences would be most pronounced when only looking at Christian participants, as it isolates the ingroup. Additionally, participants in the Christian and Sikh conditions were expected to process more experientially than participants in the control condition due to the presence of religious symbols and the salience of religion. These participants were also expected to rely more heavily on intuitive-experiential factors rather than analytical-rational factors when making sentencing decisions. Due to previous findings that emotional content (including religious material) sparks experiential thinking, it was also anticipated that those who rated themselves with high Religious Identification and Devotionalism would be more likely to utilize experiential thinking when making sentencing decisions, especially when religion was salient. Hypothesis 4. Participants in the Christian and Sikh conditions will process more experientially due to the presence of religious symbols. Hypothesis 5a. Sentences and conviction rates will to be higher in the Sikh condition and lower in the Christian due to ingroup/outgroup biases. Hypothesis 5b. These differences in sentencing and conviction rates will be exaggerated in the all-Christian group to due increased ingroup concentration. Hypothesis 6. Those high in Religious Identification and Devotionalism will be more likely to think experientially. RESULTS General The overall sample size included 133 participants (38% males) with ages ranging from 18 to 64 (with a mean age of 20.9 years old). Participants who did not provide a recommended sentence, or who provided a range for recommended sentence, were not used in sentencing data but were included in the overall data (N=5). One participant who P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 63 suggested life was entered at 70 years to match the next highest sentence recommendation. 59 participants (44%) were Christians, 14 (10.5%) were Jewish, one (0.7%) was Hindu, four (3%) were Buddhist, one (0.7%) was Muslim, 16 (12%) were Atheists, 16 (12%) were Agnostics, 14 (10.5%) indicated that they believed in God but had no particular faith, and two (1.5%) indicated that their faith was “other” than those listed (one Quaker and one Celtic Pagan). Each cell had an average of 22.16 participants, with cell sizes ranging from 22 to 23 participants. Results were also examined in a sample of Christian-only participants (N=59). Two participants in this group failed to provide valid sentence recommendations, putting the sample size at 57 for tests regarding sentences. Each cell had an average of 9.8 participants, will cell sizes ranging from 6 to 13 participants. Comprehension Check One false option was added to questionnaires to ensure participants’ close attention to the case summary. When participants were asked to rate what influenced their sentencing decisions, the false option read: “Evidence that the husband tried to break his wife’s arm.” No such evidence was provided in the case summary, yet 68 participants (51%) marked that this evidence influenced their decision. Only 15 (11%) participants marked that this did not influence them at all. These data may indicate low levels of thorough comprehension and attention in the participant pool during the conduction of this study, though ample significant data was found. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 64 Effects of Perspective Taking Perspective taking instructions. It was found that perspective taking (PT) instructions were effective in all three conditions (F (1,127) =4.81, p<.05) (N=133), causing higher scores on the perspective taking scale in each PT condition. Sum of Perspective Taking in PT and No-PT Conditions (N=133) Condition No PT With PT Instructions Instructions 28.23 30.32 Control (4.49) (4.82) 28.23 30.55 Christian (5.69) (4.26) 27.78 29.00 Sikh (4.11) (5.91) Table 2: Sum of Perspective Taking with & without Perspective Taking instructions When examining only Christian participants (N=59), perspective taking instructions were calculated as slightly less effective, though still significant in raising one’s level of perspective taking (F (1, 53) =3.48, p<.10), partially caused by the fact that perspective taking scales were higher in the Sikh condition when no perspective taking instructions were given (M=28.38, SD=3.45) than when instructions were present (M=26.22, SD=5.97). Sum of Perspective Taking in PT and No-PT Conditions for all Christian Participants (N=59) Condition No PT With PT Instructions Instructions 27.67 31.45 Control (3.82) (6.24) 25.13 30.67 Christian (4.61) (3.72) 28.38 26.22 Sikh (3.45) (5.97) Table 2: Sum of Perspective Taking with & without Perspective Taking instructions for all Christian participants P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 65 These results are slightly peculiar, indicating that Christian participants had lower levels of PT in the Sikh condition when given PT instructions than when not given instructions to take the perspective of the defendant. These results may reflect reluctance to take the perspective of an outgroup member, but the implications of such data are debatable. Most important to recognize is the overall significant effect of the perspective taking instructions in the general participant pool as well as the Christian-only pool. Sentencing. Recall the study by Galinsky and Moskowitz (2000), which determined that perspective taking decreases ingroup/outgroup biases and often leads to more positive evaluations of the target. As such, it was expected that the presence of perspective taking instructions would decrease the average recommended sentence and conviction rates in all three conditions. To investigate this expectation, a 3x2 ANOVA was run with between subjects factors of religious conditions and perspective taking. Supporting hypothesis 1a, perspective taking instructions lowered sentences in all three conditions (F (1,122) =2.94, p< .10), with the average recommended sentence within perspective taking conditions reported at 17.28 years (SD=1.57), in contrast to the average recommended sentence of 21.07 years reported without perspective taking (SD=1.57) (See Figure 1). This was the main effect of the perspective taking instructions. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 66 Recommended Sentences: All Participants (N=128) 25 Sentence (Years) 20 15 21.6 20.84 20.79 17.05 18.02 16.76 10 5 0 Figure 1: Recommended Sentences with & without Perspective Taking instructions for all participants. Control Christian Additionally, convictions of aggravated sexual abuse were consistently lower Religious Condition Sikh No Perspective Taking Instructions Perspective Taking Instructions when instructions for perspective taking were present, running an average 53% conviction rate in perspective taking conditions, as opposed to 65.5% without PT, though these data were not found to be significant through a chi square test, indicating x² (2, N=133) = 1.539, p> .05, as seen in Table 3. These data reflect expected findings and demonstrate the altruistic effects of perspective taking, resulting in lower sentences and conviction rates. These data also point to existing juror biases. Because perspective taking lowers bias, its effect demonstrates the prior existence of bias. Rates of Conviction for Aggravated Sexual Abuse: All Participants (N=133) Control Christian Sikh No Perspective Taking 59.1% 63.6% 73.9% Perspective Taking 54.5% 54.5% 50% Table 3: Percent of guilty verdicts for aggravated sexual abuse in conditions with & without Perspective Taking P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 67 Also important to note is the significantly higher tendency to convict the Sikh defendant over the Christian and control defendants in the non-PT conditions. These data support hypothesis 1b, reflecting outgroup punitiveness. According to the data, the Sikh defendant had a 10.3% higher rate of conviction than the Christian defendant, and a 14.8% higher rate than the control defendant. Observing the power of perspective taking, the discrepancies in conviction rates were greatly reduced and altered in the perspective taking conditions, with the Sikh defendant’s rate of conviction becoming the lowest of all three conditions. The same pattern in conviction rates was observed in the Christian-only data as well (see Table 4), with an even larger discrepancy between conviction rates in each condition. There, the conviction rates of the Sikh defendant dropped to 33.3%. Interestingly, rates for the Christian defendant did not reflect the same pattern as the control and Sikh conditions, with the percent of guilty verdicts with PT jumping to 80% (see Table 4). These results indicate that Christian participants were more likely to convict the Christian defendant when perspective taking was present than not (MnoPT=62.5%; MPT=80%). These conviction rates are in direct opposition to the pattern observed in the control (MnoPT=54.5%; MPT=54.5%) and Sikh conditions (MnoPT=83.3%; MPT=33.3%). These results were found to be significant through a chi square test x² (2, N=56) = 20.88, p< .0001. Rates of Conviction for Aggravated Sexual Abuse: Only Christian Participants (N=59) Control Christian Sikh No Perspective Taking 54.5% 62.5% 83.3% Perspective Taking 54.5% 80% 33.3% Table 4: Percent of guilty verdicts for aggravated sexual abuse in conditions with & without Perspective Taking; Only Christian participants. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 68 All-Christian participant sentencing followed the same pattern, not reflecting the same sentencing results as the general participant pool (See Figure 2). Like the general participant pool, higher sentences were recommended when perspective taking instructions were absent in the control condition (MnoPT=24.68, SDnoPT=17.14; MPT=14.82, SDPT=7.44) and Sikh condition (MnoPT=23.92, SDnoPT=11.01; MPT=14.89, SDPT=7.11). But within the Christian condition, higher sentences were suggested in the perspective taking condition (MnoPT=15.75, SDnoPT=7.09; MPT=28.33, SDPT=21.6). By completing a 3x2 univariate ANOVA between perspective taking and religious conditions, these data were found to be significant (F (2, 51) =4.067, p<.05). Recommended Sentences: Christian Participants Only (N=57) 30 28.33 Sentence (Years) 25 20 24.68 23.92 15 14.82 15.75 14.89 10 5 0 Figure 2: Recommended Sentences from Christian participants withChristian & without Perspective Taking instructions Control Sikh Religious Condition These results may indicate several Taking things. Instructions It appears as though ingroup perspective No Perspective Perspective Taking Instructions taking increased the negative evaluation of the target, pointing to the black sheep effect and directly contradicting hypotheses of increased altruism (e.g., Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000). This may have occurred due to participants’ increased identification with the P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 69 target and cognitive accessibility of the self-concept, escalating the sense of deviance performed by the defendant (Davis et al., 1996). These results are in line with the Social Identity Theory, supporting the rejection of the deviant ingroup member. Clearly, perspective taking instructions influenced participants, affecting their evaluation of the defendant and altering their sentencing decisions. Additionally, the religious symbols within the case influence participants, as they altered reactions to the perspective taking instructions; while PT instructions in the control and Sikh conditions generated the expected altruistic reactions within Christian participants, knowledge of the Christian defendant’s ingroup status affected Christian participants’ reactions to placing themselves in the shoes of the defendant. Recognizing the small participant pool in the Christian condition (N=14), and due to the significant effects of the independent variables, further research should be conducted to explain these findings, looking into the effects of perspective taking on ingroup and outgroup biases. These results may reflect an additional danger in supplying jurors with knowledge of the defendant’s religious affiliation, sparking ingroup biases that directly affect a defendant’s likelihood of conviction. Rational-experiential processing. It was hypothesized that instructions of perspective taking would decrease levels of intuitive-experiential thinking and increase levels of analytical-rational thinking. A univariate ANOVA evaluating the effects of perspective taking and religious conditions on REI scales indicated no significant effect of perspective taking on intuitive-experiential thinking (F (1,127)=.004, p>.05) or analytical-rational thinking (F (1,127)=2.31, p>.05), not giving support to hypothesis 2. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 70 Similarly, the CEST measures did not indicate any effect of perspective taking on heuristic (F (1,127) =1.45, p>.05) or logical thinking (F (1,127) =1.45, p>.05). Likewise, in the all-Christian participant group, perspective taking did not have a significant effect on analytical-rational (F (1, 53) =.019, p>.05) or intuitive-experiential (F (1, 53) =.004, p>.05) processes as measured by the REI scale. But, when measured by the CEST vignettes, data indicated that perspective taking significantly increased logical thinking (F (1, 53) =3.14, p<.05) and decreased heuristic thinking (F (1, 53) =3.14, p<.05), as hypothesized (See Tables 5a & 5b). Note that PT had the strongest effects on rational/experiential thinking when religion was present, pointing to an effect of the religious material on participant thought processes. CEST Vignette Data Mean Heuristic Condition No Perspective Taking Control Christian Sikh 1.50 (1.08) 1.13 (0.99) 1.77 (1.09) Perspective Taking 1.45 (0.93) .67 (1.03) .78 (1.09) Table 5a: CEST Vignettes, Heuristic Means data of only Christian participants Mean Logical Condition No Perspective Taking Control Christian Sikh 1.50 (1.08) 1.88 (0.99) 1.23 (1.09) Perspective Taking 1.55 (0.93) 2.33 (1.03) 2.22 (1.09) Table 5b: CEST Vignettes, Logical Means data of only Christian participants P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 71 Perspective taking, rational-experiential thinking and sentencing decisions. Significant positive correlations were found between the sum of perspective taking and rational thinking, r (131) =.179, p<.05. Additionally, negative correlations were found between PT and reliance on one’s instincts to make the sentencing decision, r(131)=-.174, p<.05, as well as basing one’s sentencing decision on personal religious beliefs, r(131)=-.194, p<.05, both experiential factors, all supporting hypothesis 3a. Reliance on instincts correlated with the sum of one’s Religious Identification, r (131) =.191, p<.05 and strongly correlated with experiential thinking, r (131) =.330, p<.01. All of these correlations point to a negative relationship between perspective taking and experiential processing and a positive correlation between perspective taking and rational processes. These data show that the more participants utilized perspective taking, the more they used rational thinking. It also indicates participants’ greater reliance on instincts and religion when perspective taking was not being employed. Rational-experiential thinking and sentencing in relation to perspective taking. Several analysis indicated that intuitive-experiential thinking, as reported by REI, positively correlated with basing one’s sentencing decision on “feelings about this case,” r(131)=.178, p<.05, “instincts about this case,” r(131)=.33, p<.01, and “feelings about the defendant,” r(131)=.177, p<.05. There were no correlations between judge’s instructions, logic and reason, or any evidence presented in the case, in line with hypothesis 3b. These data did not reflect significant correlations between rational thought and what influenced participant’s sentencing decisions, except for a negative correlation between “evidence that the couple had been estranged,” which may be argued as a logical or emotional piece of evidence (r(1, 131)=-.177, p<.05). P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 72 These data help support the argument that if PT instructions are applied within courts, jury members’ decisions will be influenced more by the information provided from the courts rather than personal and emotional reactions. Religious identification and devotionalism. Along similar lines, data from the control condition showed lower levels of Religious Identification as well as Devotionalism in the perspective taking conditions. Religious Identification in the control group without perspective taking instructions had an average Religious ID of 16.05 (SD=7.386), while with perspective taking the number dropped to an average of 12.57 (SD=6.169). Mirroring this pattern, levels of Devotionalism in the control condition without PT averaged at 38.86 (SD=12.616), and with PT at 32.05 (SD=12.28). These data indicate the power of perspective taking in reducing one’s reliance on religious beliefs. Interesting to note is the perspective taking instruction’s lack of effect in the conditions containing religious symbols (Sikh and Christian). Within these conditions, levels of RI and Devotionalism appeared to be insignificantly altered by the presence of perspective taking, with levels barely shifting. These data may be used to further support the use of perspective taking instructions as well as the need to expel religious symbols from courtrooms. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 73 Religious Identification: All Participants (N=127) 18 Religious Identification 16 14 16.05 13.86 12 14.86 12.57 10 11.68 10.95 8 6 4 2 0 Figure 3: Religious Identification of all participants with & without Perspective Taking instructions Control Christian Sikh Religious Condition No Perspective Taking Instructions Perspective Taking Instructions Devotionalism: All Participants (N=133) 45 Devotionalism 40 35 30 38.86 38.09 38.18 32.61 32.05 30.45 25 20 15 10 5 0 Figure 4: Devotionalism of allControl participants with & without Perspective Taking instructions Christian Sikh Religious Condition No Perspective Taking Instructions Perspective Taking Instructions P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 74 Effects of Religious Conditions Rational-experiential thinking. It was hypothesized that the presence of religious symbols would increase levels of intuitive-experiential thinking in participants. Interestingly, data indicated the opposite effect, going against hypothesis 4. A univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to examine the effects of religious material on REI, combining the Christian and Sikh conditions. Contrary to expectations, it was found that religion had a significant positive effect on analytical-rational thinking (F (1, 131) =6.059, p<.05) with the highest levels of rationality reported within conditions with religious material (M=20.76, SD=2.82), over the control condition (M=19.41, SD=3.3). Furthermore, when all three conditions were considered separately, it was found that religious material had a positive effect on analytical-rational thinking (F (2,127) =3.4, p<.05). A Bonferroni post hoc test indicated that the highest levels of rationality were recorded in the Christian condition, with a difference of 1.64 (significance of p<.05) between the Christian condition and control condition. These results directly contradict expected results. Additionally, religion was found to have no significant effect on intuitiveexperiential processes as measured by REI when religious conditions (Christian and Sikh) were considered together (F (1,131) =.044, p>.05) or apart (F (2,127) =1.13, p>.05). No effect was found in the CEST vignette measures either, showing no effect of religious condition on logical or heuristic processing (F (2,127) =.805, p>.05). Sum of Perspective Taking. Religious condition (Control, Christian, or Sikh) was not expected to significantly affect levels of perspective taking, and analysis P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 75 confirmed this prediction (F (2, 130) =.553, p>.05). Means and standard deviations of the three groups were recorded as follows: (MControl=29.27, SDControl=4.727; MChristian=29.39, SDChristian=5.109; MSikh=28.38, SDSikh=5.051). Sentencing. Based on our understanding of ingroup favoritism, it was expected that sentences in the Christian condition would be lower than the Sikh and control conditions (as there are far more Christians in the participant pool than Sikhs). Additionally, the Sikh condition was projected to have higher sentences due to outgroup punitiveness. Results did not reflect the anticipated pattern, going against hypothesis 5a. The control condition’s average sentence was calculated at 18.92 years (SD=11.7), the Christian condition at 19.43 years (SD=14.58) and the Sikh condition at an average of 19.18 years (SD=10.87). These data were not found to be significantly different from one another (F (2, 122) =.018, p>.05), showing no effect of religious condition on average sentencing. Sentencing was also examined in the all-Christian participant group (N=57). Contrary to expectations, results did not indicate ingroup favoritism, going against hypothesis 5b. Again, it was expected that the presence of religious symbols would result in higher sentences for the outgroup member (Sikh) and lower for the ingroup member (Christian). Yet, it was found that the highest sentence of 21.14 years was reported in the Christian condition (SD=17.76). Second lowest was the Sikh condition with a recommended average sentence of 20.05 years (SD=10.39) and the lowest sentence in the control conditions averaged 19.75 years (SD=13.84). However, these findings were not statistically significant (F (2, 51) =.206, p> .05). These data may be the result of the black sheep effect, as Christians received the highest P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 76 recommended sentence. Still, the reasons for this change in average sentencing are unclear. Moreover, it must be noted that these sentences are an average of PT and no-PT conditions. Recall Figures 1 & 2 for sentencing between PT and no-PT conditions. Factors that influenced the sentencing decisions. Factors found to correlate with recommended sentences were one’s reliance on “personal feelings about rape” (r (126) =.235, p<.01) and certainty in sentencing decision (r (126) =.336, p<.01); the more certain someone was, the higher the sentence and the stronger their feelings about rape in general. Most importantly, basing one’s decision on “personal feelings about rape” strongly correlated with basing one’s decision on “feelings about the defendant,” r (131) =.355, p<.01, “instincts about this case” r (131) =.252, p<.01, “personal religious beliefs,” r (126) =.318, p<.01, and “feelings about this case,” r (131) =.406, p<.01. All of these reasons reflect heuristic, feeling-based thinking. None of these factors positively correlated with judge’s instructions (r (131)=-.07, p>.05) or several questions regarding evidence presented within the case summary, with the only exception found between personal feelings of rape and evidence that the defendant showed no regret, r(131)=.217, p<.05. These correlations suggest that those who proposed higher sentences were also processing experientially. For a full chart of correlations see Appendix D.63 Reported levels of religious identification. It was found that a participant’s reported Religious Identification was significantly effected by the religious condition Also important to note is sentencing’s negative correlation with “evidence that the couple had been estranged,” r (126) =-.265, p<.01), which may be considered intuitive-experiential or analytical-rational. 63 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 77 of the study (F (2,121) =2.72, p<.10). A univariate ANOVA revealed the largest discrepancy in Religious Identification between the Christian (M=14.35, SD=1.06) and Sikh conditions (M=11.32, SD=1.05) (See Figure 5a). These results reflect a direct relationship between the way in which one perceives his/her own religious identity and the religious affiliation of the defendant before them. Most interesting in the reported RI is the significant drop in the Sikh condition. This change may be due to several factors, including higher rational thinking or the social desirability effect. It is also a possibility that these results are symptomatic of a problem with random assignment; it is possible that more religious participants happened to be assigned to the Christian condition. All participants (N=133) 20 15 14.31 14.36 10 11.33 5 0 Control Christian Sikh Religious Identification Figure 5a: Reported Religious Identification of all participants The discrepancies in Religious Identification between conditions were even more pronounced in the all-Christian participant pool (See Figure 5b). Religious conditions had a significant effect on one’s reported RI (F (2, 53) =4.32, p<.05), with a difference of 6.65 (p<.05) between the Christian and Sikh conditions, showing higher RI with a Christian defendant and lower RI with a Sikh defendant. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 78 Christian Participants (N=59) 20 15 18.29 16.39 10 11.64 5 0 Figure 5b: Reported Religious Identification ofChristian Christian participants Control Sikh Religious Identification These data indicate the substantial effects of the religious symbols within the trial summary on participant’s evaluations of their religious affiliation and identity. Christian participants reported far higher Religious Identification when the defendant of the trial was of Christian faith than when no religion was mentioned. Moreover, Christian participants’ evaluation of their Religious Identification significantly dropped with the Sikh defendant. Reasons for this are unclear, though higher rationality and social desirability effect may be argued. Religious identification and factors that influenced sentencing decisions. In the general participant pool it was found that RI negatively correlated with one’s level of analytical-rational thinking (as measured by REI),64 r (125) =-.179, p<.05, and positively correlated with a participant’s reliance on “instincts about the case” in making one’s sentencing decision (r (125) =.191, p<.05), supporting hypothesis 6. Interestingly, RI was found to be positively correlated with being influenced by logic and reason, (r (131) =.193, p<.05). Previous research indicates that logic and reason should correlate with analytical-rational, not intuitive-experiential thought, but results of RI do not reflect this expectation (Epstein et al., 1996). 64 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 79 Furthermore, reliance on one’s instincts in making the sentencing decision was correlated with intuitive-experiential thinking (as measured by the REI), r (131) =.330, p<.01, and several other feeling-based decisions, linking Religious Identification and experiential processing (see Appendix D for a full listing). This indicates higher levels of experiential thinking as well as reliance on feeling-based reasoning for sentencing decisions when levels of RI were higher. It should also be noted that Religious Identification significantly correlated with measures of Devotionalism (r (125) =.611, p<.01), linking the two measures. Additionally, all of these correlations were amplified when Atheist participants were removed. For a chart, please see Appendix E. Devotionalism. Religious conditions were found have a causal affect on one’s reported levels of Devotionalism (F (2, 127) =3.42, p<.05). Post hoc tests showed the largest discrepancy in reported Devotionalism between the Christian and Sikh conditions (6.58, p<.05) (See Figure 6a). As with Religious Identification, the all-Christian participant pool amplified these differences (F (2, 53) =3.65, p<.05), showing a discrepancy of 10.62 (p<.05) between the Christian and Sikh conditions (See Figure 6b). Again, these data suggest that the religion of the defendant in the trial influenced participants’ evaluations of their own religious devotionalism. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 80 All Participants 45 40 35 30 25 20 15 10 5 0 38.14 35.45 31.56 Control Christian Sikh Figure 6a: Reported Devotionalism of all participants Devotionalism Christian Participants 45 40 35 30 25 20 15 10 5 0 40.04 42.57 31.95 Control Christian Devotionalism Figure 6b: Reported Devotionalism of Christian participants Sikh P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s , a n d J u r y B i a s e s | 81 Devotionalism and factors that influenced sentencing decisions. In the general participant pool, levels of Devotionalism were found to be positively correlated with a participant’s reliance on “personal religious beliefs” when making sentencing decisions, r (131) =.217, p<.05. This correlation suggests that the higher levels of Devotionalism observed in the all-Christian participant group indicate increased reliance on religious beliefs when faced with a Christian defendant. Again, lower levels of Devotionalism in the Sikh condition, as compared to the control condition, raise several questions, such as whether or not participants were more rational in that condition, or whether they wanted to appear less biased. Gender When looking at gender, no significant differences were found between genders in regards to recommended sentence (F (1,126) =.016, p>.05) or guilt of aggravated sexual abuse, x² (1, N=133) = 0.21, p> .05. Females recorded higher scores of perspective taking, F (1, 131) =11.82, p<.05, indicating an elevated tendency for females to take the perspective of others. In regards to all other dependant variables, no significant differences were found between women and men: levels of devotionalism (F (1,131) =1.979, p>.05), religious identification (F (1,125) =.677, p>.05), rational processing through REI (F (1,131) =.035, p>.05), experiential processing through REI (F (1,131) =.004, p>.05), rational processing by CEST (F(1,131)=.072, p>.05) or experiential processing by CEST (F(1,131)=.072, p>.05). P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 82 Gender Sentence Guilty PT Dev RI Rat. REI Exp. REI Rat. CEST Exp. CEST Male 17.94 (1.57) 18.57 (1.23) 54% 27.1 (0.68) 30.13 (0.53) 36.73 (1.71) 34.07 (1.33) 13.74 (.99) 12.98 (0.78) 20.36 (0.43) 20.31 (0.33) 16.33 (0.6) 16.44 (0.46) 1.76 (.89) 1.81 (1.03) 1.24 (.89) 1.19 (1.03) Female 62.7% Table 6: Gender Data DISCUSSION As hypothesized, perspective taking lowered sentences and correlated positively with rational thinking; data suggest that as participants employed perspective taking, rational thinking increased and sentences decreased. Interestingly, this implies present biases within the jury pool. Furthermore, REI measures did not report changes in intuitive-experiential and analytical-rational thinking from the presence of religious symbols. Yet, one’s level of Devotionalism and Religious Identification shifted depending on the study condition, with higher RI and Devotionalism scores in the Christian condition, and lower in the Sikh condition. Devotionalism was found to be positively correlated with reliance on one’s religious beliefs and experiential thinking and RI scores were strongly correlated with reliance on instincts, a characteristic of experiential thought. These data suggest that although REI measures did not directly reflect the impact of religious material, changes in thought processes occurred within the participant pool, depending upon the presence or absence of religious symbols. Furthermore, perspective taking was found to reduce levels of RI and Devotionalism in the control condition, but had no significant effects when religious symbols were present, even though the average sentence was reduced. As such, while PT was able to reduce bias and experiential thinking, other mental processes were not P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 83 affected by PT in the presence of religious symbols. These data provide strong support for the inclusion of perspective taking instructions and the erasure of religious symbols from the courts. Furthermore, several factors impact the decision-making process. This study alone measured attitudes towards oneself (in terms of religiosity and devotion), attitudes towards the defendant, and the impact of legal materials (e.g., judge’s instructions and evidence of the case) as well as extra legal materials (e.g., intuition, feelings, religion). Findings of this study highlight the complexity of decision-making and the ways in which the decision-making process may be altered. Most importantly, the impact of perspective taking on sentencing implies the presence of juror biases. As found in previous studies, the increased self-other overlap caused by perspective taking diminishes accessibility and application of ingroup and outgroup stereotypes, and as such is suggested as an effective strategy for debiasing social thought (Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000; Vescio et al., 2003). Data showing the decrease in sentencing and increase in rational thought as a result of perspective taking support these earlier findings. Furthermore, the larger effects of perspective taking (lowering sentences by the most significant degree) in the Sikh condition, signify the presence of outgroup biases, curtailed by the presence of perspective taking. PT also had the greatest effect in Sikh conviction rates. As such, this study illustrates that religious symbols of a Guru Granth Sahib (book) and Ek-Onkar (tattoo) are enough to generate outgroup status of the defendant. Keep in mind that the way in which the Sikh defendant swore in was identical to the way in which the Christian defendant swore in, “I swear to P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 84 tell the truth, the whole truth, and nothing but the truth so help me God,” thus highlighting the impact of the symbols alone, regardless of the mention of God. Furthermore, data regarding Devotionalism and Religious Identification reflect the different modes of thinking utilized by jurors, changes in how jurors view themselves, and how jurors come to their respective decisions (e.g., relying on judge’s instructions, evidence, feelings, or intuitions). Uses of rational and experiential thinking shifted between religious conditions, as did levels of religiousness and devotion. These changes correlated with participant’s dependence on legal and extra legal materials when making their decisions. These fluctuations, changing in response to the religion of the defendant, show the instability caused by the presence of religious material in courts. Perspective Taking: Conclusions The results of this study exhibited the beneficial effects of perspective taking. Most importantly, perspective taking lowered sentences in all three conditions, illustrating the power of perspective taking in reducing biases. These claims were evidenced through perspective taking measures’ positive correlations with rational thought measures and negative correlations with religious influence and instincts. Furthermore, perspective taking’s impact in reducing levels of Devotionalism and Religious Identification in the control condition further demonstrated the power of perspective taking in inhibiting one’s reliance upon religion. (Interesting to note is the dampened effect of perspective taking on RI and Devotionalism in the two conditions containing religious symbols, though the final result (sentencing) did reflect reduced biases in lowered sentencing. These data point to the importance of using perspective P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 85 taking in the courtroom in an effort to reduce bias, but also to the importance of eliminating the presence of religious symbols within the courts. Without eradicating the presence of religious symbols, perspective taking instructions cannot have their full effect in decision-making. Although sentences were lowered, addressing how jurors come to their decisions is still relevant.) Due to perspective taking’s significant effect on rational and experiential thinking, as well as levels of religious identity and devotion, perspective taking affected what influenced juror decisions. Participants with higher levels of experiential thought were more influenced by feeling and intuition-based factors than evidence or judge’s instructions, whereas that those who utilized perspective taking and measured higher in rationality were influenced by judge’s instructions and logic, over heuristics. Furthermore, as observed in the Sikh sentencing condition, perspective taking counteracted outgroup biases by greatly reducing Sikh defendant’s sentence and conviction rates. Without perspective taking the Sikh defendant received a sentence of 21.6 years, the highest of all three conditions, and with perspective taking the sentence reduced to 16.76 years. As stated earlier, these data point to the grave importance of integrating perspective taking instructions into judge’s instructions. When jurors were advised to consider the perspective of the defendant, biases lowered and rational thinking increased. Both of these phenomena are essential to the proper functioning of the jury decision-making. Yet, adding perspective taking instructions to judge’s instructions is not the only necessary alteration. Evidence of Christians recommending higher sentences in the perspective taking conditions to Christian defendants presents a conflict. These data P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 86 indicate a hindering effect of perspective taking instructions, as it may increase the occurrence of the black sheep effect. As such, these data represent the dangers in supplying jurors with information regarding the defendant’s religious beliefs. These data also show that courts cannot simply integrate perspective taking into court instructions with the hopes that it will counteract any religious, racial, or class biases present within the court. Instead, religious content must be erased, as perspective taking can increase biases within ingroups. If a tattoo on a defendant is enough to spark the black sheep effect, this evidence provides substantial support for the erasure of all religious symbols and content within courts. Another conflict is observed in lower levels of PT within the Sikh condition (with the all-Christian participants group) when perspective taking instructions were given. Reasons for this result are unclear, and should be given more attention. These data may indicate one’s difficulty in taking the perspective of an outgroup member. Consequently, this too could help support the argument that religious content must never be given to reveal the religious affiliation of the defendant so as to reduce outgroup biases. Religious Conditions: Conclusions It was anticipated that religious conditions would affect the length of recommended sentences with higher sentences in the Sikh condition and lower in the Christian, and though the average sentence and rate of conviction without perspective taking was highest in the Sikh condition, data did not indicate a significant effect. Considering the evidence that several other decision-making factors shifted in response to the religious affiliation of the defendant, sentencing’s failure to reflect these changes may P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 87 be contributed to the lack of judgment standards provided and the large scale in which participants could recommend sentences (4 ½ years to life in prison). Providing participants with such an expansive scale allowed for innumerable interpretations of the appropriate sentence, thus compromising sentencing data. Furthermore, it was expected that religious content would produce intuitiveexperiential processing. Instead, results indicated that religious material had no effect on experiential thinking as measured by REI or CEST and instead showed increased levels of analytical-rational thinking. In fact, rational thinking was highest when the defendant was of the same religion as the participant. It is unclear why these results would occur. It is a possibility that the effects of religious material were not significant enough to be detected by rational-experiential measures. Yet, changes in experiential and rational thinking were illustrated in measures of Devotionalism and Religious Identification. The overall data regarding Devotionalism and RI reflected the anticipated obstructive effects of religious symbols on jury decisionmaking. First and foremost, the fluctuation in reported Devotionalism and Religious Identification highlights the effects of the religious symbols used in the trial summary, altering the way in which jurors judged themselves, depending upon the religion of the defendant before them. These changes alone indicate the dangers in supplying jurors with knowledge of the defendant’s religious affiliation. Furthermore, negative correlations between RI and rational processes, as well as Devotionalism’s correlation with experiential and religion-based decisions (e.g., instincts, feelings, religion, etc.), indicate the expected hindrances of religion on the jury decisionmaking process. These correlations, related to the elevated levels of Devotionalism and P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 88 RI reported in the all-Christian participant Christian condition, suggest that jurors rely more heavily on religion and think less rationally when the defendant is of one’s religious ingroup. These findings also suggest that the presence of religious symbols can change how jurors evaluate a case before them, increasing experiential thinking and reliance on feeling and instincts over evidence presented in a case. These findings are essential in understanding that religious material within courtrooms alters juror decision-making, compromising proper jury evaluations and verdicts. What remains less clear is the causation behind the lower Devotionalism and RI levels recorded in the Sikh condition. Two plausible explanations for these data are higher rational thinking (when faced with an outgroup member) and the social desirability effect. The social desirability effect, not giving honest answers in order to conform with societal norms, commonly takes place when respondents are asked their opinions on controversial issues, such as race and religion (e.g., Schuman & Converse, 1971; Kane &Macaulay, 1993).65 Amherst, Massachusetts, where this study was conducted, is a highly liberal area where “political correctness” is held in high esteem. Therefore, it appears likely that the pool of participants within this study would have the desire to appear less biased against an outgroup member by reducing their reported religious affiliation. In doing so, participants aim to reduce an image of outgroup prejudice, reflecting the social desirability effect. Also, evidence of shifting levels of Devotionalism and Religious Identification are not frequently reported, suggesting an effect of participant pool. And lastly, increased levels of rationality when faced with 65 As cited in Streb et al. (2008). P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 89 outgroup members is not an easily explained phenomenon, and is therefore an unlikely cause for the downward shifts in Devotionalism and Religious Identification observed. Limitations It should be noted that measures within this study may suffer from problems such as transparency of questions, weak attention, and the social desirability effect, as discussed earlier. One question was added to the questionnaire to check for adequate attention to details in the case study, which had participants mark whether they were influenced by evidence of an event that had not been included in the trial summary. As noted earlier, over half of the participants marked that the non-existent evidence influenced their sentencing decision. These data imply a lack of attention and understanding of the case summary by participants. Therefore, the answers in this study may have been overly influenced by prior attitudes and beliefs. As Giner-Sorolla et al. (2002) writes, “Prior attitudes and beliefs have been shown to influence legal decisions especially when under conditions of low ability to process information- such as high case complexity, cognitive load, or time pressure” (p. 508). The lack of accuracy in noting this evidence as not affecting one’s sentencing decisions indicates low cognitive load and understanding of the material. Evidence within this study should also be taken with a grain of salt due to low ecological validity. This study did not involve juror deliberations, limiting the interaction of jurors and the possibility of shifting judgments based on others’ opinions. Additionally, the materials of the study were typed out, not acted or filmed. This presentation of information decreases its feeling of importance. Similarly, the fact that P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 90 participants were not put in an actual court with a real defendant dampens the gravity of the participants’ decisions. Lastly, the issue of domestic abuse may be a limiting factor due to some religious teaching’s support of abuse, as well as participants’ emotional reactions to the topic. Spilka et al. (2003) note that several religious teachings condone abusive behavior as an acceptable form of punishment (de Jonge, 1995; Kroeger & Beck, 1996; Volcano Press, 1995). For example, several Biblical passages may be seen as condoning physical abuse by conveying messages such as the man is the head of the household. At the same time, religious institutions have commonly taken stances against domestic abuse (Volcano Press, 1995). Therefore, this study may be affected by whether or not one’s religious teachings condone or prohibit abuse. Similarly, the measures through the Rational-Experiential Inventory do not control for one’s emotional reaction to the issue of sexual and physical domestic abuse. While these statistics were read to represent one’s reaction to religious materials, it is likely that participants’ emotional states were altered by the content of abuse within the trial summary. Therefore, high levels of experiential thinking may have been read inaccurately as a reaction to religious symbols rather than content of abuse. Furthermore, this topic may have conjured up emotions for some participants while not affecting others. Also, while this same scene was depicted in each condition, there is the possibility of inaccurate random assignment. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 91 Suggestions for Future Research Considering the presence of several unexpected findings within this study, further research should be conducted. Specifically, it is important to gain a better understanding of ingroup biases and how they manifest in juror decision-making as well as verdicts. Also, one’s ability to take the perspective of outgroup members must be further investigated as well. Recommendations for the inclusion of perspective taking may not take place without further understanding of one’s ability to take the perspective of another while in a position of judgment. Additional studies should be run to validate and replicate the findings of this study. Further research should be conducted to verify the presence of juror biases and the power of perspective taking in negating such biases. Furthermore, the power of religious symbols over the decision-making process should be given more attention. Lastly, variations in case seriousness should be studied in order to gain a better understanding of the black sheep effect as well as similarity-leniency within religious groups, especially in a time of judgment. Implications for the Future Considering the findings that all three conviction rates dropped when perspective taking was added, and that perspective taking increased rational thought and reduced reliance on religious beliefs, perspective taking instructions must be added to jury instructions. Furthermore, as evidenced by the differences in sentencing and conviction rates between defendants when PT was not applied, ingroup and outgroup biases clearly influenced the decisions of participants. Simply knowing the religion of the defendant caused sentencing and conviction rates to fluctuate between conditions. As such, jury P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 92 biases cannot be ignored. Evidence from this study suggests that the inclusion of perspective taking instructions can greatly improve the impartiality of jury pools, the ideal in a time of judgment. Furthermore, again considering the findings such as sentence and conviction differences between defendants, as well as the possibility of the social desirability effect in response to the Sikh defendant’s religion and the black sheep effect within the Christian pool, simply adding perspective taking instructions will not solve the problem of bias within the courtroom. As such, religious symbols must be eradicated from United States courtrooms. If the presence of a simple religious sign such as the Bible or EkOnkar is enough to change how participants judge a defendant in a mock trial, one must assume that even more blatant religious symbols faced in real-life trials would be far more influential and emotionally significant. As cited by the Sixth Amendment, all citizens of the United States have the right to a trial with an impartial jury.66 The evidence of this study clearly exposes the biases held within jurors when religion of the defendant is known, as such compromising the adherence to the Sixth Amendment and the proper functioning of the United States jury processes. To sentence a defendant while relying on intuition over evidence goes directly against the purpose of the Sixth Amendment. With consideration given to these findings as well as United States law, changes must be made in our legal system to erase and prohibit any sign of religion within U.S. courtrooms. Without this change, jurors around the country will continue to 66“In all criminal prosecutions, the accused shall enjoy the right to a speedy and public trial, by an impartial jury of the State and district where in the crime shall have been committed, which district shall have been previously ascertained by law, and to be informed of the nature and cause of the accusation; to be confronted with the witnesses against him; to have compulsory process for obtaining witnesses in his favor, and to have the Assistance of Counsel for his defense”; U.S. Const., amend. VI. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 93 be unfairly influenced by outside, personal factors, risking the validity and impartiality of the trial process. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 94 Chapter Three: Regulating Religion & Implications “The ordinary rules for the administration of law are rather simple and not difficult to follow. But judges should be students of life, even more than of law. Biology and psychology, which form the basis of understanding human conduct, should be taken into account. Without fair knowledge of the mechanism of man, and the motives and urges that govern his life, it is idle to venture to fathom a situation; but, with some knowledge, officers and the public can be most useful in preserving and protecting those who most need such help. The life of almost any unfortunate, if rightly understood, can be readjusted to some plan of order and system, instead of left to drift on to ruin, the victim of ignorance, hatred and chance.” -Darrow (1940, p. 494). While the debiasing power of perspective taking instructions remains the most significant finding of this study, the assertion that religious symbols be removed from courts must be further discussed due to several controversies surrounding the separation of church and state. Acknowledging the presence of religious biases and the hampering effects of religious content on jurors’ abilities to think rationally, one is faced with the dilemma of regulating religion. As concluded in the previous chapter, perspective taking should be encouraged during jury instructions to reduce juror biases and the expression of such biases while in court. But the results of this study also indicated that religious symbols influenced jurors’ self-judgment, sentencing, and convictions. Moreover, levels of Devotionalism and religious identity shifted depending on the religion of the defendant, regardless of the presence of perspective taking. Although other measurements showed a reduction in bias (such as sentencing), it is still of utmost importance to regulate the changes observed, caused by the presence of religious symbols (especially because Devotionalism and Religious Identification correlated with experiential thinking and basing one’s decision on religious convictions). Without the removal of religiouslybased symbols, religious biases may continue to be felt in jurors even with the utilization P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 95 of perspective taking. Therefore, it is suggested that religious symbols be removed from U.S. courtrooms. This suggestion is made with the acknowledgment that many people believe the erasure of religion within courts is impossible, citing various difficulties surrounding the prohibition of religion in legal matters, such as government ties to religion (for more information regarding government entanglement with religion, including U.S. legal history surrounding the Establishment Clause and Free Exercise Clause, the “Lemon test,” faith-based rehabilitation, and the difficulties in regulating the government’s role in religious matters, please see Appendix F) or furthermore, unnecessary and wrong, noting American ties to Christianity and the benefits of invoking God in a time of judgment (e.g., creating unity, the sense of a higher judge, morality; e.g., Echoes of Grace: From the Prison to the State House, 2007; Rothchild et al., 2007; Complicity or Justice and Mercy?, 2007; Connelly, 1999). While respecting the arguments of American Christian traditions and the difficulty in separating church and state, it has become apparent through several psychological studies (e.g., Miller & Bornstein, 2005; Giner-Sorolla et al., 2002; Howard & Redfering, 1983; Jackson & Hunsberger, 1999; Johnson, 1984; Kenworthy, 2003; Kerr et al., 1995), that allowing religion within courtrooms creates bias. Including the study just conducted, it has been shown that religious beliefs hinder, provoke prejudice, and alter the decisions of participants. When put in a decision-making position, the presence of religious symbols, whether historically traditional or not, risks the proper functioning of our judicial system. As such, without condemning Christianity or religious roots of the American nation, it must be understood that the presence of such religious fervor when in P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 96 a time of decision-making is not beneficial to the defendant or jurors. As observed in the study, the invocation of religion increases ingroup and outgroup biases already present within the courtroom, illustrating the biasing effects of religion within U.S. courts. While religion may continue to play a role in several American traditions, the presence of religion within courtrooms is problematic if we wish to achieve optimal rationality and impartially within jury pools. Durkheim: Emphasizing Citizenship In the article Civil Religion in America, Bellah argues that it is not one religion alone that unites Americans, but a “civil religion,” which entails “common elements of religious orientation that the great majority of Americans share” (1967, p.171). The inauguration of the President is one example of a civil religious event. Bellah cites Dwight Eisenhower as saying, “Our government makes no sense unless it is founded in a deeply felt religious faith- and I don’t care what it is” (p.170). This statement, Bellah points out, negates the necessary presence of one specific religion. But according to Bellah, civil religion cannot replace traditional religion because religion supplies the awareness that the nation faces a higher judge: God. This awareness of authority and formidable judge is essential to the functioning of a courtroom setting and maintaining a law-abiding society. Jurors, defendants, and lawyer alike must feel the gravity of the situation and also respect and adhere to the procedures within the court. Emile Durkheim also credits religion for maintaining social order, citing the social roots of religion in lowering acts of deviance. Today, according to Durkheim, religion plays an important role in legitimizing and reinforcing society’s values and P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 97 norms (Durkheim, 1961). Yet, Durkheim does not assume these collective sentiments and ideas must be “religion,” as we think of it today. Unlike Bellah, Durkheim believes that the social root of religion is based on the concept of “collective effervescence” (1961). Collective effervescence is the primary form of religion, comprised of collective action (a group performing an action together, such as going to church), idealization (an idea that becomes known as a revelation, such as God), and concept formation (creating a new idea, such as Heaven). When these three things occur together, Durkheim proposes an electrical charge ignites between people that produces community and productivity. To Durkheim, “cults” are the secondary form of religion, which includes the titles most think of as “religion”: Christianity, Hinduism, Judaism, etc. But to Durkheim, religion can also include symbols, sports teams, bands, a concert, anything that brings people together under a common idea and excitement. “What essential difference is there between an assembly of Christians celebrating the principal dates of the life of Christ…and a reunion of citizens commemorating the promulgation of a new moral or legal system or some great event in the national life?” writes Durkheim (1961, p.475). Unlike the common perspective of religion, Durkheim sees the secondary forms of religion as fluid and inessential, but, no society can last without the primary form of religion, collective effervescence: “There can be no society which does not feel the need of upholding and reaffirming at regular intervals the collective sentiments and collective ideas which make its unity and its personality” (Durkheim, 1961, p.474-5). Therefore, according to Durkheimian theory and the importance of authority, as discussed by Bellah, I propose that Christian symbols and moral traditions within the courtroom be replaced with a secondary form of patriotism, an emphasis on citizenship P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 98 and the nation. Instead of invoking religious morals, citizen responsibility and legal regulations should be at the forefront of every person’s minds. Through this, secularism within courtrooms may be achieved. Further, secularism can provide the same collective effervescence as religion as long as it maintains the same elements: collective action, idealization, and concept formation. By banishing religious symbols while adding national symbols, attention would simply shift to another collective action. The ideal of justice and the concept of a safe and law abiding community will be present. Put together, these elements would result in a collective surge of patriotism, thus creating the same foundational social roots necessary for the stabilization of society, expressing the shared moral values of all Americans. In order to achieve collective effervescence and social order through secular nationalism, several things must be replaced in the courtroom setting. There should be no swearing to God, no Bible, and no religious symbols allowed on any individual within the courtroom. The presence of the Ten Commandments or any other religiously significant phrases would not be tolerated or deemed legal within the courtroom setting (Associated Press, 2004). Neither would prosecutorial references to the Bible or God. Instead, the United States flag should be present, along with the U.S. Constitution and other symbols representing the nation as a whole and emphasizing one’s role as a citizen in upholding the laws that regulate social order within this country. In doing so, the court will maintain the same authoritative air created by the current presence of religion, as suggested by Bellah. The judge will still play as formidable a role as before, acting as the person who upholds and enacts the laws of the United States, instead of invoking P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 99 God-like qualities and jurors will still feel a moral responsibility to provide a fair and just sentence. Furthermore, Durkheim provides an argument in support of the termination of the Bible-based swearing in process: using the Bible for swearing in assumes that each defendant believes in God. If they do not, then the act of swearing on the Bible is obsolete. As such, the United States should replace the use of religious material within courtrooms with signs of national pride and citizenship. By doing so, defendants will swear on their own pride and morals by covering their hearts instead of swearing by a God they may or may not believe in. Another alternative would be swearing on the Constitution, as such invoking the laws and traditions of the United States of America. The influence of what a defendant swears in on was clearly observed in this study. Recall that both the Sikh and Christian defendants used the words “so help me God”; the only difference in their oath was the holy book on which they placed their hands. This difference alone provoked the changes in perspective taking and rational thinking observed in the findings of the study. Considering this evidence, removing the Bible and creating one secular book or image on which defendants may take their oath will remove additional religious biases present within jurors. Emphasizing national pride and commitment to one’s personal religious morals will also reduce these biases. And, according to Durkheim, this may be achieved without compromising the court’s feeling of authority and power. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 100 Conclusion As hypothesized, the findings of this study indicated that perspective taking had a significant negative effect on sentencing as well as a positive correlation with rational thinking. As participants employed perspective taking, rational thinking increased and sentences decreased. Most importantly, the impact of perspective taking on sentencing implies the presence of juror biases, as perspective taking has been found decrease group stereotyping and debias social thought, therefore indicating bias in the non perspective taking conditions (Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000; Vescio et al., 2003). Supporting these findings, changes in intuitive-experiential and analytical-rational thinking were reflected in levels of Devotionalism, Religious Identification, and some measures by the Cognitive Experiential Self-Theory vignettes. Levels of Devotionalism and Religious Identification shifted depending on the study condition, with higher levels in the Christian condition, and lower in the Sikh condition. Devotionalism was found to be positively correlated with reliance on one’s religious beliefs and experiential thinking and Religious Identification scores were heavily correlated with reliance on instincts, a characteristic of experiential thought. Shifts of thought processes between religious conditions correlated with participant’s dependence on legal and extra legal materials when making their decisions, illustrating the instability caused by the presence of religious material in courts. These findings support the hypothesis that ingroup and outgroup biases are present within courtroom today. It also suggests that the presence of religious symbols may increase the expression of such biases, though instructions to take the perspective of the defendant can negate bias. Considering these findings, it is suggested that perspective P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 101 taking instructions be administered during the recitation of the judge’s instructions. Jurors should be urged to take the perspective of the defendant when making their decisions. These instructions will help diminish the presence and influence of personal biases within the courtroom. Additionally, it is suggested that a fundamentalism scale be added to the process of voir dire, in order to determine jurors who will likely be unable to avoid prejudiced decisions and experiential thinking (e.g., Jackson et al., 1997; Herek, 1987; Griffen et al., 1987; Unger, 2002; Hunsberger et al., 1999; Spilka et al., 2003; McFarland, 1989; Miller & Hayward, 2007). As noted, fundamentalist beliefs, the ascription to a religion’s definition of the “inerrant truth about humanity,” drastically hinder the decision-making process, plausibly altering the perception of a defendant as well as a case (Altemeyer & Hunsberger, 1992). There has been consistent criticism of the “death-qualifying”67 procedures currently used during voir dire for their inability to detect juror biases. “If voir dire questions can be utilized to indentify jurors who would both follow the law and be impartial, then the process would help the court maintain an image of impartiality,” writes Nason (2005). While we still do not have a complete understanding of juror bias, we must take advantage of the knowledge that has been acquired regarding juror prejudice. The utilization the fundamentalism scale would improve the current voir dire jury selection methods as well as reduce bias in non capital punishment cases, fulfilling the ideal voir dire selection. Additionally, this method would also inhibit discrimination “Death qualifying” refers to choosing jurors who are able to follow the juror’s oath and the law, despite whatever personal feelings they may have for or against the death penalty. See, Wainwright v. Witt, 469 U.S. 412, 424 (1985). 67 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 102 against religious groups as a whole, as the scale would focus on individual beliefs, not the title of one’s religious affiliation (the current laws set in Batson v. Kentucky68 and J.E.B. v. Alabama69 regarding preemptory challenges forbid race and gender-based juror discrimination, with several courts extending the law to cover religion-based discrimination,70 claiming unconstitutional discrimination toward the excluded juror).71 Because of the overwhelming evidence pointing towards fundamentalism’s inherent ties to right-wing authoritarianism, prejudice towards outgroups, and lower levels of complex thought, I believe that individuals with high scores on the religious fundamentalism scales should not be allowed to participate in juror panels and deliberations. While according to this study’s findings perspective taking instructions would be beneficial in courts, the integration of a fundamentalism test is also essential. This study also highlights the negative effects of religious material within courts. Seeing as data shifted depending on the religious affiliation of the juror, and that perspective taking instructions were less influential on the Devotionalism and Religious Identification scales when religious symbols were present, it must also be suggested that religion be removed from the courts. As discussed earlier, emphasis on citizenship and Batson v. Kentucky, 476 U.S. 79 (1985); Batson held that race-based peremptory challenges violate the rights of the defendant and potential juror: “The Equal Protection Clause guarantees the defendant that the State will not exclude members of his race from the jury venire on account of race…[B]y denying a person participation in jury service on account of his race, the State unconstitutionally discriminated against the excluded juror.” 476 U.S. at 86-87. (Barton, 1995) 69 J.E.B. v. Alabama, 114 S. Ct. 1419 (1994); Ruled that it was unconstitutional to remove a juror based on gender. 70 See, United States v. Carolene Products Co., 204 U.S. 144, 152 n.4 (1938), where the Court listed religion along with race as a suspect category. 71 As stated in Barton (1995), “A government religious classification which results in members of a religion being denied the opportunity to serve on a jury clearly constitutes a ‘burden’ on the free exercise of that religion” (p. 207). Distributing a fundamentalism scale to each individual, regardless of religious affiliation, would bypass this constitutional conflict. 68 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 103 national pride should replace the cohesive effects of religion. Yet, understanding the difficulty in replacing religion within the public sphere, and the innate connotations held between “judgment” and “God,” the role of perspective taking in negating juror biases and increasing rational thought remains the most integral and valuable finding of this study. Further conclusions. Findings of this study also emphasize the need for further psychological analysis of religion. While the issue of religion is seen by many as a taboo subject, recognizing the complications and intricacies of faith, studies such as this demonstrate how strongly religious values dictate individual behaviors and how integral religion is in the lives of many Americans. Evidence that religious values alter how people think and judge others should be of key concern to psychologists researching issues in and out of the courtroom. Thought processes, ingroup and outgroup biases, and the ability to take the perspective of others are issues relevant every day. While chapter one reviewed the difficulties in measuring an individual’s religious beliefs and the several scales that have been created and often disproven, religion itself is not a matter psychologists can push aside. God is a powerful concept that overwhelms how many individuals think, act, and feel, and as such is an integral issue for psychology. The results of this study not only emphasize the need to integrate perspective taking in courtrooms, but also the pervasiveness of religious thought and religious reliance in the general public. The effects observed from participants reading a case scenario with the written mention of religious symbols indicates the possibility of even stronger effects of religion in everyday life. Because the issue of faith is so personal and P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 104 powerful, psychologists should dedicate significant time and energy in gaining a better understanding of how and why religion affects individuals the way it does, what about religion is so enticing to the human psyche, why it is relied upon so often, and what the consequences of these behaviors are. Moreover, where else is religion used to sway people? Where are other religious symbols present? All in all, the issue of religion deserves more attention from psychologists studying behaviors in and out of the legal arena, as it affects millions of Americans every day and can drastically improve our understanding of human behaviors and motives. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 105 Appendix A This is the trial summary that was administered. The information in brackets changed depending on the condition. The Trial: [The trial began by Johnson raising his hand and putting the other over his heart, repeating, “I swear to tell the truth, the whole truth, and nothing but the truth.” After being sworn in, the trial proceeded.] [The trial began by Matthews raising his hand and swearing on the Bible, repeating, “I swear to tell the truth, the whole truth, and nothing but the truth, so help me God.” After being sworn in, the trial proceeded.] [The trial began by Rajpal raising his hand and swearing in on the Guru Granth Sahib, repeating, “I swear to tell the truth, the whole truth, and nothing but the truth, so help me God.” After being sworn in, the trial proceeded.] The following case facts were established during the trial: On May 9th, 1995 [Robert Johnson/Paul Matthews/Talib Rajpal] arrived at the home of his wife, [Rebecca Johnson/Mary Matthews/Roopali Rajpal], after her two young children by another man had left for school, and began choking and pushing her. She fell, her leg folded awkwardly under her, and her ankle broke. The two had been having marital difficulties for quite some time. The victim testified that she and [Johnson/Matthews/Rajpal] had been married about a year when they became estranged. Their marriage was stormy and they had many arguments after which [Mr. Johnson/Matthews/Rajpal] would move out to stay with his mother. [Mr. Johnson/Matthews/Rajpal] also admitted to having numerous affairs during their marriage. Both spouses testified that until the May 9, 1995 incident that resulted in [Johnson’s/Matthews’/Rajpal’s] conviction, he had never struck his wife. [Mr. Johnson/Matthews/Rajpal], an unemployed guitar teacher, testified that his wife slipped and broke her ankle as she attacked him with a kitchen knife. He then grabbed her by the hand and threw her over the bed. With this, the knife buried itself in the mattress, and [Johnson/Matthews/Rajpal] claims he took the opportunity to take it away. He said that he delayed calling an ambulance for three hours, even though she claimed to be intense pain, because she was getting dressed and putting on her makeup. "You know how women are," he said on the stand. According to [Rebecca Johnson’s/Mary Matthews’/Roopali Rajpal’s] testimony, [Robert Johnson/Paul Matthews/Talib Rajpal] came over to pick up papers he had left behind at the house. According to the victim, upon his arrival he grabbed his wife by the neck and began saying that she would have to learn how to respect him, because he was P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 106 her husband and she had to do whatever he said, that this thing of her doing whatever she wanted was going to come to an end. [Mr. Johnson/Matthews/Rajpal] then grabbed a kitchen knife and began yelling that it was too late for both of them and they both were going to die. He began to rip her clothes off, she said, but when she screamed in pain he ordered her to take them off. Then he sexually abused her. At one point, [Johnson/Matthews/Rajpal] tried to stab her but she moved aside and the knife pierced the mattress. Then he broke down and asked her forgiveness. She finally convinced him to call an ambulance around 11:30 A.M., about three hours after she broke her ankle. At the hospital, the victim called a friend who worked for the Department of Correction. When the friend arrived, she got [Mr. Johnson/Matthews/Rajpal] to leave his wife's side and got a hospital social worker to interview the victim, prosecutors said. [Mrs. Johnson/Matthews/Rajpal] says that her husband never expressed any remorse over the attack. [Mrs. Johnson/Matthews/Rajpal] said that she did not know what had sparked that particular outbreak, but that she was sure she had seen her husband outside her window a few days prior. While the man she saw was dressed in black, head mostly hidden by a cap, she noticed his tattoo [of the Christian cross/ of the Sikh symbol Ek-Onkar] on his neck, and was thus able to identify him. [Johnson/Matthews/Rajpal] never confronted her husband about the alleged spying. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 107 Appendix B This is the dependant measures survey designed to measure the participant’s sentencing decision, confidence in the decision, what influenced the decision, a participant’s religious identification and devotionalism, rational and experiential thinking, as well as perspective taking. The dependent measure survey contained measures of: ï‚· The participant’s sentencing decision (guilty or not guilty; 4 ½ years to life sentence) ï‚· One question measuring how certain the participant is of his/her verdict (10 points) ï‚· One question asking how strongly the participant agrees that the defendant is guilty of all four counts (10 points) ï‚· One question asking how strongly the participant feels that the defendant is guilty of aggravated sexual abuse (10 points) ï‚· One question asking if the participant’s opinion regarding rape and adultery is based on his/her religious beliefs (10 points) ï‚· Fourteen questions concerning how influenced participant’s were by thirteen elements (judge’s instructions, evidence that the husband broke his wife’s ankle, the opinion that the defendant showed no regret, evidence that the husband tried to stab his wife, evidence that the couple had been estranged, personal religious beliefs, personal feelings about rape, feelings about this case, instincts about this case, feelings about the defendant, logic and reason, common sense, other) with one comprehension check of “evidence that the husband tried to break his wife’s arm.” (10 points) ï‚· One manipulation check for instruction comprehension concerning whether the participant was trying to take the perspective of the defendant (10 points). ï‚· A blank for participants to write their age ï‚· A question about gender (check Male or Female) ï‚· Two questions about student status (yes/no) and if yes, a blank to write major course of study. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 108 ï‚· One question about ethnic background (check African American, Asian American, Hispanic American, Native American, White American or other) ï‚· One question about religious background (check Christian, Jewish, Hindu, Buddhist, Muslim, Atheist, Agnostic, “I believe in God, but do not have a particular faith” or Other) ï‚· One question regarding how closely the participant follows his/her religion (10 points; Miller, 2006) and two questions regarding ingroup identification and religious identification: “To what extent do you identify with other members of your religious group?” and “To what extent do you feel close to other members of your religious group?” (10 point scale, Levin & Sidanius, 1999). These were scored together to indicate participant’s Religious Identification. ï‚· A 7-item Devotionalism measure (10 point scale; Putney & Middleton, 1961) ï‚· The 10-item Rational-Experiential Inventory (REI; 5 point scale; Epstein et al., 1996) ï‚· A 13-item perspective taking measure (Interpersonal Reactivity Index; Davis 1980, 1983). ï‚· CEST measures (3 vignettes; Epstein et al., 1996). P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 109 Appendix C These are the vignettes administered from Epstein et al. (1999). These were meant to be a non-self report measure of rationality and one’s ability to recognize the rational decision. 1. Ms. Crane and Ms. Tees were scheduled to leave the airport at the same time, but on different flights. They traveled from town in the same limousine, were caught in a traffic jam, and arrived at the airport 30 minutes after their scheduled departure of their flights. Ms. Crane was told that her flight had left on time. Ms. Tees was told that her flight had been delayed and had left only 5 minutes ago. Who do you think felt her dawdling was more foolish? Ms. Crane, Ms. Tees, or neither? Who do you think should have felt her dawdling was more foolish? Ms. Crane, Ms. Tees, or neither? Why? 2. Carol almost never takes hitch-hikers in her car. Yesterday she gave a man a ride and was robbed. Nancy frequently takes hitch-hikers in her car. Yesterday she gave a man a ride and was robbed. Who do you think felt more upset about giving the hitchhiker a ride? Carol, Nancy, or neither one? Who do you think should have felt more upset about giving the hitch-hiker a ride? Carol, Nancy, or neither one? Why? 3. Steve had heard from his friend Jonathan about the fantastic cake he had eaten during his vacation, when on a visit to Turkey. As Steve had a sweet tooth he was determined to try it. Fortunately, he heard about a delicatessen store in the next town, which supposedly had a good selection of foreign foods. The next day he drove there and sure enough he found what he was looking for. The cake was very expensive, and cost him $4.50 for a single piece. Now image a slightly different version. Bill, another friend of Jonathan, had heard exactly the same thing. Assume that everything is the same except that when Bill went to the delicatessen, he was fortunate enough to run into a special promotion and got the same pastry for 25 cents. When both of them tasted the cake, they decided it was too sweet for their taste. Who do you think will be more likely to take more bites before discarding the cake? Steve, Bill, or neither one? Who should take more bites before discarding the cake? Steve, Bill, or neither one? Why? P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 110 Appendix D This chart represents the correlations between what participants based their sentencing decision on. Correlations judge judge religion personal feelings instincts defendant logic common sense Pearson Correlation Pearson Correlation Pearson Correlation Pearson Correlation Pearson Correlation Pearson Correlation Pearson Correlation Pearson Correlation religion personal feelings instincts defendant logic .031 -.070 .318(**) .025 .292(**) .406(**) -.105 .170 .252(**) .638(**) .126 .182(*) .355(**) .602(**) .454(**) .185(* ) .109 -.033 .003 -.013 -.141 .072 .099 .037 .210(*) .145 .050 ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed). N=133 KEY judge= “Judge’s instructions” religion= “Your personal religious beliefs” personal= “Your personal feelings about rape” feelings= “My feelings about this case” instincts= “My instincts about this case” defendant= “My feelings about the defendant” logic= “Logic and reason” common sense= “Common sense” .500(**) common sense P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 111 Appendix E This chart represents the correlations between what religious participants based their sentencing decision on and their levels of rational and experiential thinking as well as perspective taking. This chart does not include participants who labeled themselves atheists, in order to demonstrate the strong ties between religious beliefs and experiential thinking. Correlations without Atheist Participants SUM OF RELI ID SUM OF RELI ID instincts SUM DEV SUM RATIONAL SUM EXPERIENTI AL SUM PT Pearson Correlation Pearson Correlation Pearson Correlation Pearson Correlation Pearson Correlation Pearson Correlation instincts SUM DEV SUM RATIONAL SUM EXPERIENTI AL .243(**) .609(**) .047 -.204(*) -.005 -.066 .155 .360(**) .102 -.005 -.197(*) -.199(*) -.064 .185(*) ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed). N=117 KEY SUM OF RELI ID= Religious Identification instincts=“My instincts about this case” SUM DEV= Devotionalism SUM RATIONAL= REI Rational Score SUM EXPERIENTIAL= REI Experiential Score SUM PT= IRI Perspective Taking Score Appendix D -.125 SUM PT P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 112 Appendix F: Regulating Religion The Religion Clauses Religious content is frequently present in U.S. legal conflicts and arguments. The number of legal cases regarding religious issues is too plentiful to count, including religious objections to wars,72 public education73 and marriage laws.74 The most current cases regarding religion include the American Jewish Congress vs. Corporation for National Community Services,75 fighting direct public funds for religious teachings and Lown et al. v. Salvation Army et al.,76 fighting religious selectivity. As such, the U.S. government has attempted to control legal entanglement with religion in several ways, the most prominent being the establishment of the Religion Clauses: The Establishment Clause as well as the Free Exercise Clause. These clauses state that, “Congress shall make no law respecting an establishment of religion or prohibiting the free exercise thereof."77 This First Amendment clause was intended to afford protection against sponsorship, financial support, and active involvement of the government in religious activity; it was put in place so that the legal system would have rules to follow in regard to religious cases. See e.g., United States v. Seeger, 380, U.S. 246; 85 S. Ct. 850 (1965); Welch vs. U.S. 398 U.S. 338; 90 S. Ct. 1702 (1970) 73 See e.g., Wisconsin v. Yoder, 406 U.S. 205 (1972) 74 See e.g., United States v. Reynolds, 397 U.S. 14 (1970) 75 American Jewish Congress v. Corporation for National and Community Service, case No. 1:02CV01948, U.S. District Court for the District of Columbia 76 S.D.N.Y., No. 04 Civ. 1562 77 U. S. Constitution, Amend. 1 72 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 113 Yet the language of these Clauses is frequently debated, and the true meaning and intention of the Clauses has been called “at best opaque.”78 The uncertainty and incomplete understanding of the wording has caused innumerable debates regarding the appropriate reading of the Clauses, including when and how they should be applied. Some of the best examples portraying the controversy surrounding the Religion Clauses lie in the cases of Everson v. Board of Education, Engel v. Vitale, the “Lemon test,” and differences in interpretation between two United States Supreme Court Justices: Rehnquist and O’Connell. In the case of Everson v. Board of Education (1947), the U.S. Supreme Court debated the reimbursement to parents of children attending a parochial school for bus transportation expenses. After a lengthy debate concerning whether or not the reimbursement violated the First Amendment, the Court decided to uphold the statute, basing their decision on New Jersey’s claim that the money was applied to all citizens, regardless of religious affiliation. Yet, the decision was not made with complete certainty of its constitutionality. At the time of the trial, Mr. Justice Black, representing the majority, wrote that the decision carried to “the verge” of forbidden territory under the Religion Clauses. This occurrence, as stated earlier in Lemon v. Kurtzman (1971), “compels acknowledgement…that we can only dimly perceive the lines of demarcation in this extraordinarily sensitive area of constitutional law.” Disconcertingly, Mr. Justice 78As noted by Chief Justice Burger; Lemon v. Kurtzman, (1971), pg. 3. The authors of the Clause do not forbid the establishment of a church or official religion, instead they instruct that no law shall be made “respecting an establishment of religion”. The definition of “respecting” and its intended purpose have been argued time and again, blurring the lines as to where and when religious acts are legal in the United States. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 114 Black’s remarks are but one example of government officials expressing their uncertainty of the meaning of the Religion Clauses, and how best to apply them. In an unrelated but similar trial concerning religion and education, the Supreme Court ruled in the opposite direction as Everson v. Board of Education (1947). In one of the most controversial legal battles concerning prayer, Engel v. Vitale (1962), the Court held that the recitation of an allegedly nondenominational prayer in public schools violated the Establishment Clause. In delivering the opinion of the Court, Mr. Justice Black stated that, “there can…be no doubt that New York’s program of daily classroom invocation of God’s blessings as prescribed in the Regent’s prayer is a religious activity.” Later he wrote that it made no difference if the content of the prayer was said to be nondenominational, it was simply the presence of religion that was the problem. In opposition, Justice Stewart passionately argued against Justice Black, arguing that forbidding prayer was unconstitutional and unlawful. In the end, the Court went in accordance to Black’s position, though the case was fraught with significant disagreement largely caused by the space of interpretation left open by the Religion Clauses. In an effort to define how and when to follow the Religion Clauses of the first Amendment, the case Lemon v. Kurtzman (1971) created what is now known as the “Lemon test.” The “Lemon test” states that, “[a] statue must have a secular legislative purpose; second, it’s principal or primary effect must be one that neither advances nor inhibits religion…; [and] finally, the statue must not foster ‘an excessive government entanglement with religion’” (Lemon v. Kurtzman, 1971). In the case of Lemon v. Kurtzman, the Court decided on the constitutionality of the Rhode Island Salary P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 115 Supplement Act, which asked the state to supplement teacher’s salaries of secular subjects in nonpublic elementary schools, and the Pennsylvania Nonpublic Elementary and Secondary Education Act, which called for the government to directly reimburse the school for textbooks, supplies, and teacher’s salaries in secular subjects. While recognizing that “religious values pervade the fabric of our national life,” the court felt that the religious motivations of these educational institutions qualified as “excessive entanglement with religion” and did not uphold either act. Since the creation of the “Lemon Test,” courts around the country have referenced the three pronged test in determining whether or not a religious situation can and should be upheld by the government. Yet, even with this additional attempt at clarification, the “Lemon test” has not achieved complete success in disentangling the issues surrounding the understanding of the Establishment Clause. Most recently, this debate was observed through two recent United States Justices: Justices Rehnquist and O’Connor, who both made key contributions to Religion Clause jurisprudence. Chief Justice William Rehnquist has argued that the reading of the Establishment Clause ignores the importance of religion in the lives of individuals and communities in the United States (Lupu & Tuttle, 2005). In the case of Wallace v. Jaffree (1985), Rehnquist asserted that secularization and separationism (complete separation of church and state) is not required by the Establishment Clause, dissenting that the majority’s reading of the Establishment Clause was incorrect. 79 According to Rehnquist, the Establishment Clause should not be 79 The Court held unconstitutional an Alabama statute that instituted a one-minute period for “meditation or silent prayer” at the opening of each day in public schools; 472 U.S. 38, 91 (1985) (Rehnquist, J., dissenting). P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 116 interpreted based on Thomas Jefferson’s claim supporting “a wall of separation” between church and state, as the history following clearly indicates that this was a minority opinion and that Jefferson himself approved support of religious movements (Lupu & Tuttle, 2005). Going further, in the case of Bowen v. Kendrick (1988), Rehnquist challenged the illegality of direct payment of public funds to religious organizations, claiming that these payments do not necessarily advance religion, and thus should be legal.80 Rehnquist drew a line between pervasively sectarian institutions and religious organizations in general, claiming that giving government funds to religious organizations does not go against the third prong of the “Lemon test” (excessive entanglement of government and religion). They, unlike pervasive sectarian institutions, are far more likely to abide by government rules and regulate the pervasiveness of religion in their works (Lupu & Tuttle, 2005). In response, over the next fifteen years, the Court’s decisions on the direct funding of religious entities reflected the opinion of Rehnquist, allowing direct aid to religious schools in several cases.81 This shift in Supreme Court decisions exemplifies the fluidity and uncertainty surrounding the application of these laws, changing years of Court verdicts based on one new interpretation of a text. Unlike Rehnquist, Justice Sandra Day O’Connor’s main concern with the Establishment Clause and “Lemon test” is the issue of governmental endorsement of 80 487 U.S. 589 (1988); This case involved “Adolescent Family Life Act (AFLA), which awarded grants for teaching and counseling teenagers about sexual abstinence and encouraged grantees to make subgrants to religious organizations and other non-profit community groups, in order to expand the range of providers for the counseling and educational services” (Lupu & Tuttle, 2005, p.5). 81 See e.g., Agostini v. Felton, 521 U.S. 203 (1997), and Mitchell v. Helms, 530 U.S. 793, 810 (2000). P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 117 religion.82 She has expressed that it is constitutionally necessary that government not aid activities that are religious in content, and that the government establish, “reasonable safeguards against diversion of public aid to impermissible purposes” (Lulu & Tuttle, 2005). For O’Connor, the Clause prohibits official governmental support or endorsement of indoctrination into a religious faith, and does not take issue with the idea of secularism or separation, like Rehnquist.83 Both of these Justices view the purpose of the Religion Clauses differently and as such interpret the wording in contrasting manners. As such, it is easy to see that even today Supreme Court Justices differ on their readings of the Establishment Clause and “Lemon test,” resulting in inconsistent verdicts and also compromising an appropriate measure of the separation between church and state (whatever that may be). The three court cases of Everson v. Board of Education (1947), Engel v. Vitale (1962), and Lemon v. Kurtzman (1971), as well as the differing opinions of Justices Rehnquist and O’Connor demonstrate our country’s battle in regulating the role of religion in legal matters. This concept is important to note when considering the findings of this study. Though it was found that religion impacts juror decisions, the issue of removing religious symbols from the courtroom is fraught with controversy. While it may be argued that the removal of religion will increase rational thought and reliance on evidence and the judge’s instructions to come to a verdict, this seems unlikely to occur given that the “Endorsement [of religion] sends a message to nonadherents that they are outsiders, not full members of the political community, and an accompanying message to adherents that they are insiders, favored members of the political community. Disapproval sends the opposite message.” Lynch v. Donnelly, 465 U.S. 668, 688 (1984) (O’Connor, J., concurring); As cited in Lupu & Tuttle (2005, p. 10). 83 Mitchell v. Helms (2000), 530 U.S. 793, 844-45 (O’Connor, J., concurring in the judgment), as cited in Lupu & Tuttle, 2005, p. 16. 82 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 118 United States has shown its inability to separate itself from religion throughout history, and continues to do so today. This fact serves to emphasize the importance of integrating perspective taking instructions to counter the effects of religious biases. Faith-Based Rehabilitation In recent years, government leniency concerning “charitable choice”84 provisions towards religious groups as well as prison population increases has created an emergence of government funded faith-based rehabilitation prison programs in the United States. These programs exemplify the government’s increasing participation in religious matters and the ongoing troubles besieging the interpretation of the Religion Clauses. With the creation of President George W. Bush’s “Office of Faith-Based and Community Initiatives,” government funded faith-based programs were quickly put to work, providing large grants for projects commonly favored by conservative Christians, such as Charles Colson’s Prison Fellowship Ministries85 and Teen Challenge, a drug rehabilitation program that encourages religious conversion.86 Many claim that the recent resurgence of political religious action was due to President Bush’s religious fervor and the need to retain religious voters.87 In 1996, President Bill Clinton has signed into law a bill that included the option of “charitable choice” provisions, thus allowing religious groups the opportunity to compete for government grants (Jacoby, 2009). 85 According to the New York Times, “Prison Fellowship Ministries was founded by Charles, W. Colson, an ally of President Bush and an influential evangelical who went to prison for his role in the Watergate cover-up in the Nixon administration” (Banerjee, 2007). 86 According to an article by the New York Times, Teen Challenge goes so far as to call teenage converts from Judaism to Christianity, “completed Jews” (Jacoby, 2009). 87 President Bush’s brother, Jed Bush, claimed that he and his brother believe that the best way to rehabilitate prisoners is to “lead them to God.” As cited in Roy (2005); Price, J.H. (2003, December 30). Where punishment must fit the faith, Washington Times, A1. 84 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 119 One of the best-known and most controversial programs funded by the Bush Administration is known as the InnerChange Freedom Initiative (IFI), an organization affiliated with the Prison Fellowship Ministries and the Iowa Corrections Department. IFI is a key example of a program that challenges the role of the Religion Clauses and also highlights the everlasting and ever-growing entanglement of government with religious matters. Based on Christian philosophies, IFI aimed to rehabilitate prisoners by “spiritual and moral regeneration” through education and psychological programming, which included religious instructions and one-on-one mentoring (Roy, 2005). Though participation in the program was voluntary and claimed to be tolerant of various religious beliefs, the curriculum of the prison was indisputably Christian.88 The prison’s brochure labeled the program a “24-hour-a-day, Christ-centered, biblically based program that promotes personal transformation through the power of the Gospel” (Banerjee, 2007). “The notion of transformation,” notes Rothchild (2007), “has played a central role within the Christian tradition” (p. 82). Additionally, it was later discovered that prisoners participating in IFI were given privileges over other inmates, and had to accept Christianity in order to participate. In response, on December 3, 2007, a federal appeals panel ruled that the program violated the Religion Clauses due to its effect of advancing and endorsing religion (Banerjee, 2007). The case had been filed over four years earlier by Americans United for Separation of Church and State and was decided upon by a panel of judges of the United States Court of Appeals for the Eighth Circuit in St. Louis. In an article by Roy (2005) writes that during one session an instructor announced, “For those of you who are Muslim, Jesus is God…I’m sorry if I’ve offended you, but Jesus is God.” 88 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 120 Banerjee (2007), Professor Robert Tuttle is quoted saying that the decision, “reaffirm[s] the obligation of government not to fund programs that intermingle secular and religious content.” This decision is said to have supplied more clarity for what institutions pass the constitutional test. Even so, governmental funding to faith-based initiatives has not slowed. This year, President Barack Obama, though promising the prohibition of proselytizing and religious hiring from federally financed programs, has left the Bush Administration’s orders intact. In fact, the now-titled “White House Office for Religion-based and Neighborhood Programs” expands upon Bush policies by providing government support to religious organizations that supply social services (Zeleny & Goodstein, 2009). R. Albert Mohler Jr., president of the Southern Baptist Theological Seminary even claims that Obama’s goal of funding religious organizations while prohibiting proselytizing and religious hiring is completely irrational and unrealistic (Jacoby, 2009). These two elements are essential to the functioning of any religious organization, he stated. Halfheartedly recognizing this contradiction, the Obama administration has admitted that there is a “lack of clarity in this area” and will thus review each case on a case-by-case basis. The decision to uphold government support of religious institutions directly contradicts several previous Court decisions based upon the upholding of the Religion Clauses. Furthermore, the recognition of the possible unconstitutionality of this law begs one to question why government continues its connections with religion. Some claim political reasons, such as gaining approval from the religious right, while others argue P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 121 that religion is innate in our legal and government structures, and therefore should be included. Innate Ties between Religion and Law Many claim that the difficulty of separating church and state lies within America’s innate ties to religion. While our Founding Fathers stated that church and state are to remain separate, the laws and traditions of this country grew from seeds of Christian faith and moral standing. We share a public religion that Robert Bellah labels the “civil religion”, that “provide[s] a religious dimension for the whole fabric of American life (1967, p.171). Christianity, writes William Connolly, “is already inscribed in the prediscursive dispositions and cultural instincts of the civilization” (1999).89 This “subterranean flow” of religion, as Connolly labels it, “persists despite the disestablishment of religion” (Smith, 1997).90 Sunday, Connolly writes, is a prime example of a common word heavily overlaid with religious connotations, based on Christian traditions. These connotations, whether we are aware of them or not, “color all judgments” we make concerning the day (Connolly, 1999).91 Christianity, it seems, maintains an innate connection to United States law. A key example of this perspective was expressed by Justice Stewart in the dissent of Engel v. Vitale (1962). Mr. Justice Stewart argued that, “to deny the wish of these children to join in reciting this prayer is to deny them the opportunity of sharing in the According to Connolly, there is a “subterranean flow” of religion running through our culture, constantly mixing private religion and the public sector. 90 Smith labels this the “defacto disestablishment.” 91 The issue of Sunday closings was also discussed earlier in Chapter 1 with regards to McGowan v. Maryland (1961). 89 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 122 spiritual heritage of our Nation.” He then went on to list the ways in which Americans are a religious people, including the addition of the words “under God” in the National Anthem, the words “In God We Trust” printed on dollar bills, and the fact that both the Senate and the House of Representatives open their daily Sessions with a prayer. “We are a religious people, whose institutions presuppose a Supreme Being,” Stewart cites.92 Stewart argues that God and religion are everywhere, and we need to let people pray if they so desire. Stewart’s argument exemplifies the role of religion in the United States and the extraordinary challenge of demarcating places and times in which religious beliefs are appropriate. His statement that “we are a religious people,” supports the claims of Connolly and Bellah that whether we are aware of it or not, religion permeates American culture and traditions. In the end, Engel ruled against school prayer, provoking public retaliation and extensive ridicule of the Court, which, according to many, “had betrayed the American way of life” (Feldman, 2000). Engel eventually led to a surge of requests to add a Christian amendment to the Constitution; in fact, the 1964 platform of the Republican Party proposed such an amendment, clearly supporting the argument of Justice Stewart that we are a religious people, with our founding in Christianity. For these reasons, many argue that secularization and the separation of church and state are impossible. Christianity simply becomes underwritten as culture, claims Tocqueville, without most citizens’ recognition (Tocqueville et al., 2000). Tocqueville writes that American mores, traditions and customs, are controlled by religion, as is reason. He considers these Christian dogmas to be political barriers, as citizens do not know how to step outside of them, limiting our liberties to act outside of religious 92 Zorach v. Clauson, 343 U.S. 306, 313. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 123 regulations. “For the Americans the idea of Christianity and liberty are so completely mingled that it is almost impossible to get them to conceive of the one without the other” (Tocqueville et al., 2000, p. 293). Even Supreme Court Justices have difficulty separating their religious beliefs from their legal decisions. “Because most Supreme Court justices are themselves largely embedded in the symbols and structures of American Christian society, they tend to reach decisions that manifest and then reproduce those very symbols and structures” (Feldman, 2000, p.4). Concerns regarding this pattern arose when Supreme Court Justice Scalia recently made religious remarks at a law school breakfast. His remarks prompted many to raise concerns regarding his ability to objectively and evenhandedly make judgment, especially concerning church-state issues (Saphire, 1998). Many argue that it is simply impossible to escape the Christian roots, even within the basic traditions of the judicial system. For example, in March v. Chambers (1983), the Supreme Court upheld the practice of having a publicly paid chaplain, in this case a Protestant minister, open state legislative sessions with a prayer.93 The Court came to this conclusion by claiming it was American tradition to have a public funded chaplain. As Feldman (2000) states: The Court’s reliance on history tends to give a constitutional imprimatur to the preexisting symbols and structures of American society to the symbols and structures of Christian domination. Indeed, in Marsh, the Court stressed that it 93 March v. Chambers, 463 U.S. 783 (1983) P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 124 sought to acknowledge ‘beliefs widely held among the people of this country’; such beliefs unavoidably manifest Christian values and practices (p.4). If Christian influence is truly unavoidable in American culture, many question why church and state should be separated (e.g., Echoes of Grace: From the Prison to the State House, 2007; Rothchild et al., 2007; Complicity or Justice and Mercy?, 2007; Connelly, 1999). Many believe that Judeo-Christian beliefs play an essential role of in American culture and that Christian morality can aid our society. “Churches are explicitly value-bearing institutions…Churches ought to articulate a vision of the good ordering of society,” writes Livezey (2007, p.43). “Laws are not enough” (Livezey, 2007, p.43). Going back to Judefind v. State in 1894, in arguing the legality of Sunday closings, it was stated that, “It is undoubtedly true that rest from secular employment on Sunday does have a tendency to foster and encourage the Christian religion,” which is all the more reason to enforce it (As cited in McGowan v. Maryland, 1960, p.447).94 Even today, many judges and court officials support the incorporation of Christianity in legal decisions. For example, on December 14, 2004 Alabama Judge Ashley McKathan conducted a trial while wearing a judicial robe embroidered with the Ten Commandments, easily legible throughout the court. McKathan justified his behavior, telling The Associated Press that he believes the Ten Commandments represent the truth "and you can't divorce the law from the truth. ... The Ten Commandments can help a judge know the difference between right and wrong" (2004). McKathan stated that 94 Judefind v. State, 78 Md. 510, 514, 28 A. 405, 406 (1894). P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 125 he believes the law is based on more than just words written in law books.95 Former Alabama Chief Justice Roy Moore supported McKathan’s decision, saying, “I applaud Judge McKathan. It is time for our judiciary to recognize the moral basis of our law” (Associated Press, 2004).96 In light of such actions, it is plain to see that there are many Americans who believe that Christianity is inseparable from U.S. legal and governmental decisions, and moreover should not be separated due to the beneficial factors of the Christian faith. Four years later, McKathan ordered a group prayer in the courtroom, asking that everyone hold hands and pray (Associated Press, 2008). 96 According to the Associated Press (2004), former Alabama Chief Justice Moore was “removed from office in 2003 for refusing to remove a Ten Commandments monument from the rotunda of the Alabama Judicial Building in Montgomery.” 95 P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 126 References (2005, 3 21) Ex-prosecutor says he kept Jews off juries. Retrieved December 8, 2007, from CNN.com Web site: http://www.cnn.com/2005/LAW/03/21/jurors.ethnicity.ap /index.html Allport, G.W. & J.M. Ross. (1967). Personal religious orientation and prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 432-43. Altemeyer, B. (1996). The Authoritarian Specter. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Altemeyer, B., & Hunsberger, B. (1992). Authoritarianism, religious fundamentalism, quest, and prejudice. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 2, 113-133. Amato, P.R. (1979). Juror-defendant similarity and the assessment of guilt in politically motivated crimes. Australian Journal of Psychology, 31, 79-88. Argyle, M. (1959). Religious Behavior. Glencoe, IL: Free Press. Associated Press, (2004, December 15). Judge's robe bears Ten Commandments. FoxNews.com, Retrieved March 13, 2009, from http://www.foxnews.com/story/0,2933,141541,00.html Associated Press, (2008, August, 14). Alabama judge faces complaint for ordering group to pray in court. FoxNews.com, Retrieved March 13, 2009, from http://www.foxnews.com/story/0,2933,404166,00.html Banerjee, N. (2007, December 4). Court bars state effort using faith in prisons. New York Times. Baron, R.A. (1979). Effects of victim pain cues, victim’s race, and level of prior instigations upon physical aggression. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 9, 110-114. Barton, B.H. (1995). Religion-Based peremptory challenges after Batson v. Kentucky and J.E.B. v. Alabama: An equal protection and first amendment analysis. Michigan Law Review, 94, 1, 191-216. Batson, C.D. (1991). The Altruism Question: Toward a Social-Psychological Answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Batson, C.D. & Burris, C.T. (1994). Personal religion: Depressant or stimulant of prejudice and discrimination? In M.P. Zanna & J.P. Olsen. (Eds). The Psychology of Prejudice: The Ontario Symposium, 7, 149-69. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Batson, C.D., Flink, C.H., Schoenrade, P.A., Fultz, J., & Pych, V. (1986). Religious orientation and overt and covert racial prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 175-81. Batson, C.D., Naifeh, S.J., & Pate, S. (1978). Social desirability, religiosity, and prejudice. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 17, 31-41. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 127 Batson, C.D., Schoenrade, P., & Ventis, W. L. (1993). Religion and the Individual: A Social Psychological Perspective. New York: Oxford University Press. Bellah, R. (1967). Civil religion in America. Beyond Belief. 169-189. Belli, M. (1963). Modern Trials (abridged ed.). Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merrill. Biernat, M., Vescio, T.K., & Billings, L.S. (1999). Black sheep and expectancy violation: Integrating two models of social judgment. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 1031-41. Billiet, J.B. (1995). Church involvement, individuals, and ethnic prejudice among Flemish Roman Catholics: New evidence of a moderating effect. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 43, 224-233. Birnbaum, J. H. (1995, May 15). The gospel according to Ralph. Time, pp. 18-27. Bjarnason, Thoroddur, & Welch, M. R. (2004). Father knows best: Parishes, priests, and american catholic parishioners' attitudes toward capital punishment. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 43:1, 103-118. Blaine, B. and J. Crocker. 1995. Religiousness, race, and psychological well-being: Exploring social psychological mediators. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 21:1031-41. Bodenhausen, G.V., & Wyer, R.S. (1985). Effects of stereotypes on decision-making and information processing strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 267282. Bowen v. Kendrick, 487 U.S. 589 (1988) Brewer, M.B. (1997). On the social origins of human nature. In C. McGarty & S.A. Haslam (Eds.), The message of social psychology (pp.54-62). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Brewer, M.B. (1999). The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love or outgroup hate?. Journal of Social Issues, 55(3), 429-444. Bright, D.A. & Goodman-Delahunty, J. (2004). The influence of verbal gruesome evidence on mock juror verdicts. Psychiatry, Psychology, and Law, 11, 154-166. Bright, D. A. & Goodman-Delahunty, J. (2006).Gruesome evidence and emotion: Anger, blame, and jury decision-making. Law and Human Behavior. 30, 183-202. Cacioppo, J.T., & Petty, R.E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116-131. Caporael, L.R. (1997). The evolution of truly social cognition: The core configurations model. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 276-298. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 128 Chen, S., Duckworth, K., & Chaiken, S. (1999). Motivated heuristic and systematic processing. Psychological Inquiry,10:1, 44-49. Clark, W. H. (1958). The Psychology of Religion. New York: Macmillan Commonwealth v. Daniels, 644 A.2d 1175 (Pa. 1994). Darrow, C. (1940). Attorney for the defense. The Bedside Esquire, 485. Daudistel, H.C., Hosch, H.M., Holmes, M.D., & Graves, J.B. (1999). Effects of defendant ethnicity on juries’ dispositions of felony cases. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 29, 317-336. Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85. Davis, M.H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113-126. Davis, M.H., Conklin, L., Smith, A., & Luce, C. (1996). Effect of perspective taking on the cognitive representation of persons: A merging of self and other. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 713-726. De Jonge, J. (1995). On breaking wills: The theological roots of violence in families. Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 14, 26-36. Deaux, K. (1996). Social identification. In Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles, edited by E.T. Higgins and A.W. Kruglanski, 777-98. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. Donahue, M.J. (1985). Intrinsic and extrinsic religiousness: Review and meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 100-08. Douglas, K.S., Lyon, D.R., & Ogloff, J.R.P. (1997). The impact of graphic photographic evidence on mock juror decisions in a murder trial: Probative and prejudicial. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 485-501. Duck, R.J., & Hunsberger, B. (1999). Religious orientation and prejudice: The of religious proscription, right-wing authoritarianism and social desirability. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 9, 157-179. Egland, T.T. (2004). Prejudiced by the presence of god: Keeping religious material out of death penalty deliberations. Capital Defense Journal, 16, 2, 337-366. Eisinga, R., Konig, R., & Sheepers, P. (1995). Orthodox religious beliefs and anti-Semitism: A replication of Glock and Stark in the Netherlands. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 24, 214-223. Engel v. Vitale, 370 U.S. 421 (1962). P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 129 Epstein, S. (1990). Cognitive-Experiential self theory. In L. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp.165-192). New York: Guilford Press. Epstein (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49(8), 709-724. Epstein, S., Lipson, A., Holstein, C., & Huh, E. (1992). Irrational reactions to negative outcomes: Evidence for two conceptual systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 813-825. Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual differences in intuitiveexperiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 390-405. Euben, R.L. (1999). Enemy in the mirror: Islamic fundamentalism and the limits of modern rationalism. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Everson v. Board of Education, 330 U.S. 1 (1947) Feldman, S. (2000). Law & Religion. New York, N.Y.: New York University Press. Frey & Gaertner, S.L. (1986). Helping and avoidance of inappropriate interracial behavior: A strategy that can perpetuate a non-prejudiced self image. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 80-85. Fulton, A.S., Gorsuch, R.L., & Maynard, E.A. (1999). Religious orientation, antihomosexual sentiment, and fundamentalism among Christians. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 38, 14-22. Gaertner, S.L., & Dovidio, J.F. (1997). The subtlety of White racism, arousal, and helping behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 691-707. Gaertner, S.L., Dovidio, J.F., & Johnson, G. (1982). Race of victim, non-responsive bystanders, and helping behavior. Journal of Social Psychology, 117, 69-77. Galinsky, A.D., & Moskowitz, G.B. (2000). Perspective-Taking: Decreasing stereotype expression, stereotype accessibility, and in-Group favoritism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(4), 708-724. Giner-Sorolla, R., Chaiken, S., & Lutz, S. (2002). Validity beliefs and ideology can influence legal case judgments differently. Law and Human Behavior, 26(5), 507-526. Glock, C.Y. & Stark, R. (1966). Christian beliefs and anti-Semitism. New York: Harper & Row. Gorbert, J.J., & Jordan, W.E. (1990). Jury selection: The law, art, and science of selecting a jury (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Gorsuch, R.L. & Aleshire, D. (1974). Christian faith and ethnic prejudice: A review and P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 130 interpretation of research. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 13, 281-307. Gorsuch, R.L., & Hao, J.Y. (1993). Forgiveness: An exploratory factor analysis and its relationship to religious variables. Review of Religious Research, 34, 333-347. Griffin, G.A.E., Gorsuch, R. L., & Davis, A. (1987). A cross cultural investigation of religion orientation, social norms, and prejudice. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 26, 358-65. Griffitt, W., & Jackson, T. (1973). Simulated jury decisions: The influence of jury-defendant attitude similarity-dissimilarity. Social Behavior and Personality, 1, 1-7. Hamilton, M. (2004). Public servants and the First Amendment. CNN.com, Retrieved March 13, 2009, from http://www.cnn.com/2004/LAW/12/17/hamilton.first.amendment/ Herek, G.M. (1987) Religious orientation and prejudice: A comparison of racial and sexual attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 281-307. Highler v. State, 854 N.E. 2d 823 (Ind. 2006). Howard, W.G. & Redfering, D. (1983). The dynamics of jury decision-making: A case study. Social Behavior and Personality, 11(2), 83-89. Hunsberger, B. (1995). Religion and prejudice: The role of religious fundamentalism, quest, and right-wing authoritarianism. Journal of Social Issues, 51, 112-39. Hunsberger, B. & Jackson, L.M. (2005). Religion, meaning, and prejudice. Journal of Social Issues, 61(4), 807-826. Hunsberger, B., Owusu, V., & Duck, R. (1999). Religion and prejudice in Ghana and Canada: Religious fundamentalism, right-wing authoritarianism and attitudes towards homosexuals and women. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 5, 27-51. Hunsberger, B., & Platanow, E. (1986). Religion and helping charitable causes. Journal of Psychology, 120, 517-528. Jackson, J. W. (1993). Realistic group conflict theory: A review and evaluation of the theoretical and empirical literature. Psychological Record, 43, 3, 395-413. Jackson, L.M., & Esses, V.M. (1997). Of scripture and ascription : The relation between religious fundamentalism and intergroup helping. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 893-906. Jackson, L.M. & Hunsberger, B. (1999). An intergroup perspective on religion and prejudice. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 38(4), 509-523. Jacoby, S. (2009, March 1). Keeping the faith, ignoring the history. New York Times. Johnson, S.D. (1984). Religion as a defense in a mock-jury trial. The Journal of Social P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 131 Psychology, 125(2), 213-220. Jones, E.E., & Nisbett, R.E. (1971). The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press. Juergensmeyer, M. (2000). Terror in the mind of God: The global rise of religious violence. Berkeley: University of California Press. Jung, C.G. (1964/1968). Analytical psychology: Its theory and practice. New York: Pantheon Press. Kane, J.G., Craig, S. C., & Wald, K.D. (2004). “Religion and Presidential Politics in Florida: A List Experiment.” Social Science Quarterly 85:281–293. Kenworthy, J.B. (2003). Explaining the belief in god for self, in-group, and out-group targets. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 42(1), 137-146. Kerr, N.L., Hymes, R.W., Anderson, A.B., & Weathers, J.E. (1995). Defendant-Juror similarity and mock juror judgments. Law and Human Behavior, 19(6), 545-567. Kirkpatrick, L.A. (1993). Fundamentalism, Christian orthodoxy, and intrinsic religious orientation as predictors of discriminatory attitudes. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 32, 256-68. Kirkpatrick, L.A. & Epstein, S. (1992). Cognitive-Experiential Self-Theory and subjective probability: Further evidence for two conceptual systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 63(4), 534-544. Krause, N., & Ingersoll-Dayton, B. (2001). Religion and the process of forgiveness in late life. Review of Religious Research, 42, 252-276. Kroeger, C.C., & Beck, J.R. (Eds.) (1996). Women, abuse, and the Bible: How scripture can be used to hurt or to heal. Grand Rapids, MI: Baker Books. Lauderdale, P., Smith-Cunnien, P., Parker, J., & Inverarity, J. (1984). External threat and the definition of deviance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 1058-1068. Laythe, B., Finkel, F., & Kirkpatrick, L.A. (2002). Predicting prejudice from religious fundamentalism and right-wing authoritarianism: A multiple-regression approach. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 40(1), 1-10. Lemon v. Kurtzman, 403 U.S. 602 (1971). Levin, S., & Sidanius, J. (1999). Social dominance and social identity in the United States and Israel: Ingroup favoritism or outgroup derogation?. Political Psychology. 20(1), 99 – 126. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 132 Liptak, A (2009, 2, 2). A life term for rape at 13: Cruel and unusual?. The New York Times. Lumsden, C.J., & Wilson, E.O. (1983). Promethean fire: Reflections on the origin of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Marques, J.M. (1990). The black sheep effect: Outgroup homogeneity in social comparison settings. In D. Abrams & m. Hogg (Eds.), Social identity theory: Constructive and critical advances (pp.131-151). London: Harvester Wheatsheaf. Marques, J.M., & Yzerbyt, V.Y. (1988). The black sheep effect : Judgmental extremity towards ingroup members in inter- and intragroup situations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 287-292. Marques, J.M., Yzerbyt, V.Y., & Leyens, J.P. (1988). The black sheep effect : Extremity of judgment towards in-group members as a function of group identification. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 1-16. McCullough, M.E. & Worthington, E.L., Jr. (1999). Religion and the forgiving personality. Journal of Personality, 67, 1142-1164. McFarland, S.G. (1989). Religious orientation and the targets of discrimination. Journal of the Scientific Study of Religion, 28(3), 324-336. McGowan v. Maryland, 366 U.S. 420 (1961). McHoskey, J. 1996. Authoritarianism and ethical ideology. Journal of Social Psychology, 136, 709-717. McKeon, R. (ed.). (1941). The basic works of Aristotle. New York: Random House Miller, M. K. (2006). Religion in Criminal Justice. El Paso, Texas: LFB Scholarly Publishing LLC. Miller, M.K., & Bornstein, B. H. (2005). Religious appeals in closing arguments: Impermissible input or benign banter?. Law and Psychology Review, 29. Miller, M.K., & Bornstein, B.H. (2006). The use of religion in death penalty sentencing trials. Law and Human Behavior, 30, 675-684. Miller, M.K., & Hayward, R. D. (2007). Religious characteristics and the death penalty. Law and Human Behavior. Morris, R.J., Hood, R.W., & Watson, P.J. (1989). A second look at religious orientation, social desirability and prejudice. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27, 81-84. Nason, J. (2005). Mandatory voir dire questions in capital cases: A potential solution to the biases of death qualification. Roger Williams University Law Review, 1, 211-248. 10:211. P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 133 Oliver, E. & Griffitt, W. (1976). Emotional arousal and objective judgment. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 8, 399-400. Ostow, M. (1990). The fundamentalist phenomenon: A psychological perspective. In N.J. Cohen (Ed.), The fundamentalist phenomenon (pp.99-125). Grand Rapids, MI: Eerdmans. Perrin, R.D. (2000). Religiosity and honesty: Continuing the search for the consequential dimension. Review of Religious Research, 41, 534-544. People v. Harlan. No. 94 CR 187 (Adams Co., Colo. Dist. Ct. May 23, 2003) Pfeifer, J.E. (1999). Perceptual biases and mock juror decision making: Minority religions in court. Social Justice Research, 12(4), 409-419. Putney, S., & Middleton, R. (1961). Dimensions and correlates of religious ideologies. Social Forces, 39, 285-290. Ponton, M.O. & Gorsuch, R.L. (1988). Prejudice and religion revisited: A cross-cultural investigation with a Venezuelan sample. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 27(2), 260-271. Report to Congress: Analysis of Penalties for Federal Rape Cases, U.S. Sentencing Commission. (1993). Rogers, R.W., & Prentice-Dunn, S. (1981). Deindividuation and anger-mediated interracial aggression: Unmasking regressive racism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 63-73. Rothchild, J., Boulton, M.M., & Jung, K. (Eds.). (2007). Doing justice to mercy: Religion, law, & criminal justice. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia Press. Rothchild, Jonathan. (Ed.). (2000). Complicity or justice and mercy?. In Doing Justice to Mercy: Religion, Law, & Criminal Justice (1st ed.), Charlottesville: University of Virginia Press. Rothchild, Jonathan. (Ed.). (2000). Echoes of grace: From the prison to the state house. In Doing Justice to Mercy: Religion, Law, & Criminal Justice (1st ed.), Charlottesville: University of Virginia Press. Sandoval v. Calderon, 241 F.3d 765 (9th Circ. 2000). Saphire, R.B. (1997). Religion and recusal. Marquette Law Review, 81, 351. Schoenfeld, E. (1978). Image of man: The effect of religion on trust. Review of Religious Research, 20, 61-67. Schuman, H., & Converse, J.M. (1971). “The Effects of Black and White Interviewers on Black P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 134 Responses in 1968.” Public Opinion Quarterly 35:44–68. Sidanius, J. (1993). The psychology of group conflict and the dynamics of oppression: A social dominance perspective. In S. Iyengar & W. McGuire (Eds.), Explorations in political psychology (pp. 183-219). Durham, NC: Duke University Press. Smith, S.D. (1997).Legal discourse and the de facto disestablishment. Marquette Law Review. 81, 203-227. Spilka, B., Hood, W.H., Hunsberger, B., & Gorsuch, R. (2003). The Psychology of Religion. New York: Guilford Press. Spilka, B., Kojetin, B., & Macintosh, D. (1985). Forms and measures of personal faith: Questions, correlates, and distinctions. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 24, 437-442. State v. Fuller, 862 A.2d 1130 (N.J. 2004). State v. Purcell, 18 P.3d 113 (Ariz. 2001). Stephan, C.W., & Stephan, W.G. (1986). Habla ingles? The effects of language translation on simulated juror decisions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 16, 577-589. Streb, M.J., Burrell, B., Frederick, B., & Genovese, M.A. (2008). Social desirability effects and support for a female American president. Public Opinion Quarterly, 72(1), 76-89. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J.C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W.G. Austin (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp.7-24). Chicago: Nelson-Hall. Taylor, T.S., & Hosch, H.M. (2004). An examination of jury verdicts for evidence of a Similarity-Leniency Effect, an Out-Group Punitiveness Effect or a Black Sheep Effect. Law and Human Behavior, 28(5), 587-598. Taylor, S.E., & Mette, D.R. (1971). When similarity breeds contempt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 20, 75-81. Tittle, C. & Welch, M. (1983) Religiosity and deviance: Toward a contingency theory of constraining effects, Social Forces. 3, 61. Tocqueville, A., Mayer, J.P., Sandage, S.A., & Lawrence, G. (2000). Democracy in America. HarperCollins. Tversky & Kahneman. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124 – 1131. U.S. v. DeJesus, 347 F.3d 500 (3rd Cir. 2003). Unger, R.K. (2002). Them and us: Hidden ideologies- differences in degree or kind? Analysis P e r s p e c t i v e T a k i n g , R e l i g i o u s S y m b o l s a n d J u r o r B i a s e s | 135 of Social Issues and Public Policy, 43-52. Van Pooijen, J., & Lam, J. (2007). Retributive justice and social catergorizations: The perceived fairness of punishment depends on intergroup status. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 1244-1255. Vescio, T.K., Sechrist, G.B., & Paolucci, M.P. (2003). Perspective taking and prejudice reduction: the meditational role of empathy arousal and situational attributions. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 455-472. Volcano Press. (1995). Family violence and religion. Volcano, CA: Author. Wallace v. Jaffree, 472 U.S. 38, 91 (1985) Weber, J.G. (1994). The nature of ethnocentric attribution bias: Ingroup protection or enhancement?. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 482-504. Welch, M., Tittle, C., & Peete, T. (1991). Religion and deviance among adult Catholics: A test of the "moral communities" hypothesis. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 30(2), 159. Whitehead, A.N. (1926). Religion in the making. New York: Macmillan. Wilder, D.A., Simon, A.F., & Faith, M. (1996). Enhancing the impact of counterstereotypic information: Dispositional attributions for deviance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 276-87. Young, R.L. (1992). Religious orientation, race and support for the death penalty. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 31(1), 76-87.