CHAPTER 8: MORAL DEVELOPMENT, VALUES, AND RELIGION INTRODUCTION Moral development is a multifaceted aspect of individual growth involving cognition, behavior, motivation, and self-assessment based on personal interpretations of right and wrong. The thoughts, feelings, and behavior of moral development may have an intrapersonal or interpersonal focus. Piaget explained two types of moral cognition: heteronomous morality and autonomous morality. Young thinkers in early stages of moral development believe consequences of misbehavior are delivered immediately, as immanent justice. Older children think more about cooperation in social issues. Kohlberg described three levels of qualitatively distinct aspects of moral reasoning. Level 1: Preconventional Reasoning—reasoning is based on external rewards and punishment Stage 1: Heteronomous morality—morality is based on avoidance of punishment Stage 2: Individualism, instrumental purpose, and change—mutual pursuit of own interests Level 2: Conventional Reasoning—morality is based on internal values and external standards Stage 3: Mutual interpersonal expectations, conformity—value for trust, caring, and loyalty Stage 4: Social systems morality—value for the social order, law, justice, and duty Level 3: Postconventional Reasoning—morality is internalized Stage 5: Social contract or individual rights—explores options, makes own decision Stage 6: Universal ethical principles—conscience decides conflict between law and conscience Kohlberg hypothesized that the moral orientation of youngsters evolves in conjunction with cognitive development and external factors such as cognitive conflict, peer relations, and role-taking opportunities. Kohlberg’s critics argue that he places too much emphasis on thought versus behavior. Kohlberg’s theory does not show respect for cultural traditions found in eastern or third world countries. It is not clear that students can apply Kohlberg’s dilemmas to their daily IM 8 | 1 experiences. Gilligan proposes that Kohlberg places too much emphasis on the Western justice perspective, and too little appreciation is shown for the care perspective. Another perspective recognizes a moral domain, a social-conventional domain, and a personal domain as factors that promote justice, consistency within social groups, and personal choice. From the behavioral perspective, moral behavior depends on consistency of consequences, clarity of societal rules, quality of the modeled behavior, and competency of the adolescent. Models of moral behavior include parents, peers, and media figures. Competencies are based on cognitive capacity, demonstrated behavior, and awareness of morality rules. Social cognitive theory emphasizes the distinction between moral thought and moral action; that is, performing moral behavior when it is warranted. Altruism seems to develop from reciprocity in personal experiences; having experienced help, care, and forgiveness from others, an adolescent is able to offer the same assistance to others. Moral feelings are based on experienced child-rearing strategies, empathetic nature, and emotions in moral development. Psychoanalytic theory focuses on the superego, the moral branch of the personality, influenced by components of the superego called the ego ideal and conscience. According to Freud, parents influence moral development by employing love withdrawal and power assertion. These punitive strategies have not been found to be as effective as induction, explaining responsibility as it relates to consequences for others. Empathy is an emotional response that depends on perspective to understand the emotional condition of others. A lack of empathy correlates highly with antisocial and violent behavior. Many developmentalists believe that the contrast between positive sympathetic and negative shameful feelings contributes to adolescents’ complete moral development. Families and schools are important contexts for moral development. Parents play a primary role in moral development. Parents who use induction as a form of discipline will encourage positive moral development. Moral education is recognized as an aspect of instruction that occurs consciously and overtly as well as unconsciously or covertly. Hidden curriculum—creates an atmosphere covertly teaching ethical and unethical behavior. Character education—teaches moral behavior and avoidance of behavior that harms self or others. Values clarification—helps students identify purpose and merit in their lives. Cognitive moral education—develops democratic values such as cooperation, trust, and responsibility. Service learning—incorporates the community to provide applied learning settings that shift the focus from the student to those who require the skills the student must learn. Values are reflected in ones beliefs in politics, religion, money, friends, career, and selfrespect. Values change with time, and the current trend is toward concern for self over those for others. Good personal adjustment depends on self-fulfillment and strong commitment to others. IM 8 | 2 Adults usually introduce religion to present moral and ethical ideals and to maintain religious tradition. A majority of adolescents report engaging in religious behavior and believe that religion is important. Religious organizations help adolescents with identity formation. Piaget’s cognitive developmental theory may be applied to developmental processes that children demonstrate as they acquire knowledge about biblical figures, moral concepts, and hypothetical understanding of religious material. They are preoperational intuitive religious thought (unsystematic and fragmented), concrete operational religious thought (focused on literal details), and formal operational religious thought (abstract, hypothetical religious understanding). Fowler’s theory of religious development focuses on how individuals find meaning in their life. Religious adolescents demonstrate greater self-discipline in avoiding premarital sex and other risky behavior, but are less likely to use contraception if they are sexually active. Cults offer alternative affiliations and values that may be characterized as dangerous or deviant. Cults serve the members of the group, not the community or nonmembers. Cults’ leaders use overbearing authoritarian methods, are deceptive and coercive, and replace a previous identity with a new identity that would not have been acceptable initially. Most members of cults are psychologically healthy. TOTAL TEACHING PACKAGE OUTLINE Chapter 8: Moral Development, Values, and Religion HEADING I. DOMAINS OF MORAL DEVELOPMENT Moral Thought RESOURCE Learning Goal: 1 Image Gallery: 136 Lecture Topic: 8.1 Critical Thinking Exercise: 8.1 Discussion Topic: 8.2 Essay Question: 1 WWW: Institute for Global Ethics www.globalethics.org/ Learning Goal: 1 Critical Thinking Exercise: 8.1 Essay Question: 2 Piaget’s Ideas and Cognitive Disequilibrium Theory Critical Thinking Exercise: 8.2 Kohlberg’s Theory Critical Thinking Exercise: 8.2 Research Articles: 8.1, 8.2 Research Project: 8.2 WWW: Exploring Moral Development; Kohlberg’s Theory of Moral Development; Kohlberg’s Moral Dilemmas; Kohlberg’s Moral Stages at www.mhhe.com/santrocka10 Kohlberg’s Level 1: Preconventional Reasoning Kohlberg’s Level 2: Conventional Reasoning Kohlberg’s Level 3: Postconventional Reasoning In-Class Activity: 8.1 Short Scenarios: 8.1, 8.2 Research Articles: 8.1, 8.2 Research Project: 8.2 Influences on the Kohlberg Stages Research Article: 8.1 IM 8 | 3 Why Is Kohlberg’s Theory Important for Understanding Moral Development in Adolescence? Kohlberg’s Critics Essay Question: 3 Moral Thought and Moral Behavior Essay Question: 3 Assessment of Moral Reasoning Short Scenario: 8.2, 8.3 Research Project: 8.2 Culture and Moral Development Discussion Topic: 8.1 Gender and the Care Perspective Short Scenario: 8.3 Essay Question: 4 WWW: In a Different Voice; Exploring Girls’ Voices at www.mhhe.com/santrocka10 Reasoning in Different Social Cognitive Domains Lecture Topic: 8.2 Discussion Topic: 8.1 Short Scenario: 8.2 Moral Behavior Learning Goal: 1 Lecture Topic: 8.2 Critical Thinking Exercise: 8.1 Basic Processes Short Scenario: 8.2 Social Cognitive Theory of Moral Development Essay Question: 5 Prosocial Behavior Discussion Topic: 8.2 Essay Question: 6 Short Scenario: 8.2 Moral Feeling II. Research Article: 8.2 Learning Goal: 4 Critical Thinking Exercise: 8.1 Psychoanalytic Theory Essay Question: 7 Empathy Lecture Topic: 8.1 The Contemporary Perspective Discussion Topic: 8.1 CONTEXTS OF MORAL DEVELOPMENT Learning Goal: 2 Lecture Topic: 8.3 Research Project: 8.1 Essay Question: 8 Parenting Parental Discipline Parenting Moral Children and Adolescence Schools The Hidden Curriculum Character Education Values Clarification Short Scenario: 8.2 Discussion Topic: 7.2 Essay Question: 8 In-Class Activity: 8.2 Research Project: 8.1 IM 8 | 4 Cognitive Moral Education III. Service Learning Through the Eyes of Adolescents: Finding a Way to Get a Playground VALUES, RELIGION, AND CULTS Learning Goal: 3 Critical Thinking Exercise: 8.3 Values WWW: Values of American College Freshman; National Service Learning Clearinghouse; Give Five; Kids Who Care; Volunteer Matching Online at www.mhhe.com/santrocka10 Religion Developmental Change Fowler’s Life-Span Developmental Theory Religious Indoctrination and Parenting Religiousness and Sexuality in Adolescence Learning Goal: 3 Image Gallery: 13 Research Article: 8.2 Essay Question: 9 Careers in Adolescent Development: Constance Flanagan, Professor of Youth Civic Development Cults Learning Goal: 3 Discussion Topic: 8.3 Essay Question: 10 WWW: Cult 101; Cults and Mind Control; Social Psychological Aspects of Cults; The Heaven’s Gate Website; Dangerous Cults at www.mhhe.com/santrocka10 SUGGESTED LECTURE TOPICS Topic 8.1—Eisenberg’s Model of Moral Development Eisenberg suggests that Kohlberg’s model is too rigid; she suggests that children’s moral reasoning is not very predictable because children can reason from several different levels rather than using one level and only being able to advance. In Eisenberg’s model, the higher levels are found only in older children, but children can use any of the levels for which they are capable. A child who can use high-level abstract reasoning doesn’t have to use it. Eisenberg’s model gives equal value to justice-oriented and caring-oriented moral reasoning. The six levels are: A. Self-centered reasoning. At this level the individual is concerned with consequences to oneself. One may choose to assist or not assist because of (a) personal benefit or loss; (b) the expectation of reciprocity; (c) one needs the other, or likes or dislikes the other. Preschoolers and early elementary-school children frequently use this level. B. Needs-oriented reasoning. Concern for needs is expressed, without role taking or empathy, even though there may be a conflict with one’s own needs. Some preschoolers and many school-age children use this mode of reasoning. C. Stereotyped and/or approval-oriented reasoning. This level involves stereotyped ideas of good/bad people and good/bad behavior and the desire to win approval. Some school-age children and adolescents use this level. IM 8 | 5 D. Empathetic reasoning. The individual can use some role taking, empathy, and recognition of the other’s humanness. There is awareness of the emotional consequences of helping (feeling good) or not helping (feeling guilty). This is the common level for a few older school-age children and many adolescents. E. Partly internalized principles. Justifications for actions involve internalized values (e.g., concern for others’ rights). The ideas are not clearly thought out or strongly stated. This is the common mode for a few adolescents and adults. F. Strongly internalized principles. Justifications for actions are based on strongly felt internalized values (e.g., wanting to improve society, belief in equality of all). Emotional consequences involve self-respect and living up to one’s own values. This reasoning is rare. Reference Eisenberg, N. (1989). The development of prosocial values. In N. Eisenberg, J. Reykowski, & E. Staub (Eds.). Social and moral values: Individual and social perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Topic 8.2—Egalitarian Motive In some societies, such as with Filipino peasants, the central cultural orientation is an egalitarian motive and the belief that all people should be equal. For the Filipinos, society provides a stable central government, sufficient agricultural and natural resources, over 50 percent literacy in English, and yet poor health, malnutrition, and much disease exist. The peasants live in a subsistence economy, do not plan ahead, do not allow government programs to improve agriculture and business, and deliberately avoid individual accomplishments. Americans view this pattern as irresponsible, illogical, and lazy. But from the Filipino perspective, the foundation of happiness is social approval rather than personal prestige and material possessions. The peasants are guided by these aspects of the egalitarian motive: 1. Pakiksama. More importance is placed on good feelings among people than on personal achievements. 2. Desirability of just meeting one’s needs. The prevailing attitude is that it is sufficient to just meet the day’s needs. 3. Leveling. Any attempts at personal improvement are discouraged by negative results such as teasing, threats, attacks, and gossip. 4. “All have a right to live” belief. While advancement is discouraged, the peasants do help people who are having great difficulty in meeting their everyday existence needs. 5. Hiya. Failure to succeed would lead to feelings of embarrassment and inferiority called hiya. IM 8 | 6 Do the cultural effects of the egalitarian motive surprise you? Are there other effects of the egalitarian motive that surprise you? Are there other effects of this motive on a culture? Would it be possible to combine the typical American motives and the egalitarian motive? What do you think the result would be? Could you change some aspects of the egalitarian motive to make it a healthier motive? References Guthrie, G. M. 1970. The psychology of modernization in the rural Philippines. I.P.C. Paper No. 8. Quezon City: Ateneo de Manila University Press. Madigan, F. C. (Ed.) (1967). Human factors in Philippine rural development. Cagayan de Oro City: Xavier University Press. Topic 8.3—Ethics Education Professionals are well aware of the importance of moral and ethical education in preparation for employment, particularly in areas such as social services, psychology, law, and medicine. Professional organizations have had ethical guidelines for decades, and in the case of medical ethics, for centuries. Books that discuss business ethics or ethics in daily life recently have become relatively popular. Adolescents are faced with increasingly difficult challenges to behaving morally and ethically as substance abuse, sexual harassment, bullying, cheating, and vandalism increase in educational settings. The importance of establishing acceptable codes of behavior suggests that moral and ethical behavior need to be addressed at the high school level. Although ethics education cannot and should not be the focal point in all coursework, it can be integrated into all levels and types of educational preparation. The basic goals of ethics education are: to sensitize students to ethical issues and potential consequences of their actions; to increase students’ ability to recognize ethical issues; to develop the ability to analyze issues and make good decisions; and to accept the lack of clarity that often occurs before and after making ethical decisions. Ethics must be related both to current experiences and to potential future difficulties. Issues that students experience regularly afford the best learning examples for ethical decision making. Students also must be prepared to recognize potential difficulties; thus, they must be presented with some slightly more abstract issues than they are familiar with currently. Several strategies are used: Discussion of moral and ethical behavior may be conducted in separate training sessions or in conjunction with other areas of study. For example, high school students might learn how to respond to plagiarism in English class and perjury in law and government, or about lying and cheating as a topic in an ethics class. IM 8 | 7 Case studies provide students with the opportunity to solve ethical dilemmas that they might observe or experience. In these exercises, students think about the choices that they might make from the position of an objective observer. For example, suppose a student was told in confidence who took the money from a club treasury. The student recognizes that the planned party will not be held unless the money is returned. On the one hand, if the student tells the club advisor who took the money, the confidant will feel betrayed. If she doesn’t say anything, everyone will be deprived of the opportunity for a party. Role playing exercises get students directly involved in responding to difficult situations. For example, give one student the information that he has been at a party and is too drunk to drive, but can’t admit it to anyone. It could jeopardize his position on the team headed for a state championship. A second student is given the information that she recognizes that her friend is drunk for the fifth weekend in a row. Beside that, she has smelled alcohol on her friend’s breath all week, even in the morning. All ethical dilemmas have one or more moral issues that are present and must be decided upon for the good of those involved. Learning to weigh the relative importance and impact of each potential outcome of the moral dilemma allows students to make decisions with which they can be most comfortable. Issues that students are likely to be faced with include confidences of friends versus negative consequences for an individual or group, and freedom to choose versus harm to the individual or others. Although many educators believe that ethical behavior depends on an individual’s character, fundamentals of ethical behavior have been established in nonprofessionals and professionals in a wide variety of employment settings. The acquisition of basic ethical awareness, knowledge of ethical behavior, and application of ethical decision making capabilities represent invaluable interpersonal competence and commitment to the welfare of others. Reference Patterson, J. B., & Curl, R. M. (1990). Ethics education in supported employment preparation. Rehabilitation Education, 4, 247–259. CLASSROOM DISCUSSIONS AND ACTIVITIES Discussion Topics Discussion 8.1—An Age of Ethical Relativity Western culture emphasizes individualism as the foundation of relationships between people. As such, the individual is responsible for making moral decisions, responding to individual conscience, determining responsibility, and establishing personal values through education. Kenneth Gergen explains that the individual acts on these responsibilities based on relationships with the broader culture and the broader concerns of the culture. Two theoretical foundations have been used to explain individual action: the romanticism view of deep inner standards that guide benevolent propensities and the modernist morality based on Darwinism, scientism, and increased cultural consciousness. Neither considers the relationship or interdependence of the IM 8 | 8 individual with the greater culture. The consequence of considering relationships in identifying significance and meaning to moral action is what Gergen identifies as constructivist relativism. From this perspective, Kohlberg’s form of independent decision making would not be as beneficial as Gilligan’s socially centered decision-making processes. Current Gallup Polls suggest that Americans are becoming increasingly comfortable with moral relativism. In March 2000, almost 90 percent of Americans said religion was important in their lives, but only 20 percent believed theirs was the only true path to God. About 45 percent believed that their views and the views of others were as important as religious teachings. Only 24 percent of those polled viewed themselves as part of the religious right, a group that believes in biblical literalism. How do students determine the truth? What percentage of students decides about moral behavior based on internal feelings? What percentage base decisions on scientific information? When might it be wrong to tell the truth? What additional evidence do students have that ethical relativism influences the lives of adolescents? References The Gallup Organization. (2000, March). Gallup poll topics: A–Z—Religion. Princeton: Author. www.gallup.com Gergen, K. J. (1992). Social construction and moral action. In D. N. Robinson (Ed.), Social discourse and moral judgment (pp. 9–27). San Diego: Academic Press, Inc. Lamborn, S. D., Fischer, K. W., & Pipp, S. (1994). Constructive criticism and social lies: A developmental sequence for understanding honesty and kindness in social interactions. Developmental Psychology, 30, 495–508. Discussion 8.2—Does Altruism Really Exist? Do we really care about others, what happens to them, how they feel, and whether life is positive for them? Or do we always have ourselves as the target of our concern? Do we help others for their sake, or only for our own? A terminal value occurs if we value others for their own sake. An instrumental value takes place if we value others because it is to our advantage to do so. Which is truer of human nature? Some psychologists seem to believe that altruism, valuing and pursuing another person’s welfare as a goal, is a fantasy. These psychologists see humans as social egoists. As William James noted in 1890: “We know how little it matters to us whether some man, a man taken at large and in the abstract, proves a failure or succeeds in life—he may be hanged for aught we care—but we know the utter momentousness and terribleness of the alternative when the man is the one whose name we ourselves bear.” In addition to William James, Freud’s psychoanalytic theory and Skinner’s behaviorism makes strong social egoist assumptions. Social egoism as a dominant psychological philosophy is also exhibited in the amount of research done on such self-emphasizing topics as self-awareness, self-monitoring, selfIM 8 | 9 presentation, self-handicapping, self-deception, self-evaluation, self-affirmation, self-discrepancy, self-expansion, and self-esteem. Many theories in social psychology assume that people are out for themselves: Among them are social comparison theory, social exchange theory, and equity theory. Yet, Batson and his colleagues (1990) conducted a series of research studies that suggests that there is altruism or true empathy-induced helping. His studies looked at alternatives such as helping (1) to reduce aversive empathic arousal, (2) to avoid social and self-punishments (e.g., shame and guilt), and (3) to seek social and self-rewards, and found that some helping occurs because of altruism. Batson (1990) concludes, “All the research . . . suggests that our capacity for altruistic caring is limited to those for whom we feel empathy. In study after study, when empathy for the person in need is low, the pattern of helping suggests underlying egoistic motivation. It is not that we never help people for whom we feel little empathy; we often do . . . We care for them instrumentally rather than terminally.” (p. 344) Furthermore, even when we have concern for others, we might not act on it if the cost of helping is painfully high. Reference Batson, C. D. (1990). How social an animal? The human capacity for caring. American Psychologist, 45, 336–346. Discussion 8.3—Satanism Satanism has been identified as a destructive religion that seduces individuals who feel alienated, alone, angry, desperate, and powerless. The promises of Satanism are power, dominance, and gratification to those who join the cult. Adolescents are seduced by these promises and have psychological needs that are met by participation in Satanic worship including: Sense of belonging—resolves the adolescent’s identity crisis; Mastery and structure—establishes order and a sense of accomplishment; Power and control—provides opportunity for power in destructive rituals; Rebellion—satisfies normal processes of experimenting with values; Curiosity and relief from boredom—satisfies normal curiosity about occults; Self-esteem—achievement of identity resolves difficulties of low self-esteem; Validation of anger and violence—resolves alienation by violent means of attaining power. Feelings of gratification that come from participation in the cult make it a difficult and lengthy process to leave. Contact and patterns of involvement can be used to determine level of involvement. Experimental use—”dabbling” with friends Social/recreational use—practices magic and learns rituals Situational use—Satanic practices in response to stressful situations IM 8 | 10 Intensified use—deeply involved; formal cult initiation has occurred Compulsive use—secretively and compulsively engaging in ritualistic behavior Chronic addiction—Satanism is a way of life; leaving the cult is nearly impossible Involvement often correlates with a significant event. Family participation often helps to successfully extricate adolescents from cult involvement, particularly after establishing a strong relationship between the therapist and client. Success also depends on developing alternative healthy activities, including elimination of probable substance abuse. Reference Clark, C. M. (1994). Clinical assessment of adolescents involved in Satanism. Adolescence, 29, 461–468. In-Class Activities Activity 8.1—Kohlberg’s Ethical Dilemmas Present the four moral dilemmas and have students respond to them and explain their responses (essentially Kohlberg’s tasks). Then have the students break into small groups and analyze their results according to Kohlberg’s six stages. Do their reasons fit into his categories? Why or why not? How do their patterns of results support or not support his stage model? Are all their responses in one stage? What kind of mixture or patterning is evident? Dilemma 1: In Europe, a woman is near death from cancer. There is one drug that the doctors think might save her. It is a form of radium that a druggist in the same town has recently discovered. The drug is expensive to make, but the druggist is charging ten times what the drug cost him to make. He paid $200 for the radium and is charging $2,000 for a small dose of the drug. The sick woman’s husband, Heinz, goes to everyone he knows to borrow the money, but he can get together only about $1,000. He tells the druggist his wife is dying and asks him to sell the drug for $1,000 now and the rest later. The druggist says, “No, I discovered the drug and I’m going to make money from it.” Heinz is desperate and considers breaking into the man’s store to steal the drug for his wife. Should he? Why or why not? Dilemma 2: John and Mary are taking a class together and are strongly attracted to one another. They want to have sex together, but John is married, although the marriage is having difficulties. Should they sleep together? Why or why not? Dilemma 3: Dr. Johnson makes decisions about which patients have access to a kidney machine. Patients who do not get access will die. There are far more people who need the machine than can be accommodated, so there is a waiting list for those not yet on it. Dr. Johnson’s young daughter is injured in a car accident and has kidney damage. She needs access to the machine to live. Should Dr. Johnson take another patient off the machine to put his daughter on? Why or why not? Dilemma 4: You are shopping with a friend when you notice that your friend is shoplifting. You look around and notice that the store manager is watching you. What should you do? Why? IM 8 | 11 Reference King, M. B., & Clark, D. E. (1989). Instructor’s manual to accompany J. W. Santrock and S. R. Yussen’s Child development: An introduction (4th ed.). Dubuque: Wm. C. Brown. Activity 8.2—Values Clarification Exercise The world as we know it is coming to an end. There is a fallout shelter in central Canada to which six people will be admitted during this third world war. You are the administrator and you must make the selections. The supply of air, food, water, and availability of space is adequate for only six people for three months, but ten people are asking to be admitted. For the survival of the human race, you must decide which six of them will be saved. You have exactly 30 minutes to make up your mind before Washington goes up in smoke. These are your choices: 1. A 16-year-old girl of questionable IQ, a high school dropout, pregnant. 2. A policeman with a gun (which cannot be taken from him—he was recently thrown off the force for brutality). 3. A clergyman, 75. 4. A woman physician, 36, known to be a confirmed racist. 5. A male violinist, 46, who served seven years for pushing narcotics. 6. A 20-year-old Black militant, no special skills. 7. A former prostitute, female, 39. 8. An architect, a male homosexual. 9. A 26-year-old law student. 10. The law student’s 25-year-old wife who spent the last nine months in a mental hospital, still heavily sedated. They refuse to be separated. Ask students to choose the six people they will admit to the shelter. After they have completed their assignment, ask students to choose by casting votes of the ten applicants and determine which will be admitted into the shelter. Ask students to debate the decisions they made. Reference Santrock, J. W. (1998). Adolescence: An introduction (7th ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill. IM 8 | 12 Critical Thinking Exercises Exercise 8.1—Moral Development, Values, and Religion The chapter discussion of moral development, values, and religion presents another opportunity to study clearly measurable developmental changes during adolescence. The developmental issues discussed in Chapter 2 receive variable emphasis for the topics in Chapter 8. Which issue appears to receive the least attention in this chapter? Circle the letter of the best answer, explain why it is the best answer, and why each of the other answers is not as appropriate. A. Development results from biological, social, and cognitive processes. B. There are periods of development. C. Nature and nurture interact to produce development. D. Development may be either continuous or discontinuous. E. Early experience is often important for later development. Exercise 8.2—Kohlberg’s Theory of Moral Development Lawrence Kohlberg’s theory of moral development represents a standard against which other theories are compared. One question to ask is how this approach relates to the various theories and studies about moral development presented in the chapter. Based on Santrock’s summary of Kohlberg’s theory in Chapter 8, which of the following statements does not accurately represent a way in which Kohlberg’s approach applies what we know about moral development? Circle the letter of the best answer, explain why it is the best answer, and why each of the other answers is not as appropriate. A. Piaget’s formal operational thinker is comparable to a person at Kohlberg’s Level 3: Postconventional Reasoning. B. Kohlberg’s approach to understanding moral development is to investigate cognitive processes, not moral behavior. C. Kohlberg’s theory emphasizes discontinuity in moral development across time. D. Kohlberg’s approach is slightly different from the social cognitive theory of moral development. E. Kohlberg’s theory can be used successfully as the foundation for moral education. IM 8 | 13 Exercise 8.3—Values, Religion, and Cults In the section of Chapter 8 entitled “Values, Religion, and Cults,” Santrock discusses contemporary patterns and trends in adolescents’ values. Which of the following is an assumption, rather than an inference or an observation, which motivates Santrock to write about this topic? Circle the letter of the best answer, explain why it is the best answer, and why each of the other answers is not as appropriate. A. Adolescents carry with them a set of values that influences their thoughts, feelings, and actions. B. College freshmen are strongly motivated to be well off financially. C. Talking about values and exposure to religion helps adolescents make good decisions when they encounter new dilemmas in daily life. D. There was an increase between 1986 and 1995 in the percentage of freshmen that said they were strongly interested in participating in community action programs. E. Religious institutions created by adults are designed to introduce certain beliefs to children and adolescents and ensure that they will carry on a religious tradition. Answer Key for Critical Thinking Exercises Exercise 8.1 A. This is not the best answer. The chapter suggests sex differences in emphasis of moral behavior; it covers social and cognitive processes extensively. B. This is not the best answer. Piaget and Kohlberg, both of which receive research support for their claims, treat developmental periods in the theories. C. This is the best answer. There is no discussion of the influence of nature and nurture in this chapter. D. This is not the best answer. The theories of Piaget and Kohlberg, and theories derived from them (e.g., Hoffman’s, Gilligan’s) explain discreet, discontinuous stages of moral development. E. This is not the best answer. Although there is limited treatment of this idea, it is included in the discussion of psychoanalytic accounts of the development of moral feeling. IM 8 | 14 Exercise 8.2 A. This is not the best answer. This statement correctly identifies the comparison between Piaget’s formal operations, individuals who think logically, deductively, and abstractly; postconventional reasoning is based on completely internalized morality. B. This is not the best answer. Kohlberg believed that moral development is based primarily on moral reasoning, not the behavior that an individual demonstrates. C. This is not the best answer. Kohlberg described moral development as unfolding in a series of stages, and at each stage the children and adolescents describe qualitatively distinct thought processes. D. This is the best answer. It is an inaccurate explanation of Kohlberg’s theory because social cognitive theory of moral development distinguishes between moral competence in ability versus performance. E. This is not the best answer. Higgins, Power, and Kohlberg (1983) demonstrated that moral education was successful in three schools following a democratic approach to school management. Exercise 8.3 A. This is an observation. Santrock states this as a principle at the beginning of the section with a reference. He argues for this position with additional supporting references as evidence. B. This is an observation. It summarizes findings portrayed in Figure 8.3 from a study of college student values. In short, college freshmen say they are highly motivated to be well off financially. C. This is an assumption. Santrock proposes that adolescents who are exposed to values clarification and attend religious services are more likely to hear the necessary information about avoiding risky behavior, but they also benefit from spending time with good models. It is not clear how well adolescents generalize the information they receive to new problems they experience at school or in the community. D. This is an observation. It is one of the observations that led to the interpretation just discussed in C. The statement reports an observed shift in what freshmen say about their interest in community action programs. E. This is an inference. Santrock introduces the idea that adults create environments for children to transmit specific religious values. There are no data to suggest what percentage of parents use religious educational settings as a mechanism to teach values, to introduce students to acceptable peer groups, or to avoid undesirable influences by unacceptable IM 8 | 15 peers. The data do show, however, that adults adopt the religions they were exposed to as youths. Short Scenarios Scenario 8.1 Brandon has been at the Roosevelt Senior High for three years, and has great plans for writing a book about his high school experience. He was lucky enough to have two professional parents who provided him with interesting opportunities for academic achievement, participation in school activities, and travel around the United States and Europe. Brandon has learned a lot about people, the way they respond to the circumstances of their lives, and the way they respond to other people. Of course he understands his classmates at school better than anyone else he has observed. He has noticed that students living in the poorer neighborhoods, and whose parents have inconsistent employment, look out for themselves. They do a good job of keeping track of the teachers and principal so they don’t often get caught breaking rules, but they take advantage of the system at every opportunity. Most kids want to be liked. They don’t really think for themselves; they do and say what most of their friends do and say. Even those who think they are very enlightened check with friends or authority figures before making any decision. Brandon has only a few personal friends who seem to be individualists, but also are considerate of others’ feelings and beliefs. Most of them have jobs, support school and community events, or visit and help older people. Brandon writes in his journal every night to remember interesting events and the contradictions he observes between what students and teachers say and do. It also gives him a chance to reflect on his experiences and perspectives and those of his classmates. Someday he will have enough material to write a book of short stories. Brandon has had a variety of experiences as a result of an upper middle-class SES. Traveling around the United States and Europe has given Brandon an opportunity to observe a wide variety of cultures and belief systems, offering points of comparison. As his exposure to Western cultures has been most dominant, the Kohlberg theory of moral development would provide an accurate assessment of his development. The pattern of taking care of self could be described as Kohlberg’s Level 1 behavior. The social cognitive theory of moral development explains the importance of distinguishing between the ability and the willingness to produce moral behavior. Wanting to be liked and going along with community rules are aspects of Kohlberg’s Level 2. Brandon describes his friends as engaging in postconventional reasoning. Scenario 8.2 All that Jim has to do is turn on his killer smile, and he can charm almost anyone into almost anything. He has tried it often enough to know that it works. Other people know it too. His teammates have started asking him to buy alcohol and tobacco products for them. The coach IM 8 | 16 wants him to help raise money for the team. And his parents ask him to attend all of the social events they host for their business and civic activities. Even though he wants to remain good friends with his teammates, he knows that he will get into serious trouble with the law, the coach, and his parents if he is caught buying liquor. In addition to that, he knows it isn’t healthy to drink after extensive workouts or games. It is even more dangerous for his friends, and anyone else on the road, if they drink and drive. He could go into the liquor store and not be so convincing, but that would solve only part of the problem. He must think of some strategy to convince them that they should stay sober. Maybe he could get his parents and the coach to foot the bill for a pro ball player to come and give everyone a motivational “be smart” lecture. He’ll try that approach first. Jim is conflicted because his adolescent friends encourage him to manipulate others; he does not want to get in trouble—Kohlberg’s Level 1 thinking. Jim’s parents have modeled altruistic and philanthropic behavior to support community. His coach wants him to help raise money for the team, and Jim’s altruistic behavior becomes apparent if he does so. Jim’s parents ask him to attend social events that they host to further their business and philanthropic interests or their altruism. Part of the dilemma is whether to help his friends so they will continue to like him versus refusing to help and having everyone angry with him represent Kohlberg’s Stage 2 thinking. Jim feels discomfort from the pressure that he gets from his friends, but he also wants to remain friends with them. Acting on these thoughts and feelings to find a solution demonstrates moral competence and behavior described by the social cognitive theory. Jim has identified a solution by exchanging his support to his coach and parents for a speaker that could help his friends, an example of reciprocity in altruism. The speaker would provide an opportunity for character education. Scenario 8.3 Lisa and her girlfriend Rhonda have just had the scare of a lifetime. Two crazy people were driving down the freeway at what must have been 100 m.p.h. and one of them actually fired a gun! Lisa and Rhonda were fine, but the older gentleman driving in front of them didn’t fare so well. His car swerved off the road and ran into a guardrail. Lisa slowed down immediately and pulled over to see what could be done to help him. Lisa phoned 911 and asked for an ambulance as soon as she noticed the gash on the man’s forehead. The first aid kit and blanket her mother insisted on keeping in the car were certainly helpful, as was Rhonda, who volunteers at the hospital. As they took care of the wound, the man complained that he had not gotten the car license plate number and that the police had not yet arrived. Lisa and Rhonda were so thankful that nobody had been hurt seriously they had not thought about the police. They realized that they wouldn’t be much help to the police when they IM 8 | 17 did arrive. They were proud anyway, since their team effort had enabled them to deal with the man’s emergency medical needs. Everyone else seemed to have paid attention to the car. Lisa and Rhonda demonstrated moral sensitivity by thinking first about the consequences of the gunshot. Lisa and Rhonda demonstrated altruism by helping the older man at the scene. Lisa was prepared for the emergency with the cell phone and first aid kit, indications of foresight to support the performance of moral behavior, and motivation to help others. Lisa and Rhonda were concerned that the injured man received attention, not the justice that the driver and occupants of the speeding car should receive. Rhonda may have received some direct instruction during her hospital work about handling a medical emergency that allowed her to act quickly and appropriately. CURRENT RESEARCH ARTICLES Article 8.1—Moral Decision Making and Moral Judgment Development Research on moral judgment investigated the influence of a wide range of environmental factors including educational practices, social influences, and family and peer relationships. The results generally support the stage theorist’s perspective in that moral development is hierarchical and can be influenced by contextual experiences. Recently, studies have focused on intraindividual correlates of change, specifically at the phases of consolidation and transition as they relate to the individual’s development of moral judgment. Consolidation refers to the individual’s consistent pattern of responses across items that show a preference for one stage. Highly variable response patterns reflect low stage consolidation, and reflecting a pattern of transition. Response patterns have been suggested as predictors of stage transition. This research investigated the possibility that one can predict the usefulness of Kohlberg’s moral stages increases during developmental phases of consolidation and diminishes during periods of transition. Subjects were identified from data sets of students representing junior high school, senior high school, college, and graduate school. Over 47,000 participants were included in three crosssectional samples (46,679) and two longitudinal samples (822). Analysis showed that the Defining Issues Test (DIT) scores formed a distribution that reflected a normal sample. The DIT, a measure of moral development based on Kohlberg’s stages of moral reasoning, was used to assess study participants. The DIT includes six stories of social dilemma about which participants must form solutions. Solutions are ranked and given a P score, based on a scale from 0 to 95, that reflects assessed preferences for postconventional moral orientation. The DIT assesses developmental shifts between adolescence and adulthood, focusing on conventional to postconventional reasoning. The results show that participants’ reliance on the Kohlbergian moral framework was greatest during periods of consolidation and lowest during periods of transition. As participants demonstrated movement toward periods of consolidation, the usefulness of the moral stage IM 8 | 18 information increased. Participants demonstrating high mixture in stages of reasoning were experiencing periods of transition; transition periods were associated with confusion about interpreting real-life events and, subsequently, how to respond based on the confusion. As a result, the authors concluded that it might be necessary to develop strategies for moral education that address current abilities and strategies students demonstrate during transition and consolidation periods. As the utility of Kohlberg’s stages decreases, it becomes increasingly necessary for other systems to be in place to guide moral decision-making processes. Reference Thoma, S. J., & Rest, J. R. (1999). The relationship between moral decision making and patterns of consolidation and transition in moral judgment development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 323–334. Article 8.2—Individual Moral Judgment Theories of moral development explain developmental changes through (a) socialization of the individual to cultural ideology and (b) the individual’s cognitive processes of social and moral meaning. Theories differ in explaining the importance of one process over another. Other aspects of moral development include autonomy and heteronomy: Cognitive developmentalists emphasize the development of autonomy; social learning theories and cultural psychology focus on cultural transmission, thus emphasizing heteronomy. Piaget argued that the individual develops from heteronomy to autonomy. Kohlberg theorized that the individual fluctuates between heteronomy and autonomy within each stage. The position used as a foundation for this investigation was that both processes influence the development of moral thinking and that they occur simultaneously, in parallel, and reciprocally. Moral thinking is used in this research to describe the individual’s perspective on issues such as abortion, rights of homosexual individuals, or religion in public schools. Cultural ideology is another basic process used here to refer to values, norms, and standards that are independent of the individual and shared mutually by a group of people as part of the norm. The question for this investigation was how individual moral thinking and group cultural ideology interrelate. Two studies were completed to determine the association between political attitudes and moral judgments. Study 1 included two congregations of about 300 members each, from the same neighborhood in Minneapolis/St. Paul, MN. One church was a liberal congregation from the United Church of Christ (UCC); the second was a conservative American Baptist congregation. A random sample of 100 members from each was mailed a set of questionnaires. Respondents included 87 Baptists and 80 UCC members. Questionnaires measuring independent variables included measures from the Inventory of Religious Belief, the Intrinsic Religious Motivation Scale, and a measure of political conservatism-liberalism. The Defining Issues Test (DIT) was used to measure moral judgment. Opinions about pubic policy issues were dependent variables measured by the ATHRI; public policy issues included items such as euthanasia, due process rights of the accused, and free speech. Demographic variables were measured on several variables such as education, sex, and occupation. The two churches were chosen for similarity on these variables, and in fact the results showed a high degree of similarity across variables. IM 8 | 19 The results showed that the combined responses for religious ideology, political identity, and moral judgment produced a multiple correlation of .79 that established anticipation of members’ moral thinking. All major independent and dependent variables were significantly different between the two churches in the expected directions. With the exception of education (the more conservative Baptist group had a higher reported level of education), the groups differed in the expected directions. The two congregations had opposite positions on human rights public policy issues and polarities also were found in ideological and moral judgments (DIT P scores were 32.44 for Baptists and 41.72 for UCC members). Study 2 was completed as a cross-replication of Study 1. Sixty-two undergraduate students completed all of the protocols and demonstrated consistency on the DIT. The materials and procedure were the same as in Study 1. The results of Study 2 indicated that the student sample was more like the UCC sample than the Baptist sample. Generally, students were more liberal on measures of political conservatism, theocentrism, fundamentalist, Orthodoxy, moral judgment and public policy issues. The combined results also indicated that religion alone does not predict responses to questions about public policy, but the underlying orientation toward fundamentalist beliefs does so. Middle America, usually depicting the middle class socioeconomically and conservative politically, refers here to the middle range of moral judgment that corresponds to religious fundamentalism. Overall, the results suggest that political identity (liberal or conservative), religious fundamentalism, moral judgment, and views on public policy were significantly interrelated. Liberal and conservative position viewpoints transcended categories of questions. A person who demonstrates high religious fundamentalism, supports strong religious authority, and has strong respect for civil authorities. Religious authoritarianism was associated with high Stage 4 DIT scores. Further, beliefs that authority should not be questioned or scrutinized, blocks the individual from making the transition to postconventional thinking. The main point of the article is that consistency across moral judgment, religious fundamentalism, and political identity produce highly predictive decisions about moral thinking and public policy issues. Reference Narvaez, D.; Getz, I.; Rest, J. R., & Thoma, S. J. (1999). Individual moral judgment and cultural ideologies. Developmental Psychology, 35, 478–488. STUDENT RESEARCH PROJECTS Research Project 8.1—Applying Ethics in Human Research Objective. Students will be able to assess and compare the knowledge of research ethics for human subjects by college students in psychology classes that teach research design. Type/Length of Activity. Out-of-class activity; variable. IM 8 | 20 Directions. Students should contact psychology professors in three courses that teach sections of research design for human subjects’ research. Explain to professors that their students may participate in an assessment of students’ ability to apply the ethical procedures and guidelines for human subjects to three research descriptions. The research descriptions will not adequately comply with the principles and guidelines in the handout. The assessments can be administered during class time with specified time limits or individually, having students discuss the ethical considerations of each. Students will need to make copies of the ethical principles and guidelines to distribute to students serving as their subjects. At the time of administration, read the descriptions of each of the research plans. Be prepared to answer questions that participants might ask. Three Ethical Principles The following ethical principles should be followed to plan and complete research with human subjects. 1. Beneficence—maximizing beneficial outcomes and minimizing potential risk or harm. 2. Respect—concern for autonomy of person and courtesy. 3. Justice—fair procedures and fair distribution of costs and benefits. The three ethical principles can be acted upon in five guidelines for scientific behavior: 1. Valid research design. Valid research designs must be used to accurately determine research results. Valid research designs consider theoretical orientation, possible data collection procedures, and previous research designs and results. 2. Identification of consequences. The potential risks and benefits of the research should be identified before beginning the research and measures taken to assure that they will not affect the subject or the research results. 3. Selection of subjects. The research participants should (1) be chosen based on the purpose of the study, (2) derive some benefit from participation, and (3) be free from coercive strategies that would influence their participation or the participation of disadvantaged subjects. 4. Voluntary informed consent. Participants should be given adequate information about the study to make knowledgeable decisions about their participation. Informed consent should be obtained before the research begins, in language that is appropriate to potential subjects, and with the knowledge that they can withdraw at any time. 5. Compensation for injury. The researcher is responsible for negative effects of the research and should compensate subjects for personal injury. IM 8 | 21 Research Plans These research plans are intended to assess students’ ability to apply the ethical principles given to you separately to the following research plans. Explain orally, or provide a written document for, each of the research plans and ask students to decide which of the ethical principles have been supported and which have been neglected. Compare their answers with those provided below. Research Plan 1. This exercise is designed to assess the effects of competition in solving a Piagetian problem in logical thinking. Half of the subjects will be told that the research is investigating the problem solving strategies used by subjects and the other half will be told that the investigation will identify the person who uses the best problem solving strategies. Answer. This study requires that some information be withheld from the subjects. (Neither group has a clear idea about the purpose of the investigation.) The informed consent should include the information that subjects can choose not to participate because they will not know ahead of time what the research is all about. The consent should explain that a full disclosure of the information will be provided after the research is complete. Those uncomfortable with these parameters can refuse participation. Research Plan 2. You plan to compare the cognitive abilities of college sophomores with those of retired adults over 70 years of age. The sophomores will be recruited from college classes and participants will get an A in their course and nonvolunteers will have their grade lowered. Retired people will be recruited from a retirement community by doing a door-to-door request for participants. Those who agree will be asked to complete some puzzles without explaining in detail what the study is designed to test. They probably would not be interested or understand the details anyway. Answer. In order for the comparison to be meaningful, comparable educational experiences, vision, test-taking skills, and motivation should be established. Thus, some screening procedures would be necessary as opposed to the recruitment strategies proposed here. Further, older people would probably be wary of letting a stranger come in at night for whatever reason. The sophomores in the psychology class are being coerced into participating in the study by the incentive of consequences to their grades. Research Plan 3. You plan to investigate the effects of watching television on the immediate and long-term behavior of children. Based on parental cooperation, one group will be exposed to television programs with violent content, a second group will be exposed to primetime situation comedies, and a third group will be exposed to educational programs from the Discovery channel every day for a month. At the end of the month, and a year later, each group will be asked to complete a questionnaire about solving conflict with their classmates. Answer. Subject selection may be skewed, especially if a group of parents allows the viewing of violent programs versus educational programs, other family characteristics may be influencing behavior patterns. Further, the long-term effects of watching violent programs could be very troublesome, and the researcher could be held responsible for deviant developmental patterns. It is unethical to set a child up for poor peer interaction. IM 8 | 22 Wrap-Up. After completing the data collection, the answers should be analyzed to determine students’ ability to respond to the research plans. Students could be required to write a full research paper or a short report of the results. The information should be shared with the professors of each class and with the psychology class for which the research project was completed. Reference Sieber, J. E. (1990). Three exercises on the ethics of research on humans. In Activities handbook for the teaching of psychology (pp. 266–270). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Research Project 8.2—Assessing Students’ Level of Moral Development Objective. Following the completion of this project, students conducting the investigation will be able to compare the level of moral and ethical development demonstrated by selected college students in comparison to Kohlberg’s six stages. Type/Length of Activity. Out-of-class activity; 4 hours. Directions. Look for famous quotes or quotes by famous authors on the Internet. Numerous resources are available through Google. Choose one quote from each of Kohlberg’s stages as shown. Write each quote on a file card (do not provide the stage number), and label each quote randomly by an alphabetical letter. Find six college students (three female and three male) who are willing to rank the quotes from highest moral reasoning to lowest moral reasoning. Compare their answers to your answers. Also ask each participant to pick out the quote that best reflects his/her own beliefs. Record their answers. You may use the data sheet in the Student Handouts section of this Instructor’s Manual. Example Quotes “Every disorder of the soul is its own punishment.”—Saint Augustine (Stage 1) “ ‘Am I living in a way which is deeply satisfying to me, and which truly expresses me?’ This I think is perhaps the most important question for the creative individual.”—Carl Rogers (Stage 2) “What you do not want others to do to you, do not do to others.”—Confucius (Stage 3) “My country right or wrong.”—Anonymous (Stage 4) “Morality is the observance of the rights of others.”—Dagobert D. Runes (Stage 5) “A world at peace will be one where the rights of every human being—dignity, liberty, and the basic rights of education and health care—will be respected.”—Kris Kristofferson (Stage 6) 1. Why did you pick the quotes that you did? Do you think your choices influenced the results? 2. How closely did the subjects reflect Kohlberg’s hierarchy? IM 8 | 23 3. Some research findings suggest that Kohlberg’s six stages are more typical of male morality than female morality. Did your male subjects reflect Kohlberg’s rating more closely than did females? 4. Gilligan’s research suggests that females choose caring morality over justice morality, and that males prefer the reverse. Do your subjects’ choices reflect this gender difference? 5. Evaluate the use of quotations in moral development research. What are the advantages and disadvantages? Wrap-Up. Write a 4-page research paper to explain your procedures and summarize your results. Include your quote choices and research results. Discuss possible gender differences in moral development and what you attribute these differences to. Be prepared to discuss your results in class. ESSAY QUESTIONS Review the guidelines for “Answering Essay Questions” before students respond to these questions. 1. Define moral development and explain why thoughts, feelings, and behaviors are important aspects of the individual to understand. 2. Compare and contrast Piaget and Kohlberg’s theories of moral development. Which seems more relevant to the study of adolescence? Why? 3. What are the criticisms of Kohlberg’s theory? Explain their importance to contemporary dilemmas that adolescents might encounter. 4. Compare and contrast Kohlberg and Gilligan’s theories of moral development. 5. What are the similarities and differences between the behavioral and social cognitive perspectives of moral development? 6. Define altruism and discuss the roles that moral thinking and feeling play in expressions of altruism. 7. Outline the psychoanalytic explanation of moral development. What approaches do researchers recommend that parents watch for or adopt? 8. Explain the characteristics of character education, values clarification, cognitive moral education, and service learning. What are the strengths and weaknesses of each approach? IM 8 | 24 9. Characterize adolescents’ involvement in religion and whether their involvement relates to other aspects of their lives. 10. Compare and contrast cults with mainstream religious groups. What are the characteristics of adolescents who are attracted to cults? References Borba, M. (2001). Building moral intelligence: The seven essential virtues that teach kids to do the right thing. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Gurian, M. (2000). The good son: Shaping the moral development of our boys and young men. New York: Penguin Putman Inc. Hoffman, M. L. (2002). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University. VIDEO Adolescent and Parent Emotions (VAD) Overview Child developmentalists who have studied child-rearing techniques and moral development have focused on parents’ discipline techniques. These include love withdrawal, power assertion, and induction. Moral development theorist and researcher Martin Hoffman believes that any discipline produces arousal on the adolescent’s part. Love withdrawal and power assertion are likely to evoke a very high level of arousal, with love withdrawal generating considerable anxiety and power assertion considerable hostility. Induction is more likely to produce a moderate level of arousal in adolescents, a level that permits them to attend to the cognitive rationales parents offer. When a parent uses power assertion or love withdrawal, the adolescent may be so aroused that, even if the parent gives accompanying explanations about the consequences for others of the adolescent’s actions, the adolescent might not attend to them. Power assertion presents parents as weak models of self-control—as individuals who cannot control their feelings. Accordingly, adolescents may imitate this model of poor self-control when they face stressful circumstances. The use of induction, however, focuses the adolescent’s attention on the action’s consequences for others, not on the adolescent’s own shortcomings. In this segment, Dr. Reed Larson discusses how parents and adolescents, respectively, tend to experience and express their emotions. From his comments, consider how Hoffman may have reached his conclusions about the connection between emotional arousal and the effectiveness of one parental discipline technique over another. Pre-Test 1. Induction is a discipline technique in which a parent a. attempts to gain control over the adolescent or the adolescent’s resources. Examples include spanking, threatening, or removing privileges. Incorrect. This discipline technique is power assertion. IM 8 | 25 b. withholds attention or love from the adolescent, as when the parent refuses to talk to the adolescent or states a dislike for the child. Incorrect. This discipline technique is love withdrawal. c. uses reason and explanation of how the adolescent’s actions may affect others. Correct. In induction, a parent might say, “If you yell at her, she’ll be angry and it will be harder to solve the root problem.” d. refrains from expressing emotion. Incorrect. Whether or not the parent expresses emotion does not necessarily constitute a discipline technique. Post-Test 1. In Dr. Larson’s observations, adolescents tend to experience emotions differently from their parents in the following manner. a. Adolescents feel annoyed more often than their parents do. Incorrect. There does not appear to be a big difference in how frequently adolescents or parents feel an emotion. b. Adolescents tend to be unhappy and their parents tend to be happy. Incorrect. There does not appear to be a big difference in happiness versus unhappiness between adolescents and their parents. c. Adolescents experience greater range and intensity of emotion, or arousal, than their parents do. Correct. Dr. Larson observes parental emotions as more moderate and adolescent emotions as more extreme. d. Adolescents experience lower levels of arousal than their parents do. Incorrect. On the contrary, Dr. Larson observes adolescent arousal as higher than that experienced by parents. IM 8 | 26