Emotional intelligence and performance Running head: EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND PERFORMANCE AT WORK Emotional Intelligence and Performance at Work Vanessa Urch Druskat Whittemore School of Business and Economics The University of New Hampshire Durham, NH 03824 USA Ph 603-862-3348 Fax 603-862-3383 Email Vanessa.druskat@unh.edu Peter J Jordan Griffith Business School Griffith University Australia Ph 37353717 Fax 37353887 Email Peter.Jordan@griffith.edu.au Article submitted for inclusion in Cherniss, C. (Ed) American Psychologist Special Edition on Emotional Intelligence. Acknowledgements: We would like to thank Cary Cherniss for his invaluable comments on drafts, and Josina Fluehr-Lobban for her help with our literature review. 1 Emotional intelligence and performance 2 Abstract A growing body of research reveals that emotion has a pervasive influence in organizations. Thus, some scholars have argued that emotional intelligence abilities that enable one to think intelligently about emotion can support constructive behavior and decision-making in organizations and enhance the performance of individuals, groups, and organizations. But, theory and research on emotional intelligence have emerged quickly and important questions about the concept abound. In this article, we review the current state of the interdisciplinary literature on the link between emotional intelligence and work performance. We suggest that theory and research are progressively moving in two directions (1) one aimed towards recognition of emotional intelligence as a bonafide type of intelligence that, among other things, supports the development of constructive social relationships and work performance, (2) the second aimed towards identifying the social and emotional skills and competencies made possible by emotional intelligence and that supports the development of social relationships and work performance. We show that, despite using different measurement techniques, researchers aiming in both directions demonstrate that, in certain contexts, emotional intelligence predicts work performance over and above measures of personality and general mental ability. We also suggest directions for theory and research that can advance knowledge and practice. Keywords: Emotions, Emotional Intelligence, Work Performance. Emotional intelligence and performance 3 Emotional Intelligence and Performance at Work These are exciting times for researchers interested in the study of emotions in organizations. It is no coincidence that the growing fascination over a possible link between emotional intelligence and work performance has occurred amid the current “affective revolution” (Barsade, Brief, & Spataro, 2003) in organizational studies. Despite the inseparable link between emotion and cognition (Damasio, 1994) the “affective revolution” is a movement of the pendulum away from an earlier dominant focus on the value of cognition and rational thinking that occurred during the “cognitive revolution” in the 1970’s and 1980’s (see Barker, Abrams, Tiyaamornwong, et al., 2000). At that time, emotion was relatively absent from organizational research and exploring emotion in organizational life was “largely deemphasized, marginalized, or ignored” (Martin, Knopoff, & Beckman, 1998, p. 429). The pendulum has now swung towards recognition that employee emotions are unavoidable and necessary and that they influence work behavior and outcomes. The notion that emotion influences processes and performance at work is not new (see Homans, 1950; Rothlisberger & Dickson, 1939), what is new is the mounting research evidence linking emotion to performance and other valuable consequences in organizations (Ashkanasy, Zerbe & Hartel, 2005). It is within this context that research on emotional intelligence in the workplace has taken-off in the last decade. It started in the early 1990’s with the publication of Mayer and Salovey’s first theoretical article on the topic (Salovey & Mayer, 1990) and Goleman’s exciting and user-friendly books (Goleman, 1995, 1998). As we will discuss, it shows no signs of slowing down. There is understandable excitement over the idea of a form of intelligence that facilitates perceiving, understanding, and managing emotion, and that enables the use of emotion to improve thought and action. Emotional intelligence and performance 4 For practicing managers in organizations, emotional intelligence can help explain why employees with strong academic backgrounds or cognitive skills are not always the best team members or leaders. Regardless of the need for additional construct clarification and measurement testing (see Murphy, 2006), its mere existence as a concept has broadened managers’ ideas about the abilities and skills necessary for success in certain job contexts. This is significant because although research has shown that failure in managerial-level positions is usually due to a lack of emotional and interpersonal skills (Lombardo, Ruderman, & McCauley, 1988) discussions about such topics have been isolated and few. Practicing managers want frameworks, assessments, and developmental tools that can help them avoid costly hiring and promotion mistakes, and that can help them develop the capabilities of their most talented employees. They are therefore particularly enthusiastic about the growing evidence that emotional intelligence is not fixed and can be developed by those with the motivation to do so (Boyatzis, Stubbs, & Taylor, 2002; Caruso & Salovey, 2004). At the same time, some prominent Industrial/Organizational psychologists have fiercely criticized conceptualizations and research on emotional intelligence (Matthews, Zeidner, & Roberts, 2002) and notions that it offers researchers and organizations anything new (Landy, 2005; Locke, 2005). As valuable gate-keepers, their role has been to hold a mirror to researchers, to ask them to temper enthusiasm until good reliable data is collected over time, and to outline questions and concerns that must be addressed if research and theory are to move forward in an effective and sustainable way. Our purpose in this article is to present and discuss the current state of knowledge on the link between emotional intelligence and workplace performance—at multiple levels in organizations. Our review is aimed at identifying promising directions for future theory and research. We begin by weighing in on the current controversies over the concept of emotional Emotional intelligence and performance 5 intelligence because of their centrality to understanding the current state of the literature and their relevance to the new directions we propose. Background and Controversies When Daniel Goleman’s first book on emotional intelligence (Goleman, 1995) appeared, a small fraction of it addressed emotional intelligence in the workplace. The face validity of the concept led practicing managers to quickly pursue more information. Goleman responded with a second book focused on the value of EI in the workplace (Goleman, 1998). Goleman is a former student of the late David C. McClelland who arguably spearheaded the idea of job competency modelling (see McClelland, 1973). Thus, he turned to competency research to provide answers about the manifestation of emotional intelligence in the workplace. Goleman defined an “emotional competence” as “a learned capability based on emotional intelligence that results in outstanding performance at work,” and proposed that both social and emotional competencies fell under this definition (Goleman, 1998, p. 24). Goleman’s theory is rooted in data from decades of inductive qualitative competency research carried out by McClelland and his former students (see Boyatzis, 1982; Spencer & Spencer, 1993). That research revealed that competencies falling theoretically under the categories of: self-awareness, self-management, social awareness, and social skills were significantly more often demonstrated by the highest performing managers than the competencies theoretically categorized as “cognitive” (e.g., systems thinking, pattern recognition). At a later time, Goleman and his colleague Richard Boyatzis developed a selfand other- report survey (i.e., 360 degrees) that assesses perceived demonstration of the competencies in the model (see Sala, 2002; Boyatzis, Goleman, & Rhee, 2000). As discussed, research on the role of emotions in organizations was already burgeoning and this facilitated the speed with which researchers turned their attention to emotional intelligence. The relative newness of the concept meant that researchers needed to Emotional intelligence and performance 6 develop their own models and measurement instruments. For example, Reuven Bar-On who had been studying emotional well-being offered his definition of “emotional-social intelligence” and his self-report instrument which soon was shown to predict the job performance of recruiters in the U.S. Air Force, recruits in the Israeli Defense Forces (BarOn, 1997, 2006) and several others. This was followed by emotional intelligence models and self-report instruments developed by Schutte and colleagues (1998; 2000), Jordan and colleagues (Jordan, Ashkanasy, Hartel, & Hooper, 2002), Wong and Law (2002) and others (for more examples, see article by Gowing & O’Leary this issue). At the same time, the originators of the emotional intelligence concept, Mayer and Salovey, were working with their colleagues toward another end (Mayer, Salovey, Caruso, & Sitarenios, 2001). Their definition and their research and writing present emotional intelligence as a legitimate parallel to more widely recognized forms of intelligence (e.g., verbal or spatial intelligence). Their research program has been aimed at testing whether emotional intelligence meets traditional standards as a form of intelligence (see Mayer, Caruso, & Salovey, 2000). Toward that end, they developed, tested, and revised an ability measure that, unlike the other self-report (or other-report) measures, requires subjects to reason accurately in the areas of perceiving, assimilating, understanding, and managing emotions (see Mayer, Panter, Salovey, Sitaraneos, 2005; Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2002). Critics of emotional intelligence favor the Mayer and Salovey model and stream of research and their measurement instrument as a more legitimate test of emotional intelligence (see Daus & Ashkanasy, 2005; Matthews, Zeidner, & Roberts, 2002; Murphy, 2006). On the whole, they focus their complaints at the more organizationally focused models of emotional intelligence. They complain about the of the lack of a consistent definition of emotional intelligence, the overly broad nature of some definitions (for example, Goleman’s framework includes twenty competencies and Bar-On’s includes fifteen) (Locke, 2005), the similarity Emotional intelligence and performance 7 between some definitions of EI and personality characteristics (Matthews et al., 2002), the lack of reliable and valid measurement instruments, and the lack of rigorous research demonstrating that emotional intelligence adds predictive validity over and above what personality measures and cognitive intelligence measures already offer (Landy, 2005). They also condemn the “opportunistic” behavior of those they perceive to exaggerate the value of emotional intelligence for their own financial gain (see, Fineman, 2004; Landy, 2005; Locke, 2005; Matthews et al., 2002). Some critics demand to see the numbers supporting the claims made by advocates like Goleman whose theory is based on research that is primarily inductive, qualitative, and proprietary because most of it was conducted by students of McClelland’s while working for his consulting firm, which is now owned by the Hay Group (for details on their research methods see Boyatzis, 1998) (Landy, 2005, 2006). Several of these important criticisms point to problems that are predictable during the early stages of construct development (see Weick, 1989). We address two of them in this article: (1) the quality of research that supports the link between emotional intelligence and work performance, and (2) the inconsistencies of models, definitions and measurements of the construct. Two Conceptions of Emotional Intelligence To begin addressing the inconsistencies in models and definitions of emotional intelligence, we make what seems like a natural proposition -- that two categories of theory on emotional intelligence exist. Each involves a distinct but valid conception of emotional intelligence, and research and theory in each category aim toward related but distinct purposes. Others have also discussed the existence of two categories of theory. For example, Petrides and his colleagues (Petrides, Furnham, & Frederickson, 2004) discuss one category as the study of “ability emotional intelligence,” defined as emotion- related cognitive abilities measured via performance tests. They propose the second category to be “trait Emotional intelligence and performance 8 emotional intelligence,” defined as emotion-related dispositions or self-perceptions measured via self-report. Offermann and her colleagues (Offermann, Bailey, Vasilopoulos, Seal, Sass, 2004) also discuss two categories of theory. One category defines emotional intelligence as a legitimate intelligence or set of cognitive abilities that involve emotion. The second category defines a “mixed-model” of emotional intelligence that includes skills and personality characteristics that may be products of emotional intelligence (see also Matthews et al., 2002). A recent meta-analysis supports the two category distinction (Van Rooy, Viswesvaran & Pluta, 2005). It demonstrated that researchers studying “mixed-models” of emotional intelligence use a variety of self-report measures, yet their measures overlap significantly and correlate .71 among themselves. They correlate .14 to the more ability-focused measures use to study emotional intelligence as a type of intelligence (Van Rooy, Viswesvaran & Pluta, 2005). However, as will be discussed in greater depth below, researchers in these distinct groups have found that their constructs, despite being assessed through very different measures, both predict work performance over and above personality traits and general mental ability in certain contexts (e.g., Côté & Miners, 2006; Law, Wong, & Song, 2004; Van Rooy & Viswesvaran, 2004). Thus, for the purposes of this article, we label the two conceptions of emotional intelligence as focused upon: (1) emotional intelligence, defined as the capacity to carry out accurate reasoning about emotions and to use emotions to enhance thought (see Mayer & Salovey, 1990), and (2) expressed emotional intelligence, defined as the behaviors or actions resulting from the strength of one’s capacity for emotional intelligence. The label “expressed emotional intelligence” acknowledges that this conception aims to represent skills and behaviors --not personality traits as some critics have argued. Personality traits are generally defined as relatively stable and enduring characteristics of a person (Snyder & Ickes, 1985). Emotional intelligence and performance 9 But, the skills in expressed emotional intelligence models can change and develop over time (see Bar-On, Handley, & Fund, 2006; Boyatzis et al., 2002; Ciarrochi & Godsell, 2006). For example, as a person’s emotional intelligence increases, a person can increase in her capacity to understand and/or control her emotions (see Bar-On’s dimensions of “emotional selfawareness” and “impulse control,” Goleman’s dimensions of “self-awareness” and “selfcontrol,” or Shutte’s dimensions of “emotional awareness” and emotional control”). We also preferred the label “expressed emotional intelligence” to one that refers to the construct by the way it is measured (e.g., self-concept or self-report emotional intelligence), because of the danger of identifying a construct by the way it is measured (e.g., IQ is that which IQ measures), which can steer theory and research away from addressing the most fundamental question at this early stage in the history of this concept-- What exactly is emotional intelligence (see Oatley, 2004, p. 221)? Hereafter, we separately review research falling under the two conceptions of emotional intelligence. Emotional Intelligence and Work Performance in Different Job Contexts If there is a discernable fact emerging from research on the antecedents of work performance it is that context matters. The predictors of performance in a job making cars on an assembly line differ from those in a job processing insurance claims, or leading a company. Actions and behaviors relevant to performance will also vary by type of organization. For example, those of an administrative assistant in a university setting may differ from those of an administrative assistant in a fast-paced high-technology firm. A list of the ways that context can influence the skills, behaviors, and strategies that predict work performance could be endless. As there has not been enough research done on emotional intelligence to examine patterns in highly specific job or organizational contexts, below we present and discuss the research in three broad contextual categories: (1) individual Emotional intelligence and performance 10 contributors, (2) leadership roles, and (3) group or team tasks. As discussed above, we separate our discussion by the category of emotional intelligence studied. Individual Contributors Individual contributors in organizations usually do not have leadership or managerial responsibilities. During the course of their work day, events lead to the experience of a wide range of emotions, e.g., joy, worry, love, frustration, that can have an effect on one’s work performance (Ashkanasy, 2003). Because most jobs involve the need to interact with other people, some of these emotions may occur during interpersonal interactions (Côté & Miners, 2006). Thus, emotional intelligence abilities that enable understanding, managing or using emotions toward constructive ends may influence the performance of individual contributors. Emotional intelligence in individual contributor roles. We located four studies published in peer-reviewed journal articles examining the influence of emotional intelligence (as intelligence) on work performance (see Table 1). Lam and Kirby (2002) studied the individual cognitive-based performance of college students under stressful circumstances that involved difficult problems and very short time allotments. After controlling for general mental ability, performance was significantly predicted by the EI branches of perceiving emotions, and regulating emotions, but not the third branch they studied, understanding emotions. A related study (discussed in our section on team contexts) also found that individual performance on a cognitive task was predicted by perceiving emotions, but not other branches of EI (Day and Carroll, 2004). It appears that cognitive tasks are enhanced by the ability to monitor and be aware of one’s own emotion. When one is under great pressure while completing a cognitive task, the ability to regulate or manage one’s emotions also becomes of significant importance. ------------------------------Insert Table 1 about here Emotional intelligence and performance 11 ---------------------------------Lopes and his colleagues (Lopes, Grewal, Kadis, Gall, & Salovey, 2006) conducted a field study examining the influence of emotional intelligence on the work performance of individual contributors whose tasks were cognitive, but also involved an interpersonal component. They studied analysts and clerical and administrative staff from the finance department of a U.S. insurance company. In this job context, the EI branch of perceiving emotion was not related to performance. However, the three other EI branches they studied were related to performance. Using emotions to facilitate thought and action (e.g., drawing on emotions in the service of achieving goals) was related to company rank. The EI branches of understanding emotion and managing emotion were found to be significantly related to both company rank and percent merit pay increase. These two EI branches that are more strategic in nature and are related to managing oneself around others in the organization (see Mayer, Salovey & Caruso, 2004) make a difference to employee rank and merit pay, despite the fact that these are individual contributor roles. This supports the notion that individual contributor roles requiring interpersonal interaction benefit from emotional intelligence. So far, we have only seen the relevance of the perceiving emotions branch of EI in highly cognitive tasks. In the next study, Elfenbein and Ambady (2002) took a closer look at the perceiving emotion branch in a field study of employees whose jobs were largely dependent on interpersonal interaction. Using an interesting theoretical twist, they looked specifically at a subset of the perceiving emotion ability they labelled “eavesdropping on feelings.” Eavesdropping skill was defined as the ability to extract non-verbal emotions from vocal tones better than from facial expressions. Study participants were recent college graduates doing a year of community service work that included organizing after school programs, serving as school teaching assistants, and working in teams on community-related tasks. Results showed that in a task involving a great deal of interpersonal interactions Emotional intelligence and performance 12 employee effectiveness was related to skill at eavesdropping on positive feelings, e.g., happiness, but not to skill at eavesdropping on negative feelings, e.g., anger, fear, or sadness, which had a negative effect on ratings of employee effectiveness. In this highly interpersonal context, it may be that eavesdropping on negative feelings distracts from the task by producing an unconstructive amount of empathy, or it may be that it provides access to feelings that make others uncomfortable and would better go unnoticed (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002). Recognizing a co-worker or clients’ emotions (i.e., perceiving emotions) might be most useful if the perceiver is also skilled in managing emotions (e.g., skill at turning anger into constructive behavior), unfortunately that EI branch was not assessed (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002). Côté and Miners (2006) studied primarily individual contributors working in various jobs at a large public university. Jobs included building and grounds (1% of sample), business and finance (7%), computer and mathematical (16%), education and library (28%), maintenance (1%), legal (1%), personal care (1%), protective service (2%), administrative support (23%) and managers (21%). This study assessed the influence of total EI and did not examine the four branches of EI discussed by Mayer and Salovey (Mayer, Salovey & Caruso, 2002). Findings revealed what the authors labelled a compensatory model: Total EI became a stronger predictor of performance as cognitive intelligence decreased. It is interesting to compare this finding to that of a study conducted with non-managerial employees in Hong Kong. Here it was found that total EI was significantly correlated with employee performance, however, the amount of emotional labor required in the job (i.e., how often employees must manage or act out certain emotions in a job) moderated the EI – performance link such that EI became more important the more often the employees job required emotional labor (Wong and Law, 2002). Together these studies show that in certain situations, which can be internal (less cognitive ability) or external (emotionally challenging Emotional intelligence and performance 13 job) to the person, emotional intelligence becomes increasingly important for individual performers. Future theory and research should continue to examine moderators between the EI –performance relationship. It is worth noting that three of the studies in this category found relationships between EI and performance while controlling for the effects of personality traits, cognitive intelligence or both on performance. This provides a valuable response to criticisms that EI does not demonstrate predictive validity over and above what personality measures and cognitive intelligence measures already offer (see Landy, 2005). Expressed emotional intelligence in individual contributor roles. We located seven studies examining the role of expressed emotional intelligence on the work performance of individual contributors (see Table 1). Five different self-report measures were use to study Expressed EI in these studies; one study also used peer-ratings of EI. The jobs studied included a miscellaneous group of study participants who had attended courses, workshops, and seminars (Petrides & Furnham, 2006) ballet dancers (Petrides, Niven & Mouskounti, 2006), salespeople (Rozell, Pettijohn & Parker, 2006), accounting majors interviewing for jobs at the Big 5 certified public accounting firms (Chia, 2005), workers in a cigarette factory in China (Law et al., 2004), mental health professionals (Nikolaou & Tsaousis, 2002) and debt collectors (Bachman, Stein, Campbell, Sitarenios, 2000). Again, it can be argued that all of these individual contributor roles required interacting with others, whether in a ballet performance where one seeks to connect emotionally with the audience and other dancers on stage, or in a sales role, job interview, or mental health facility where interpersonal interactions are the primary focus of the work, or among debt collects speaking on the phone and engaging in what we can assume are challenging conversations. Six of the studies examined the correlation between performance and total expressed EI (i.e., all EI dimensions aggregated), and all of these found a significant positive Emotional intelligence and performance 14 correlation. However, Petrides and Furnham (2006) found the significant link only for their female study participants. This finding was consistent with their earlier study showing that men are more likely to estimate themselves as having higher IQ and lower EI and women are more likely to estimate themselves as higher in EI and lower in IQ (Petrides, Furnham, & Martin, 2004). Similarly, another study found that females rate themselves as demonstrating more EI than do males (Van Rooy, Alonso, & Viswesvaran, 2005). It is clear that additional research should examine sex differences in self-ratings of expressed emotional intelligence. Another pattern evident in these studies of expressed EI in individual contributors was the effect of EI on job stress and job commitment. Two studies showed a relationship between lowered levels of work stress. Nikolaou and Tsaousis (2002) found that EI was linked to lowered stress among mental health workers – an important finding considering the high levels of stress in an occupation that requires constant care-taking. Petrides and Furnham (2006) found the same link in their diverse participant sample; they also found that EI was linked to significantly less stress in men as well as women. Three studies also found a link between EI and commitment or dedication to the work (Petrides & Furhnam, 2006; Law et al., 2004; Nikolaou & Tsaousis, 2002). Petrides and Furnham (2006) also found that for both men and women, total expressed EI was significantly related to perceived job control (p < .01), and job satisfaction (p < .01). Unfortunately none of these studies carefully assessed which EI sub-scales accounted for the most variance in these outcomes. Stress would likely be reduced by competence in understanding one’s emotions and their effects (i.e., understanding emotion) coupled with the ability to manage those emotions within the context of one’s goals (i.e., managing emotion). Commitment, perceived job control and job satisfaction might also be influenced by the EI dimension of using emotions, which enables one to activate emotions on the service of one’s goals. Of course, research is necessary to test those hypotheses. Emotional intelligence and performance 15 It should be noted that one of these studies controlled for the influence of the Big Five personality traits. After controlling for personality, total EI was shown to account for more than 10% of the variance for both job-performance and contextual performance (i.e., interpersonal facilitation while carrying out the role and job dedication) (Law et al., 2004). Summary of research on individual contributors. The thirteen studies we reviewed so far reveal that emotional intelligence –measured as a form of intelligence and as skills that grow out of that intelligence -- is significantly associated with job performance in a number of job contexts. Studies that examine the specific branches or dimensions of EI are most helpful for improving understanding of exactly why EI matters to the performance of individual contributors. Based on this review, it seems reasonable to assume that when an individual contributor is working alone on a purely cognitive task, the ability to perceive emotions, or recognize their presence, aids performance. If the individual task is carried out under stressful circumstances, the ability to manage emotions or effectively control them then also becomes essential to performance. Most of the other individual tasks studied included an interpersonal component to the job, whether it was working to interpersonally facilitate the performance of others in a cigarette factory, dancing on stage, interacting with clients who haven’t paid their bills, or assigning patients in a mental health institution, the ability to use, understand, and manage emotions supported effective performance. . Leadership Roles In this section, we review studies examining the influence of emotional intelligence on the performance of leaders and managers. The studies include team leaders, first-line managers, government leaders, and executives (see Table 2). There is not yet enough research on each specific role to examine them separately, thus we grouped them together and henceforth use the terms leader and manager interchangeably. When authors provide details Emotional intelligence and performance 16 about the specific responsibilities managed by their study participants, we present it to aid interpretation of their study results. ------------------------------Insert Table 2 about here ---------------------------------In general, a leader’s role involves some or all of the following responsibilities: interpreting events for followers, choosing objectives for the group or organization, organizing and planning work activities to accomplish the objectives, motivating followers to achieve the objectives, maintaining cooperative relationships and teamwork, and enlisting support and cooperation from people outside the group or organization (Yukl, 1997). A study of newly hired or promoted leaders showed that the greatest challenge is in adjusting to having one’s own performance dependent on the motivation of others (Hill, 1992). It is likely that emotional intelligence would help a leader meet these responsibilities. To begin with, emotion is integral to motivation; it influences and directs the persistence of effort required to obtain objectives (Lord & Kanfer, 2002). Therefore, the ability to recognize, use, understand and manage emotion might enable a leader to effectively motivate others. For example, research reveals that the way a leader manages his own emotions influences the emotions of employees. Leader behavior during interpersonal interactions with employees has been found to have a significant influence on employee positive and negative emotions; moreover, employees are more likely to hold onto and remember the negative emotions than the positive emotions that result from these interactions (Dasborough, 2006). Other research has demonstrated that leaders’ moods are particularly contagious in the workplace and significantly influence the moods of their employees (Bono & Ilies, 2006; Sy, Côté, & Saavedra, 2005). This is relevant because moods and the valence of emotions influence employee cognition and performance; positive affect increases an employee’s Emotional intelligence and performance 17 cognitive flexibility and creative problem solving ability (Amabile, Barsade, Mueller, & Staw, 2005; Isen, 1993). Negative affect such as fear or anxiety can hurt an employee’s ability to effectively process information (Holmer, 1994). Emotional intelligence in managerial and leadership roles. We located two studies examining the influence of emotional intelligence (as intelligence) on leader work performance and one that examined whether EI influenced leadership emergence in teams. Rosete and Ciarrochi (2007, 2005) conducted two studies with senior executives in Australian public service companies. In the first study, they examined the relationship between EI and leadership effectiveness defined as (1) the achievement of last year’s goals and (2) ratings of “how” leaders achieved business goals, i.e., did they build effective working relationships along the way? They found that total EI scores were significantly related to “how” goals were achieved, but not to the achievement of those goals. Closer examination revealed that two branches of EI were significantly correlated with “how” goals were achieved: perceiving emotions and understanding emotions. Regression analyses showed that perceiving emotions accounted for the most variance (18%) in “how” goals were achieved and remained a significant predictor after controlling for personality and cognitive intelligence. It is interesting to note that in this environment the strongest predictor of goal achievement was the personality trait “dominance.” To look more closely at how EI affects leadership behavior in this organization, the study authors also examined the link between EI and ratings of the leader on the list of “core leadership capabilities” in the Australian Public Service Commission Leadership Capability Framework. The core capabilities were assessed using a 360-rating process in which the leaders, their managers, and a minimum of three of their employees rated the frequency with which they were demonstrated by a leader. Total EI was significantly correlated with “cultivates productive working relationships” and “exemplifies personal drive and integrity.” Emotional intelligence and performance 18 The EI branch of understanding emotions was the one most frequently correlated to leader capabilities and was significantly related to: “shapes strategic thinking”, “communicates with influence” and “cultivates productive working relationships” In a second study, Rosete and Ciarrochi (2007) examined another 122 senior executives at an Australian public service company. Similar to the first study, leadership performance was defined as (1) “achieving business outcomes” and (2) “effective interpersonal behavior.” This time total EI was significantly correlated with both performance outcomes. Moreover, similar to the last study, after controlling for personality and cognitive intelligence the two EI branches of perceiving emotions and managing emotions significantly predicted “effective interpersonal behavior.” However, this time, both branches also significantly predicted “achieving business outcomes.” A third study examined whether Total EI predicted leader emergence in undergraduate work teams who performed a ten-week consulting project for a course requirement (Côté, Lopes & Salovey, 2006). Results showed that after controlling for personality, total EI predicted a student’s emergence as a leader. Interestingly, the higher team members’ scored on the personality trait of emotional instability, the more strongly total EI predicted leadership emergence. This suggests that a leaders’ EI ability may be even more relevant in challenging interpersonal contexts (Côté et al., 2006). Overall, these studies show that EI supports effective leader performance. The most consistent predictor of performance in the first two studies was perceiving emotion (i.e., awareness of emotions and the ability to differentiate emotions in oneself and others), which significantly predicted the building of effective relationships in both studies and also significantly predicted the achievement of business outcomes in the second study. In the second study, managing emotions also significantly predicted the achievement of business Emotional intelligence and performance 19 outcomes. In both studies, these branches of EI predicted the performance outcomes over and above measures of personality traits and cognitive intelligence. So for it appears, that for leaders, emotional intelligence is linked to two seemingly distinct aspects of the role. These are evident in the leadership capabilities with which total EI correlated in the first study: “cultivates productive working relationships” and “exemplifies personal drive and integrity.” Through abilities like perceiving and managing emotions, which played a large role in these studies, leaders are able to understand others and manage themselves well enough to build relationships and focus their drive toward achieving goals. Additional theory and research is needed to determine more precisely how this happens. Expressed emotional intelligence in leadership roles. We located eight peer-reviewed studies examining the influence of expressed EI on leader effectiveness (two are published in one article -- see Table 2). Overall, these studies revealed clear and consistent evidence of significant links between expressed EI and leader performance, job satisfaction and health and well-being. Five of the studies found that leaders’ total expressed EI score significantly correlated with leader performance (Carmeli, 2003; Dulewicz, Higgs, & Slaski, 2003; Semadar, Robins, & Ferris, 2006; Slaski & Cartwright, 2002; Sy, Tram, O’Hara, et al., 2006). Five studies found a significant correlation between leader total expressed EI and the leader’s job satisfaction or morale (Carmeli, 2003; Dulewicz, et al, 2003; Slaski & Cartwright, 2002; Sy et al., 2006). Two studies found a significant correlation between leader total expressed EI and lower stress (Dulewicz, et al, 2003; Slaski & Cartwright, 2002). Finally, two studies found a significant correlation between leader total expressed EI and work-life balance (Carmeli, 2003; Slaski & Cartwright, 2002). It appears that expressed EI is not only good for leader performance, but also for leader health and well-being. Three of the studies in this category examined correlations between manager’s EI and employee performance and satisfaction (Sy et al., 2006; Wong & Law, 2002). One of these Emotional intelligence and performance 20 studies conduced with food service workers and their managers showed a significant correlation between manager’s total expressed EI and employee performance and job satisfaction (Sy et al., 2006). Similarly, the second study conducted with middle and upperlevel managers in Hong Kong also found a significant correlation between managers’ total expressed EI and employee performance and job satisfaction (Wong & Law, 2002). The third study, again conducted by Wong and Law (2002) with middle-level administrators working in the Hong Kong government, found that manager’s total EI was significantly correlated with employee job satisfaction, but not performance. It is interesting to speculate about what it was in the job or job context in the third study that negated the EI-performance link, but not the EI-job satisfaction link seen in the other two. Two of these studies, found evidence that leader expressed EI became more useful when employees were faced with challenging interpersonal circumstances. The study of food service workers found that the lower employees’ scored on a measure of expressed EI the more strongly manager expressed EI was correlated with the employees’ job satisfaction (p < .05) and performance (p < .07) (Sy et al., 2006). The study conducted with middle- and upper-level managers in Hong-Kong found that leader expressed EI was more significantly associated with employee performance the more the employee’s job required emotional labor. As discussed earlier, emotional labor is work that necessitates managing or acting out certain emotions in a job, for example, emotional labor involves providing customer service with a smile regardless of one’s true feelings (see Hochschild, 1983). The findings in these studies are consistent with the earlier study demonstrating that the link between EI and leader emergence in a team increased as average team member emotional instability increased (Côté et al., 2006). Together they suggest that leader EI or expressed EI is particularly beneficial when employees face interpersonally challenging situations – either for reasons that are internal to the employee (i.e., lack of EI) or external to Emotional intelligence and performance 21 the employee (i.e., co-workers are emotionally unstable or the work involves emotional labor). It is also important to point out the similarity between these findings and that of the study conducted by Côté and Miners (2006) revealing that the lower an individual’s cognitive intelligence, the more relevant emotional intelligence becomes to employee performance. There is a clear and consistent patterns suggesting that emotional intelligence can aid employees facing internal and external challenges at work. In contrast to the studies demonstrating the important link between leader expressed EI and employee needs, two studies in this category found the interpersonal or relationship oriented dimensions of EI less necessary for strong leader performance. These studies set out to determine the dimensions of expressed EI that were most strongly associated with the performance of middle-level managers in large retail organizations. Despite using different measures, both found that the EI dimensions focused on recognizing and managing one’s own emotions were linked to performance, but that those expressed EI dimensions focused on interpersonal sensitivity or interpersonal relationships were not. Dulewicz and colleagues (2003) found that manger performance was significantly correlated with the expressed EI self-awareness, emotional resilience and motivation – but not interpersonal sensitivity. Slaski and Cartwright (2002) found that manager performance was significantly correlated with expressed intrapersonal EI, adaptability, stress management, and general mood -- but not interpersonal EI. It may be that middle-level managers in the retail industry have less direct contact with employees and less need to develop relationships, or it may be that the way these manager’s regulate themselves and their moods matters most to achieving objectives in this environment. These are questions for future theory and research. Similar results occurred in a study by Semadar and colleagues (Semadar et al., 2006) who examined the influence of several social effectiveness constructs on the performance of leaders in the Australian division of a global motor manufacturing company, including Emotional intelligence and performance 22 expressed EI and political skill. Leaders participating in the study worked in a number of areas including manufacturing (11%), stamping (7%), casing (8%), marking/sales (16%), human resources (13%), product development (23%,) and others such as IT, finance, etc., (21%). Both expressed EI and political skill were found to be significantly correlated with leader performance. However, regression analyses revealed that only political skill provided significant unique contribution to job performance. The dimensions of behavior that define political skill include: network building, interpersonal influence, social astuteness and apparent sincerity. Similar to the two studies discussed above, these omit the empathy and interpersonal skill branches of EI included in the measure of expressed EI used in this study (Semadar et al., 2006). Summary of research on leaders and managers. Overall, the eleven studies testing the influence of EI (as intelligence) and expressed EI shows a clear pattern of results, which doesn’t differ much between the two categories of EI theory. The pattern suggests that EI affects leader performance in two ways – through relationships with employees that increase an employee’s ability to manage herself and her situation well – in essence bringing out the best in employees. Also, through enabling the leader to perceive and manage his own emotions in the service of influencing others and achieving goals. Transformational leadership theory may help clarify how EI enables a leader to bring out the best in employees (Ashkanasy & Tse, 2000; Barbuto & Burbach, 2006). Brown and Msohavi (2005) have proposed that EI may be the “X-factor” that advances and reinvigorates a more complete understanding of leader social influence and may be the key antecedent to transformational leadership (p.869), which involves inspiring employees to grow and achieve more than they thought was possible. Transformational leaders achieve this by engaging followers in four ways: idealized influence (i.e., they exhibit charisma that induces identification with the leader and her vision), inspirational motivation (i.e., inspiring the Emotional intelligence and performance 23 highest levels of motivation), intellectual stimulation (i.e., recognizing how to challenge individuals in order to stretch and develop their capabilities), and individualized consideration (i.e., understanding the unique qualities of each individual) – all of which have consistently been linked positively to leader performance (Lowe, Kroek, & Sivasubramaniam, 1996). Several researchers have tested these hypotheses. One study, using the Bar-On measure found that expressed EI accounted for 34% of the variance in transformational leadership in construction executives (Butler & Chinowsky, 2006). The dimensions of expressed EI that best predicted transformational leadership were interpersonal skills and empathy. Similar results were found in a second study examining expressed EI and transformational leadership with 80 elected public officials in the U. S. (Barbuto & Burbach, 2004). This study found that the expressed EI dimensions of empathic response and interpersonal skills were significantly correlated to staff member ratings of their leaders’ demonstration of transformational leadership behaviors. Finally, a third study examined expressed EI and transformational leadership with 63 managers of a large U. S. -based information services and technology firm (Sosik & Mergerian, 1999). Here it was found that when leaders scored high on self-awareness, their expressed EI (especially the dimensions of interpersonal control and personal efficacy) were significantly related to all four dimensions of transformational leadership behavior. These authors proposed that self-awareness increases a leader’s ability to translate EI abilities into transformational leadership. Overall, these three studies support the idea that the interpersonal dimensions of EI may link to leadership performance because they help develop and transform employees and bring out their highest levels of motivation and skills. Work Team Contexts Emotional intelligence and performance 24 The studies in our two previous categories of individual contributors and managers and leaders consistently showed that EI predicts performance, thus we expect similar results from research conducted in team contexts. In this section, the work conducted in teams requires team members to interact to complete their work. The length of time these team members must interact varies from one hour to indefinitely. In any team situation, team member interaction can be smooth and easy as seen in cohesive groups or it can be strained and conflict ridden as often seen during particular stages or phases in a group’s development (Gersick, 1988; Tuckman, 1965). It takes emotion to bind a group together and it takes emotion to allow minority dissent to occur (Nemeth, 1994). Indeed, in teams (as elsewhere) every interaction produces emotion (Kemper, 2000) and when team members are stuck together working under tight-deadlines, teams can be hotbeds for emotion (Barsade & Gibson, 1998). It is important to note that positive emotion in teams is not always beneficial for performance outcomes (Janis, 1982) and negative emotions are not always harmful to performance (Jehn & Mannix, 2001). Due to the preponderance and complexity of emotion in team contexts, it makes sense that research on the influence of emotional intelligence could advance group theory. Emotional intelligence at the team level has been conceptualized and studied several ways (see Elfenbein, 2006). The most common method for studying team-level emotional intelligence is to average team member responses on emotional intelligence measures. This produces a team average that is than compared to team performance or other outcomes such as team process effectiveness. Jordan and his colleagues (Jordan et al., 2002) employed a slightly different method for studying team-level emotional intelligence. They developed an expressed emotional intelligence measure customized specifically for the team context. Their measure, the Workgroup Emotional Intelligence Profile (WEIP), is based on Mayer and Salovey’s (1997) Emotional intelligence and performance 25 theory of emotional intelligence. However, team-level emotional intelligence is assessed using items worded for the team context, e.g., When I am frustrated with fellow team members I can overcome my frustration. A third way of conceptualizing team-level emotional intelligence grows out of the idea that teams are greater than the sum of their parts and that team emotional intelligence is better measured by the patterns of interactions among team members than by the skills of individual members (Elfbenbein, 2006). This method is seen in the work of Druskat and Wolff (2001) who propose and measure of a model of emotionally intelligent group norms. They have argued that emotionally intelligent norms sanction and make predictable behaviors that are emotionally intelligent within a team context because they lead to the development of group trust, group identity, group efficacy, and networks with relevant groups and individuals outside of the team. They also argue that emotionally intelligent norms bring about emotionally intelligent behavior without relying on the individual emotional intelligence of every team member (Druskat & Wolff, 2001). As can be seen in Table 3, the fourteen studies we review in this section use all three conceptualizations of team-level emotional intelligence. We have categorized them as either examining emotional intelligence or expressed emotional intelligence in teams. ------------------------------Insert Table 3 about here ---------------------------------Emotional intelligence in team contexts. We located three research articles that examined team emotional intelligence as a form of intelligence. The first study examined team member ability at the emotional intelligence branch of Perceiving Emotions in teammates (Elfenbein, Polzer, Ambady, 2006). Team members in each team worked together for one week prior to data collection on community service projects such as disaster relief, Emotional intelligence and performance 26 assisting local charities, and after-school programs. The purpose of the study was to determine whether some teams, as a whole, were better at perceiving each others’ non-verbal emotions than other teams. The researchers used a measure they developed and labelled Team Emotion Recognition Accuracy (TERA) that involved video-taping team members displaying specific emotions. Later teammates were asked to describe the non-verbal emotion being exhibit. The study results demonstrated that teams were significantly better at rating the emotions of their teammates than non-teammates. Moreover, some teams, as a whole, were significantly better at rating the accuracy of teammates’ emotions than other teams. The teams did not differ significantly in demographics or on the Big Five personality traits (Elfenbein et al., 2006). The findings in this study are relevant for two reasons. First, they demonstrate that the EI branch of perceiving emotion can manifest itself as a team-level variable. That is, it is a team property in the sense that it does not reside only in the individual members. The individuals in the teams that scored higher on perceiving non-verbal emotions were no better at perceiving emotions than members in teams who scored lower at perceiving non-verbal emotions. Instead, members in the higher scoring teams were better at perceiving the nonverbal emotions of the members within their team—suggesting that there was something these teams did that enabled members to perceive each others non-verbal emotions better than other teams. Second, previous research has demonstrated that team’s with higher scores on perceiving non-verbal emotions rate their teams as having less conflict, making decisions more collaboratively, accomplishing more work, and have a more psychologically safe environment than teams with lower scores on perceiving non-verbal emotions (see Elfenbein, 2006). The second study in this category examined teams of undergraduate students who worked together for a short while to complete a cognitive decision-making task that was first Emotional intelligence and performance 27 conducted individually and then as a team (Day & Carroll, 2004). In this study, the EI branch of perceiving emotions at the individual-level was significantly related to individual performance. None of the EI branches when aggregated to the team-level were significantly related to performance. However, all EI branches when aggregated to the team-level were significantly correlated with team sportsmanship behavior and all except team-level perceiving emotions were related team civic virtue. In sum, this study reveals that in shortterm teams completing a cognitive task, team-level EI influences the way team members treat and help one another – but it does not affect team performance (Day & Carroll, 2004). The final study in this category examined teams in a financial services division of a large insurance company (Feyerhem & Rice, 2002). Employees were responsible for processing paperwork and customer service. They worked in large teams of about fifteen members. The team-level EI branches of managing emotions in self and managing emotions in others were both significantly related to team performance. Assessments of the influence of team-leader EI also produced interesting results. Team leader EI was not significantly related to team performance. However, team leader score on the EI branch of understanding emotions was significantly and positively related to team customer service, but significantly negatively related to team ratings of accuracy, productivity and commitment to continuous improvement. In other words, the stronger the team leader’s ability to understand emotions and their outcomes, the better teams were at customer service but the worse they were at their more cognitive responsibilities. Due to the relevance of EI managing emotions to performance, the team’s work clearly required members to manage their emotions. This may have been particularly necessary when employees were interacting with customers. Thus, team leaders who understood the difficulty of the situation may have helped customers work more effectively with their customers. Recall that in previous studies team leader EI has been beneficial to Emotional intelligence and performance 28 team members in challenging interpersonal contexts (Côté et al., 2006; Sy et al., 2006). We can assume that at least some of the customers calling their insurance company were experiencing difficult circumstances. However, when it was time for team members to focus on their more cognitive tasks that required accuracy and continuous improvement, team leader understanding appears to have detracted from performance. Future research should examine more closely the influence of EI in work roles that involve switching back and forth between intense cognitive decision making tasks and difficult customer interactions. In this section, we have reviewed three very different studies on the role of emotional intelligence in work team contexts. So far, it is clear that emotional intelligence is relevant to team functioning; however, no clear patterns emerge among the data perhaps because of the very different work tasks and work contexts studied. Expressed emotional intelligence in team contexts. We identified eight studies examining expressed emotional intelligence in team contexts (see Table 3; one article contains two studies). Seven of the eight studies demonstrate that expressed emotional intelligence at the team-level is significantly correlated with team performance (Druskat, Wolff, Messer & Stubbs, 2007; Foo, Elfenbein, Tan, & Aik, 2005; Frye, Bennett, & Caldwell, 2006; Jordan, Ashkanasy, Hartel, & Hooper, 2002; Jordan & Troth, 2004; and Offermann, et al., 2004). Two of these studies demonstrated these results while controlling for Big Five personality traits and cognitive intelligence (Foo et al., 2005; Offermann et al., 2004). The first two studies in this category by Druskat and colleagues (Druskat et al., 2007), examines expressed emotional intelligence as a set of team-level norms. The first study examined these norms in graduate student teams working together for a full year. The second examined the same team-level emotional intelligence norms in teams working in six companies that included engineering teams, bank-teller teams, financial service teams, and Emotional intelligence and performance 29 manufacturing teams. In both studies, results showed significant correlations between team performance and norms of: Interpersonal understanding, proactive problem solving, organizational understanding and building external relationships. The second study in this category was conducted with self-managing work teams in a distributor of promotional products (Frye et al., 2006). Here it was found that team taskorientation was significantly correlated with the expressed team-level dimensions of interpersonal EI and adaptability EI. Team maintenance orientation, that is the team’s willingness to take care of member and relationship needs, was significantly correlated with the expressed team-level dimension of general mood EI (Frye et al., 2006). The third study examined team-level emotional intelligence in dyads working on a negotiation task (Foo et al, 2005). Here it was found that higher total emotional intelligence in a dyad produced a better joint negotiation outcome. Also, the dyad member with the highest EI score rated herself as having the most positive experience in the negotiation exercise. However, despite the fact that dyads with higher joint EI produced better outcomes, the member with the highest level of EI in the dyad significantly more often did not benefit from the outcome because he allowed his counterpart to benefit from it. This finding suggests two opposing hypotheses. Either the member with the highest level of EI demonstrated too much empathy, or that member was investing in a long-term relationship, which can be an effective strategy in negotiations. In the fourth study, Jordan and Troth (2004) studied individual and team performance. They found that emotional intelligence was not predictive of individual performance in a cognitive decision making task. When the same task was completed by teams, not only were the teams with higher expressed team-level emotional intelligence more successful, but their performance on the task was predicted by their team-level emotional intelligence score. This finding raises an interesting point about the nature of task performance in teams that is Emotional intelligence and performance 30 relevant for all the studies we have reviewed that have been conducted in team contexts. Although a task may be cognitive in nature, when it is completed in a team, emotion emerges as a factor determining performance. In other words the group member’s emotions need to be understood and managed to enable maximum group performance. The result of effective emotion management is seen in the team’s scoring higher on team-level emotion intelligence in this study – they were significantly more likely to use collaboration as a conflict management style, which is well known to be an effective conflict management style in teams (Jordan & Troth, 2004). Offermann and her colleagues (Offermann et al., 2004) found a similar outcome in their study of expressed emotional intelligence and individual and team performance. Their study showed that cognitive ability predicted individual performance while team-level expressed emotional intelligence predicted team performance and positive team attitudes. They also examined leader emergence and found that the expressed emotional intelligence dimensions of self-awareness and relationship management were most strongly correlated with both team leader emergence and effectiveness. Jordan and colleagues (Jordan et al., 2002), examined the links between expressed team-level emotional intelligence and team performance in a longitudinal study. Performance in this study was measured by independent raters’ scores on two variables: (1) team process effectiveness and (2) team goal focus during team meetings. Jordan and his colleagues found a performance difference in the first 3 weeks of their study with high average emotional intelligence teams outperforming teams with low average emotional intelligence. In an interesting twist, after the first three weeks the performance difference eroded to the point where there was no performance difference between the two groups after 10 weeks. Jordan et al. (2002) speculate that teams compensated for lower emotional intelligence through team development processes. Emotional intelligence and performance 31 Finally, Rapisarda (2002) examined learning teams in an executive-level management program that worked together for the full two years of their program. She found that teamlevel expressed EI was more strongly related to team cohesiveness than to team performance. The dimensions of expressed team-level EI that were most strongly and consistently (with team member and faculty raters) correlated with positive team outcomes cohesiveness included empathy and achievement orientation. Summary of research on teams, dyads, and their members. The studies in this section reveal several interesting patterns. First, three of the studies found that dimensions of expressed team-level emotional intelligence associated with good team member relationships were significantly correlated with performance (Druskat, et al., 2007, Frye et al, 2006; Rapisarda, 2002). The Offermann study (Offermann et al., 2004) also, once again, demonstrated the relevance of team leader’s relationship management EI and self-awareness EI being supportive of team performance. Another pattern in this set of studies is the consistent finding that team’s and team members with higher levels of expressed EI have better attitudes (Offermann et al., 2004), better maintenance of their teams needs (Frye et al, 2006) and more fun (Foo et al., 2005) in these intensely interpersonal situations. Finally, it should be noted that a three of two of these studies found positive relationships between team-level emotional intelligence and performance while controlling for Big Five personality traits and cognitive intelligence. Emotional Intelligence at the Organizational-Level Research has revealed that emotional intelligence and expressed emotional intelligence exists at both the individual- and team-levels. However, there is some evidence that it can also exist at the organizational level (see Ashkanasy 2003, Huy, 1999; Martin et al., 1998), that is in an organization’s culture. Huy (1999) labels organizational emotional intelligence “emotional capability,” defined as an organization’s ability to acknowledge, Emotional intelligence and performance 32 recognize, monitor, discriminate, and attend to its members’ emotions. Like team-level emotional intelligence, it is manifested in an organization’s norms, routines, and emotional display rules. Indeed, Ashkanasy and his colleagues are finding emerging evidence that a similar construct they label “emotional climate” affects organizational performance (Ashkanasy, 2003; Ashkanasy & Nicholson, 2003). A form of emotional intelligence manifested at the organizational level requires a departure from traditional masculine organizational cultures that discourage expression or discussions of emotions. (see Martin et al., 1998). Indeed, it may be that organizational cultures that encourage employees to think intelligently about emotion may have a positive influence how employees deal with everything from day to day stresses of customers complaints to more substantial organizational change (see Huy, 1999) and finally to crises that arise in organizations such as major recalls of defective equipment or industrial sabotage and international terrorist events. Further theory and research is clearly required in this area. Ideas for Future Research and Theory on Emotional Intelligence in the Workplace We noted at the beginning of this article that we would conclude by providing concrete suggestions about the kind of research that can advance theory and the practical use of emotional intelligence concepts in the workplace. It is important to recognize that research and theory emotional intelligence and work performance is in relative infancy when compared to the more established research streams in personality and cognitive intelligence. Yet as discussed throughout this article, there are some broad questions that when answered can provide a broader foundation for understanding the influence of emotional intelligence on work performance. Multilevel Research There is a significant amount of research that needs to be done to examine the existence and manifestation of emotional intelligence to and the five levels of analysis Emotional intelligence and performance 33 suggested by8 Ashkanasy (2003). We have already noted some research linking emotional intelligence to between person level (individual) and group level behavior, however, there is still more research to be done regarding the within person level and at the organizational level. Ashkanasy’s (2003) multi-level model of emotions in organizations provides an insight into the pervasive nature of emotions in organizations. If emotions are pervasive in organizations, then emotional intelligence may have an impact on how these emotions emerge in the workplace at all levels. Emotional intelligence research needs to be expanded to examine its impact at varying levels of organizations. Curvilinear Relationships and Interactions At this stage, particularly in business, there is an assumption that emotional intelligence is a linear construct and that more is better. A question arises, however, as to whether too much emotional intelligence can be detrimental to performance. For instance, as discussed earlier, Foo, Elfenbein, Tan, & Aik (2005) found that individuals with high emotional intelligence actually ended up with lower performance in a negotiation simulation because they were more likely to concede ground to achieve an integrative solution. Clearly, there are cases where emotional intelligence may lead to performance losses, however as discussed by the authors, sometimes losses due to actions taken to build solid relationships can turn into performance gains in the long-run. If we look at the major abilities of emotional intelligence the question becomes when might “too much” be a detriment. If we have too much emotional control then we border on alexithymia and this clearly will have detrimental effects on our ability to work with and relate to other people (Lane, Sechrest, Reidel, Weldon, Kaszniak, & Schwartz, 1996). The other end of this continuum, however, may be equally debilitating. If we have too much emotional awareness we might adopt an oversensitive approach to life and work and become distressed over minor issues that dominate our lives and influence our relationships. Ideally, Emotional intelligence and performance 34 emotional intelligence enables us to think intelligently about emotion and act in ways that marshal emotions in the service of goals. Finding the best balance between awareness and control for each of our individual lives may take a life-time. It also is interesting to speculate which measure of EI would best determine whether a person balances the branches of emotional intelligence well – a test of EI as intelligence, or a test of expressed EI rated by self or others. Theorists and researchers could examine the concept of too much or too little emotional intelligence in a number of ways. The first would be to acknowledge that individual emotional intelligence abilities may have a curvilinear relationship with performance, rather than the linear relationships that have been proposed and tested. The other, as discussed above, is to consider the individual factors of emotional intelligence to be interrelated. It may be that high levels of emotional awareness or sensitivity are beneficial as long as they are combined with high levels of emotional regulation. The best leaders may be highly attuned to their employees’ emotions, but still resist the impulse to intervene when an employee encounters a challenge because it might not produce the best long-term outcome or the intellectual stimulation so vital to successful transformational leadership (see Bass & Avolio, 1994). In sum, future research should test for curvilinear relationships and interactions between the branches of emotional intelligence. Total Emotional Intelligence Versus Branches of Emotional Intelligence A current problem with research on emotional intelligence is the propensity for researchers to treating emotional intelligence as one-dimensional construct (i.e., Total EI) when all theorists describe emotional intelligence as a multidimensional construct. When examining personality, it makes little sense to give an overall single digit personality score combining extraversion, neuroticism, openness to experience, conscientiousness, and agreeableness. This diminishes the explanatory power of the construct. The explanatory Emotional intelligence and performance 35 power of emotional intelligence is also diminished by not examining the different branches of emotional intelligence. As our review has demonstrated, we know have enough evidence to acknowledge that total scores of emotional intelligence and expressed emotional intelligence predict emotional intelligence in some contexts. The emotional intelligence construct has now reached what Weick (1989) refers to as a more advanced stage of construct development. At this stage, relative comprehensive and believable relationships have been developed. Future theory and research must now focus on deeper explanations of those relationships that can be best obtained by examining emotional intelligence as a multi-dimensional construct. The Importance of Context Two often theory and research in organizational studies ignores the role of context (Avolio, 2007). In fact, different types of jobs and different industries require different skills and have different makers for job success. Some jobs require skills at the individual level and some at the group level, while others such as leadership may require a range of skills from the within person to the organizational level. To draw out these relationships, emotional intelligence research in business needs to focus on the use of multifaceted measures of emotional intelligence, at different levels of analysis, to increase our understanding of this construct. Conclusion We opened this article by outlining the vigorous debate around which emotional intelligence has persisted since Daniel Goleman first highlighted its potential for business (Goleman, 1995). We proposed that researchers have, perhaps inadvertently, created two streams of research – one focused on examining emotional intelligence as a legitimate form of intelligence. The second focused on defining the behaviors and skills that grow out of power of that intelligence. The latter group has more actively studied the influence of emotional intelligence on workplace performance. However, despite using very different Emotional intelligence and performance 36 measures for the two constructs, both categories of researcher have clearly and consistently demonstrated a strong positive association between emotional intelligence and work performance in some contexts. New theory is needed to take this research to the next level of determining in exactly which job contexts the different branches of emotional intelligence matter most, how much emotional intelligence is enough (see Boyatzis, 2006), and exactly how do the branches of emotional intelligence work together to provide performance advantages. We look forward to the next wave of emotional intelligence theory and research. Emotional intelligence and performance 37 References Amabile, T. M., Barsade, S. G., Mueller, J., & Staw, B. M. (2005). Affect and creativity at work. Administrative Science Quarterly, 50, 367-403. Ashkanasy, N. M. (2003). Emotions in organizations: A multilevel perspective. In F. Dansereau and F. J. Yammarino (Eds.), Research in multi-level issues, vol. 2: Multi-level issues in organizational behavior and strategy (pp. 9-54). Oxford, UK: Elsevier Science. Ashkanasy, N. M., & Nicholson, G. J. (2003). Climate of fear in organizational settings: Construct definition, measurement, and a test of theory. Australian Journal of Psychology, 55, 24-29. Ashkanasy, N. M., & Tse, B. (2000). Transformational leadership as management of emotion: A conceptual review. In N. M. Ashkanasy, C. E. J. Hartel & W. J. Zerbe (Eds.), Emotions in the workplace (pp. 221-235). Westport, CT: Quorum Books. Ashkanasy, N. M., Zerbe, W.J., & Härtel, C. E. J. (Eds.). (2005). Research on emotion in organizations, Volume 1: Affect and its effects in organizational settings. Oxford, UK: Elsevier Science. Avolio, B. J. (2007). Promoting more integrative strategies for leadership theorybuilding. American Psychologist, 62(1), 25-33. Bachman, J., Stein, S., Campbell, K., & Sitarenios, G. (2000). Emotional intelligence in the collection of debt. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 8(3), 176-182. Barbuto, J. E., & Burbach, M. E. (2006). The emotional intelligence of transformational leaders: A field study of elected officials. The Journal of Social Psychology, 146(1), 51-64. Bar-On, R. (1997). Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory: A measure of Emotional Intelligence. Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems, Inc. Emotional intelligence and performance 38 Bar-On, R., Handley, R., & Fund, S. (2006). The impact of emotional intelligence on performance. In V. U. Druskat, F. Sala & G. Mount (Eds.), Linking emotional intelligence and performance at work: Current research evidence with individuals and groups (pp. 3-19). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Barker, V. E., Abrams, J. R., Tiyaamornwong, V., Seibold, D. R., Duggan, A., Park, H. S., et al. (2000). New contexts for relational communication in groups. Small Group Research, 31(4), 470-503. Barsade, S. G., Brief, A. P., & Spataro, S. E. (2003). The affective revolution in organizational behavior: The emergence of a paradigm. In G. Greenberg (Ed.), OB: The state of the science (2nd. ed., pp. 3-52). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Barsade, S. G., & Gibson, D. E. (1998). Group emotion: A view from top and bottom. In D. Gruenfeld (Ed.), Composition (Vol. 1, pp. 81-102). Stamford, CT: JAI Press. Bass, B. M., & Avolio, B. J. (1994). Improving organizational effectiveness: Through transformational leadership. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Bono, J. E. (2006). Charisma, positive emotions and mood contagion. Leadership Quarterly, 17(4), 317-334. Boyatzis, R. E. (1998). Transforming qualitative information: Thematic analysis and code development. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Boyatzis, R. E., Goleman, D., & Rhee, K. S. (2000). Clustering competencies in emotional intelligence. In R. Bar-On & J. D. Parker (Eds.), The handbook of emotional intelligence (pp. 343-362). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Boyatzis, R. E., Stubbs, E., & Taylor, S. (2002). Learning cognitive and emotional intelligence competencies through graduate management education. Academy of Management Journal on Learning and Education, 1, 150-162. Emotional intelligence and performance 39 Brown, F. W., & Moshavi, D. (2005). Transformational leadership and emotional intelligence: A potential pathway for an increased understanding of interpersonal influence. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 867-871. Butler, C. J., & Chinowsky, P. S. (2006). Emotional intelligence and leadership behavior in construction executives. Journal of Management in Engineering, 22(3), 119-125. Carmeli, A. (2003). The relationship between emotional intelligence and work attitudes, behavior and outcomes: An examination among senior managers. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 18, 788-813. Caruso, D. R. & Salovey, P. (2004). The Emotionally Intelligent Manager: How to Develop and Use the Four Key Emotional Skills of Leadership. San Francisco: Jossey Bass. Chia, Y. M. (2005). Job offers of multi-national accounting firms: The effects of emotional intelligence, extra-curricular activities, and academic performance. Accounting Education: An International Journal, 14(1), 75-93. Ciarrochi, J., & Godsell, C. (2006). Mindfulness-based emotional intelligence: Research and training. In V. U. Druskat, F. Sala & G. Mount (Eds.), Linking emotional intelligence and performance at work: Current research evidence with individuals and groups (pp. 21-52). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Côté, S., Lopes, P. N., & Salovey, P. (2006). Emotional intelligence predicts leadership emergence in emotionally challenging groups, Annual Conference for the Society of Industrial and Organizational Psychology. Dallas, TX. Côté, S., & Miners, C. T. H. (2006). Emotional intelligence, cognitive intelligence, and job performance. Administrative Science Quarterly, 51, 1-28. Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. New York: Avon. Emotional intelligence and performance 40 Dasborough, M. T. (2006). Cognitive asymmetry in employee emotional reactions to leadership behaviors. Leadership Quarterly, 17(2), 163-178. Daus, C. S., & Ashkanasy, N. M. (2005). The case for the ability based model of emotional intelligence in organizational behavior. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 453-466. Day, A. L., & Carroll, S. A. (2004). Using an ability-based measure of emotional intelligence to predict individual performance, group performance, and group citizenship behaviors. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(6), 1443-1458. Druskat, V. U., & Wolff, S. B. (2001). Group emotional competence and its influence on group effectiveness. In C. Cherniss & D. Goleman (Eds.), Emotional competence in organizations (pp. 132-155). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Druskat, V. U., Wolff, S. B., Messer, T. E., & Stubbs, E. (2007). Emotionally competent group norms and group effectiveness. Manuscript submitted for publication. Dulewicz, V., Higgs, M., & Slaski, M. (2003). Measuring emotional intelligence: Construct and criterion-related validity. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 18(5), 405-420. Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). Predicting workplace outcomes from the ability to eavesdrop on feelings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(5), 963-971. Elfenbein, H. A., Polzer, J. T., & Ambady, N. (2006). Can teams have emotional skills? The case of emotion recognition. Manuscript submitted for publication. Elfenbein, H. A. (2006). Team emotional intelligence: What it can mean and how it can affect performance. In V. U. Druskat, F. Sala & G. Mount (Eds.), Linking emotional intelligence and performance at work: Current research evidence with individuals and groups, (pp. 165-184). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Ekman, P. (1992). Facial expressions of emotion: New findings, new questions. Psychological Science, 33, 4-38. Emotional intelligence and performance 41 Feyerhem, A. E., & Rice, C. L. (2002). Emotional intelligence and team performance: The good, the bad and the ugly. The International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 10(4), 343-362. Fineman, S. (2004). Getting the measure of emotion -- and the cautionary tale of emotional intelligence. Human Relations, 57(6), 719-740. Foo, M. D., Elfenbein, H., Tan, H. H., & Aik, V. C. (2005). Emotional intelligence and negotiation: The tension between creating and claiming value. International Journal of Conflict Management, 15(4), 411 – 429. Frye, C. M., Bennett, R., & Caldwell, S. (2006). Team emotional intelligence and team interpersonal process effectiveness. Mid-American Journal of Business, 21(1), 49-56. Gersick, C. G. (1988). Time and transition in work teams: Toward a new model of group development. Academy of Management Journal, 31, 9-41. Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence: Why it can matter more than IQ. New York: Bantam. Goleman, D. (1998). Working with emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books. Hill, L. A. (1992). Becoming a manager: How new managers master the challenges of leadership. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Business School Press. Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The managed heart: Commercialization of human feeling. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Holmer, L. L. (1994). Developing emotional capacity and organizational health. In R. H. Kilmann, I. Kilmann & Associates (Eds.), Managing ego energy: The transformation of personal meaning into organizational success (pp. 49-72). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Homans, G. (1950). The human group. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Emotional intelligence and performance 42 Humphrey, R. H. (2002). The many faces of emotional leadership. Leadership Quarterly, 13: 493-504 Huy, Q. N. (1999). Emotional capability, emotional intelligence, and radical change. Academy of Management Review, 24(2), 325-345. Gersick, C. G. (1988). Time and transition in work teams: Toward a new model of group development. Academy of Management Journal, 31, 9-41. Jordan, P. J., Ashkanasy, N. M., Hartel, C. E. J., & Hooper, G. S. (2002). Workgroup emotional intelligence: Scale development and relationship to team process effectiveness and goal focus. Human Resource Management Review, 12, 195-214. Jordan, P.J., & Troth, A.C. (2004). Managing emotions during team problem solving: Emotional intelligence and conflict resolution. Human Performance, 17(2), 195-218. Kemper, T. D. (2000). Social models in the explanation of emotions. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 45-58). New York: Guildford Press. Landy, F. J. (2005). Some historical and scientific issues related to research on emotional intelligence. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 411-424. Lam, L.T. & Kirby, S.L. (2002). Is emotional intelligence an advantage? An exploration of the impact of emotional and general intelligence on individual performance. Journal of Social Psychology, 142, 133-43. Lane, R.D., Sechrest, L., Reidel, R., Weldon, V., Kaszniak, A., & Schwartz G.E. (1996). Impaired verbal and nonverbal emotion recognition in alexithymia. Psychosomatic Medicine, 58: 203-210. Law, K. S., Wong, C. S., & Song, L. J. (2004). The construct and criterion validity of emotional intelligence and its potential utility for management studies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(3), 483-496. Emotional intelligence and performance 43 Locke, E. A. (2005). Why emotional intelligence is an invalid concept. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 425-431. Lombardo, M. M., Ruderman, M. N., & McCauley, C. D. (1988). Explanations of success and derailment in upper-level management positions. Journal of Business and Psychology, 2(3), 199-216. Lopes, P. N., Grewal, D., Kadis, J., Gall, M., & Salovey, P. (2006). Evidence that emotional intelligence is related to job performance and affect and attitudes at work. Psicothema, 18, 132-138. Lord, R. G., & Kanfer, R. (2002). Emotions and organizational behavior. In R. G. Lord, R. J. Klimoski & R. Kanfer (Eds.), Emotions in the workplace (pp. 5-19). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Lowe, K. B., Kroek, K. G., & Sivasubramaniam, N. (1996). Transformational leadership: A meta-analytic review of the MLQ literature. Leadership Quarterly, 7, 385-425. Martin, J., Knopoff, K., & Beckman, C. (1998). An alternative to bureaucratic impersonality and emotional labor: Bounded emotionality at the body shop. Administrative Science Quarterly, 43, 429-469. Matthews, G., Zeidner, M., & Roberts, R. D. (2002). Emotional intelligence: Science and myth. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Mayer, J. D, & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P. Salovey & D. Sluyter (Eds.). Emotional development and emotional intelligence: Implications for educators (pp. 3-31). New York: Basic Books. Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2002). Mayer-Salovey-Caruso emotional intelligence test (MSCEIT) user's manual. Toronto, Canada: MHS Publishers. Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2004). Emotional intelligence: Theory, findings, and implications. Psychological Inquiry, 15(3), 197-215. Emotional intelligence and performance 44 McClelland, D. C. (1973). Testing for competence rather than for "intelligence". American Psychologist, 28(1), 1-14. Murphy, K. R. (Ed.) (2006). A Critique of emotional intelligence: What are the problems and how can they be fixed? Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Nemeth, C. J. (1994). The value of minority dissent. In A. Mucchi-Faina, A. Maass & S. Moscovici (Eds.), Minority influence (pp. 3-31). Chicago: Nelson Hall. Nikolaou, I., & Tsaousis, I. (2002). Emotional intelligence in the workplace: Exploring its effects on occupational stress and organizational commitment. The International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 10(4), 327-342. Oatley, K. (2004). Emotional intelligence and the intelligence of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 15(3), 216-238. Offermann, L. R., Bailey, J. R., Vasilopoulos, N. L., Seal, C., & Sass, M. (2004). The relative contribution of emotional competence and cognitive ability to individual and team performance. Human Performance, 17(2), 219-243. Palmer, B., & Stough, C. (2001). Workplace SUEIT: Interim technical manual. Melbourne, Australia: Swinburne University of Technology. Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2004). Technical manual of the trait emotional intellience questionnaire (TEIQue). London: University of London, Institute of Education. Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2006). The role of trait emotional intelligence in a gender-specific model of organizational variables. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36(2), 552-569. Petrides, K. V., Furnham, A., & Martin, N. G. (2004). Estimates of emotional and psychometric intelligence: Evidence for gender-based stereotypes. Journal of Social Psychology, 144, 149-162. Emotional intelligence and performance 45 Petrides, K. V., Niven, L., & Mouskounti, T. (2006). The trait emotional intelligence of ballet dancers and musicians. Psicothema, 18, 101-107. Rapisarda, B. A. (2002). The impact of emotional intelligence on work team cohesiveness and performance. The International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 10(4), 363-379. Rosete, D., & Ciarrochi, J. (2007). A critical evaluation of the role of ability based emotional intelligence in leadership effectiveness. Manuscript submitted for publication. Rosete, D., & Ciarrochi, J. (2005). Emotional intelligence and its relationship to workplace performance outcomes of leadership effectiveness. Leadership & Organization Development Journal, 26(5), 388-399. Roethlisberger, F. J., & Dickson, W. J. (1939). Management and the worker. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Rozell, E. J., Pettijohn, C. E., & Parker, R. S. (2006). Emotional intelligence and dispositional affectivity as predictors of performance in salespeople. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 14(2), 113-124. Sala, F. (2002). Emotional competence inventory: Technical manual. Boston: The Hay Group. Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 9, 185-211. Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggarty, D. J., Cooper, J. T., Golden, C. J., & Dornheim, L. (1998). Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence. Personality & Individual Differences, 25(2), 167-177. Schutte, N.S., Schuettpelz, E. & Malouff, J.M. (2000). Emotional intelligence and task performance. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 20, 347-354. Emotional intelligence and performance 46 Semadar, A., Robins, G., & Ferris, G. R. (2006). Comparing the validity of multiple social effectiveness constructs in the prediction of managerial job performance. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27, 443-461. Sivanathan, N., & Fekken, G. C. (2002). Emotional intelligence, moral reasoning and transformational leadership. Leadership & Organization Development Journal, 23(3/4), 198205. Slaski, M., & Cartwright, S. (2002). Health, performance and emotional intelligence: An exploratory study of retail managers. Stress and Health, 18, 63-68. Snyder, M., & Ickes, W. (1985). Personality and social behavior. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (3rd ed., Vol. II, pp. 883-947). New York: Random House. Sosik, J. J., & Megerian, L. E. (1999). Understanding leader emotional intelligence and performance: The role of self-other agreement on transformational leadership perceptions. Group and Organization Management, 24(3), 376-390. Sy, T., Tram, S., & O'Hara, L. A. (2006). Relation of employee and manager emotional intelligence to job satisfaction and performance. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 68, 461-473. Sy, T., Côté, S., & Saavedra, R. (2005). The contagious leader: Impact of the leader's mood on the mood of group members, group affective tone, and group processes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(2), 295-305. Tuckman, B. (1965). Developing sequences in small groups. Psychological Bulletin, 63, 384-399. Van Rooy, D. L., Alsonso, A., & Viswesvaran, C. (2005). Group differences in emotional intelligence scores: Theoretical and practical implications. Personality & Individual Differences, 38(3), 689-700. Emotional intelligence and performance 47 Van Rooy, D. L., & Viswesvaran, C. (2004). Emotional intelligence: A meta-analytic investigation of predictive validity and nomological net. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 65(1), 71-95. Van Rooy, D. L., Viswesvaran, C., & Pluta, P. (2005). An evaluation of construct validity: What is this thing called emotional intelligence? Human Performance, 18(4), 445462. Weick, K. (1989). Theory construction as disciplined imagination. Academy of Management Review, 14, 516-531. Wong, C. S., & Law, K. S. (2002). The Effect of leader and follower emotional intelligence on performance and attitude: An exploratory study. Leadership Quarterly, 13, 243-274. Yukl, G. A. (1997). Leadership in organizations (4th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Emotional intelligence and performance 48 Emotional intelligence and performance 49 Table 1 Peer Reviewed Journal Studies Examining Emotional Intelligence and Expressed Emotional Intelligence in Individual Roles Emotional Intelligence in Individual Roles Author(s) Sample EI Measure Dependent Variables & Controls Key Results 1 Côté & Miners, 2006 175 full-time employees of a large public university MSCEIT V2.0 (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2002) Dependent variable: (1) Supervisor performance ratings, (2) organizational citizenship behavior at organizational (OCBO) and individual (OCBI) Controls: (1) Cognitive intelligence using the Culture Fair Intelligence Test, (2) Big Five Personality (3) LMX (relationship with supervisor), (4) education level, (5) hours worked per week, (6) and occupation. High Total EI compensates for low cognitive intelligence (in other words EI becomes a stronger predictor of task performance the lower one’s cognitive intelligence) High Total EI compensates for low OCBO No evidence that Total EI and cognitive intelligence predict job performance differently in jobs with different emotional demands. 2 Lopes, Grewal, Kadis, Gall, & Salovey, 2006 44 analysts and clerical employees from the finance department of a Fortune 400 insurance company MSCEIT V2.0 (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2002) Dependent variables: (1) Work performance measures: (a) salary, (b) percent merit increase, (c) company rank, (2) Interpersonal facilitation (rated by peers and supervisors), (3) Interpersonal sensitivity and sociability (rated by peers and supervisors), (4) Contribution to a positive work environment (rated by peers and supervisors), (5) Affect and attitudes at work measured using scales of job, mood, stress tolerance (rated by peers and supervisors) Controls: (1) Verbal ability (2) Big Five personality (3) trait affect using Positive and negative affect scales, (4) level of education Total EI: related to company rank (p < .01) and % merit increase (p < .05), but not salary. EI--Perceiving Emotion not sig. related to performance. EI--Using Emotion: related to company rank (p < .05). EI--Understanding Emotion: related to company rank (p < .05) and % merit increase (p < .05). EI--Managing Emotion: related to company rank (p < .01), and marginally to % merit increase (p < .10) Total EI remained sig. related to % merit increase after controlling personality traits Total EI remained sig. related to company rank after controlling for level of education and trait-positive affect (which were both sig. correlated with rank). Total EI sig. related to peer-rated interpersonal facilitation and peer-rated sociability. (after controlling for level of education Total EI remained sig. related to supervisor rated sociability, -trait negative affect, -liking, contribution to a positive work environment, -trait negative affect, and -stress-tolerance) Emotional intelligence and performance Author(s) Sample 50 EI Measure Dependent Variables & Controls Key Results 3 Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002 69 full-time employees in a year-long public service program. The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy (DANVA) test (Nowicki, 2000) Dependent variables: (1) Senior staff rating of employee effectiveness (success and perceived fit with the organization), (2) peer ratings of effectiveness (success and perceived fit with the organization). Controls: Gender, age, ethic group membership Accuracy in recognizing emotions through non-verbal means predicted ratings of effectiveness (for staff ratings more than peer ratings). Accuracy in eavesdropping on emotions (scored as extent to which employees could extract nonverbal information more accurately through vocal tones than facial expressions) predicted effectiveness for employees who were better at recognizing positive emotions and was a negative predictor of performance for employees who were better at recognizing negative emotions. 4 Lam & Kirby, 2002 304 students at a university in the Western U.S. MEIS (Mayer, Salovey & Caruso, 1997) Dependent variable: Cognitive performance on eight problems selected from the Burney (1974) logical reasoning test taken under stressful circumstances. Controls: General intelligence using the Shipley Institute of Living IQ Scale All findings occur after controlling for general intelligence. Regressions showed Total EI contributed to individual cognitive-based performance (F = 11.37, p < .001), Perceiving Emotions contributed to individual cognitive-based performance (F = 23.24; p < .001), Regulating Emotions contributed to individual cognitive-based performance (F = 7.59; p < .01) Understanding Emotions did not contribute to individual cognitive-based performance over and above general intelligence. Dependent variables: (1) Occupational Achievement (based on salary and level in organizational hierarchy), (2) Job Control, (3) Job Stress, (4) Overall Job Satisfaction, and (5) Organizational Commitment All results reported separately for men and women. For men: Total EI sig. correlated with job control (p < .05), job stress (p < .05), and job satisfaction (p < .01). For women: Total EI sig. correlated with occupational achievement (p < .01), job control (p < .01), job satisfaction (p < .05), and job commitment (p < .01). Total EI was related to lower levels of stress and higher levels of perceived control, satisfaction, and commitment. Expressed Emotional Intelligence in Individual Roles 1 Petrides & 167 working adults Trait Emotional Furnham, 2006 Intelligence Questionnaire-Short Form (TEIQue-SF); (Petrides & Furnham, 2004) Emotional intelligence and performance 51 Author(s) Petrides, Niven & Mouskounti, 2006 Sample 34 ballet students at the English National Ballet School. EI Measure Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire(TEIQue) (Petrides & Furnham, 2004) Dependent Variables & Controls Dependent variables: Dancing Quality rated by 5 teachers using Ballet Dancing Ability Rating Scale. Controls: Big Five Personality Traits Key Results Quality sig. correlated with Total EI (p < .05) and EI dimensions of Self Motivation (p < .01), Emotional Expression (p < .05), Adaptability (p < .05), and Happiness (p < .05). Quality sig. correlated with Big Five Agreeableness (p < .01), and Openness to Experience (p < .05). 3 Rozell, Pettijohn & Parker, 2006 103 salespeople employed by a U.S. nationwide medical devices company Schutte emotional intelligence scale (Schutte et al., 1998) Dependent variables: (1) Selfreported sales performance based on objectives met and comparisons to peers.(2) positive and negative affect via PANAS scale Highest performers scored sig. higher on Total EI than lowest performers (p < .05). Performance levels were also sig. related to EI dimensions of Emotional Awareness (p < .05) and Emotional Self/Other control (p < .02), but not External Emotional Control. Performance was not sig. related to PANAS 4 Chia, 2005 91 Ethnic Chinese Accounting Majors at a university in the Asia-Pacific Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-i) (Bar-On, 1997) Dependent variable: Number of job offers in “Big 5 Certified Public Accounting Firms” Controls: (1) Academic Performance i.e., grades, (2) Extra-curricular activities, (3) Number of initial job interviews, and (4) Number of 2nd round job interviews. Academic performance and extra curricular activities were most predictive of obtaining initial interviews. EI had no relationship to initial interviews. Total EI was most predictive of obtaining job offers, (t=6.61, p < .001), followed by 2 nd round job interviews (t=4.46, p < .001), and initial job interviews (t=2.54, p < .01). EI was more strongly correlated to final job offers (r = .57, p <.01), than academic performance (r = .32, p <.01), and extra-curricular activities (r = .43, p <.01). 5 Law, Wong, & Song, 2004 165 cigarette factory workers in the Anhui province of China Wong and Law Emotional Intelligence Scale (WLEIS) rated by self and by peers Dependent variables: (1) Task performance: supervisory assessment, and (2) contextual performance: interpersonal facilitation and job dedication Controls: (1) Big Five Personality Inventory, (2) demographics, (3) measures of loyalty to supervisor, (4) and trust in supervisor. Peer rating of EI predicted task performance (p < .01), interpersonal facilitation (p < .01), and job dedication (p < .01) after controlling for demographics, loyalty to and trust in supervisor, and Big Five. Self-rating of EI predicted task performance (p < .05) interpersonal facilitation (p < .01), and job dedication (p < .01) after including the same controls as above. After controlling for relevant variables and the Big Five personality dimensions, peer-rated EI still accounted for more than 10% of the variance in in-role and extra-role performance. 2 Emotional intelligence and performance 5 6 Author(s) Nikolaou & Tsaousis, 2002 Bachman, Stein, Campbell & Sitarenios, 2000 Sample 212 mental health professionals: medical & psychological, paraprofessionals, and administrative staff. 36 Account Officers at a debt collection agency. 52 EI Measure Emotional intelligence Questionnaire (EIQ) (developed by 2nd author). Dependent Variables & Controls Dependent variables: (1) Organizational stress and (2) organizational commitment Key Results Total EI was sig. correlated with lower stress (p < .01) and higher organizational commitment (p < .01). All EI dimensions were also sig. correlated to lower stress and all but one (Perception & Appraisal of Emotion) were sig. correlated to organizational commitment Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-i) (Bar-On, 1997) Dependent variables: Performance broken into two groups (a) “consistently high producers.” High performing group (n = 24), and (b) “consistently low producers” (n = 12). “Consistently high producers” scored higher than the lower producers on: Total EI ( p < .05), and EI dimensions of Intrapersonal (p < .01), Adaptability (p < .05), and General Mood (p < .01) Emotional intelligence and performance 53 Table 2 Peer Reviewed Journal Studies Examining Emotional Intelligence and Expressed Emotional Intelligence in Managerial and Leadership Roles Emotional Intelligence in Managerial and Leadership Roles 1 2 Author(s) Rosete & Ciarrochi, 2007 Sample 122 executives from a large Australian public service company EI Measure MSCEIT V2.0 (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2002) and The Swinburne University Emotional Intelligence Test (SUEIT; Palmer & Stough, 2001) Dependent Variables & Controls Dependent variables: Manager performance ratings on: (a) business outcomes achieved, (b) effective interpersonal behavior Controls: (1) Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire, (2) Reasoning ability measure, and (3) Cognitive intelligence, WASI. Key Results Total EI correlated with “achieved business outcomes (p < .01), effective interpersonal behavior (p <.01). SUEIT EI not sig. correlated with either performance outcome but sig. correlated with Total EI (p <.01). Total EI predicted “business outcomes” after controlling for reasoning ability and personality Total EI predicted “effective interpersonal behavior” after controlling for reasoning ability and personality. EI branches of Perceiving Emotion and Managing Emotion predicted “business outcomes” after controlling for “reasoning” and personality scales. All four branches of EI predict “effective int. behavior” after controlling for reasoning ability and personality. Rosete & Ciarrochi, 2005 41 senior executives in a large Australian public services organization MSCEIT V2.0 (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2002) Dependent variables: (1) Performance rated by supervisor for (a) achievement of last year’s goals, (b) how goals were achieved --did they build effective relationships, and (2) 360 degree assessment of 40 core leadership capabilities (rated by manager and at least 3 employees) Controls: (1) Sixteen Personality Factor, (2) Cognitive ability (WASI). Total EI correlated with rating of effectiveness in “How” goals were achieved (p < .05) Total EI not sig. related to achievement of goals EI Perceiving Emotion is strongest predictor in rating effectiveness of “How” goals were achieved (p < .01; 18% of variance) followed by personality characteristic of “privateness” (p < .05; 10%) Perceiving Emotions sig. predicated “How” goals were achieved over and above personality characteristics and cognitive intelligence. Total EI correlated sig. with leadership capabilities “Cultivates Productive Working Relationships” (p < .05) and “Exemplifies Personal Drive and Integrity” (p < .05). EI branch that correlated with the most leadership capabilities was Understanding Emotion, which correlated with “Shapes Strategic Thinking” (p < .05), “Cultivates Productive Working Rel.” (p < .05), and “Communicates with Influence” (p < .05). Emotional intelligence and performance 3 Author(s) Côté, Lopes, & Salovey, 2006 Sample 138 (40 teams) undergraduate students in an organizational behavior course EI Measure MSCEIT V2.0 (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2002) Expressed Emotional Intelligence in Managerial and Leadership Roles 4 Semadar, 136 leaders in the The Swinburne Robins, & Australian division University Emotional Ferris, 2006 of a global motorIntelligence Test manufacturing (SUEIT; Palmer & company Stough, 2001) 54 Dependent Variables & Controls Dependent variable: Leadership Emergence Controls: Big Five personality traits Key Results Total EI predicted leadership emergence after controlling for Big Five. The positive association between EI and leadership emergence gets stronger the lower group members score on emotional stability – the more unstable the group, the more EI predicts leader emergence Dependent variable: Job performance via annual performance appraisals by senior managers. Controls: (1) self-monitoring, (2) leadership self-efficacy, (3) political skill, (4) gender, (5) seniority. Job performance correlated to EI (p< .01), political skill (p< .01), and leadership self-efficacy (p< .05). Regression analysis showed only political skill provided significant unique contribution to job performance. EI and political skill were correlated r = .71, p< .01. 5 Sy, Tram, & O’Hara, 2006 187 food service workers and 62 food-service managers Wong & Law Emotional Intelligence Survey (WLEIS) (Wong & Law, 2002) Dependent variables: (1) Job performance assessed by managers and (2) job satisfaction. Controls: Big Five Personality Factors (John, Donahue, and Kentle, 1991). Managers Total EI correlated with employee job performance (p< .01) and job satisfaction (p< .01). After controlling for Big Five --Manager’s Total EI correlated more positively with job satisfaction for employees with low EI than for employees with high EI (p < .05), and more positively with job performance for employees with low EI than for employees with high EI at a marginal level of significance (p < .07). Employees with higher Total EI have higher job performance (p< .05) after controlling for Big Five. Employees with higher Total EI have higher job satisfaction (p< .001) after controlling for Big Five. 6 Carmeli, 2003 98 senior level managers in local Israeli government Schutte Trait Emotional Intelligence Test (Schutte et al. 1998) Dependent variables: (1) Job performance – self-rated, (2) job sat., (3) affective commitment to the org., (4) org. citizenship behavior, (5) work-life balance, and (6) job involvement, (7) intent to leave. Controls: org. size, tenure, income. Regressions showed Total EI sig. predicts job performance (p < .01), job satisfaction (p < .01), affective commitment to the organization (p < .05), organizational commitment (p < .001), and work-life balance (p < .05). Emotional intelligence and performance Author(s) Sample EI Measure 55 Dependent Variables & Controls Key Results 7 Dulewicz, Higgs, & Slaski, 2003 59 middlemanagers from a large retail organization Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (EIQ) developed by the authors Dependent variables: (1) performance ratings by manager on 16 critical success factors determined by the organization, e.g., setting objectives, planning, organizing, teamwork, etc., (2) morale, and (3) stress. Total EI was correlated with performance (p < .001), morale (p < .001) and lower stress (p < .001). Performance was also sig. correlated with dimensions of Self-Awareness (p < .001), Emotional Resilience, (p < .001), and Motivation (p < .001). But not with dimensions of Interpersonal Sensitivity, Influence, Intuitiveness, or Conscientiousness. 8 Slaski & Cartwright, 2002 224 middle managers in a major retail organization in the United Kingdom Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-i). (Bar-On, 1997) Dependent variables: (1) management performance rated by immediate managers on 16 critical success factors, e.g., setting objectives, planning, organizing, teamwork, etc., (2) general health and well-being, (3) morale, (4) stress, and (5) quality of work life. Total EI was correlated with performance (p< .01), better health and well-being (p< .01), morale (p< .01), lower stress (p< .01), and better quality of work life (p< .01). Performance was sig. correlated with EI dimensions of Intrapersonal EI (p < .05), Adaptability EI (p < .05), Stress Management EI (p < .05), and General Mood EI (p < .05), but not Interpersonal EI 9 Wong & Law (2002) Study 1: 60 middle and upper-level managers and their subordinates in Hong Kong. Study 2: 146 middle level administrators in Hong Kong Government Wong & Law Emotional Intelligence Survey (WLEIS) rated by managers (developed by authors) Dependent variables for study 1: (1) Job performance (manager ratings), (2) emotional labor, (3) job satisfaction, (4) organizational commitment, and (5) turnover intention Dependent variables for study 2: (1) job performance, (2) job satisfaction, (3) organization citizenship behaviors Study 1: Total EI of subordinates is correlated with job performance (p < .01), job satisfaction (p < .01). EI Regulation of Emotion (p < .01) and Others’ Emotion Appraisal (p < .01) were only EI dimensions sig. correlated with performance. Emotional labor of the job moderates subordinate’s EI – job performance relationship, such that EI is more relevant to job performance if the job requires emotional labor. Study 2: Manager’s EI is correlated with subordinates job satisfaction (p < .01) and organization citizenship behavior (p < .05), but not performance. 10 Dulewicz & Higgs (2000) 100 general managers taking a management course EI competencies measured via selfreport (e.g., listening, sensitivity, motivating others, achievement motive, emotional resilience) Dependent variable: managerial advancement – assessed 10 years after competency assessment. Comparisons: Cognitive competencies (e.g., judgment, planning), and managerial competencies (e.g., supervising) EI competencies accounted for 36% of variance in advancement, cognitive competencies accounted for 27% of variance in advancement, and managerial competencies accounted for 16% of variance in advancement. Emotional intelligence and performance 56 Table 3 Peer Reviewed Journal Studies Examining Emotional Intelligence and Expressed Emotional Intelligence in Team Contexts Author(s) Sample EI Measure Dependent Variables & Controls Key Results Emotional Intelligence in Team Contexts 1 Elfenbein, Polzer, & Ambady, 2007 68 employees of a community service non-profit organization randomly assigned to 14 teams working together for one week before data collection. Emotion recognition: Team emotion recognition accuracy (TERA) test developed by the authors. Dependent variable: Accuracy at perceiving emotions Controls: (1) NEO-PPI five-factor personality scale (Costa & McCrae, 1992), and (2) Demographics Teams varied sig. in their members’ average level of accuracy at Perceiving teammate’s expressions of anger and embarrassment. By contrast, teams did not differ in their accuracy at perceiving the emotions of random others (non teammates). This reveals that perceiving team members emotions is a team-level skill that some teams exhibit sig. better than others. No sig. differences found among high and low Perceiving teams in Big Five or demographics. 2 Day & Carroll, 2004 246 Canadian students in 47 teams working together for one short task MSCEIT V1.1 (Mayer et al., 2000) (Team-level EI is measured as the average of team member EI scores). Dependent Variables: (1) Performance based on (a) individual and (b) team score on a cognitive decision-making task (selecting whom to downsize), (2) Organization citizenship behaviors at the (a) individual level and (b) team-level. EI branch of Perceiving Emotion was sig. correlated with individual-level performance (p < .01). Team-level EI was not sig. correlated with team performance. All team- level EI branches were sig. correlated with team-organization citizenship branch of sportsmanship (p < .05). Team-level EI branches (all except Perceiving Emotion) were sig. correlated with organization citizenship branch of civic virtue. 3 Feyerhem & Rice, 2002 164 employees in 11 teams in a regional processing office of a financial services division of large insurance company in the mid-western U.S. The Multifactor Emotional Intelligence Scale (MEIS) (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 1997) (Team-level EI is measured as the average of team member EI scores). Dependent variables: (1) 2 managers ranked all teams on performance and (2) ratings of team performance by team members, team leaders and managers. Team-level EI branches of Managing Emotions in Others sig. correlated with performance rankings (p < .05) and performance ratings (p < .05). Team-level EI branches of Managing Emotions in Self sig. correlated with performance rankings (p < .05) and performance ratings (p < .05). Team leader EI was not correlated with performance. Team leader Understanding Emotions was sig. positively correlated with ratings of team customer service (p < .05), but sig. negatively correlated with team accuracy (p < .01), productivity (p < .01), and commitment to continuous improvement (p < .01). Emotional intelligence and performance Author(s) Sample EI Measure Dependent Variables & Controls Key Results Dependent variables in study 1: faculty ratings of team effectiveness at 1 month and 6 months post norm assessments. Dependent variables in study 2: team effectiveness rated by managers at least 2 months post norm assessment. Study 1: Team performance was sig. correlated with Team-level EI dimensions of Interpersonal Understanding (p < .01), Team Self-Evaluation (p < .05), Proactive Problem Solving (p < .01), Organizational Understanding (p < .01), and Building External Relationships (p < .05). Study 2: Team performance was sig. correlated with Team-level EI dimensions of Interpersonal Understanding (p < .01), Proactive Problem Solving (p < .01), and Building External Relationships (p < .05) and marginally sig. correlated with Organizational Understanding (p < .10). Bar-On, 1997 – Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-i). (Team-level EI is measured as the average of team member EI scores). Dependent variables: Team process effectiveness: (a) Team task orientation and (b) team maintenance function Team-level Total EI did not predict team task orientation or team maintenance function Team task orientation was sig. correlated with 2 EI dimensions: Team-level Interpersonal EI (p < .05), and Team-level Adaptability EI (p < .05). Team maintenance function was sig. related to teamlevel General Mood EI (p < .05). Wong, Law, & Wong, 2004 – forced choice measure Dependent variables: (1) subjective experience in a dyad negotiation and (2) Objective performance scores in dyad negotiation. Controls: (1) Big Five personality inventory (NEO-IPIP --omitting agreeableness), (2) Grade point average (GPA) All findings occur after controlling for Big Five and GPA In dyad negotiations, higher Dyad-level EI produced sig. better joint negotiation outcomes. Partner with higher EI had a sig. more positive experience than counterpart. The individual in the partnership with the highest EI earned a sig. lower objective score for him/herself, suggesting that higher EI allows you to create value – but your negotiating partner benefits from that value. Expressed Emotional Intelligence in Team Contexts 4 Druskat, Wolff, Study 1: 382 Group emotional Messer, & MBA students in competence norms Stubbs, 2007 48 teams together (GEC Norms) in all courses for (survey measures a year. Study 2: team-level norms; 905 employees developed by (109 teams) in six authors) diverse companies 5 Frye, Bennett & Caldwell, 2006 6 Foo, Elfenbein, Tan, & Aik, 2005 130 employees in 33 self-managing teams at the headquarters of a distributor of promotional products in the Mid-Western U.S. 164 Business undergraduates in Asia (82 dyads) 57 Emotional intelligence and performance Author(s) Sample EI Measure 58 Dependent Variables & Controls Key Results 7 Offermann, Bailey, Vasilopoulos, Seal, & Sass, 2004 425 business administration students in 89 teams in the MidAtlantic U.S. Emotional Competence Inventory (ECI) – self-assessment version (Boyatzis & Goleman, 2002). (Team-level EI is measured as the average of team member EI scores). Dependent variables: (1) Individual performance (exam grades), (2) team score in a cognitive problem-solving task (blizzard” survival), (3) team project grade, (4) team attitudes survey, and (5) leadership emergence and effectiveness (both measured by peer ratings). Controls: (1) Cognitive ability – (SAT score), and (2) Big Five personality traits Team-level EI was sig. correlated with team project grade (p < .05) and team attitudes (p < .05; with the relationship management cluster accounting for most of the variance), but not team problem-solving score. EI was sig. correlated with leader emergence (p < .01) and leader effectiveness (p < .01), with dimensions of Self-Awareness (p < .01) and Relationship Management (p < .01) the most strongly correlated with both. Regressions showed that when included along with the Big Five, the EI dimensions still added significant incremental validity in the prediction of leadership emergence (R² = .047, p = .002) and marginally significant incremental validity in predicting leadership effectiveness (R² = .023, p = .076). EI was not sig. correlated with individual performance. Team-level (averages) Big Five traits were not sig. correlated with team project grades and team-level (average) cognitive ability was not sig. correlated with team performance measures or team attitudes. 8 Jordan & Troth, 2004 350 Australian students in 108 teams in an introductory management course Workgroup Emotional Intelligence Profile, Version 6 (WEIP-6) –– self-report measure developed by the authors to measure EI in teams. (Team-level EI is measured as the average of team member EI scores.) Dependent variables: (1) Performance based on (a) individual and (b) team score on a problemsolving task: (survival exercise), (2) Interpersonal conflict handling styles Teams with higher team-level WEIP scores, score higher on team-level performance (p < .05). Team’s average scores on ability to Manage their Own Emotions was sig. correlated to higher team performance. Ability to Manage Other’s Emotions was not linked to team performance. Regression analysis with subscales of the WEIP revealed that team performance was best predicted by Management of One’s Own Emotions (p < .01) and Discussion of Own Emotions (p < .05), which was negatively associated with performance. Awareness of Own Emotions was not a significant predictor. Teams with higher team-level WEIP scores used collaboration as their preferred style of conflict resolution. Emotional intelligence and performance Author(s) Sample EI Measure Dependent Variables & Controls 59 Key Results 9 Jordan, Ashkanasy, Hartel, & Hooper, 2002 448 Australian undergraduates in 44 teams in a managerial skills course for nine weeks Workgroup Emotional Intelligence Profile, Version 3 (WEIP-3) – self-report measure developed by the authors to measure EI in teams. (Team-level EI is measured as the average of team member EI scores). Dependent variables: Team performance ratings based on “log sheets” submitted weekly for 9 weeks by teams and coded for (a) team process effectiveness and (b) team goal focus Teams with high team-level WEIP scores performed well on process effectiveness and goal focus right from the start of the semester. Teams with low team-level WEIP scores initially performed poorly on process effectiveness and goal focus, but improved over time so that by the end of nine weeks there were no sig. differences between groups. 10 Rapisarda, 2002 91 executive MBA students working in 18 study groups for a period of two years Emotional Competence Inventory (ECI) -360 degree version (Boyatzis & Goleman, 2002). Dependent variables: (1) Team performance ratings by team members and faculty, and (2) team cohesiveness ratings by team members and faculty. Dimensions of ECI sig. correlated with student ratings of cohesiveness included: Achievement Orientation, Empathy, Influence, Communication, Leadership, Conflict Management, Self-control Adaptability and Building Bonds. Dimensions of ECI sig. correlated with faculty ratings of cohesiveness were Empathy (p < .05), and marginally sig. were Achievement Orientation and Influence (p < .10) Two dimensions of ECI were marginally sig correlated with student ratings of performance: Achievement Orientation and Empathy (p < .10). One dimensions of ECI was sig. correlated with faculty ratings of performance: Empathy (p < .05).