Rigor, Relevance and Relationships in Educational Reform: The Story of the Hope Study By Ronald J. Newell and Mark J. Van Ryzin EdVisions Schools is a Gates Foundation intermediary whose focus is creating and sustaining small, innovative secondary schools. Two of our design essentials are project-based learning and advisory grouping. Project-based learning emphasizes individual and collaborative projects as the foundation for learning and includes very little of the whole-class instruction typically found in traditional secondary schools (Newell, 2003). Advisory grouping provides a stable sense of support for adolescents by matching them with an adult and a group of peers over an extended period of time (Newell, 2003). During our history, we have been interested in not only developing learning cultures that support adolescent emotional and psychological growth, but also in finding some way to assess the growth of these developmental assets. Our interest is based upon the rather sobering results reported by research on traditional secondary schools, which reveals year-by-year decreases in student motivation, engagement, and interest in school (Eccles, Midgley, & Adler, 1984; Gottfried, Fleming, & Gottfried, 2001; Harter, 1981; Hirsch & Rapkin, 1987; Marks, 2000). Further, adolescents show an increase in the frequency of high risk behaviors and psychological disturbances, such as depression and eating disorders (Dryfoos, 1990; Kazdin, 1993). Anecdotally, we knew that adolescents in EdVisions’ settings were happier, more satisfied, and had better attendance records than in their previous schools, but our stories, though compelling, did not constitute scientific proof that our pedagogical model was indeed benefiting youth development. To evaluate our pedagogical model, we turned to stage-environment fit theory, which states that traditional middle and high school environments are often not able to meet the developmental needs of adolescents, increasing the risk of negative outcomes, like drops in motivation and engagement (Eccles & Midgley, 1989; Eccles, et al., 1993; Roeser, Eccles, & Strobel, 1998). The theory also asserts that a better match in terms of adolescent needs and the educational environment should result in more positive outcomes (Midgley, Feldlaufer, & Eccles, 1989). Thus, we asked the basic question: is our pedagogical model a good stageenvironment fit for adolescents? We were interested in determining whether the EdVisions learning community was a healthy place for adolescent psychological development. We decided that surveys measuring student perceptions of autonomy, belongingness, goal orientation, engagement and hope would give good indications of how adolescents perceive the school environment and how, in turn, they respond. This grouping of surveys has become known within EdVisions as the Hope Study. From preliminary surveys results in ‘04-‘05, we have determined that EdVisions sites are psychologically healthier places for adolescents than traditional secondary schools, and that the project-based learning and full-time advisory systems are design essentials that increase levels of engagement and hope in our students. The Gates Foundation and EdVisions believe that the new three R’s in the 21 st Century ought to be Rigor, Relevance and Relationships. EdVisions believes that the survey components from the Hope Study can give definitive indications as to whether or not our replication sites are holding rigor, relevance and relationships in high regard. The Hope Study survey results can be used to determine the development of design principles that create powerful learning environments for adolescents. The Hope Study Survey Components Autonomy: having choices, setting your own goals and timelines, choosing what to study and when, viewpoints are accepted. Project-based learning enables students to pursue their own interests and passions, what is “in the moment” rather than imposed. Autonomy affects motivation and has a direct relationship to persistence and engagement, as well as graduation rates (Deci, Nezlek, & Sheinman, 1981; Deci, Schwartz, Sheinman, & Ryan, 1981; Flink, Boggiano, & Barrett, 1990; Ryan & Grolnick, 1986; Vallerand & Bissonnette, 1992; Vansteenkiste, Simons, Lens, Sheldon, & Deci, 2004). Among the 3 R’s, autonomy is directly related to Relevance; in EdVisions schools, students are encouraged to pursue those academic interests that are most relevant. Belongingness: perceiving high levels of support from peers and teachers, in both personal issues and academic issues. The need for strong, mutually supportive relationships is a fundamental human need (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). In the school setting, belongingness refers to the quality of interpersonal relationships with teachers and peers. High levels of belongingness lead to increases in motivation, positive social behavior and academic achievement (Wentzel, 1994, 1997, 1998; Wentzel, Barry, & Caldwell, 2004; Wentzel & Caldwell, 1997). In contrast, socially rejected students show lower levels of engagement, have higher levels of academic and behavioral problems (DeRosier, Kupersmidt, & Patterson, 1994), and can be at significant risk of dropping out of school and eventually running afoul of the law (Parker & Asher, 1987). Belongingness can also enhance school adjustment, perceived competence, and self-esteem (Barrera, Chassin, & Rogosch, 1993; Cauce, 1986; Kurdek & Sinclair, 1988). Belongingness is a direct measure of Relationships. EdVisions’ schools foster these positive relationships through the practice of advisory grouping. Goal Orientation: perceiving high expectations from teachers; belief that effort will be recognized; emphasis on deep understanding rather than shallow recitation. Students who perceive a positive or “task” goal orientation in school will seek challenges and show persistence in the face of adversity, use more effective learning strategies, have more positive attitudes and demonstrate more engagement in learning (Roeser, Midgley, & Urdan, 1996). On the other hand, students who perceive a negative or “performance” goal orientation seek to avoid challenge and, in the face of failure, attribute their results to lack of ability and exhibit a “learned helplessness” response, which refers to a negative emotional response and a defeatist attitude (Eccles & Midgley, 1989). Goal orientation is a good measure of the Rigor of an educational environment; schools that are uniformly rigorous and emphasize deep understanding will see high scores in “task” goal orientation, whereas schools that favor some students over others or who emphasize shallow understanding will see high scores in “performance” goal orientation. In EdVisions’ schools, both project-based learning and advisory grouping come together to inspire a positive “task” goal orientation. Engagement: a reflection of the student work ethic, concentration and attention paid to school work; behavioral and emotional involvement in school. A higher level of engagement is a reflection of the amount of effort and passion a student will put into learning (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004). A lower level of engagement usually means a student will not complete work on time and will not learn much of value. An engaged learner will do superior work, obtain a deeper level of understanding, and retain the knowledge and skills for a longer period of time. Hope: an individual’s conception of their ability to conceptualize goals, develop strategies for goal completion, and initiate and sustain motivation toward those goals. Higher hope students set more challenging goals for themselves and tend to perceive they will be more successful at attaining those goals, even if at first they do not succeed (Snyder, et al., 1991). Hope can predict college grade point averages, higher graduation rates, optimism about life, more physical health, more self-esteem, and greater levels of happiness (Snyder, 2002; Snyder, et al., 2002). Results To assess the impact of project-based learning and advisory grouping, an Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) was conducted using data from three schools located in the same rural area southwest of Minneapolis. The three schools had similar students and similar socio-economic conditions. One of the schools was a longterm EdVisions site, one was a relatively new EdVisions site, and one was a typical traditional rural Minnesota comprehensive high school. Data at all three schools was collected multiple times, so longitudinal measures of change could be captured for the same students. The data indicated that student perceptions of autonomy, teacher support, and “task” goal orientation were significantly higher in EdVisions sites, while perceptions of a “performance” goal orientation where higher in the traditional school. As a result, students in the EdVisions schools demonstrated higher levels of engagement, and student hope scores grew over a relatively short span of time (five months), whereas they fell slightly for students in the traditional high school. In fact, the levels of autonomy and engagement were nearly twice as high in EdVisions sites. Only peer support was relatively similar, although EdVisions students showed slightly higher levels. In EdVisions schools, hope grew from an aggregate score of 48.87 to 50.69 (+1.82) in the older EdVisions site, from 47.47 to 49.45 in the newer EdVisions site (+1.98), and fell slightly from 48.59 to 48.35 (-.24) in the traditional site. This growth in hope among the EdVisions students is phenomenal when considering the fact that hope index scores are generally quite stable during adolescence and adulthood unless a significant intervention is introduced. The hope score increases were accompanied with other indicators of success in EdVisions sites. Percentile rank scores in reading and math went up in every EdVisions site where data was available for three years, increasing in aggregate 6.3 percentiles in reading, and 8.7 percentiles in math. ACT scores at the nine oldest EdVisions sites (22.4) averaged higher than the Minnesota state average (22.2) in ‘04-‘05. Although there is not enough comparable data to make a definitive statement, there is indication that as hope scores go up, standardized academic measures will follow. The 3 R’s As can be seen, all of the above attributes of learning communities ultimately are brought together in the three R’s (Rigor, Relevance, and Relationships). As measurement instruments, the Hope Study components can indeed connect the three R’s to student outcomes and can provide insights into what the learning community does to provide students with a challenging, supportive and stimulating place in which to learn. By measuring the components mentioned above, the survey instruments provide educational reformers with feedback regarding whether their changes are enhancing the ability of students to achieve their goals, whether they are able to engage them in wanting to learn. As previously mentioned, academic results soon follow the 3 R’s. We believe that students rarely adopt higher academic learning goals in response to “surface” reforms, such as changes in curriculum or subject matter; however, if the reforms permit higher levels of autonomy for students, encourage higher levels of peer and teacher support, and provide higher levels of “task” goal orientation, then academic results will not only appear but will be longer lasting. When new school cultures are created, the general assessment practice is to immediately look for academic gains as measured by grades, standardized tests, or criterion referenced academic scores. Even when student performance is measured, it is more than likely tied to academic performances rather than life skills. Generally, dispositional goals are dismissed as too difficult to discern or as irrelevant. Unfortunately, when only academic gains are measured, there may be an immediate level of growth in response to change in the environment, but these gains will be short-lived if not accompanied by deeper levels of change in the learning community along the lines of the 3 R’s. Simply making greater demands on students will not ensure long term success, especially for the students who have had little success in traditional settings. But when long term changes in the learning community are geared towards Rigor, Relevance and Relationships, long-term academic success is more likely. By measuring the 3 R’s in the Hope Study, EdVisions is able to help build greater capacity for reform and therefore have more long-term effects on more children’s lives. References Barrera, M., Jr., Chassin, L., & Rogosch, F. (1993). Effects of social support and conflict on adolescent children of alcoholic and nonalcoholic fathers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 602-612. Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497-529. Cauce, A. M. (1986). Social networks and social competence: Exploring the effects of early adolescent friendships. American Journal of Community Psychology, 14, 607-628. Deci, E. L., Nezlek, J., & Sheinman, L. (1981). Characteristics of the rewarder and intrinsic motivation of the rewardee. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 1-10. Deci, E. L., Schwartz, A. J., Sheinman, L., & Ryan, R. M. (1981). An instrument to access adults’ orientations toward control versus autonomy with children: Reflections on intrinsic motivation and perceived competence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 642-650. DeRosier, M. E., Kupersmidt, J. B., & Patterson, C. J. (1994). Children’s academic and behavioral adjustment as a function of the chronicity and proximity of peer rejection. Child Development, 65, 1799-1813. Dryfoos, J. G. (1990). Adolescents at risk: Prevalence and prevention. New York: Oxford University Press. Eccles, J. S., & Midgley, C. (1989). Stage/environment fit: Developmentally appropriate classrooms for early adolescents. In R.E. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in education (Vol. 3, pp. 139-186). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Eccles, J., Midgley, C., & Adler, T. (1984). Grade-related changes in the school environment: Effects of achievement motivation. In J.G. Nicholls (Ed.), The development of achievement motivation (pp. 283-331). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Midgley, C., Reuman, D., MacIver, D., & Feldlaufer, H. (1993). Negative effects of traditional middle schools on students’ motivation. The Elementary School Journal, 93, 553-574. Flink, C., Boggiano, A. K., & Barrett, M. (1990). Controlling teaching strategies: Undermining children’s self-determination and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 916-924. Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74, 59-109. Gottfried, A. E., Fleming, J. S., & Gottfried, A. W. (2001). Continuity of academic intrinsic motivation from childhood through late adolescent: A longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 3-13. Harter, S. (1981). A new self-report scale of intrinsic versus extrinsic orientation in the classroom: Motivational and informational components. Developmental Psychology, 17, 300-312. Hirsch, B. J., & Rapkin, B. D. (1987). The transition to junior high school: A longitudinal study of self-esteem, psychological symptomatology, school life, and social support. Child Development, 58, 1235-1243. Kazdin, A. E. (1993). Adolescent mental health: Prevention and treatment programs. American Psychologist, 48, 127-141. Kurdek, L. A., & Sinclair, R. J. (1988). Adjustment of young adolescents in twoparent nuclear, stepfather, and mother-custody families. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 91-96. Marks, H. M. (2000). Student engagement in instructional activity: Patterns in the elementary, middle and high school years. American Educational Research Journal, 37, 153-184. Midgley, C., Feldlaufer, H., & Eccles, J. S. (1989). Student/teacher relations and attitudes toward mathematics before and after the transition to junior high school. Child Development, 60, 981-992. Newell, R. J. (2003). Passion for learning: How project-based learning meets the needs of 21st-century students. Lanham, MD: Scarecrow Press. Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations and later personal adjustment: Are low-accepted children at risk? Psychological Bulletin, 102, 357-389. Roeser, R. W., Eccles, J. S., & Strobel, K. R. (1998). Linking the study of schooling and mental health: Selected issues and empirical illustrations at the level of the individual. Educational Psychologist, 33, 153-176. Roeser, R. W., Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. C. (1996). Perceptions of the school psychological environment and early adolescents’ psychological and behavioral functioning in school: The mediating role of goals and belonging. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 408-422. Ryan, R. M., & Grolnick, W. S. (1986). Origins and pawns in the classroom: Selfreport and projective assessments of individual differences in children’s perceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 550-558. Snyder, C.R. (2002). Hope theory: Rainbows in the mind. Psychological Inquiry, 13, 249-275. Snyder, C.R., Harris, C., Anderson, J.R., Holleran, S.A., Irving, L.M., Sigmon, S.T., Yoshinobu, L., Gibb, J., Langelle, C. & Harney, P. (1991). The will and the ways: Development and validation of an individual-differences measure of hope. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 575-585. Snyder, C.R., Shorey, H.S., Cheavens, J., Pulvers, K.M., Adams, V.H., & Wiklund, C. (2002). Hope and academic success in college. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 820-826. Vallerand, R. J. & Bissonnette, R. (1992). Intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivational styles as predictors of behavior: A prospective study. Journal of Personality, 60, 599-620. Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Sheldon, K. M., & Deci, E. L. (2004). Motivating learning, performance and persistence: The synergistic effects of intrinsic goal contents and autonomy-supportive contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 246-260. Wentzel, K. R. (1994). Relations of social goal pursuit to social acceptance, classroom behavior, and perceived social support. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 173-182. Wentzel, K. R. (1997). Student motivation in middle school: The role of perceived pedagogical caring. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(3), 411-419. Wentzel, K. R. (1998). Social relationships and motivation in middle school: The role of parents, teachers, and peers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 202209. Wentzel, K. R., Barry, C. M., & Caldwell, K. A. (2004). Friendships in middle school: Influences on motivation and school adjustment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 195-203. Wentzel, K. R., & Caldwell, K. (1997). Friendships, peer acceptance, and group membership: Relations to academic achievement in middle school. Child Development, 68, 1198-1209.