Laurence Steinberg, Ph.D. Distinguished University Professor Laura H. Carnell Professor of Psychology Sir Cyril Chantler Plain Packaging Review C/o King’s College London Room 1.2 Hodgkin Building Guy’s Campus London SE1 1UL By Mail and By Email (Christopher.1.cox@kcl.ac.uk) Re: Independent review of the public health evidence on standardized packaging of tobacco 7 January 2013 Dear Sir Cyril, I write this letter in response to your call for evidence on whether the introduction of standardized packaging is likely to lead to a decrease in the consumption of tobacco, including in particular a decrease in the risk of children taking up and becoming addicted to smoking. I have considerable expertise in the area of adolescent judgment, decisionmaking, and risk-taking, and have published widely on these topics.1 I have considered the topic of smoking by minors extensively in recent years. 1 I am the Distinguished University Professor and Laura H. Carnell Professor of Psychology at Temple University in Philadelphia and have published extensively on adolescent judgment, decision-making, and risk-taking. I am a former President of the Society for Research on Adolescence (the largest professional organization of scholars interested in this stage of development) and of the Division of Developmental Psychology of the American Psychological Association. I am the author of more than 350 scholarly articles and numerous books on adolescent development, including a leading textbook on the subject that is used in colleges in the USA and elsewhere. Department of Psychology Temple University Philadelphia, PA 19122 215-204-7485 215-204-5539 (fax) lds@temple.edu It is common ground between us, and among right-thinking people generally, that adolescents should be discouraged or prevented from taking up smoking. There are many policy options available to governments to achieve this, and you have been asked to consider whether the introduction of standardized packaging in England would be likely to be effective. My view is that it would not. The introduction of standardized packaging would be unlikely to affect underage smoking, either in terms of initiation or progression from experimentation to regular use. Adolescents’ experimentation with, and use of, tobacco is best viewed as a specific example of their propensity to engage in risk-taking more generally. Measures to prevent or reduce youth smoking are only likely to be effective if they are informed by scientific research on risk-taking and decision-making in this age group. I know of no scientific evidence that suggests, nor would my understanding of the current research on adolescent decision-making suggest, that cigarette packaging is relevant to adolescents’ decisions to experiment with or continue smoking. These opinions are consistent with the contents of the November 2010 report on “Adolescent Decision-Making and the Prevention of Underage Smoking” (copy enclosed) that I prepared at the request of external legal counsel to Japan Tobacco International in response to a consultation request from the European Commission with regard to the proposed Second Tobacco Products Directive and to assist Governments and other stakeholders considering standardized packaging and similar regulatory proposals. On the specific question before you, I concluded in 2010 in that report that: “… there is no evidence that changes in cigarette packaging affect adolescents’ experimentation with or use of cigarettes… [T]he impact of changes in cigarette packaging on adolescent smoking is, at best, likely to be very small” (paragraph 75). My view remains unchanged today, despite the further studies published since 2010 (which I have reviewed) and the introduction of standardized packaging in Australia. In passing, I should say that while I understand that Japan Tobacco International is, via their external legal counsel, aware that I am writing to you, the views that I set out below are my own, based on my experience as an independent scientist with extensive knowledge based on research in the area of adolescent decision-making. They reflect the best contemporary science on this topic, which I summarize below. I would be happy to discuss my views further with you directly, if you would find that helpful. In greater detail: 1. In light of the fact that smoking during adolescence substantially elevates the likelihood for regular adult smoking, and, consequently, for the serious health consequences associated with chronic smoking, many have called for policies and Department of Psychology Temple University Philadelphia, PA 19122 215-204-7485 215-204-5539 (fax) lds@temple.edu practices designed to reduce the number of adolescents who experiment with tobacco and the number of those who experiment who progress to regular smoking, as well as measures designed to encourage regular adolescent smokers to quit. It appears that the most significant reductions in smoking prevalence are likely to come from policies and practices that reduce the number of individuals who try cigarettes, rather than from those that encourage cessation among regular smokers (Gilpin et al., 2006). So it makes sense to focus on policy measures that will discourage or prevent adolescents from experimenting with cigarettes. The question is whether standardized packaging is likely to have that effect, in light of what we know about adolescents’ decision-making and their risk-taking behaviour generally including why adolescents experiment with tobacco. 2. The high rate of risky behavior among adolescents relative to adults, despite massive, ongoing, and costly efforts to educate teenagers about its potentially harmful consequences (including a tremendous investment in health education), has been the focus of much theorizing and empirical research by developmental scientists for at least three decades (Steinberg, 2008). This work has disproved several widely-held beliefs about adolescent risk-taking: for instance, that adolescents are more likely than adults to believe they are invulnerable; that adolescents are deficient in their information-processing, or that they think about risk in fundamentally different ways from adults; and that adolescents do not perceive risks where adults do, or are less risk-averse than adults. None of these assertions is correct. Indeed, most studies find few, if any, age differences in individuals’ evaluation of the risks inherent in a wide range of potentially dangerous behaviors (e.g., smoking, driving while drunk, having unprotected sex). Research consistently shows that adolescents are well aware of the health risks of smoking2 but that some start smoking anyway. Given the nearuniversal acknowledgement among teenagers in the UK of the health risks of smoking, changing cigarette packaging will likely have no impact on adolescents’ awareness of the potential harmful consequences of smoking. 3. Adolescents are knowledgeable, logical, reality-based, and accurate in the ways in which they think about risky activities, including smoking – no different from adults, in fact. But they do engage in higher levels of risky behavior than adults. The explanation for this apparent paradox which is highly relevant to your assessment, for reasons that I explain later – is explained by more contemporary models of adolescent e.g. “When asked about their beliefs about smoking, the majority of pupils reported strong agreement with the negative effects of smoking. Almost all the pupils thought smoking can cause lung cancer (99%), makes your clothes smell (97%), harms unborn babies (97%), can harm non-smokers health (96%) and can cause heart disease (93%).” The NHS Information Centre, Lifestyles Statistics, “Statistics on Smoking: England” (2012), at p.47; available at: https://catalogue.ic.nhs.uk/publications/publichealth/smoking/smok-eng-2012/smok-eng-2012-rep.pdf. 2 Department of Psychology Temple University Philadelphia, PA 19122 215-204-7485 215-204-5539 (fax) lds@temple.edu decision-making that draw on recent advances in developmental neuroscience. The dominant framework to emerge is what has been called a “maturational imbalance” or “dual systems model” (Casey et al., 2011; Steinberg, 2010). This posits that heightened risk taking in adolescence is a natural by-product of the asynchronous maturation of the “reward system” or “incentive processing system” of the brain, which is responsive to emotion, reward and novelty, and the “cognitive control system,” which is critical for impulse control, emotion regulation, and planned decision making. It has been suggested that the incentive processing system becomes especially aroused early in adolescence, and that this arousal pushes the adolescent to engage in sensation-seeking in the pursuit of immediate rewards, but that this arousal takes place before the cognitive control system has fully matured. Later in adolescence and in early adulthood, there is a decrease in the incentive system’s responsiveness to rewarding and emotionally arousing social stimuli and improvements in the cognitive control system. This leads to a decline in risk-taking behavior that continues into adulthood. There is growing support in the scientific literature for numerous aspects of the dual-systems model, in the fields of both neurobiology and psychology (see Engle, 2013, for a recent series of articles on the adolescent brain). 4. These more contemporary models explain a series of observations about adolescent decision-making that inform why minors take risks and why some experiment with cigarettes: a. Adolescents are especially sensitive to rewards (Galvan, 2010), including rewarding stimuli like social status or admiration (Burnett et al., 2011). Thus, when faced with two alternative courses of action (e.g., trying versus forgoing smoking), adolescents will pay greater attention to the potential rewards of each alternative (e.g., gaining the admiration of one’s peers versus pleasing one’s parents) than to their risks. b. Compared to adults, adolescents are more likely to focus on the immediate consequences of a decision, rather than think about the longer-term ones (Steinberg et al., 2009). This does not appear to be due to poor impulse control but instead to the generally weaker orientation to the future evinced by young people, especially during the early adolescent years (i.e., before 16). Thus, it is not so much that teenagers are incapable of delaying gratification (as one might see in a small child) as it is that they just prefer not to. c. Compared to adults, adolescents are more likely to pay attention to and focus on the immediate and short-term drawbacks of a choice (e.g., that smoking will cause bad breath, or that not smoking will lead to social exclusion by peers) than Department of Psychology Temple University Philadelphia, PA 19122 215-204-7485 215-204-5539 (fax) lds@temple.edu on the longer-term ones (e.g., that smoking may cause lung cancer or heart disease). To a sensation-seeking 15-year-old focused on what he experiences to be the here-and-now rewards of smoking, preventing experimentation with cigarettes by emphasizing the possibility of developing a disease 40 years in the future is not likely to be a deterrent. Conversely, making cigarettes less affordable to minors has been proven to be an effective short-term deterrent to their smoking3; in contrast, the introduction of standardized packaging emphasizing the costs of smoking, and, in particular, the potential long-term health consequences of smoking would not have any impact within the relevant short term timeframes of adolescent decision-making. d. Adolescents’ decisions about risk-taking are more easily swayed than are adults’ by the influence of their peers. Susceptibility to peer influence is high during early and mid-adolescence and declines steadily until about age 18, at which point it levels off (Steinberg & Monahan, 2007). Peer influence tends to exacerbate adolescents’ sensitivity to rewards and their preference for immediate rewards (Albert et al., 2013), which may lead them to engage in more risky behavior when they are with their peers than when they are alone (Gardner & Steinberg, 2005). The role of peer and societal influences as the primary drivers for smoking uptake by adolescents is widely recognized (e.g., Osaki et al., 2008). e. Owing to immaturity in brain regions associated with cognitive control, adolescents are less able to regulate their behavior than are adults (Casey et al., 2011; Steinberg et al., 2008). Although it is unlikely that adolescents’ decisions to purchase cigarettes are impulsive, because in order to circumvent age restrictions on the sale of tobacco, they must devise some sort of plan (e.g., decide which retail store is least likely to ask for ID, rehearse what they will say to the salesclerk in case ID is requested or to a stranger or someone they know over 18 to ask for a proxy purchase), their decision to try cigarettes for the first time may be made on the spur of the moment – and particularly in circumstances where peer influence is strong and cigarettes are readily available. f. Adolescents’ decision-making is more easily disrupted by emotional and social arousal than is that of adults (Albert & Steinberg, 2011). An important implication of this is that conventional research that finds few differences between 3 The price of cigarettes and the ease of availability of tobacco have repeatedly been shown to be key factors in smoking uptake by minors (Allison et al., 1999; Li, Stanton, & Feigelman, 2000; Petraitis et al., 1995; Sen & Wirjanto, 2009). Research indicates that adolescents, because of their relatively more limited discretionary income, are especially sensitive to cigarette pricing; changes in cigarette prices have an especially large impact on underage smoking, which rises as cigarette prices fall and declines as prices rise (Francis, 2000; Leverett et al., 2002; Waller et al., 2003). Department of Psychology Temple University Philadelphia, PA 19122 215-204-7485 215-204-5539 (fax) lds@temple.edu adolescents and adults in the way they think about risk may reach very different conclusions than would be the case if the same decision making were studied when individuals were actually in the moment. To be more concrete about it, asking questions during a focus group or telephone survey about the potential risks of smoking or the impact of packaging on likely cigarette use may yield very different responses than one would get if one asked the same questions to a group of adolescents who were drinking beer with their friends at a party (and this is a significant limitation on the subset of the standardized packaging research that does consider adolescent behavior). Accordingly, it is wise to interpret the results of research on adolescents’ responses to hypothetical changes in packaging or other aspects of marketing with great caution. How adolescents respond to a hypothetical cigarette package when they are alone and completing a research questionnaire may bear no or little resemblance to how they respond to a real package when they see one at a party. 5. Taken together, and applied specifically to standardized packaging, these features of adolescent decision-making suggest that: a. The desire to experiment with risky activity, including smoking, is a normative aspect of adolescent behavior. It is highly improbable that the color or other features of cigarette packaging that would be addressed by the introduction of standardized packaging have any impact on an adolescent’s desire to engage or not to engage in risk-taking behavior of this sort. b. Given adolescents’ innate and natural propensity for risk-taking, the sheer availability of cigarettes within the adolescent’s social network is likely a stronger influence on his or her initial experimentation with smoking than is the type of cigarettes (e.g., brand, flavor, filtered or non-filtered) that are available or the packaging in which these cigarettes are contained. It is highly improbable that an adolescent who is interested in trying smoking will decline a cigarette from a friend because of the packaging of the cigarette that is offered, or agree to smoke that cigarette because of its packaging. c. Because of this, measures which focus on packaging are unlikely to make any significant impact on experimentation or use of tobacco products by adolescents. Measures that focus on the availability of cigarettes (i.e., removing them from adolescents’ social networks, for example, by raising the minimum legal purchase age or by criminalizing proxy purchasing, as I understand has been done in Scotland) and on their price (e.g., raising the tax on them) are far more likely to be effective. Department of Psychology Temple University Philadelphia, PA 19122 215-204-7485 215-204-5539 (fax) lds@temple.edu d. Those adolescents who may find standardized packaging less appealing are most likely to take measures to personalize their packages similar to the way many adolescents do for their smartphones. There has been a sharp rise in sales of such personalized cases or labels since the introduction of graphic health warnings or standardized packaging (in Australia). e. Adolescents, who cannot legally buy cigarettes, often obtain them from friends, often without seeing the packaging in which the cigarettes were initially supplied. We also know that minors who smoke in England and Wales are more likely to ask someone else to buy them for them (proxy purchasing) rather than try to buy them in shops, or to obtain them from friends or family4 – here, too, they may well not see cigarette packaging before obtaining the cigarettes. Thus, the ways in which adolescents, who may be experimenters or irregular users, and adults, who are likely to be regular smokers, obtain cigarettes are very different, which diminishes the reach and force of policies aimed at regulating how cigarettes are packaged. f. A frequently-stated goal of standardized packaging is to make on-package health warnings more salient. However, as already noted, it is abundantly clear that adolescents are aware of and understand the risks of smoking and know that it has harmful long-term health consequences. It is simply that they privilege short-term reward over long-term risk and are more susceptible to making decisions about smoking experimentation “in the moment” and under the influence of their peers. Therefore, even if standardized packaging were shown to increase the prominence of health warnings, this would in my view have no influence on adolescent experimentation with, or use of, tobacco products. g. There are relatively few studies that consider the effect of standardized packaging on the smoking behavior of adolescents (as opposed to adults), and those that do exist have used similar methodologies to those used in the adult studies. One must be very cautious in attempting to extrapolate the results of studies involving adults “Smoking, drinking and drug use among young people in England in 2012”, National Statistics and HSCIC (2012). Available at: https://catalogue.ic.nhs.uk/publications/public-health/surveys/smok-drin-drugyoun-peop-eng-2012/smok-drin-drug-youn-peop-eng-2012-repo.pdf. At page 9: “Rather than trying to buy cigarettes in shops, pupils were more likely to have asked someone else to buy cigarettes for them (proxy purchasing) […] In 2012, 8% of pupils had asked somebody to buy cigarettes on their behalf, and nine out of ten (88%) were successful at least once.” At page 17: “Pupils who smoked were most likely to get cigarettes by being given them by other people (69%), typically by other friends (57%).” 4 Department of Psychology Temple University Philadelphia, PA 19122 215-204-7485 215-204-5539 (fax) lds@temple.edu to adolescents, given the differences in adults’ and adolescents’ decision-making that I have described. h. I know of no studies that directly address whether levels of smoking initiation or progression among adolescents would be different, were standardized packaging required. Instead, studies ask consumers to imagine how they might behave if they had the opportunity to purchase cigarettes in standardized packages. Demonstrating that, when presented with a particular pack design by a researcher, a person will say that he or she would be more likely to quit smoking, or never to take it up, is not the same as demonstrating that the packaging actually leads to a change in behavior. There is a long tradition of research showing that it is far easier to change what people say they will do than to change their actual behavior. i. The choice presented to research subjects in the typical standardized packaging experiment is between a product in a conventional package and one in a standardized package. This might be informative to questions about the sorts of packages that a manufacturer interested in selling a product ought to choose. However, if the question under consideration is whether mandating standardized packaging would reduce the prevalence of smoking, it is not at all clear that research comparing standardized and conventional packaging is the least bit informative. If someone is thirsty, he may prefer a beverage in a colorful bottle to one in a plain bottle. But it is unlikely that, if given the choice between a plainly bottled beverage and nothing, a thirsty person will choose to forgo purchasing any beverage at all. Indeed, in one focus group study in which smokers rated cigarettes in plain brown packages less appealing, the participants explicitly noted during follow-up questioning that this preference was a consequence of the comparison between standardized packaging and other forms of packaging, and that they did not believe that standardized packaging would reduce the appeal of smoking if all cigarettes were sold in this form (Moodie, Mackintosh et al., 2011). 6. Overall, it is unlikely that standardized packaging will affect underage smoking at all, either in terms of initiation or progression, given that research indicates (1) that adolescents’ decisions to smoke are heavily influenced by the behavior of their peers and are hardly impacted by the communication of information about the health risks of smoking; (2) that the desire to experiment with risky activity, including smoking, is a normative aspect of adolescent behavior; and (3) that adolescents are likely to obtain cigarettes from friends, rather than retail outlets, and in many cases where the cigarettes in question have been removed from their package. The extant research on the likely impact of standardized packaging measures provides little evidence to suggest the contrary. Limiting adolescents’ access to cigarettes, through the more vigilant enforcement of minimum legal purchase age (MLPA) laws and through Department of Psychology Temple University Philadelphia, PA 19122 215-204-7485 215-204-5539 (fax) lds@temple.edu increasing the price of cigarettes are proven strategies for reducing underage smoking and should continue to be the focus of attempts to deter adolescents from experimenting with tobacco. Please do not hesitate to contact me. at lds@temple.edu, if you would like to discuss these insights further. Copies of the various references above are enclosed. Sincerely, Enc. (hard copy only) - various papers. Department of Psychology Temple University Philadelphia, PA 19122 215-204-7485 215-204-5539 (fax) lds@temple.edu References Albert, D., & Steinberg, L. (2011). Judgment and decision making in adolescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, 211-224. Albert, D., Chein, J., & Steinberg, L. (2013). The teenage brain: Peer influences on adolescent decision-making. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 114-120. Allison, K. et al. (1999). Adolescent substance use: Preliminary examinations of school and neighborhood context. American Journal of Community Psychology, 27, 111–141. Burnett, S., Sebastian, C., Kadosh, K., & Blakemore, S-J. (2011). The social brain in adolescence: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging and behavioural studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1654-1664. Casey, B. J., Jones, R., & Somerville, L. (2011). Braking and accelerating of the adolescent brain. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, 21-33. Engle, R. (2013). The teenage brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22 (2) (entire issue). Francis, D. (2000). Dramatic rise in teenage smoking. NBER Digest. October 25. Galvan, A. (2010). Adolescent development of the reward system. Frontiers of Human Neuroscience, 4, 1-9. Gardner, M., & Steinberg, L. (2005). Peer influence on risk-taking, risk preference, and risky decision-making in adolescence and adulthood: An experimental study. Developmental Psychology, 41, 625-635. Gilpin. E. et al. (2006). What contributed to the major decline in per capita cigarette consumption during California’s comprehensive tobacco control programme? Tobacco Control, 15, 308-16. Leverett, M. et al. (2002). Tobacco use: The impact of prices. The Journal of Law and Medical Ethics, 30 (suppl), 88-95. Li, X., Stanton, B., & Feigelman, S. (2000). Impact of perceived parental monitoring on adolescent risk behavior over 4 years. Journal of Adolescent Health, 27, 49–56. Moodie, C., Mackintosh, A. et al. (2011). Young adult smokers’ perceptions of plain packaging: a pilot naturalistic study. Tobacco Control, 20, 367-373. National Statistics and Health and Social Care Information Centre (2012). Smoking, drinking and drug use among young people in England in 2012. Department of Psychology Temple University Philadelphia, PA 19122 215-204-7485 215-204-5539 (fax) lds@temple.edu NHS Information Centre (2012). Statistics on Smoking: England. Osaki, Y. et al. (2008). Decrease in the prevalence of smoking among Japanese adolescents and its possible causes: Periodic nationwide cross-sectional surveys. Environmental Health and Preventive Medicine, 13, 219–226. Petraitis, J., Flay, B., & Miller, T. (1995). Reviewing theories of adolescent substance use: Organizing pieces in the puzzle. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 67–86. Sen, A., & Wirjanto, T.(2009). Estimating the impacts of cigarette taxes on youth smoking participation, initiation, and persistence: Empirical evidence from Canada. Health Economics, 19, 1264–1280. Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28, 78-106. Steinberg, L. (2010). A dual systems model of adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 216-224. Steinberg, L., Albert, D., Cauffman, E., Banich, M., Graham, S., & Woolard, J. (2008). Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and selfreport: Evidence for a dual systems model. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1764-1778. Steinberg, L., & Monahan, K. (2007). Age differences in resistance to peer influence. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1531-1543. Waller, J. et al. (2003). The early 1990s cigarette price decrease and trends in youth smoking in Ontario. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 94, 31-35. Department of Psychology Temple University Philadelphia, PA 19122 215-204-7485 215-204-5539 (fax) lds@temple.edu