Collaborative Research: Development of song culture in zebra finches - vocal and sensory changes over generations PIs: City College of New York: Ofer Tchernichovski & Lucas Parra Weill Medical College of Cornell University: Henning Voss Baylor College of Medicine: Santosh Helekar Project Summary Culture consists of traits inherited epigenetically through social learning. Oscine songbirds exhibit song learning and provide biologically tractable models of culture: members of a species show individual variation in song and geographically separated groups have local song dialects. How does song culture develop over multiple generations? In zebra finches, birds kept isolated during the sensitive period for song learning develop an abnormal isolate song. Interestingly, in an isolated colony founded by an isolate, wild-type songs emerge within 3-4 generations, due to biased imitation that gradually transforms the isolate song into a normal, species-typical song. In the proposed research we would like to study both vocal and sensory changes that take place during the cumulative cultural transition. We will combine continuous analysis of vocal changes with non-invasive fMRI brain imaging to investigate how auditory selectivity to songs develops in males and in females, and how it drives cultural changes. The non-invasive fMRI will allow repeated measurements of auditory selectivity side by side with measurements of vocal changes during individual development and over generations as culture emerges de novo in controlled environments. Of particular interest is the role of imitation biases in males, and that of female auditory/perceptual selectivity in driving cultural changes. We will confirm that the BOLD response changes measured above are due to changes in neuronal activity and not primarily due to hemodynamic and vascular factors by measuring the local cerebral blood flow with arterial spin labeling, and separately recording local field potential responses in parallel experiments. Our research could potentially bridge between studies of vocal and sensory changes (which are often studied in reduced preparations) and field ethology studies of cultural changes in vocal communication. This project will contribute to the training of postdoctoral scholars and graduate students. It will provide scientific education and research training to minority undergraduate students at CCNY. We will also involve a few Bronx high school students in the research. We will disseminate our findings through electronic and print media. . 1 Objectives This proposal is based on two recent findings: the first is that the transition from an isolate song into a wild type song can be studied in a controlled laboratory environment [1], where vocal changes leading to wild-type culture can be measured and analyzed continuously in different social conditions. The second finding is that non-invasive fMRI imaging can capture auditory selectivity to song stimuli in awake mildly sedated birds [2-4]. In males, such selectivity depends on early exposure to songs. In females, however, auditory selectivity to songs develops even without exposure to songs, suggesting that females might have an important role in guiding the development of song culture. To examine the role of imitation biases and song-selectivity in the emergence of song culture we address the following objectives: Objective 1: We will examine how song culture emerges during one generation by performing continuous measurements of social interactions and of vocal changes in controlled environments: A B ♀ ♀ In an arena configuration of one tutor (center) and 10 pupils we observed a wide variety of imitation, with one or two pupils imitating perfectly, one or two imitating nothing, and the rest of the bird with partial imitation (Tchernichovski et al 2001). The separation between the birds will allow us to record relatively clean song development from each bird using directional microphones (so that only one pupil can sing in the microphone direction) The separation between the birds will allow us to use automatic video tracking of each bird, continuously over development. This will allow us to quantify how much each pupil communicate with the tutor and with other pupils. In configuration B, we replace two pupils by adult females and visually separate some pupils from the females (black bars). This will allow us to examine the effect of females presence on tutor behavior and on the neighboring pupils We will perform these experiments both with wild-type tutor and with isolate tutor – to obtain the first generation of progress toward wt songs (which is the strongest effect). During song development we will perform fMRI in three age groups 40, 60, 90, and will compare the progression of imitation and social interactions in each bird with fMRI results. Experiments will be repeated to test for possible (unlikely) effect of the fMRI on song development – it is unlikely since social and live training will usually override that of passive playbacks. However, we will test the birds with song syllables – not entire songs, to further reduce the chance of effect. 2 Objectives 2: We will examine the developmental time course of auditory selectivity to song in males and in females, testing for experience-dependent effects. We will investigate whether there are consistent differences in males as opposed to females in developmental time course of auditory response maturation between isolates and birds raised in a semi-natural colony. This is a necessary first step in studying the role of sensory processes in the evolution of a song dialect. We will record song development males and test for coupling between song development and changes in auditory response selectivity in those males, and in their female audience. In previous fMRI studies with 3T scanner we found that blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) responses to vocal stimuli show stimulus-dependent selectivity in adult colony males but not in isolate males. Here, using a stronger magnet (a new 7T scanner) we hope to obtain higher definition brain images and measure both general song selectivity (songs vs. calls) and fine-grained song selectivity (comparing wild type songs, isolate songs and familiar colony songs). Under Objective 1 We will ask the following questions: i) When during song development do general and fine-grained song selectivities emerge in males? Time course of song selectivity development will be compared across colony raised males and isolates. Does individual variability in the progression of song imitation (similarity to tutor’s songs) correlate with the development of song selectivity, as assessed by fMRI? Are there consistent differences in BOLD responses of juveniles and adult males across wildtype and isolate songs? How responses to the bird’s own song develop in isolates versus colony raised males? ii) When during development do general and fine-grained song selectivities emerge, and how do they progress in females? Does the timing and magnitude of selectivity depend on the environment (colony vs. isolation)? iii) Assess the possible effect of repeated measurements of BOLD responses to vocal sounds on vocal development and on auditory selectivity. In case of adverse effects we will adjust the research plan. iv) We will confirm that the BOLD response changes measured above are due to changes in neuronal activity and not primarily due to hemodynamic and vascular factors by measuring the local cerebral blood flow with arterial spin labeling (ASL), and separately recording local field potential (LFP) responses in parallel experiments. Objective 3: We propose to test the following two hypotheses: a) Progressive maturation of auditory response selectivity across generations in males anticipates the de novo evolution of song culture, and b) The innately mature and normal response selectivity in females facilitates its maturation in their male companions through social interaction. Under this objective we will study how male and female song-selectivity changes during development across generations, over the course of evolution of a new song culture. Using similar methods as in Objective 1 we will examine the development of general and fine-grained song selectivity across generations in two environments. In the first “deprived environment”, a juvenile bird will imitate the song on an isolate tutor one-to-one, continuing recursively (using the pupil of one generation as the tutor of the next generation) across 3-5 generations until wild-type song emerges. In the second “social environment” same tutoring will take place, starting from a pair of male and female isolates, continuing recursively with 4 birds in each chamber (male + female tutors and male + female pupils). In these birds we will ask the following questions: i) When during development in each generation and across generations general and fine-grained song selectivities emerge, and how do they progress in males in relation to the evolution of a new song culture? Does the presence of female change the rate of vocal and sensory changes? 3 ii) Does the development of female song selectivity affect the imitation of song by males across generations? For this study the birds will be raised, isolated and tutored at CCNY in the Tchernichovski laboratory. fMRI scanning will be conducted at WMC under the direction of Henning Voss. LFP recordings will be carried out in the Tchernichovski laboratory in collaboration with Santosh Helekar. fMRI analysis will be carried out jointly by Henning Voss at WMC and Santosh Helekar at BCM. Lucas Parra at CCNY will develop statistical methods and models to perform combined analysis of multi-generational LFP, fMRI and song data. Background and Significance Many species of oscine songbirds show elaborate vocal communication with remarkable ability of juvenile birds to develop their song repertoire by imitating songs of adult individuals. Except for tropical species, females usually do not sing, but song preferences of females can affect male singing behavior indirectly by sexual selection, such that males with songs that females prefer will reproduce more, followed by epigenetic transmission of their songs to their offspring. In addition, females can affect song structure directly by behaviorally guiding song development of juvenile males (West & King, Nelson & Marler). At the population level, the cultural transmission of songs gives rise to local song dialects. Those dialects can remain stable for many years, and song-dialects are expressed not only at the vocal level but also at the perceptual level, as females prefer local dialect songs upon other dialects (). Although our knowledge about song culture stems from field studies, understanding the role of song imitation and female preferences in the development of song culture requires research in controlled environments that allow us to examine how song culture come about in an “island colony” established by an isolate founder. Songbirds raised in isolation (isolates) develop abnormal songs that differ from wild-type song of the species (Fig 1 a-b). In a recent study (Feher et al 2009) we found that species specific (wild-type) song culture can emerge de novo within 3-4 generations: we raised a juvenile zebra finch with an isolate tutor in a sound proof chamber. When adult, we used that pupil as the tutor of another pupil, continuing recursively across tutoring-generations. The outcome was a gradual modification, across generations, of the isolate song toward a wild-type song structure. The pupils did not invent new syllables. Instead, they modified the abnormal isolate syllables and made them more species typical in each generation (Fig. 2). The approximation of wild-type song in such impoverished environment indicates that even in an isolate bird who sings abnormal song there is some hidden “knowledge” of the wild-type song structure, but it takes a few recursions to emerge. In that sense, the wild-type song culture is a multi-generational phenotype – it is encoded in the founder, but takes a few recursions to emerge. However, the outcome is slightly different when a similar experiment is done in a seminatural colony. Here, a gradual transition toward a wild-type song across generations occurred as well, but birds show some “creativity”, with new syllable types invented and then incorporated into the songs. Figure 1c-e presents a cartoon illustration of how feedback might affect song development at different levels: In an isolate we have only a single level of feedback from the bird’s own song. In a oneto-one tutoring environment we have an additional input from the tutor, and in a colony setting we also have feedback from females and from siblings. Taken together, song development is shaped by input and feedback at different levels, but also by imitation is biased. Here, we propose to study the establishment of song culture by combining two approaches: First, until now we only looked at the outcome of song development in each generation. We now add to this a dynamic investigation of vocal changes during development and across generations, as song culture is established. Second, we are going to combine the vocal measurements with brain imaging that would allow us to assess – both in males and in females – how auditory response patterns to songs are shaped by experience across generations. Although the second aim is complicated, and somewhat risky, we show preliminary results (see XX) that establish the feasibility of using fMRI imaging to gain important knowledge about how experience can shape specific responses to songs. In this section we present a short review about the current knowledge about the development of auditory responses to songs, with emphasis on sensorymotor feedback during development: 4 d c isolate e One-to-one tutoring ♀♀ ♀ Colony Figure 1: isolate songs versus wild type songs. c-e in isolates we have only one level of input feedbac from bird’s own song. The tutor (d) add an additional layer of input and females (c) can add a third one… What can be gained by combined sensory and vocal measurements? At the level of the individual animal, we expect that singing skills and sensory tuning should develop concurrently, so that as auditory responses (and perception) become sensitive to subtle vocal transitions, a better singing performance can be achieved, which in turn further . But of course, sensory-motor feedback cannot explain the development of song culture, which requires no external input: Figure 2 presents the convergence from isolate songs into wild-type songs across a few generations 5 isolate F1 F2 At the population level, we expect to see similar effect at longer time scales. Since zebra finches are Figure 2: Biased imitation across generations giving rise to wild type song… 6 Song development and song culture: Songbirds are used to study vocal learning, vocal perception and production. There are interesting parallels between auditory and vocal pathways in the songbird and human brain [5], and between song development and speech development [6]. Among songbirds used in neurobiological research, zebra finches have been the most extensively studied species. In striking analogy with human speech acquisition, zebra finches require an extended critical period of tutor-dependent learning for the acquisition of song [7, 8]. Each adult male zebra finch produces a learned song that is stereotyped but spectrally and temporally distinct [9]. The learning of song involves tutoring by adult conspecific birds, and requires auditory feedback [10]. As in human speech, two stages of vocal development have been recognized, namely early sensory and late sensorimotor phase (reviewed in [11]). It is hypothesized that an auditory template of the tutor song is memorized by a young bird during the sensory learning phase [12]. This stored template is then used by the pupil to compare it with the sensory representation of its own evolving vocal output until a closely matched stable copy of the tutor song is acquired [13]. The exact nature and location of the auditory template of the tutor song are unclear. Recent experiments have suggested that it might reside in the secondary auditory area NCM [14], but we do not know much about it, nor when and how it becomes accessible to the premotor song system. Development of auditory responses in the forebrain: Auditory responses to song have been recorded in the primary auditory area in the caudal telencephalic region termed as field L, in the higher auditory areas NCM and CM, as well as in forebrain nuclei related to the production and acquisition of song: HVC, LMAN, area X of the lobus parolfactorius, and nucleus interfacialis (Nif) [15-22]. The song system nuclei show song-specific auditory responses with remarkably similar properties and selectivity, with greater responsiveness to BOS compared to conspecific song and to conspecific compared to heterospecific song [23]. Most neurons within HVC show a global synchronous response to BOS [24]. The temporal pattern of song appears to be encoded by combination-sensitive neurons that are activated by temporal sequences of multiple syllables, and structures within the song control pathway appear to be tuned to specific patterns within songs [18-20]. Song-selectivity of auditory neurons in the song nuclei emerges gradually and in parallel with song motor learning [22, 25-28]. The responsiveness to the bird's own song (BOS) is stronger than for the tutor’s song even during early song development [29-31], even when BOS is an abnormal song produced by denervation of the syrinx [29], suggesting that song selectivity might be induced by auditory feedback during vocalization. Selectivity for conspecific song is also seen in the NCM [32]. Song learning experience has a profound effect on the responsiveness of NCM neurons to song stimuli [33]. Similarities in response selectivity seen in the song nuclei also extend to neurons in NCM and CM [34, 35], suggesting that essentially a similar type of perceptually relevant information regarding song is accessed by higher auditory areas, premotor song control nuclei and song learning nuclei. How then do these areas make use of this information in a differential manner to perform their distinct functions? One possibility is that these structures might be differentially responsive during different phases of song development. To find out if this is true we need to compare the responsiveness of these areas simultaneously and at multiple times during development using a global analytical approach. fMRI-BOLD responses to song stimuli: Recently, fMRI based on the blood-oxygenation-leveldependent (BOLD) effect has been developed for songbirds [2, 36]. fMRI as a non-invasive procedure allows for longitudinal within-subject studies, and it does not interfere with subsequent extra- or intracellular recordings. In humans, fMRI is presently the most important neuroimaging technique. The BOLD effect is indirectly related to neuronal activity because increased local energy demand increases capillary blood flow and reduces deoxyhemoglobin levels [37]. Observations in monkeys show that BOLD amplitude increases approximately linearly with the LFP and to a lesser extent with single and multiple unit activity. LFPs have been shown to be better correlated than neuronal spiking activity with BOLD effect. Therefore, LFP recordings can complement fMRI by revealing the neural activations underlying the BOLD response, in addition to elucidating millisecond scale temporal information. fMRI images in turn provide high resolution 3-dimensional spatial information. Preliminary Studies Studies Relevant to Both Objectives of the Project 7 Cultural evolution of song in zebra finches Ofer, please add stuff here fMRI studies in awake birds Our group performed a series of fMRI and event related potential recording experiments in zebra finches over the past three years. We overcame technical hurdles of fMRI in small animals on 3.0 T MRI scanners, involving custom-made radiofrequency (RF) coils [38, 39] and techniques to improve the image quality for echo-planar imaging [40-42]. This knowledge can be transferred readily to the proposed experiments on a 7.0 T animal MRI scanner. A paper describing the first set of results of these experiments has been published in PNAS [2] and two additional papers are under review [43, 44]. Our group works cohesively. We presented our results in several conferences including ISMRM (2006 and 2007), SFN (2006 - 2008), Cold Spring Harbor Workshop (2007), and the IBRO World Congress, Melbourne (2007). Figure 2: Differential topography of activation of auditory areas show stimulus specific responses. (A) Averaged functional activations depending on stimulus (rows) and sagittal slice position (columns). The slice numbers as corresponding to Figure 1A are given at the bottom. (B) Comparison of averaged activation for different stimuli. Positive changes from stimulus 1 to stimulus 2 are shown in red, negative changes in blue. For example, the stimulation with BOS yields more posterior (red area, arrow) and less anterior (blue) activation than the stimulation with TONE. (C) Differences in BOLD response between primary and secondary auditory areas for stimulation with TUT. The clusters presumably correspond to activations in field L (green) and other areas including NCM (red). right left 3 5 We detected highly significant BOLD responses to songs in auditory brain areas (Figure 1), shown by, the maximum intensity projection of significantly active voxels for the whole brain and by the BOLD response time series. Figure 2A shows parasagittal slices with averaged activation clusters. Slices 4 and 5 are from the medial part, and slices 2 and 7 are most lateral. As shown, activation is mostly medial, centered around auditory nuclei field L and NCM. 7 2 X med X lat 1 0 1 1 1 2 1 3 CM L 9 8 NCM Stim 750 ms Figure 3: Local field potential responses to auditory stimuli. Bilateral local field potentials in an adult male to a conspecific song motif. Traces 8 shown are stimulus onset time-locked averages of 256 traces. X med and X lat represent area X medial and lateral portions, respectively. Stimulus-dependent differentiation of sensory BOLD response topography in auditory areas We found strong differences in the distribution of the voxels activated in response to different stimuli. Interestingly, the largest contiguous area of activation was seen with tutor song (TUT) stimulus and the smallest with the unfamiliar conspecific song (CON) stimulus. Figure 2B shows differential profiles for all possible pairs of stimuli in the two medial slices. TUT showed activation that is much more pronounced throughout the activated region when compared with TONE, and greater amount of activation in the central and rostral field L portions when compared with BOS. An example for clustering in the mediocaudal region is given in Figure 2C (See Voss et al., 2007 [2] for details). Discrimination between stimuli in the forebrain as a whole In normal colony birds, the response has been quantified by measuring the time-averaged BOLD A B C Figure 4: Social interactions and song tutoring can shape stimulus-specific patterns of BOLD activation in the forebrain. A) Sound playbacks trigger strong, and non-selective BOLD responses in isolate birds. We see activation in both posterior (NCM, field L and CM cluster) and anterior (probably area X) of the forebrain. In birds who learned songs during development (box-trained and live tutored) we see only posterior forebrain activation, with strong differences (discrimination) across stimuli (n=6 birds per group). (B) Quantification of anterior forebrain (C) Quantification of the posterior auditory cluster. Note that isolates show similar responses across sounds, whereas trained birds show strong NCM response to songs, and weak response to other sounds. response amplitude [2]. The medial parts of the brain show differential selectivity with respect to the stimulus. Stimulus discrimination was strong in the medial part of the brain (a single factor ANOVA test across the stimuli, p=0.0006) but non significant (p=0.5) in the lateral slices. Responses to TONE stimulation were significantly smaller than responses to CON or BOS stimulation. Neural responses in auditory areas and the anterior forebrain region are similar to BOLD responses We have conducted in vivo chronic LFP recordings in NCM, field L, CM and area X in 4 adult birds (Figure 3), showing faithfully reflected stimulus features in LFP responses. Similar observations have been made by others with regard to NCM multiunit responses in acute preparations [14, 45, 46]. The responses suggest right-sided lateralization and are much smaller in area X than in the posterior auditory structures, consistent with the fMRI findings below. Testing for possible effect of Diazepam on bioelectrical responses 9 To test for possible effect of mild sedation with Diazepam, which is necessary for the fMRI studies, we compared auditory evoked surface and LFP responses with and without Diazepam. There was no significant difference in the amplitude or time course, and only slight alterations in shape, between responses before and after sedation (see supplementary figures in Voss et al. [2]). Song tutoring over development can shape the selectivity of auditory responses To study the impact of tutoring on the neural representation of auditory stimuli we have compared BOLD responses in box-trained, live-tutored and untutored adults. The untutored birds were acoustically isolated from other birds and each other during the sensitive period for song learning. The other birds were trained either by playbacks (in isolation) or by live tutors in a colony. We found remarkably widespread and indiscriminate activation of the forebrain in the untutored birds in response to songs, calls and pure tone (Figure 4), with greater spread of activity in auditory areas; but in addition, the song learning area was also activated. In Right Left contrast, activity in box-tutored birds was significantly restricted and highly selective, with little activation of the anterior forebrain (AFP). NCM field Box trained birds showed only a single cluster, AEP surface centered at field L and NCM. In live-tutored birds responses were even more restricted and no consistent activation of AFP could be detected. Stim marker Both area and magnitude of AFP BOLD 750 ms response were significantly higher in isolates Figure 5: Comparison of NCM LFP and surface compared to trained birds. In isolates, AFP AEP. Traces shown are representative averages of responded to a variety of sounds (Figure 4A and 256 trials from a >150 day-old adult zebra finch. The B). We find it remarkable that several minutes of stimulus marker is a square pulse indicating the song playbacks over the entire song duration of the conspecific song motif stimulus used. development were sufficient to nearly eliminate AFP activation. Since AFP has a central role in song imitation, our results could be interpreted as “gating by learning”; namely, that the decreased sensitivity of AFP to sounds might be the neuronal mechanism of closing the sensitive period for song learning. BOLD responses do not mirror spiking activity [47] but correlate well with evoked potentials and with LFPs, reflecting the overall input and postsynaptic potentials across thousands of neurons. Simultaneous measurements of surface auditory evoked potentials (AEP) and LFP in NCM (Figure 5) verified that a significant portion of the AEP stems from an area that shows BOLD activation. Taken together, the above findings suggest that developmental tutoring might cause selective redistribution, restriction, and fine tuning of auditory representation in the forebrain. Tracking the development of auditory responses We have performed chronic LFP recordings in 9 young males during song development (age 58 to 86 days). The electrodes have remained in place in a usable condition for more than 6 weeks. We tested if AFP activation occurs in tutored juvenile birds. Figure 6 shows that on an average there is significant activation of the most lateral part of area X in 39 – 56 day-old male zebra finches trained with a live tutor. LFP recordings show larger area X responses in 58 – 60 day old juvenile birds trained with live tutors compared to live-tutored adults (101 – 1095 day old, Figure 7). The mean power of the area X responses is 3 - 4 fold greater in juveniles (n = 6) compared to adults (n = 7, p < 0.023, left; p < 0.034, right, Student’s unpaired t test) (Figure 8A). The mean power of NCM responses on the other hand is 6 – 7 folds greater in adults than in juveniles (Figure 8B). Recordings in juveniles conducted at 5 successive time points during development show a marked decrease in the mean power of area X responses after the first time point (Figure 8C; one way ANOVA p < 0.002, F = 13.17, left; p < 0.01, F = 8.09, right). In contrast, the mean power of the NCM response shows a (not significant) trend towards an increase at the last two time points (Figure 8D). Thus it appears that males undergo a distinct pattern of maturational changes in their auditory responses to song during development. In the first objective of the present project we will be tracking these maturational changes in greater detail in with fMRI and LFP recordings, and correlating them with the trajectory of development of the song output of each bird. In the second objective we will be determining the progression, if any, of these sensory changes in anticipation of the progressive approximation towards normal pattern of song over 3 – 5 generations. 10 Research Design and Methods 11 WT WT ISO ISO 12 Call2 Call1 Con2 Con1 Tone The facilities and expertise needed for the experiments proposed here are distributed across our three laboratories at Baylor College of Medicine (BCM), Weill Medical College of Cornell University (WMC) and City College of New York (CCNY). For this project zebra finches are being raised at CCNY, and cared for according to IACUC committee animal protocol review guidelines and recommendations. Procedures and Timeline: Based on the variability observed in our preliminary Anterior forebrain Auditory area activation activation results we set the sample size to 10 birds per subgroup. We will test 14 experimental subgroups: 10 in Objective 1 and 4 in Objective 2, and therefore we anticipate that a total of 120 birds will be used in this project. The Tchernichovski Lab will dedicate 10 training boxes to this project, adding up to about 40 developmental experiments per year. All fMRI scanning will be done at WMC by Dr. Voss or his postdoctoral associate with help from Dr. Helekar or a graduate forebrain student in Dr. Tchernichovski’s laboratory. Birds will be transported to WMC from CCNY individually in a sound-proof box by eye the experimenter. lateral medial Figure 6: AFP activation in juvenile tutored birds. Averaged functional BOLD activation maps (color, p < 0.005) in 5 juvenile birds (day 39-56 post hatch). Stimuli: 2 different conspecific songs, 2 different calls, and a control tone. Left Right Adult 4 Left Juvenile Right 5 X X 2 NCM NCM Stim Stim 750 ms 750 ms Figure 7: LFP traces in adult and juvenile male birds. Conspecific song motif responses in a livetutored 3 year-old adult and a 59 day-old juvenile male in training with a live tutor. Birds were recorded in the awake state. Recordings are bilateral from X and NCM. Traces are stimulus time-locked averages of >250 stimulus trials. Note the larger amplitude of the area X responses in the juvenile bird. 13 Transportation will be done by taxicab, as in our pilot project over the last three years. fMRI and LFP recordings will be done in different sets of birds under similar conditions. LFP recordings will be done at A Juveniles Adults C NCM 800 Mean Power ( m V 2) 700 600 500 NCM (right) 400 NCM (left) 300 200 100 da ys 99 /1 05 da ys 86 da ys 76 da ys 69 59 da ys 0 Age (days) B Juveniles Adults D Area X 800 Mean Power (mV2) 700 600 Area X (right) 500 Area X (left) 400 300 200 100 da ys da ys 99 /1 05 86 da ys 76 da ys 69 59 da ys 0 Age (days) Figure 8: Comparison of responses in juveniles and adults, and maturation of area X and NCM responses. A. Mean power (± SEM) of area X responses in 58 – 60 day old juveniles and 101 – 1095 day-old adults. The responses were evoked by a conspecific song motif stimulus as in Figure 10. B. Mean power (± SEM) of the NCM response under the same stimulus conditions as in A. C. Mean power (± SEM) of area X responses at five different developmental time points. The responses were evoked by a conspecific song motif stimulus as in Figure 11. D. Mean power (± SEM) of the NCM response at the same time points and under the same stimulus conditions as in C above. CCNY by a graduate student in collaboration with Dr. Helekar. Drs. Voss and Helekar will analyze the fMRI data at WMC and BCM, respectively. Dr. Parra at CCNY will develop statistical methods and models to analyze multi-generational song, fMRI and LFP data. Under ideal conditions we would perform both fMRI and LFP in the same location. However, this is not possible due to the distribution of equipment, skills and expertise. The principal investigators of this project have been collaborating for 3 years now, including co-mentoring of a PhD student and publishing together. The PIs will continue their joint leadership of this project, sharing equal responsibilities for carrying out the work. Each PI will be administratively, technically, and scientifically responsible for the conduct of his own research and that of the project personnel in his laboratory. The PI’s will make efficient use of their primary expertise, namely: Dr. Tchernichovski will supervise raising and tutoring of birds for the de novo establishment of a song culture, and continuous sound recordings and real-time analysis during song development. fMRI imaging will be conducted by Dr. Voss at WMC. The LFP recordings and fMRI data analysis will be conducted in collaboration with Dr. Helekar. Dr. 14 Parra will be in charge of developing statistical methods. The project will be carried out over 4 years. The first objective will be completed by the end of year 2. Objective 2 will be addressed over the last three years of the project. The data generated by this project will be presented scientific meetings, and published in peerreviewed journals. Procedures, software and materials developed will be freely shared with all interested researchers, students and teachers for research and education. OBJECTIVE 1 We will test the hypothesis that maturation of auditory responses to vocal stimuli during development is experience-dependent in male zebra finches but not in females. We will perform high resolution brain fMRI on sedated zebra finches at several time points during song development in these two groups of birds. We will test for changes in the spatial extent and configuration of the blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) response in all song and auditory nuclei of the forebrain. We will attempt to judge the influence of developmental vascular changes on the BOLD response by measuring the local cerebral blood flow with arterial spin labeling (ASL). To measure changes in the time course of activation we will perform local field potential recordings (LFP) from the same nuclei at corresponding developmental time points in separate sets of birds belong the two groups. Based on our preliminary studies we have already identified the auditory areas caudal medial nidopallium (NCM), field L and caudal mesopallium (CM) and the song learning nucleus area X as potential sites of recording. We will use a common set of conspecific as well as heterospecific vocal stimuli to elicit auditory responses in all birds studied with fMRI and LFPs. We will develop common standardized measures for quantitative comparison of BOLD and LFP responses at different time points, such as the variability of the amplitude, time course and spatial spread across stimuli and across birds. We will investigate whether there are consistent differences in males as opposed to females in developmental time course of auditory response maturation between isolates and birds raised in a semi-natural colony. This is a necessary first step in studying the role of sensory processes in the evolution of a song dialect. This investigation will be performed in a single generation under two environments: birds raised in a semi-natural colony, as opposed to birds raised singly in sound isolation chambers. In previous fMRI studies with 3T scanner we found that blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) responses to vocal stimuli show stimulus-dependent selectivity in adult colony males but not in isolate males. The pattern of response selectivity in females was similar to that in colony males. Here, using a 7T scanner, we hope to obtain higher definition brain images to study to what extent developmental changes in sensory responses to vocalizations contribute to the de novo evolution of a song dialect. We will examine general song selectivity (songs vs. calls) and fine-grained song selectivity (comparing wild type songs, isolate songs and familiar colony songs). Under Objective 1 We will ask the following questions: i) When during song development do general and fine-grained song selectivities emerge, and how do they progress in males? Does individual variability in the progression of song imitation correlate with the development of song preferences? ii) When during development do general and fine-grained song selectivities emerge, and how do they progress in females? Does the timing and magnitude of selectivity depend on the environment (colony vs. isolation)? fMRI procedure: We will develop and use RF coils and methods designed to perform fMRI scanning in juvenile birds. We will perform high resolution brain fMRI on trained and isolated zebra finches during development, and test for changes of the spatial extent and spatial configuration of the blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) response. All MRI scanning will be done in awake birds mildly sedated with Diazepam (60 µl, 1.66 mg/ml in normal saline, i.m.), and restrained in soft plastic tubes. The stimulation paradigm used in all experiments is given under Procedures. Wild type songs, isolate songs, familiar colony songs and calls will be used as stimuli…………………………. LFP procedure: We will perform LFP recordings with chronically implanted electrodes following similar stimulation procedure and at similar time points as in the scanning experiments described above. Electrodes will be implanted 72 hours before the first presentation of the stimulus and recording of its response. These experiments will also tell us whether the changes in BOLD response do indeed mirror changes in neural activity, rather than other parameters such as tissue perfusion, blood flow and 15 oxygenation of the hemoglobin, which are also likely to change during development. Measuring the BOLD response and the LFP response under similar conditions, and quantifying the correlation between the two, enables us to assess whether both change in similar ways with development. If nonneuronal parameters are responsible for the changes we see over development then we would not see corresponding correlated changes in LFP. Moreover, if the non-neuronal parameters change simply during the course of brain development then we would see these changes in both tutored birds and isolates. Alternatively, neural changes will be dependent on auditory experience, and would be reflected in differences between tutored birds and isolates. Bilateral LFP responses will be recorded in the auditory areas NCM, field L and CM, and the song learning nucleus area X. As shown in the preliminary studies, these areas show strong BOLD responses to auditory stimuli. fMRI would also reveal any activity that might be evident in limbic or other structures at any stage during development. If there is consistent activation of the song nucleus HVC in the awake mildly sedated state, then we will also perform LFP recordings from this nucleus. We will use a high resolution anatomical MRI scan and the new MRI atlas [48] to pin-point the location of the tip of the LFP electrode on the zebra finch stereotaxic atlas. The anatomical location of the tip will be kept constant across birds. The chronic LFP recordings will be done, as described below, under fully alert unrestrained conditions as well as after mild sedation and restraint, as in the fMRI scanning procedure. Testing for developmental vascular changes: To separate neurophysiological changes as visible in the BOLD response from vascular changes, we will perform arterial spin labeling (ASL) imaging at all time points and in all groups of birds to measure regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF). An ASL pulse sequence is now provided within our agreement with Bruker Biospin. From forepaw-stimulation experiments in rats it is known that the BOLD response is tightly correlated with rCBF, but the activated BOLD areas may be larger than areas with increased rCBF [49]. Whereas we cannot measure the cerebral metabolic rate of oxygen directly here (which would be the ultimate goal), we will relate the changes in relative BOLD response intensity during ontogeny to the accompanying changes in rCBF. Accuracy of song imitation: To assess the progress and success of vocal imitation, the vocalizations of experimental birds will be recorded at the same time points at which neural measurements are performed. The similarity between the bird’s song at each of these points and the tutor’s song (in the tutored groups) will be measured using Sound Analysis, as described elsewhere [50]. Imitation success will be correlated with the neural findings. Data analysis and anticipated results Cultural Evolution Ofer, please add here fMRI and LFP Changes We will measure the mean peak amplitude and mean area of significant BOLD activation. We will also compare the location(s) of the centroid(s) of maximum activation and the spatial configuration of activation, and perform cluster analyses on the BOLD response time traces to analyze sub-structures of activated areas. For LFP responses we will measure the root mean square (RMS) amplitude and duration of the responses elicited at each location or area. The shape of the responses will be compared in a pair-wise manner using cross-correlation analysis. For BOLD and LFP responses these parameters will be computed from the stimulus time-locked averaged traces. To make left-right comparisons, left-right ratio of each quantified parameter under each condition will be used as a determinant of the extent of lateralization. Both short and long term time-dependent changes would be noteworthy from the standpoint of response plasticity and sensory learning during development. For LFP responses we will measure the RMS amplitude/power and duration, and compare shapes by cross-correlation. Each BOLD and bioelectrical response parameter will be plotted with respect to time during development. This will allow us to observe the time course of changes due to learning of song, and correlate any sharp transformations of responses with steps in song learning at corresponding time points. Based in part by observations in our preliminary studies we hope to confirm the following: a) There would be an initial over-activation of NCM/field L/CM region in response to TUT in terms of mean peak amplitude/power and area of significant activation in the early stages of song learning. b) There would be a similar robust activation of the area X at these early stages. c) Both types of activity would show a progressive reduction over the course of song learning either as a gradual decrement or a sharp fall at the time of the closure of the sensitive period. d) The emergence of auditory representation of the 16 tutor’s song would be captured by consistency in some measure of the LFP response (such as the consistency of the shape of the response ascertained by cross-correlation analysis) and of the BOLD response (such as a fixed discrete pattern of significant activation in NCM). This, if true, would support the long-standing notion in the birdsong field that the establishment of a fixed auditory representation of the tutor’s song precedes vocal development. e) A general decrease in sensitivity and an increase in selectivity of the NCM/field L/CM response to non-vocal control sound combined with deactivation of X might also be observed, with contrasting findings in isolates. f) Accuracy of imitation varies among individuals, and we expect to see such variability among our tutored birds. We expect to find a correlation between imitation success and the observed changes in auditory responses. Specifically, we expect better tuning and maturation (as described above) of activation of auditory areas and of area X in birds achieving good imitation. We will weigh our results against the possibility that observed ontogenic changes are caused by changes in neuro-vascular coupling by measuring changes of regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) using arterial spin labeling (ASL) in response to stimulation with a vocal sound. For constant neuro-vascular coupling we would not expect a significant change of the BOLD/rCBF ratio. If we observe significant changes during certain phases of development, we will include this information into the interpretation of our results. In addition, we will generate correlation maps between the BOLD and rCBF signals and monitor local changes during development. To keep experimental efforts feasible, ASL experiments will be performed on only one stimulus (a standard female call) per bird, and the analysis will be restricted to primary and secondary auditory areas. OBJECTIVE 2 We propose to test the following two hypotheses: a) Progressive maturation of auditory response selectivity across generations in males anticipates the de novo evolution of song culture, and b) The innately mature and normal response selectivity in females facilitates its maturation in their male companions through social interaction. Under this objective we will study how male and female song-selectivity changes during development across generations, over the course of evolution of a new song culture. Using similar methods as in Objective 1 we will examine the development of general and fine-grained song selectivity across generations in two environments. In the first “deprived environment”, a juvenile bird will imitate the song on an isolate tutor one-to-one, continuing recursively (using the pupil of one generation as the tutor of the next generation) across 3-5 generations until wild-type song emerges. In the second “social environment” same tutoring will take place, starting from a pair of male and female isolates, continuing recursively with 4 birds in each chamber (male + female tutors and male + female pupils). In these birds we will ask the following questions: iii) When during development in each generation and across generations general and fine-grained song selectivities emerge, and how do they progress in males in relation to the evolution of a new song culture? Does the presence of female change the rate of vocal and sensory changes? iv) Does the development of female song selectivity affect the imitation of song by males across generations? Bird’s vocalizations produced during song learning will be continuously recorded and analyzed in real time using a modified version of the Sound Analysis system developed in the Tchernichovski laboratory [50, 51]. In each of the two experimental groups (box-trained and isolates) we will perform an initial fMRI scan for the tutor’s song and a non-vocal sound at day 41. We recognize that there are individual differences in the pace of song development. We therefore propose to scan the birds, subsequently, both at pre-specified time points in development (once weekly scans until day 90) and at time points corresponding to shifts in vocal patterns. While we might not be able to determine whether any particular sensory change precedes a vocal shift or vice versa, the LFP and fMRI measurements at pre-specified time points, independent of vocal changes, would allow us to plot developmental trajectories of auditory responses and compare those trajectories with the trajectories of vocal changes that are being continuously recorded. In this way we would be able to determine if any of the major events in song development such as the emergence of syllable types, formation of song motifs, and crystallization, are preceded or succeeded by significant changes in auditory responses. Once such correlations are discovered, causal relationship between the two events could be tested in a subsequent project. In separate groups of birds we will perform LFP recordings at corresponding time points…………………………….. 17 Data analysis and anticipated results Cultural Evolution Ofer, please add here fMRI and LFP Changes We will investigate any changes in the BOLD parameters side by side with the developing vocalized precursors for the tutor’s song components (or for the crystallized song in isolates). To confirm that these changes also occur at the same time in the underlying bioelectrical response, we will perform measurements on LFPs as in Objective 1………………… Problems and Alternative Strategies Based on our experience, we do not anticipate any serious technical problems with the fMRI experiments. Regarding in vivo electrophysiology, we have already performed preliminary experiments using the proposed methods in adult and juvenile birds. We propose to record LFPs only after 40 days post-hatch because of the difficulty of implanting chronic electrodes due to the softness and immaturity of the skull at earlier ages. If we encounter difficulty in conducting chronic recordings before 60 days post-hatch, then we will perform acute LFP recordings at these earlier time points in restrained birds using standard procedures. We have successfully performed chronic epidural surface electroencephalographic and LFP recordings around 45 – 55 days of age. Regarding difficulties in comparing differences in activation between different salient stimuli with a global measure such as fMRI, we have already demonstrated that we can measure response selectivity and differential topography using fMRI in our recent PNAS paper. In addition, we are using both fMRI and LFP recordings. With LFP, response selectivity is evident in the amplitude and shape of the waveform elicited by each stimulus. ……………. Procedures Protocol for stimulation: All stimuli (digitally normalized to RMS amplitude) used will be delivered at ~70 dB sound pressure level (exceeding the 7T scanner background noise of ~65 dB). Furthermore, we will construct a sound isolation box to reduce the background noise. The stimulation protocol will be as described in Voss et al., 2007 [2]. The same stimuli will be used for the LFP recordings, in which we will also present stimuli at the normal sound intensity of vocalizations, i.e. ~70 dB sound pressure level, in the presence of a pre-recorded background noise (~65 dB) to simulate the scanning conditions. As far as possible, stimuli of comparable durations will be chosen, and parameters such as degree of familiarity and saliency will be kept uniform across all birds. MRI scanning procedure: (Henning, please add here) Images will be acquired on a 7T animal MRI scanner (Bruker BioSpec) with parallel acquisition hardware. We will use a variety of MRI pulse sequences to acquire anatomical and functional (BOLD and rCBF) images of the brain, starting with EPI. All necessary pulse sequences are provided with our scanner. The choice of scanning parameters will be guided by our experience on a 3T MRI scanner and by literature values from songbird fMRI experiments on 7T MRI scanners. Final optimized parameters will be determined in pilot experiments. As far as possible, this optimization will be performed on phantoms. Each scanning sessions will take about 1 hr per bird. In addition, we will image anatomical changes with high-resolution contrast-enhanced anatomical imaging [52], and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) [53] in excised brains at representative time points. MRI postprocessing: The images will be despiked and motion corrected using AFNI and further processed using in-house software written in MATLAB, and ParaVision: Data will be smoothed slicewise with a 2D Gaussian filter, detrended, and temporally smoothed. Statistical significance of the BOLD and rCBF response will be defined voxel-wise with general linear modeling, including motion parameters derived from the motion correction as nuisance parameter, and then corrected for multiple testing using Gaussian random field theory. The relative strength of the BOLD effect and rCBF changes will be measured in the significantly activated areas by the percentage of signal variability in response to stimulation against overall signal intensity. The relative BOLD signal will be related to the relative rCBF signal by comparing the BOLD/rCBF ratio, averaged over all significant voxels in the brain, for all time points. In addition, correlation maps between BOLD and rCBF will be computed to estimate possible local changes of neuro-vascular coupling. We will also measure the volume of activation in selected regions. Non-isotropic shifts in anatomy during development will be analyzed by examining 18 changes in the shape of activated regions within the brain. All the statistical parametric maps (SPMs) will finally be registered to a brain template for the corresponding developmental time point, and group statistics will be performed, using standard n-way ANOVA tests for groups of birds, stimuli, and time points. The anatomical resolution on the new 7T animal scanner that we will use will be higher than in our present experiment. More importantly, we expect spatial distortions of the functional data versus the anatomy to be reduced, and we will be able to overlay the functional activations to the anatomies of individual brains at each time point. Color fiber direction maps from DTI images will provide additional information about location of fiber tracts and song nuclei which may not be evident from the anatomical images alone. Procedure for in vivo electrophysiology: The chronic depth electrodes will be implanted bilaterally under general anesthesia (xylazine - 50 mg/kg body weight and ketamine - 25 mg/kg body weight, intramuscular). The LFP Teflon-coated blunt tungsten electrodes with an impedance of 100 – 500 KΩ will be inserted into holes made in the skull at pre-specified stereotaxic coordinates (uniform across all birds) in NCM, field L, CM and area X specified by the Nixdorf and Bischof stereotaxic atlas (Nixdorf, B. and Bischof, H.–J. (1979) unpublished, Bielefeld) aligned with a MRI atlas ([48], and using a high resolution anatomical MRI scan. Two bilateral gold-plated pin reference electrodes will be implanted over the cerebellum at stereotaxic coordinates AP – -2.0, Lat – 1.3. The active and reference electrodes will be connected to the lead wires with specially designed clamp connectors. The leads will be connected to a 16-channel differential amplifier (AM-Systems, Inc). Digitally normalized stimuli will be delivered at 70 dB maximum sound pressure levels. Recordings will be conducted while the bird is awake or mildly sedated with diazepam and restrained in a soft plastic tube inside a sound-attenuated box. The signals will be low-pass filtered at 10 kHz, digitized at 5 kHz, and analyzed with MATLABbased programs developed by us. Auditory evoked LFPs will be obtained by stimulus time-locked averaging of >250 artifact-free epochs. Raising and training of zebra Finches: Ofer, please add here Brain histology: To determine the location of electrodes we will mark their locations by passing an electrolytic lesion current under deep terminal xylazine/ketamine general anesthesia. We will then perfuse and remove the brain to perform histology on cryosections using standard procedures. Coronal or sagittal sections will be stained with Nissl’s stain or with cytochrome C oxidase stain (see Braun et al. [54]). Cytochrome C oxidase histology will also be carried out in a few separate birds at ages corresponding to the fMRI scanning time points in development, to track developmental anatomical changes in auditory and song structures. This will enable us to properly interpret the locations and spatial extents of BOLD activation patterns at those time points. Analysis of birdsong: Ofer, please add here Statistical Analysis Lucas, please add here Broader Impact Educational Significance: This project provides a unique opportunity for students and postdoctoral fellows to engage in multidisciplinary research in animals, involving behavioral analysis, in vivo electrophysiology and functional magnetic resonance imaging. We will use this innovative multipronged and quantitative analytical approach to recruit students and postdoctoral researchers in our laboratories. The Tchernichovski laboratory provides research opportunities for graduate and undergraduate students from CCNY, City University of New York (CUNY). CCNY is a designated Research Center in Minority Institution (RCMI) and is dedicated to supporting and promoting the scientific careers of minority students. Its campus is located in Harlem, New York, and the college has a large proportion of minority students. Graduate students from the Speech-Language-Hearing Department of CUNY also have an opportunity to conduct research in the lab, as Dr. Tchernichovski is a faculty member of that program as well as Biology. This allows students with a background in human clinical research (speech pathology) to conduct research on an animal model of vocal learning. Dr. Tchernichovski teaches an Animal Behavior course to both graduates and undergraduates at CCNY. The course includes a section on mechanisms of learning in songbirds. A number of students have sought to conduct research in the lab (for independent study credits) after being introduced to the subject of birdsong neurobiology in the course. The graduate student in the Tchernichovski lab that will be involved with the present project will be trained to combine behavioral analysis with imaging. She/he 19 will be mentored by Dr. Tchernichovski, and will interact with other systems neuroscience faculty in our neuroscience group. She/he will participate in our weekly neuroscience seminars, and present her work at Society for Neuroscience annual meetings. She/he will also visit the Voss lab on a regular basis, and learn fMRI scanning and analysis from Dr. Voss. The Voss laboratory is integrated into an imaging core facility that provides research opportunities and support for graduate students and residents from the WMC and affiliated research institutions interested in biomedical imaging. Dr. Voss is in charge of animal magnetic resonance imaging, and will be involved in training students and postdoctoral researchers involved in this research. The Helekar laboratory has provided research internships to undergraduate students of University of Houston, University of Texas at Austin and Rice University, as well as research assistantships to science graduates interested in academic careers in science and medicine. BCM Neuroscience graduate students have the opportunity to conduct research rotations and doctoral thesis research in his laboratory. The laboratory has conducted short-term (1 - 3 months) research training for high school, undergraduate and medical students. He has held a laboratory teaching activity on songbird models of speech for Speech Pathology graduate students of University of Houston. Being an adjunct faculty member of the Biology Department of Texas A & M University, Dr. Helekar serves on the thesis committee of a graduate student studying songbird molecular neurobiology. Training minority students – The Tchernichovski laboratory has already trained a minority PhD student and several undergraduate students. For this project, CCNY will allocate resources to provide at least one undergraduate minority student with a stipend for participation in it. The student will be taught the behavioral data collection and analysis methods used in the lab, and apply them under the supervision of a PhD student and Dr. Tchernichovski. Currently, WMC boasts a record of graduating over 400 minority physicians with numbers still rising. In addition, 18% (among the highest in the nation) of its student body consists of underrepresented minorities. From this pool, Dr. Voss will try to attract medical students and postdoctoral fellows to work with him on the present project. Students at BCM and universities in the Houston area have a substantial minority representation, and Dr. Helekar will try to recruit students from this pool as undergraduate, graduate and postgraduate researchers in the laboratory. Extending across disciplines – Dr. Tchernichovski is a member of both the CUNY Neuroscience program, and the CUNY Speech and Hearing programs. We plan to continue sharing students across these disciplines to relate our findings more firmly to those of speech pathology and speech development in human infants. With more than 20 current user groups, the imaging core facility that houses Dr. Voss’ lab will serve, as before, a pool of users from the basic and cognitive neurosciences and various medical specialties, ensuring a continuous exchange of ideas across disciplines. The imaging core is at present rapidly expanding, with a new 7.0 T animal MRI scanner and a second 3.0 T human MRI scanner, attracting new groups from various institutions, including biologists from Rockefeller University. Dr. Helekar’s laboratory will continue to offer as before opportunities for research to clinical residents and fellows, as well as for collaborative projects on songbird models of speech and learning disorders for clinical researchers in the BCM Neurology, Otolaryngology and Psychiatry departments. Dissemination of Research Findings: Our (Tchernichovski, Voss and Helekar) research involving fMRI was featured on the NOVA Science Now program of the Public Broadcasting Service (http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/nova/sciencenow/0304/01.html) and the Science Friday news report of National Public Radio (http://www.sciencefriday.com/news/072407/finch072407.html). Dr. Tchernichovski continues to answer questions from students and interested readers regarding our work on the NOVA Science Now website. Our work also appeared in the German weekly newspaper “Die Zeit” (http://www.zeit.de/2008/41/N-Stottervoegel). We will continue to disseminate the findings of our project through local, national and international media in this manner. We will also explain our work and our results to the general public through our respective institutional websites. Community Outreach: The Tchernichovski lab has participated, and will continue to participate in a program with a local middle school which pairs middle school students with a graduate student mentor over the course of the school year for a science fair project. The goal of the program is to introduce the middle school students to scientific research and methods and promote interest in science. Dr. Voss gives an annual lecture within an NIH sponsored summer school for high school students interested in neuroscience. Dr. Helekar has given and plans to give talks to high school students who are members 20 of the National Science Honor Society about careers in science, his own experiences as a neuroscientist, and songbird research related to the present proposal. Two students have used his help and the resources in his laboratory to develop their science fair projects in the past. He will continue to carry out such activities in the community. Overall Future Plans: Our overall plan for the future for our research to have a broader impact is therefore as follows: Procedures, software and materials developed under the present project will be freely shared with all interested researchers, students and teachers for research and educational purposes. All three investigators will strive: 1) To train graduate students through laboratory research rotations and doctoral thesis work, 2) To provide scientific and research training to undergraduate students and opportunities for independent summer research, 3) To serve on the thesis committees of graduate students, 4) To disseminate our work through electronic and print media, and 5) To give science lectures, and provide practical science education and student research experience to middle school and high school students in the community. Postdoctoral Mentoring/Training The graduate student in the Helekar laboratory will receive a well-rounded training and experience in songbird neurophysiology. He/she will be trained to perform animal surgery and implant local field potential recording (LFP) electrodes in the brain. He/she will be taught to raise and tutor songbirds, perform song and LFP recordings, and to carry out statistical analysis of data. He/she will also gain experience in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in songbirds, and analyze fMRI data. He/she will have to present his/her work at a weekly lab meeting, and participate in a weekly journal club that requires each person to present a research paper about once every two months for general discussion. He/she will have to attend a departmental neuroscience seminar every week. He/she will also have opportunities to attend an annual departmental Neuroscience retreat and a Society for Neuroscience annual meeting to present his/her work. This will enable him/her to interact with scientists in his/her area, as well as with those working in other areas of research on the brain. The postdoctoral fellow in the Voss laboratory will have the opportunity to interact with several neuroscience and preclinical research groups with various backgrounds that use the imaging core facility. He/she will have the opportunity to present at domestic scientific conferences, and to attend scientific meetings and presentations at WMC and its two affiliated institutions, including Rockefeller University. WMC offers regular grant preparation, scientific publishing, and mentoring workshops. The postdoc program at WMC offers regular career pathway seminars. We will also ask the postdoc to present a lecture about functional MRI within our biomedical imaging teaching program and to present progress reports within our internal lab meetings. References 1. Feher, O., et al., De novo establishment of wild-type song culture in the zebra finch. Nature, 2009. 459(7246): p. 564-8. 2. Voss, H.U., et al., Functional MRI of the zebra finch brain during song stimulation suggests a lateralized response topography. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 2007. 104(25): p. 10667-72. 3. Maul, K.K., et al., Early song-tutoring experience is required to shape auditory responses in zebra finch males but not in females. in review, 2009. 4. Voss, H.U., D. Salgado-Commissariat, and S.A. Helekar, Altered BOLD response to conspecific song in zebra finches with stuttered syllables. in review, 2009. 5. Haesler, S., et al., FoxP2 expression in avian vocal learners and non-learners. J Neurosci, 2004. 24(13): p. 3164-75. 6. Doupe, A. and P. Kuhl, Birdsong and human speech: common themes and mechanisms. Annu Rev Neurosci, 1999. 22: p. 567-631. 7. Immelmann, K., Song development in the zebra finch and other estrilid finches, in Bird Vocalizations, R.A. Hinde, Editor. 1969, Cambridge University Press: Cambridge. p. 61-77. 8. Doupe, A.J. and P.K. Kuhl, Birdsong and human speech: common themes and mechanisms. Annu Rev Neurosci, 1999. 22: p. 567-631. 9. Sossinka, R. and J. Bohner, Song types in the zebra finch Poephilia guttata castanotis. Zietschrift für Tierpsychologie, 1980. 53: p. 123-132. 10. Brainard, M.S. and A.J. Doupe, Interruption of a basal ganglia-forebrain circuit prevents plasticity of learned vocalizations. Nature, 2000. 404(6779): p. 762-6. 21 11. Terpstra, N.J., et al., Localized brain activation specific to auditory memory in a female songbird. J Comp Neurol, 2006. 494(5): p. 784-91. 12. Konishi, M. and F. Nottebohm, Experimental studies in the ontogeny of avian vocalizations., in Bird Vocalizations, R.A. Hinde, Editor. 1969, Cambridge University Press: Cambridge. p. 29-48. 13. Tchernichovski, O., et al., Dynamics of the vocal imitation process: how a zebra finch learns its song. Science, 2001. 291(5513): p. 2564-9. 14. Phan, M.L., C.L. Pytte, and D.S. Vicario, Early auditory experience generates long-lasting memories that may subserve vocal learning in songbirds. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 2006. 103(4): p. 1088-93. 15. Bonke, B., D. Bonke, and H. Scheich, Connectivity of the auditory forebrain nuclei in the guinea fowl (Numida meleagris). Cell Tissue Res, 1979. 200(1): p. 101-21. 16. Muller, C. and H. Leppelsack, Feature extraction and tonotopic organization in the avian auditory forebrain. Exp Brain Res, 1985. 59(3): p. 587-99. 17. Williams, H. and F. Nottebohm, Auditory responses in avian vocal motor neurons: a motor theory for song perception in birds. Science, 1985. 229(4710): p. 279-82. 18. Margoliash, D., Preference for autogenous song by auditory neurons in a song system nucleus of the white-crowned sparrow. J Neurosci, 1986. 6(6): p. 1643-61. 19. Doupe, A. and M. Konishi, Song-selective auditory circuits in the vocal control system of the zebra finch. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 1991. 88(24): p. 11339-43. 20. Margoliash, D. and E. Fortune, Temporal and harmonic combination-sensitive neurons in the zebra finch's HVc. J Neurosci, 1992. 12(11): p. 4309-26. 21. Vicario, D. and K. Yohay, Song-selective auditory input to a forebrain vocal control nucleus in the zebra finch. J Neurobiol, 1993. 24(4): p. 488-505. 22. Doupe, A., Song- and order-selective neurons in the songbird anterior forebrain and their emergence during vocal development. J Neurosci, 1997. 17(3): p. 1147-67. 23. Margoliash, D., Functional organization of forebrain pathways for song production and perception. J Neurobiol, 1997. 33(5): p. 671-93. 24. Sutter, M. and D. Margoliash, Global synchronous response to autogenous song in zebra finch HVc. J Neurophysiol, 1994. 72(5): p. 2105-23. 25. Doupe, A. and M. Solis, Song- and order-selective neurons develop in the songbird anterior forebrain during vocal learning. J Neurobiol, 1997. 33(5): p. 694-709. 26. Solis, M. and A. Doupe, Anterior forebrain neurons develop selectivity by an intermediate stage of birdsong learning. J Neurosci, 1997. 17(16): p. 6447-62. 27. Volman, S., Development of neural selectivity for birdsong during vocal learning. J Neurosci, 1993. 13(11): p. 4737-47. 28. Amin, N., A. Doupe, and F.E. Theunissen, Development of selectivity for natural sounds in the songbird auditory forebrain. J Neurophysiol, 2007. 97(5): p. 3517-31. 29. Solis, M.M. and A.J. Doupe, Compromised neural selectivity for song in birds with impaired sensorimotor learning. Neuron, 2000. 25(1): p. 109-21. 30. Solis, M.M. and A.J. Doupe, Anterior forebrain neurons develop selectivity by an intermediate stage of birdsong learning. J Neurosci, 1997. 17(16): p. 6447-62. 31. Solis, M. and A. Doupe, Compromised neural selectivity for song in birds with impaired sensorimotor learning [see comments]. Neuron, 2000. 25(1): p. 109-21. 32. Stripling, R., S. Volman, and D. Clayton, Response modulation in the zebra finch neostriatum: relationship to nuclear gene regulation. J Neurosci, 1997. 17(10): p. 3883-93. 33. Bolhuis, J.J., et al., Localized neuronal activation in the zebra finch brain is related to the strength of song learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 2000. 97(5): p. 2282-5. 34. Theunissen, F.E. and S.S. Shaevitz, Auditory processing of vocal sounds in birds. Curr Opin Neurobiol, 2006. 16(4): p. 400-7. 35. Stripling, R., A.A. Kruse, and D.F. Clayton, Development of song responses in the zebra finch caudomedial neostriatum: role of genomic and electrophysiological activities. J Neurobiol, 2001. 48(3): p. 163-80. 36. Van Meir, V., et al., Spatiotemporal properties of the BOLD response in the songbirds' auditory circuit during a variety of listening tasks. Neuroimage, 2005. 25(4): p. 1242-55. 37. Logothetis, N.K. and B.A. Wandell, Interpreting the BOLD signal. Annu Rev Physiol, 2004. 66: p. 735-69. 22 38. Ballon, D. and H.U. Voss. High-Pass Two-Dimensional Ladder Network Resonators. in Proc.of the 13th Scientific Meeting of ISMRM. 2005. 39. Voss, H.U. and D.J. Ballon, High-pass two-dimensional ladder network resonators for magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng, 2006. 53(12 Pt 2): p. 2590-3. 40. Voss, H.U., et al. Diffusion tensor imaging at 3T with strongly reduced geometric and intensity distortions. in Proc Intl Soc Mag Reson Med. 2005. 41. Voss, H.U., et al. Correction of Spatial and Intensity Distortions in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. in Proc Intl Soc Mag Reson Med. 2005. 42. Voss, H.U., et al. Functional MRI of the songbird zebra finch at 3 Tesla. in Proc Intl Soc Mag Reson Med 2006. 43. Voss, H.U., D. Salgado-Commissariat, and S.A. Helekar, Altered Bold Sensitivity To Conspecific Song In Zebra Finches With Stuttered Syllables. Submitted preprint, 2008. 44. Maul, K.K., et al., Early song-tutoring experience is required to shape auditory responses in zebra finch males but not in females. Submitted preprint, 2008. 45. Chew, S.J., et al., Decrements in auditory responses to a repeated conspecific song are longlasting and require two periods of protein synthesis in the songbird forebrain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 1995. 92(8): p. 3406-10. 46. Terleph, T.A., C.V. Mello, and D.S. Vicario, Auditory topography and temporal response dynamics of canary caudal telencephalon. J Neurobiol, 2006. 66(3): p. 281-92. 47. Logothetis, N.K., et al., Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature, 2001. 412(6843): p. 150-7. 48. Poirier, C., et al., A three-dimensional MRI atlas of the zebra finch brain in stereotaxic coordinates. Neuroimage, 2008. 41(1): p. 1-6. 49. Shen, Q., H. Ren, and T.Q. Duong, CBF, BOLD, CBV, and CMRO(2) fMRI signal temporal dynamics at 500-msec resolution. J Magn Reson Imaging, 2008. 27(3): p. 599-606. 50. Tchernichovski, O., et al., A procedure for an automated measurement of song similarity. Anim Behav, 2000. 59: p. 1167-1176. 51. Deregnaucourt, S., et al., How sleep affects the developmental learning of bird song. Nature, 2005. 433(7027): p. 710-6. 52. Bath, K.G., et al., Quantitative intact specimen magnetic resonance microscopy at 3.0 Tesla, Magnetic Resonance Imaging. in press, 2009. 53. De Groof, G., et al., In vivo diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) of brain subdivisions and vocal pathways in songbirds. Neuroimage, 2006. 29(3): p. 754-63. 54. Braun, K., et al., Distribution of parvalbumin, cytochrome oxidase activity and 14C-2-deoxyglucose uptake in the brain of the zebra finch I. Auditory and vocal motor systems. Cell Tissue Res, 1985. 240: p. 101-115. 23