The orthographic distinctiveness effect on direct and indirect tests of memory: delineating the awareness and processing requirements Lisa Geraci and Suparna Rajaram Journal of Memory and Language 47 (2002) 273-291 Summary: Orthographically distinctive words are recalled better than common words, except when indirectly tested. This may suggest that there needs to be some sort of test awareness to create the orthographical distinctiveness effect. Leder, Helmut & Carbon, Claus-Christian (2005). When context hinders! learn-test compatibility in face recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A. Volume 58, Number 2 This examines how people perceive faces and whether they see parts of the face explicity or holistic. They examined if learning parts of the face would carry over to knowing the entire face. They found that after learning parts of the face, subjects were not able to carry this learning over when the features were presented in the full face. They concluded that this shows it is hard to ignore irrelevant parts in faces. A holistic account of the own-race effect in face recognition: Evidence from a cross-cultural study Cognition [0010-0277] Tanaka yr: 2004 vol: 93 iss: 1 pg: B1 Abstract A robust finding in the cross-cultural research is that people's memories for faces of their own race are superior to their memories for other-race faces. However, the mechanisms underlying the own-race effect have not been well defined. In this study, a holistic explanation was examined in which Caucasian and Asian participants were asked to recognize features of Caucasian and Asian faces presented in isolation and in the whole face. The main finding was that Caucasian participants recognized own-race faces more holistically than Asian faces whereas Asian participants demonstrated holistic recognition for both own-race and other-race faces. The differences in holistic recognition between Caucasian and Asian participants mirrored differences in their relative experience with own-race and other-race faces. These results suggest that the own-race effect may arise from the holistic recognition of faces from a highly familiar racial group. 1) The false memory and the mirror effects: The role of familiarity and backward association in creating false recollections David Anaki, Yifat Faran, Dorit Ben-Shalom, and Avishai Henik aRotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care, 3560 Bathurst Street, Toronto, Ont., Canada M6A 2E1 This experiment examined whether false memory for non-presented lures would be influenced by the lure’s familiarity. Their results revealed that false memory levels for low familiarity lures were higher than that for high familiarity lures, but only when the backward association strength between the presented list’s words and the lure was high. In contrast, higher false alarms were observed for high frequency unrelated distractors. They relate their results to the false memory effect, and suggest an activation/monitoring account of the effect, according to which non-presented lures are activated during encoding. 2) Sensitivity reductions in false recognition: A measure of false memories with stronger theoretical implications Westerberg, Carmen E; Marsolek, Chad J, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition. Vol 29(5), Sep 2003, pp. 747-759 This study states that signal detection analyses of recognition memory indicate that a bias to respond "old" is large for critical words that are centrally related with previously encoded word lists, is small for words that are less related, and is not observed for unrelated words. Also, recognition sensitivity has not been previously shown to differ between those conditions. In this study, they conduct three experiments, where criticalword sensitivity was lower than sensitivity for other word types, but related-word sensitivity was not lower than sensitivity for unrelated words. Literature: Glanzer, M., Hilford, A., Kim, K. (2004). Six Regularities of Source Recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. Vol. 30, 6:1176-1195. Summary: The authors/experimentors of this experiment looked at previous research that suggests source-recognition memory can be woven into a signal detection model covering source and item recognition. The authors used 5 experiments involving variables that have learned, documented effects on item recognition as seen in previous studied into this topic. The authors found 6 source-recognition premises that support the enhanced model involving source-recognition, including one premise that states a correlation between item and source recognition. The authors state that “any learning variable that increases item recognition increases source recognition” as this correlation between recognition. The study generally offers an alternative explanation for source recognition that accounts for most of the variance (99%) of the five experiments the authors used to develop the enhanced model with the advantage of unifying item and source recognition where previous studies were not able to join these recognitions under the same structure. The article focuses on recognition memory and what certain cues are used in the retrieval of certain information. The researchers of this experiment hypothesized that item and contextual cues are used in retrieving information. Another experiment on recognition hypothesized that the history of the item is an important cue that affects recognition memory. The results of this experiment conclude that all types of cues, item, context, and memory affect recognition. Criss, Amy H., Shiffrin, Richard M. (2004) Context Noise and Item Noise Jointly Determine Recognition Memory; A Comment on Dennis and Humphreys (2001). Psychological Review. Pp. 800-807. Retrieved from PsychInfo Database (AN: 0033295X) Malmberg, K.J., Nelson, T.O. The word frequency effect for recognition memory and the elevated-attention hypothesis. Memory&Cognition. 2003. 31(1), 35-43 According to the article low-frequency (LF) words are better recognized than high-frequency (HF) words since LF words attract more attention than do HF words. Then, they hypothesized that more attentional resources are allocated to Lf words than to HF words only during the early phase of encoding and that the allocation of attentional resources during the late phase of encoding is not greater for LF words than HF words. 1: J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1992 Mar;18(2):379-90. Remembering pictures: pleasure and arousal in memory. Bradley MM, Greenwald MK, Petry MC, Lang PJ. Health Science Center, University of Florida, Gainesville 32610. Incidental memory performance for pictures that varied along the affective dimensions of pleasantness and arousal was assessed. For both an immediate and delayed (1 year later) free-recall task, only the arousal dimension had a stable effect on memory performance: Pictures rated as highly arousing were remembered better than low-arousal stimuli. This effect was corroborated in a speeded recognition test, in which high-arousal materials encoded earlier in the experiment produced faster reaction times than their low-arousal counterparts. Pleasantness affected reaction time decisions only for pictures not encoded earlier. These results suggest that whereas both the dimensions of pleasantness and arousal are processed at initial encoding, long-term memory performance is mainly affected by arousal. PMID: 1532823 [PubMed - indexed for MEDLINE]