Journal of Applied Psychology 2003, Vol. 88, No. 4, 760 –763 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0021-9010/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.4.760 Children’s Face Recognition Memory: More Evidence for the Cross-Race Effect Kathy Pezdek, Iris Blandon-Gitlin, and Catherine Moore Claremont Graduate University It is well established that own-race faces are recognized more accurately than cross-race faces. However, there are mixed results regarding the developmental consistency of the cross-race effect. White and Black kindergarten children, 3rd graders, and young adults viewed a Black and a White target individual. One day later, recognition memory for each target was tested with a 6-person lineup. The interaction of race of participant by race of target face on Ag scores was significant, demonstrating an overall cross-race effect. The 2nd-order interaction with age did not approach significance; for each age group, own-race identification was more accurate than cross-race identification. The age consistency of the cross-race effect in light of the significant main effect of age suggests quantitative but not qualitative differences in face memory processing at various ages. For children, as well as adults, own-race faces are recognized more accurately than cross-race faces. An important goal for the field of applied psychology is to resolve inconsistencies in the research literature upon which expert testimony is frequently based. In the first study on this topic, Feinman and Entwisle (1976) tested White and Black children in grades one, two, three, and six on their ability to recognize photographs of White and Black children. Face recognition memory improved significantly with age, and although the race of participant by race of target face interaction was highly significant, this first-order interaction did not interact with age. The cross-race effect was evident at each age level; whereas White children were more accurate recognizing White faces than Black faces, Black children were more accurate recognizing Black faces than White faces. However, Chance, Turner, and Goldstein (1982) and Goldstein and Chance (1980) tested White children (grades one to eight) and adults on their ability to recognize photographs of White and Japanese adults. In both studies it was reported that d⬘ scores significantly increased with age, and that, whereas for children, d⬘ scores were similar for White and Japanese faces, for adults, White faces were recognized more accurately than Japanese faces. With these inconsistencies in the literature, it is difficult to know what advice expert witnesses should offer jurors regarding whether the cross-race effect operates for children as well as adults. In cognitive development research, it is important to determine which developmental effects result from quantitative differences (older participants and younger participants use the same cognitive processes to perform a task, but the older participants simply perform better) versus qualitative differences (older participants and younger participants utilize different cognitive processes to perform a task). In an analysis of variance (ANOVA) model, main effects of age suggest quantitative differences, and interactions with age suggest qualitative differences. In numerous studies, it has been reported that face recognition memory increases with age, thus suggesting quantitative differences in face memory across ages. In meta-analyses of face recognition memory studies, Pozzulo and Lindsay (1998) and Shapiro and Penrod (1986) reported that age yielded a large effect size for measures of both hits and false alarms. However, there do not appear to be major qualitative A major factor that affects the accuracy of eyewitness memory is whether the witness and the perpetrator are of the same race or of different races. Own-race faces are recognized more accurately than cross-race faces. This cross-race effect (also termed own-race bias) has been demonstrated in field settings (Brigham, Maass, Snyder, & Spaulding, 1982; Platz & Hosch, 1988) as well as in numerous laboratory studies (see Meissner & Brigham, 2001, for a review of this research). The effect is a robust one that is evident in measures of both hit rates and false alarm rates (Meissner & Brigham, 2001) as well as reaction time (Valentine, 1991) and in forensically relevant tasks such as photo lineup construction by police officers (Brigham & Ready, 1985) and the Photofit system for face reconstruction (Ellis, Davies, & McMurran, 1979). The cross-race effect in eyewitness identification has also been endorsed by attorneys (Brigham, 1981; Brigham & WolfsKeil, 1983), which accounts for the fact that expert testimony on this topic is so common. In this study, we examined the developmental consistency of the cross-race effect. In light of the fact that in real eyewitness identification situations the witnesses are often children, and in many cases involving children, the only evidence is a child’s eyewitness account, it is important to study factors that specifically relate to children’s identification accuracy. Three studies have examined developmental differences in the cross-race effect, and the results reported have been inconsistent. Kathy Pezdek, Iris Blandon-Gitlin, and Catherine Moore, Department of Psychology, Claremont Graduate University. This article is based on work supported by the National Science Foundation conducted by Kathy Pezdek under Grant SES-0111240. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation. This research was conducted as Catherine Moore’s master of arts thesis. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kathy Pezdek, Department of Psychology, Claremont Graduate University, 123 East Eighth Street, Claremont, California 91711. E-mail: kathy.pezdek@cgu.edu 760 RESEARCH REPORTS differences across age in the processes underlying face recognition memory (Baenninger, 1994; Chung, 1997; Pedelty, Levine, & Shevell, 1985). On the basis of these findings, we hypothesized in this study that face recognition memory will improve with age, and that the cross-race effect will be evident for children as well as adults. In this study, White and Black kindergarten children, third-graders, and young adults were compared on their recognition memory for a White man and a Black man. The three age groups in this study were selected from similar heterogeneous neighborhoods. Consequently, there is no reason to think that the relative amount of exposure to own-race faces as compared with cross-race faces should differ across groups. It is predicted that the first-order interaction of race of participant by race of target face will not interact with age, suggesting a consistency in the cross-race effect with age. This result would suggest that it is the relative amount of exposure to own-race faces as compared with cross-race faces that affects the size of the cross-race effect. Method Participants and Design A total of 186 children and young adults participated, including 62 kindergarten children (M age ⫽ 5.63 years; 32 male, 30 female), 62 third-graders (M age ⫽ 8.63 years; 29 male, 33 female), and 62 young adults (M age ⫽ 24.61 years; 31 male, 31 female). Half of the participants in each age group were White; half were Black. There was no statistical difference in the mean age for the White versus the Black participants in each age group. The children were recruited from public schools in the Los Angeles metropolitan area. The young adults were volunteers from psychology classes at two community colleges in the same area. Each participant viewed a White man and a Black man and was tested on their memory for each. The design was a 3 (age) ⫻ 2 (race of participant) ⫻ 2 (race of target face) mixed factorial design, with race of the target face being the only within-subjects factor. Procedure and Materials The experiment consisted of a presentation phase followed by a recognition phase. In the presentation phase, participants viewed a 2.5 min video in which two men, one White and one Black, were working at a counter, facing the camera, making cinnamon rolls. The video had the appearance of a cooking show. There was a natural conversation between the two men concerning their baking task. Prior to viewing the video participants were told that they would view a brief video in which two men were cooking, and that their task was to pay close attention to the video as they would be asked to identify the men later. This instruction was included so that any age differences that resulted could be attributed to memory processing differences rather than to age differences in attention allocated to the target faces. The children were shown the presentation tape in groups of four or five but were tested individually. The adults participated with their classmates as part of a class exercise. Participants returned the following day for the test phase. They were presented a video of a six-person lineup for each of the two target men. Each lineup included a target man and five foil individuals who matched the appearance of the target. The order of presenting the two test lineups was counterbalanced such that half of the participants viewed the White lineup first; half viewed the Black lineup first. In each test lineup, the six men first walked into the room and stood facing the camera. Then one at a time, each man stepped forward and faced the camera for a 15-s headshot. As each man stepped forward, participants were asked to respond on a scale from 1 (absolutely sure this was not the target man) to 5 (absolutely 761 sure this was the target man). This scale provides recognition memory judgments on a continuous scale rather than a discrete scale, thereby allowing for a signal detection analysis of the responses. Although this type of response does not simulate police identification procedures, it provides a more sensitive memory measure that reduces the effect of participants’ response criteria on identification accuracy. The experimenter recorded the children’s responses. The adults marked their responses on a sheet provided. Results The data were coded in terms of the signal detection measure, Ag. This measure is the nonparametric analog of d⬘and is used to test recognition accuracy in a “yes–no” forced choice test. Ag measures obtained from confidence ratings are similar to d⬘ measures computed from two alternative forced choice tests, and Ag is relatively unaffected by violations of the assumptions underlying d⬘ (Banks, 1970). Ag is the area under the receiver operator characteristic (the ROC curve). We followed the procedures specified in MacMillan and Creelman (1991) to compute Ag measures from responses to one target face and five foil faces. The Ag values ranged from .00 to 1.0 (perfect performance) with .50 being the chance rate. Essentially, higher Ag values represent better ability to distinguish the target face from the foils. A 3 (age) ⫻ 2 (race of participant) ⫻ 2 (race of target face) ANOVA was performed on the Ag data. The results of this analysis with effect sizes indicated are provided in Table 1. The interaction of race of participant by race of target face was significant, F(1, 180) ⫽ 14.98, p ⬍ .001, demonstrating an overall cross-race effect. White participants were more accurate in recognizing the White target face (M ⫽ .74, SD ⫽ .29) than the Black target face (M ⫽ .68, SD ⫽ .30), and Black participants were more accurate in recognizing the Black target face (M ⫽ .81, SD ⫽ .24) than the White target face (M ⫽ .66, SD ⫽ .28). More important, this effect did not vary across age groups; the Age ⫻ Race of Participant ⫻ Race of Target Face interaction did not approach significance, F ⬍ 1.00. As can be seen in Figure 1, for each age group, own-race identification was more accurate than cross-race identification. The only other significant effect in this analysis was the main effect of age, F(2, 180) ⫽ 13.65, p ⬍ .001. As expected, recognition accuracy was highest for the adults (M ⫽ .80, SD ⫽ .28), next highest for the third graders (M ⫽ .77, SD ⫽ .25), and lowest for the kindergarten children (M ⫽ .62, SD ⫽ .29). Table 1 Results of the Analysis of Variance With Effect Sizes Indicated Source Between subjects Participant race Age Participant Race ⫻ Age Within subjects Target race Target Race ⫻ Participant Race Target Race ⫻ Age Target Race ⫻ Participant Race ⫻ Age * p ⬍ .001. df MS F n2 1 2 2 0.05 1.08 0.22 0.65 13.65* 1.36 .00 .13 .02 1 1 2 2 0.18 0.99 0.06 0.02 2.73 14.98* 0.90 0.33 .02 .08 .01 .00 RESEARCH REPORTS 762 Figure 1. Mean Ag values as a function of the age and race of the participants and the race of the target face. White bars indicate the White target face; black bars indicate the Black target face. Discussion These findings are consistent with those of Feinman and Entwisle (1976) and support the notion that the cross-race effect is similar across age from 5 years of age through adulthood. Not only were own-race faces recognized more accurately than cross-race faces at each age level, but also the size of the cross-race effect did not vary with age. In addition, the main effect of age reported in this study supports the findings reported elsewhere that face recognition memory improves with age. In meta-analyses of face recognition memory studies, Pozzulo and Lindsay (1998) and Shapiro and Penrod (1986) reported that age yielded a large effect size for measures of both hits and false alarms. From a theoretical point of view, the age consistency of the cross-race effect, in light of the significant main effect of age, suggests quantitative but not qualitative changes with age in the cognitive processes underlying face recognition memory. In other words, the cognitive processes underlying face recognition memory are similar for participants at each age level; the older participants are simply performing these cognitive processes better. To illustrate this distinction, one model is considered for how faces might be represented in memory. This is the multidimensional space framework proposed by Valentine (1991) and Valentine and Endo (1992). According to this model, faces are represented in a hypothetical space that retains each face on the basis of various dimensions representing features or sets of features. These dimensions, including both configural and featural aspects of faces, developed from an individual’s prior experience with faces. In accordance, the representational space for own-race faces includes a better differentiation of the variant and the invariant dimensions of faces than does the representational space for cross-race faces. As a consequence, own-race faces are spread more evenly throughout the multidimensional “face space” and are more distinctively represented and more accurately recognized. On the other hand, less distinction is made between the variant and the invariant dimensions of cross-race faces, and as a consequence, cross-race faces are more tightly clustered in the face space and thus less distinctively represented and less accurately recognized. The results of this study suggest that to the extent that this multidimensional space framework can account for the cross-race effect with adults—and it does appear to fit the adult data quite well (Chiroro & Valentine, 1995)—it is likely to describe the cognitive processes underlying the cross-race effect for children as well. The significant main effect of age, however, suggests that the developmental difference is a quantitative one. For example, the representational face space for children is likely to retain less and less appropriate featural and configural information than does the representational face space for adults. Accordingly, face memory is generally less accurate for young children than for older children and adults, but the size of the cross-race effect is similar among age groups. The significant main affect of age in light of the nonsignificant interaction of race of participant by race of target face by age suggests that it is the relative amount of exposure to own-race faces as compared with cross-race faces that affects the size of the cross-race effect. Because the three age groups in this study were selected from similar heterogeneous neighborhoods, there is no reason to think that the relative amount of exposure to own-race faces as compared with cross-race faces differed across groups. It can also be predicted from this model that for both adults and children, relatively more contact with cross-race faces would reduce the cross-race effect by increasing the recognition accuracy of cross-race faces relative to own-race faces. This, in fact, has been reported in a number of studies involving adults (Brigham et al., 1982; Carroo, 1987; Chiroro & Valentine, 1995; Platz & Hosch, 1988) as well as children (Feinman & Entwisle, 1976). From an applied point of view, these results suggest that the advantage of own-race identification over cross-race identification would be expected to be less for both adults and children who have had higher levels of cross-race contact. How can we account for the differences obtained between the results of the present study and those of Chance, Turner and Goldstein (1982) and Goldstein and Chance (1980), who reported a cross-race effect for adults but not for children? First, it should be noted that in both of these previous studies, only White participants were tested, and they were compared with their ability to recognize White and Japanese target individuals. The cross-race effect is more apparent if both the race of the target faces and the race of the participants are varied. This design is necessary to reject the interpretation that perhaps the specific own-race faces included in the study were more discriminable and thus more memorable than the specific cross-race faces selected. Second, an unusual pattern resulted in the Goldstein and Chance (1980) study, raising concerns about the validity of these findings. Whereas adults recognized the White faces better than the Japanese faces, White children at all grade levels from first to sixth recognized the Japanese faces better than the White faces. In addition, Chance, Turner, and Goldstein (1982) reported that d⬘ scores for first and second graders to both White and Japanese faces were less than chance (i.e., d⬘ ⬍ 1.00). This suggests that perhaps the cross-race effect was masked by a floor effect with the children’s data in this study. One might argue that the reliability of the study was limited by the fact that each child recognized only one White face and one Black face. It is true that in typical face recognition memory research, participants are presented with dozens of faces to remember and are then tested on their ability to distinguish old from new RESEARCH REPORTS test faces. However, this is quite a different situation from the real world situation of viewing one suspect and having to pick him out of a lineup later. We designed this study to test recognition memory for only two faces, the two men interacting in the video, to more closely approximate the situation of real eyewitness identification. The fact that there was no main effect of race of face suggests that the memorability of the White and Black face did not differ. Even more important is the finding that, although White participants identified the White face better than the Black face, Black participants identified the Black face better than the White face. On the basis of the findings of this study, expert witnesses on eyewitness identification should be confident testifying that for children as well as adults, own-race faces are recognized more accurately than cross-race faces. Although face identification accuracy does improve with age, this effect appears to result from quantitative rather than qualitative changes with age in the cognitive processes underlying face recognition memory. References Baenninger, M. A. (1994). The development of face recognition: Featural or configurational procession? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 57, 337–396. Banks, W. P. (1970). Signal detection theory and human memory. Psychological Bulletin, 74, 81–99. Brigham, J. C. (1981). The accuracy of eyewitness evidence: How do attorneys see it? Florida Bar Journal, 55, 714 –721. Brigham, J. C., Maass, A., Snyder, L. D., & Spaulding, K. (1982). Accuracy of eyewitness identifications in a field setting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 673– 681. Brigham, J. C., & Ready, D. J. (1985). Own-race bias in lineup construction. Law & Human Behavior, 9, 415– 424. Brigham, J. C., & WolfsKeil, M. P. (1983). Opinions of attorneys and law enforcement personnel on the accuracy of eyewitness identifications. Law & Human Behavior, 7, 337–349. Carroo, A. W. (1987). Recognition of faces as a function of race, attitudes, and reported cross-racial friendships. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 64, 319 –325. Chance, J. E., Turner, A. L., & Goldstein, A. G. (1982). Development of 763 differential recognition for own- and other-race faces. Journal of Psychology, 112, 29 –37. Chiroro, P., & Valentine, T. (1995). An investigation of the contact hypothesis of the own-race bias in face recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 48(A), 879 – 894. Chung, M. S. (1997). Recognition of upright and inverted faces by children and adults: A test of Carey’s encoding shift hypothesis. Korean Journal of Psychology: Developmental, 10, 139 –151. Ellis, H. D., Davies, G. M., & McMurran, M. M. (1979). Recall of White and Black faces by White and Black witnesses using the Photofit system. Human Factors, 21, 55–59. Feinman, S., & Entwisle, D. R. (1976). Children’s ability to recognize other children’s faces. Child Development, 47, 506 –510. Goldstein, A. G., & Chance, J. E. (1980). Memory for faces and schema theory. Journal of Psychology, 105, 47–59. MacMillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (1991). Detection theory: A user’s guide. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Meissner, C. A., & Brigham, J. C. (2001). Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 3–35. Pedelty, L., Levine, S. C., & Shevell, S. K. (1985). Developmental changes in face processing: Results from multidimensional scaling. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 421– 436. Platz, S. J., & Hosch, H. M. (1988). Cross-racial/ethnic eyewitness identification: A field study. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 18, 972–984. Pozzulo, J. D., & Lindsay, R. C. L. (1998). Identification accuracy of children versus adults: A meta-analysis. Law and Human Behavior, 22, 549 –570. Shapiro, P. N., & Penrod, S. (1986). Meta-analysis of facial identification studies. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 139 –156. Valentine, T. (1991). A unified account of the effects of distinctiveness, inversion, and race on face recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 43(A), 161–204. Valentine, T., & Endo, M. (1992). Towards an exemplar model of face processing: The effects of race and distinctiveness. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 44(A), 671–703. Received May 28, 2002 Revision received December 4, 2002 Accepted December 19, 2002 䡲