Working Memory – Goal Maintenance Task Name Description Cognitive Construct Validity Probabilistic Reversal Learning Subjects select one stimulus from an array of concurrently presented stimuli (preferably 3); typically, more than one set of discriminanda are presented, in a randomized order across trials. Each choice is followed by positive or negative feedback (correct choice or incorrect choice). The relationship between the stimulus presented and the feedback given can be deterministic or probabilistic. Subjects must, based upon feedback alone, learn which cue in each discriminanda set are associated with positive feedback and then maintain optimized performance of the rule (rule maintenance). Once the subject meets pre-set performance criteria, feedback is adjusted without warning. In other words, the rules are "reversed" in one or more of the discriminanda sets. Subjects must update their behavior based upon the change in rules. Optimal reversal performance is often considered to involve cognitive control over prepotent responding. Acquisition of a multiple choice visual discrimination is believed to reflect implicit learning processes. Optimal performance of a visual discrimination is thought to reflect rule maintenance. Reversal of a learned visual discrimination is thought to measure cognitive control over pre-potent responding (response inhibition). (Waltz & Gold, 2007) (Lee, Groman, London, & Jentsch, 2007) (Frank & Claus, 2006) MANUSCRIPTS ON THE WEBSITE: Cools, R., Altamirano, L., & D'Esposito, M. (2006). Reversal learning in parkinson's disease depends on medication status and outcome valence. Neuropsychologia, 44(10), 1663-1673. Waltz, J. A., & Gold, J. M. (2007). Probabilistic reversal learning Neural Construct Validity Interactions between declarative systems (hippocampal and prefrontal) and implicit systems (striatum) are predicted across initial learning of a discrimination, such that increasing activation in the striatum, as a function of trials performed, occurs along with learning. The ventrolateral and ventromedial frontal cortex are particularly involved in the effective inhibition of responses at reversal. (Dias, Robbins, & Roberts, 1996, 1997) (Fellows & Farah, 2005) (Fellows & Farah, 2003) Reliability As this is a "learning" task, test-retest reliability is unavailable. Psychometric Characteristics Practice effects are unknown, but are the subject of active investigation. Depending upon how the test is conducted, there can be ceiling effects for discrimination learning. The use of multiple discriminada sets, at least 3 stimuli in each set and probabilistic feedback can avoid this. Animal Model Stage of Research Analogous tests for mice, rats and monkeys exist. There is evidence that this specific task elicits deficits in schizophrenia. (Lee et al., 2007) (Boulougouris, Dalley, & Robbins, 2007) (Chudasama & Robbins, 2006) We need to assess psychometric characteristics such as test-retest reliability, practice effects, and ceiling/floor effects for this task. We need to study whether or not performance on this task changes in response to psychological or pharmacological intervention BUT: (Cools, Altamirano, & D'Esposito, 2006; Cools, Lewis, Clark, Barker, & Robbins, 2007) impairments in schizophrenia: further evidence of orbitofrontal dysfunction. Schizophrenia Research, 93(1-3), 296303. Operation Span/ Symmetry Span “In the first published version of the Operation Span Task, 12 items were presented, with 3 items each consisting of two, three, four, and five operation word pairs, presented in ascending order. The stimuli consisted of mathematical operations followed by a to-be-remembered word, drawn from the same normed set of common fourto six-letter words as that for Turner and Engle’s version of the reading span task. When the operation word string was presented, the subjects read the operation aloud and verified whether the stated solution was correct or incorrect. They then read aloud and remembered the word for later recall. All intermediate calculations were done silently and without the aid of pencil and paper. Engle et al. (1992) developed the version of the operation span task currently used in our laboratories. The primary difference from earlier versions is the manipulation of presentation order. Rather than presenting reading span and operation span items in ascending order (items with fewer elements first), which permitted the subjects to anticipate the number of words that they would be asked to remember on any given trial, Engle et al. (1992) randomized the presentation order, effectively eliminating reliance on any strategies that come from knowing the size of the memory set.2 This modification has the added benefit of deconfounding item size and buildup of proactive interference, since recent studies have shown that proactive interference builds from trial to trial in This task has more demonstrated construct validity than all other WM tasks combined. This task has been shown to individually and at the latent variable level to be valid This task, in several versions, has been shown to be reliable over repeated administrations and also split half. Good internal consistency. Good retest reliability over up to 3 month (.7 and above). (Conway et al., 2005) (Klein & Fiss, 1999) (Unsworth, Heitz, Schrock, & Engle, 2005) The task has been used in studies where it was administered at least a dozen times and even late in the study still correlated with errors after a period of sleep deprivation (Conway et al., 2005) (Klein & Fiss, 1999) (Unsworth, Heitz, Schrock, & Engle, 2005) NONE This specific task needs to be studied in individuals with schizophrenia. Data already exists on psychometric characteristics of this task, such as testretest reliability, practice effects, ceiling/floor effects. We need to study whether or not performance on this task changes in response to psychological or pharmacological intervention. WM span tasks (Bunting, in press; Lustig, May, & Hasher, 2001; May, Hasher, & Kane, 1999). It also results in a wider range of scores than does the traditional ascending approach. A potential risk of this approach, however, is that the early presence of difficult items may discourage some subjects, particularly those who are less able, such as children, the elderly, or patients. (Our advice to researchers working with such populations is to stress to the subject that perfect recall is not expected in these tasks.)” The above taken from pp. 73-74 of Conway et al., 2005) (R W Engle, Kane, & Tuholski, 1999) (R. W. Engle, Tuholski, Laughlin, & Conway, 1999) (Kane et al., 2004) (Conway et al., 2005) Symmetry Span: In this task participants were required to recall sequences of red squares within a matrix while performing a symmetryjudgment task. In the symmetryjudgment task participants were shown an 8 x 8 matrix with some squares filled in black. Participants decided whether the design was symmetrical about its vertical axis. The pattern was symmetrical half of the time. Immediately after determining whether the pattern was symmetrical, participants were presented with a 4 x 4 matrix with one of the cells filled in red for 650 ms. At recall, participants recalled the sequence of red-square locations in the preceding displays, in the order they appeared by clicking on the cells of an empty matrix. There were three trials of each set-size with list length ranging from 2-5. The same scoring procedure as Ospan was used. The above taken from Unsworth (2006). MANUSCRIPTS ON THE WEBSITE: Conway, A. R., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user's guide. Psychon Bull Rev, 12(5), 769-786. Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. P., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W. (2005). An automated version of the operation span task. Behav Res Methods, 37(3), 498-505. AX-CPT In this task, participants are presented with cue–probe pairs and told to make a target response to an “X” (probe) but only when it follows an “A” (cue), and a nontarget response otherwise. A correct response to “X” depends upon maintaining the “goa” or “context” provided by the cue (“A” or not-“A”). One change to the standard AX-CPT was to increase the frequency of target (“AX”) trials so that they occur with a high frequency (70%), with the remaining 30% of trials distributed across three types of nontarget trials (“BX”, “AY”, and “BX” where “B” refers to any non-“A” cue and “Y” refers to any non-“X” probe). This creates two types of biases that can be used to probe the integrity of goal maintenance. The first bias, or prepotent response, is that participants expect to make a target response when they see an “X” probe, because this is the correct response on most of the trials (87.5% of trials in which an X is presented). On “BX” trials, participants have to use to the Original model demonstrating framework for context processing: (Cohen & ServanSchreiber, 1992) Formal model of AX task linking performance to DA gating: (Braver, Barch, & Cohen, 1999) Demonstration of convergence of context processing performance, and deficits in patients, across three tasks: (Cohen, Barch, Carter, & Servan-Schreiber, 1999) Evidence of trait-like impairment in schizophrenia patients: (A. W. MacDonald, 3rd et al., 2005) Evidence of specificity of impairment and relation to PFC dysfunction: (Barch, Sheline, Csernansky, & Performance on the AX-CPT improves in response to amphetamine administration in both individuals with schizophrenia (Barch & Carter, 2005) and healthy controls (Barch & Braver, 2007). BX errors had an alpha of .88 (J. D. Cohen, D. M. Barch, C. S. Carter, & D. Servan-Schreiber, 1999). This task can elicit different deficits in goal (context) processing (Barch et al., 2001; Barch, Carter et al., 2003; Barch et al., 2004; J.D. Cohen et al., 1999; Javitt, Shelley, Silipo, & Lieberman, 2000; D. Servan-Schreiber, J. D. Cohen, & S. Steingard, 1996; Stratta, Daneluzzo, Bustini, Casacchia, & Rossi, 1998; Stratta, Daneluzzo, Bustini, Prosperini, & Rossi, 2000). Simon Killcross has argued that he has a rodent paradigm that measures something similar (Haddon & Killcross, 2007). There is evidence that this specific task elicits deficits in schizophrenia. We need to assess psychometric characteristics such as test-retest reliability, practice effects, and ceiling/floor effects for this task. There is evidence that performance on this task can improve in response to psychological or pharmacological interventions. context provided by the “B” cue to inhibit this bias to respond target to an “X” (which would lead to a false alarm). Thus, impaired goal representations will lead to poor performance on “BX” trials, because the goal (context) provided by the “B” cue would not be available to override the tendency to want to response target to the “X.” The second bias is that participants expect to make a target response after they see an “A” cue, because most of the time an “X” follows the “A” cue (87.5% of “A” cue trials). However, on trials in which the “A” is not followed by an “X”, this predictive aspect of context actually creates the tendency to false alarm. Thus, intact representations of context will hurt performance on “AY” trials, because context induces an invalid expectancy, leading to worse “AY” than “BX” performance. In contrast, individuals with impaired context representations should show worse “BX” than “AY” performance. Snyder, 2003) Link to PFC impairment in unmedicated patients, specificity of impairments to schizophrenia, and further demonstrating link to disorganization symptoms of schizophrenia: (A. MacDonald et al., 2005) (Cohen & Servan-Schreiber, 1992) (Braver et al., 1999) (Cohen et al., 1999) (A. W. MacDonald, 3rd et al., 2005) (Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2003) (Barch, Sheline et al., 2003) (A. MacDonald et al., 2005) (A. W. MacDonald et al., 2003) REVIEW WITH THE DPX Dot Pattern Expectancy Task Dot Pattern Expectancy Task. This task is a variant of expectancy manipulation of the AX-CPT (Servan-Schreiber, Cohen, & Steingard, 1996). It uses dot pattern stimuli instead of letters to 1) increase difficulty of maintaining information and 2) manipulate the interference from invalid stimuli more parametrically than was possible with letters. These manipulations decrease ceiling effects, increase the likelihood of observing a differential deficit, and may increase reliability. (MacDonald et al., 2005) MANUSCRIPTS ON THE WEBSITE: DPX is formally equivalent to the expectancy AX task, upon which it is built. The conditions of interest have been shown to be statistically equivalent to the AX, which has an extensive history in the clinical cognitive neuroscience literature: Original model demonstrating framework for context processing: (Cohen & Servan-Schreiber, 1992) Formal model of AX task linking performance to DA gating: (Braver, Barch, & Cohen, 1999) This kind of goal maintenance, known as context processing, has been shown to be associated with activity in DLPFC (predominantly BA 9). (Barch et al., 1997) (A. W. MacDonald, 3rd, Cohen, Stenger, & Carter, 2000) Preliminary data from unpublished studies. Furthermore, these data have also been evaluated to determine whether a shorter version would maintain reliability: Among 210 undergraduates, coefficient alpha for AX trials was .91 across 120 trials, and .85 for The task has been found to be capable to demonstrating differential deficits in three studies, including the original study. These studies also indicate that errors rates on the AY condition are 10-20% in controls, and on the BX condition are 613%. None known. Current work includes a monkey homologue using visual locations to define valid cues and probes with an otherwise formallyequivalent expectancy manipulation. There is evidence that this specific task elicits deficits in schizophrenia. Data already exists on psychometric characteristics of this task, such as testretest reliability, practice effects, ceiling/floor effects. We need to study whether or not performance on this task changes in Cohen, J. D., Barch, D. M., Carter, C., & Servan-Schreiber, D. (1999). Context-processing deficits in schizophrenia: Converging evidence from three theoretically motivated cognitive tasks. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 120-133. MacDonald, A. W., 3rd, Goghari, V. M., Hicks, B. M., Flory, J. D., Carter, C. S., & Manuck, S. B. (2005). A convergentdivergent approach to context processing, general intellectual functioning, and the genetic liability to schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, 19(6), 814-821. Demonstration of convergence of context processing performance, and deficits in patients, across three tasks: (Cohen, Barch, Carter, & Servan-Schreiber, 1999) Demonstrating statistical equivalence of DPX to expectancy AX in general population sample (n=500), relationship to working memory and IQ, and impairments on DPX associated with unexpressed genetic liability:(A. W. MacDonald, 3rd et al., 2005) In addition: Evidence of traitlike impairment in schizophrenia patients: (A. W. MacDonald, 3rd et al., 2005) Evidence of specificity of impairment and relation to PFC dysfunction: (Barch, Sheline, Csernansky, & Snyder, 2003) Demonstration of relatively greater sensitivity of DPX relative to NBack and AX task to polymorphisms in COMT gene: (A. W. MacDonald, 3rd, Carter, Flory, Ferrell, & Manuck, 2007) Link to PFC impairment in unmedicated patients, specificity of impairments to schizophrenia, and further demonstrating link to disorganization symptoms of schizophrenia: (A. MacDonald the first 46 trials. Alpha for BX trials was likewise .76 across all 16 trials, whereas it was .71 for the first 8 trials. AY trials were not as internally consistent, with alpha=.46 for all trials and .20 for the first 8 trials. In terms of retest reliability, 70 mixed healthy and psychiatric patients 6-week retest reliability (single measure absolute ICC’s) was .78 and .56 for AX and BX trials, respectively. Among the 10 schizophrenia patients in this sample, retest reliability was .94 and .79 for AX and BX trials, respectively. For AY trials, retest reliability was lower, ICC = .57. response to psychological or pharmacological intervention. et al., 2005) Evidence that context processing is related to genetic liability to schizophrenia: (A. W. MacDonald, Pogue-Geile, Johnson, & Carter, 2003) (Cohen & Servan-Schreiber, 1992) (Braver et al., 1999) (Cohen et al., 1999) (A. W. MacDonald, 3rd et al., 2005) (Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2003) (Barch, Sheline et al., 2003) (A. MacDonald et al., 2005) (A. W. MacDonald et al., 2003) REFERENCES: Barch, D. M., & Braver, T. S. (2007). Cognitive control in schizophrenia: Psychological and neural mechanisms. In R. W. Engle, G. Sedek, U. von Hecker & A. M. McIntosh (Eds.), Cognitive limitations in aging and psychopathology. Barch, D. M., Braver, T. S., Nystom, L. E., Forman, S. D., Noll, D. C., & Cohen, J. D. (1997). Dissociating working memory from task difficulty in human prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia, 35(10), 1373-1380. Barch, D. M., & Carter, C. S. (2005). Amphetamine improves cognitive function in medicated individuals with schizophrenia and in healthy volunteers. Schizophrenia Research, 77(1), 43-58. Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., McDonald, A., Sabb, F. W., Noll, D. C., et al. (2001). Selective deficits in prefrontal cortex regions in medication naive schizophrenia patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 280-288. Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2003). Context processing deficit in schizophrenia: Diagnostic specificity, 4-week course, and relationships to clinical symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(132-143). Barch, D. M., Mitropoulou, V., Harvey, P. D., New, A. S., Silverman, J. M., & Siever, L. J. (2004). Context-processing deficits in schizotypal personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113(4), 556-568. Barch, D. M., Sheline, Y. I., Csernansky, J. G., & Snyder, A. Z. (2003). Working memory and prefrontal cortex dysfunction: Specificity to schizophrenia as compared to major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 53, 376-384. Boulougouris, V., Dalley, J. W., & Robbins, T. W. (2007). Effects of orbitofrontal, infralimbic and prelimbic cortical lesions on serial spatial reversal learning in the rat. Behav Brain Res, 179(2), 219-228. Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., & Cohen, J. D. (1999). Cognition and control in schizophrenia: A computational model of dopamine and prefrontal function. Biological Psychiatry, 46, 312-328. Chudasama, Y., & Robbins, T. W. (2006). Functions of frontostriatal systems in cognition: comparative neuropsychopharmacological studies in rats, monkeys and humans. Biological Psychiatry, 73(1), 19-38. Cohen, J. D., Barch, D. M., Carter, C., & Servan-Schreiber, D. (1999). Context-processing deficits in schizophrenia: Converging evidence from three theoretically motivated cognitive tasks. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 120-133. Cohen, J. D., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Servan-Schreiber, D. (1999). Context-processing deficits in schizophrenia: Converging evidence from three theoretically motivated cognitive tasks. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108(1), 120-133. Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97(3), 332-361. Cohen, J. D., & Servan-Schreiber, D. (1992). Context, cortex and dopamine: A connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. Psychological Review, 99(1), 45-77. Conway, A. R., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user's guide. Psychon Bull Rev, 12(5), 769-786. Cools, R., Altamirano, L., & D'Esposito, M. (2006). Reversal learning in Parkinson's disease depends on medication status and outcome valence. Neuropsychologia, 44(10), 1663-1673. Cools, R., Lewis, S. J., Clark, L., Barker, R. A., & Robbins, T. W. (2007). L-DOPA disrupts activity in the nucleus accumbens during reversal learning in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychopharmacology, 32(1), 180-189. Dias, R., Robbins, T. W., & Roberts, A. C. (1996). Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts. Nature, 380, 69-72. Dias, R., Robbins, T. W., & Roberts, A. C. (1997). Dissociable forms of inhibitory control within prefrontal cortex with an analog of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test: restrictions to novel situations and independence from "on-line" processing. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(23), 9285-9297. Engle, R. W., Kane, M. J., & Tuholski, S. W. (1999). Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory (pp. 102-134). New York: Cambridge University Press. Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309-331. Fellows, L. K., & Farah, M. J. (2003). Ventromedial frontal cortex mediates affective shifting in humans: evidence from a reversal learning paradigm. Brain, 126(Pt 8), 1830-1837. Fellows, L. K., & Farah, M. J. (2005). Different underlying impairments in decision-making following ventromedial and dorsolateral frontal lobe damage in humans. Cereb Cortex, 15(1), 58-63. Frank, M. J., & Claus, E. D. (2006). Anatomy of a decision: striato-orbitofrontal interactions in reinforcement learning, decision making, and reversal. Psychol Rev, 113(2), 300-326. Haddon, J. E., & Killcross, S. (2007). Contextual control of choice performance: behavioral, neurobiological, and neurochemical influences. Ann N Y Acad Sci, 1104, 250-269. Javitt, D. C., Shelley, A., Silipo, G., & Lieberman, J. A. (2000). Deficits in auditory and visual context-dependent processing in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 1131-1137. Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., Tuholski, S. W., Wilhelm, O., Payne, T. W., & Engle, R. W. (2004). The generality of working memory capacity: a latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. J Exp Psychol Gen, 133(2), 189-217. Klein, K., & Fiss, W. H. (1999). The reliability and stability of the Turner and Engle working memory task. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput, 31(3), 429-432. Lee, B., Groman, S., London, E. D., & Jentsch, J. D. (2007). Dopamine D2/D3 receptors play a specific role in the reversal of a learned visual discrimination in monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology, 32(10), 21252134. MacDonald, A., Carter, C. S., Kerns, J. G., Ursu, S., Barch, D. M., Holmes, A. J., et al. (2005). Specificity of prefrontal dysfunction and context processing deficts to schizophrenia in a never medicated firstepisode psychotic sample. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 475-484. MacDonald, A. W., 3rd, Carter, C. S., Flory, J. D., Ferrell, R. E., & Manuck, S. B. (2007). COMT val158Met and executive control: a test of the benefit of specific deficits to translational research. J Abnorm Psychol, 116(2), 306-312. MacDonald, A. W., 3rd, Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288(5472), 1835-1838. MacDonald, A. W., 3rd, Goghari, V. M., Hicks, B. M., Flory, J. D., Carter, C. S., & Manuck, S. B. (2005). A convergent-divergent approach to context processing, general intellectual functioning, and the genetic liability to schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, 19(6), 814-821. MacDonald, A. W., Pogue-Geile, M. F., Johnson, M. K., & Carter, C. S. (2003). A specific deficit in context processing in the unaffected siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 57-65. Servan-Schreiber, D., Cohen, J., & Steingard, S. (1996). Schizophrenic deficits in the processing of context: A test of a theoretical model. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 1105-1112. Servan-Schreiber, D., Cohen, J. D., & Steingard, S. (1996). Schizophrenic deficits in the processing of context: A test of a theoretical model. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53(Dec), 1105-1113. Stratta, P., Daneluzzo, E., Bustini, M., Casacchia, M., & Rossi, A. (1998). Schizophrenic deficits in the processing of context. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 186-187. Stratta, P., Daneluzzo, E., Bustini, M., Prosperini, P., & Rossi, A. (2000). Processing of context information in schizophrenia: relation to clinical symptoms and WCST performance. Schizophrenia Research, 44, 57-67. Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. P., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W. (2005). An automated version of the operation span task. Behav Res Methods, 37(3), 498-505. Waltz, J. A., & Gold, J. M. (2007). Probabilistic reversal learning impairments in schizophrenia: further evidence of orbitofrontal dysfunction. Schizophrenia Research, 93(1-3), 296-303.