Affective Climate 1 Running head: AFFECTIVE CLIMATE AND TEAM PERFORMANCE Affective climate’s influence on workteam performance: The role of affective climate strength. Nuria Gamero Vázquez University of Valencia Abstract Affective Climate 2 Several studies have shown that workteam members experience similar affects related to their work. These collective affects influence team processes and outcomes. Recently, the concept of affective climate has been proposed to define shared affective responses by workteam members. The aim of this study was to validate the affective climate concept and to examine its possible effects on team performance. With that purpose, we have considered the intensity dimension (the level of shared affective states) as well as the strength dimension (the degree of within-unit agreement among team members` affective experiences). The sample consisted of 133 workteams from several financial companies. The total number of participants was 581. Hierarchical regression analyses show that affective climate intensity and strength influence team performance. Further, our data suggest that affective climate strength may be an important factor to understand the link between team affective climate intensity and team performance. Key words: affective climate intensity, affective climate strength, team performance Affective Climate 3 Introduction Affects are an integral and inseparable part of organizational life (Ashforth & Humphrey, 1995). Workteam members` experiences and affective states have been an area of growing interest in the organizational studies and they are implicit in numerous organizational and psychological theories, because of their implication in the group and organizational processes and outcomes (Ashkanasy, Härtel & Zerbe, 2000; Brief & Weiss, 2002; Domagalski, 1999; Fisher & Ashkanasy, 2000; Muchinski, 2000;). Most of the research about the role of affect has been carried out at the individual level. Researchers have described a variety of different kinds of affective experiences (mood, emotion and dispositional affect, among others and they have defined all of them as “affect”) showing their important role on work units` processes and outcomes. However, recent developments in the study of affect have derived to series of promising research areas with regard to their presence at the group level (Bartel & Saavedra, 2000; Barsade, 2001; Barsade, Ward, Turner & Sonnenfeld, 2000). The present study aims to contribute to the study of affect as a group phenomenon. Our goal is to test the validity of the affective climate concept as a current and emergent construct and to study its effects on team performance. For it, we review the main contributions to the affective climate concept and, considering its dimensions of intensity (or level of shared affective states) and strength (or team members` affective homogeneity), we provide a theoretical rationale for the hypotheses tested. Affective Climate 4 1. Affective climate: a new concept 1.1. The study of collective affect. Until recently, affect as an area of study has received relatively little attention from researchers. However, during the last decade, researchers have turned its attention to the employees` affect dimensions (Domagalski, 1999) and a large number of studies on causes and consequences of affect in the workplace has been carried out. As a consequence of the proliferation of literature on individual affect, a new approach to this topic is emerging, which considers affect as a collective property of work groups. Several authors have pointed out that group members could develop shared affect. George (1990) proposed the concept of “group affective tone” and she defined it as “consistent or homogeneous affective reactions within a group” (p.77). In her study with a sales teams` sample, she showed that work groups could develop affective tones when a degree of consistency or homogeneity in the affective reactions among members was demonstrated. As George (1990, 1995) argued, “when members of a group experience similar levels of positive/negative mood at work, then the group has a positive/negative affective tone…. Conversely, if members experience dissimilar levels of positive/negative mood at work then the group does not have an affective tone” (p.781). George concluded that when most of group members experience positive (or negative) affective states, then the affective tone of the group as a whole becomes positive (or negative) as well. Sessa (1996), in her study on group emotion and conflict, demonstrated the existence of affective tone in 30 nurse teams. Results showed that shared affect is exhibited by team members through a series of vocal cues, facial expression and body movements and, therefore, that it could be observed. Other contributions come from Totterdell and colleagues` studies. They have asserted that Affective Climate 5 work group members may share group affect and they found a significant concurrent mood associated in several professional samples. Totterdell et al. (1998), in two studies with different samples, nurses and accountants, found evidence showing that the moods of team members were related to each other in teams over time. Their results showed that individual mood could be influenced by collective mood of the teammates at work, such that people’s mood could become linked to the mood of their teammates. Totterdell (2000) confirmed those data in a sample of professional cricket teams and also showed that the influence was the same, independently of shared events by the team. Across short-term meetings of 70 very diverse work group teams, Bartel and Saavedra (2000) also found convergence of mood. These authors showed that work group mood is something that can be recognized and reliably measured by members within the work group and they also showed that group mood was also rated by observers external to the group. Likewise, Barsade (2001), in a study which examined the influence of emotional contagion on team dynamics, found that contagion happened and promoted a strong convergence of group members` mood. As Barsade (2001) indicated, work group members come to develop mutually shared moods and emotions in the course of executing their tasks and that these affects may result for a subtle but continuous transfer of affective states among members. Thus, group affect studies offer “excellent external validity that shared emotions occur in organizational workteams and that these emotions can be recognized and measured” (Barsade, 2001, p. 5). Different models to explain the processes that contribute to the convergence of collective affect have been formulated. Among those studies, we highlight George’s (1990, 1996) research. According to her, there are at least four complementary Affective Climate 6 theoretical rationales supporting the existence of group affective tone: social interaction, Schneider’s (1987) attraction-selection-attrition (ASA) framework, socialization processes and social influence (Fisher, 1986) and, finally, the similarity in group tasks and outcomes for team members. On the other hand, Barsade and Gibson (1998) pointed out to a series of fundamental factors to understand and explain group emotion formation. Those elements would be developed and structured later by Kelly and Barsade (2001) in a more complete organizational model to understand affective influences on groups. In this model, these authors noted that group emotions result from the group’s affective composition and the affective context in which the group is behaving. It is through a variety of explicit and implicit processes (or bottom-up components in the model), how affective experiences that group members bring with them to the group are communicated to other group members and form the affective compositional group effects. There would be also a series of factors in the group’s affective context (or top-down components) that may amplify or constrain the group members` affective experiences. Despite theoretical production, empirical evidence points out structural elements and team processes which foster the convergence of affect at the group level. Among them, emotional contagion (Barsade, 2001; Gump & Kulik, 1997; Hatfield, Caccioppo & Brapson, 1994; Totterdell et al., 1998; Totterdell, 2000), mood regulation norms (Bartel & Saavedra, 2000), emotional comparison (Sullins, 1991; Totterdell et al., 1998), task and social interdependence and members stability (Bartel & Saavedra, 2000), member tenure and a set of factors related to being interdependent in the team and satisfied with it: more committed to the team, perceiving a better team climate, being happier and engaging in collective activity (Totterdell, 2000). Affective Climate 7 1.2. The Affective Climate Construct. The concept of climate has a wide tradition in Organizational Psychology and its important role in the different organizational experiences and outcomes has been demonstrated. Organizational research has usually considered climate as shared perceptions of unit members (i.e. perceptive climate). Nonetheless, climate can also represent other shared experiences or responses. Thus, other kind of climates can be identified. In this sense, Kirton and McCarthy (1988) proposed the concept of cognitive climate to refer to the cognitive styles that team’s members use to solve their problems. On the other hand, De Rivera (1992) proposed the concept of emotional climate to refer to shared emotional responses by a collective. According to him, the emotional climate reflects the emotional relationship between group members (i.e. how group members are related to one another). This construct has been the objective of some studies in social psychology, which have emphasized its validity and usefulness (Fernández-Dols, 1998; Hurley, 1997; Kavanagh, O’Halloran, Manicavasagar, Clark, Piatkowska, Tennant & Rosen, 1997; Páez, Ruiz, Gailly, Kornblit, Wiesenfeld & Vidal, 1997). Paez et al. (1997) define emotional climate as a collective “state of mind” which is characterized by certain predominant emotions. These predominant emotions are associated to certain predominant actions` tendency. As Paez et al. (1997) indicate, emotional climate is a subjective construct (emotions are in the mind of the individuals) as well as an objective one (they are shared and manifested in collective ways of behaving). They also point out that emotional climate is “an emergent affective phenomenon which aggregate new elements and it is distributed within group” (p.82). More recently, González-Romá, Peiró, Subirats and Mañas (2000), reconceptualized De Rivera’s concept as affective climate. They have described it as “shared affective responses by workteam’s members” (p.98). They used the concept of Affective Climate 8 “affect” because it is a wide term which traditionally includes other concepts as mood, emotion, sentiment, dispositional affect, etc. In this study, we considered two dimensions of affect: tension and optimist (negative and positive affect, respectively). These dimensions are extracted starting from a more general conceptual framework, circumplex model of affect, which represents the principal dimensions of affect and how they are interrelated (Bartel & Saavedra, 2000; Warr, 1990; Weiss & Copranzano, 1996; Yik, Russell & Barret, 1999). Therefore, we will refer to affective climate in term of tension and optimism affective climate. Although the research on this kind of climate is scarce, several studies have confirmed its existence. Peiró, González-Romá, Tordera, and Belmonte`s (2000) study with a sample of 250 public health service`s units, highlighted the importance of concept of emotional climate as a relevant phenomenon which should be empirically demostrated. On the other hand, Gonzalez-Romá, Peiró, Subirats and Mañas (2000), using a two-wave panel data design, tested the validity of the concept of affective team climate in a sample of 33 health care workteams. Their results also showed that shared perceptions of team climate influence on collective affective responses. Thus, it points out that workteams` cognitive climate is a significant predictor of workteams’ affective climate. Their research supported longitudinal studies that showed a unidirectional relationship between perceptive climate and teams` affective outcomes (GonzálezRomá, Peiró, Lloret, Mañas & Muñoz, 1996). However, despite existing studies that confirm the validity of this kind of climate, research is not enough and it is necessary a greater empirical validation of this construct. Thus, our first objective is to test the existence of affective climates within workteams. Thus, we propose the following hypothesis: Affective Climate 9 Hypothesis 1. Team members will have similar affective work experiences in tension and optimism. 2. Affective climate dimensions and their role on team performance Affective climate, as other kinds of social climate, is a construct at a collective level that is operationalized as integration or combination of similar affective experiences at individual-level. To establish the validity of a construct operationalized at a collective level, we need composition models. These models specify the functional relationship among constructs operationalized at different levels of analysis (Chan, 1998). In the typology of composition models proposed by Chan (1998), the direct consensus model is likely the most familiar within multilevel researches and the most frequently used in climate studies (González-Romá, Peiró & Tordera., 2002). In direct consensus models, the isomorphic relationship between the specified constructs at different levels (e.g. individual and unit) is based, in the case of the affective climate, on the within-unit agreement among the individuals` affective responses. Once this agreement has been demonstrated (internal consistency criterion) and differences have been observed among units (differentiation criterion), it will be justified to obtain a measure of central tendency (e.g. average) to represent the unit’s affective climate. Thus, in this kind of model, within-unit agreement is considered a prerequisite to indicate that a construct exists at a higher level and that it can be operationalized. However, in another kind of model, dispersion models, within-unit dispersion is used as the operationalization of a unit-level construct. In this model, “the degree of within-group agreement of scores from the lower-level units of attributes… [is] a focal construct as opposed to merely a statistical prerequisite for aggregation” (Chan, 1998, 239). As Chan (1998) noted “dispersion is by definition a group-level characteristic…. Affective Climate 10 because it refers to the variability within a group and variance statistic in indexing an attribute of a group as opposed to an attribute of any individual-level response” (p.239). Dispersion models suggest that the groups vary in their level of homogeneity, in contrast with the direct consensus models which propose that the groups are homogeneous with respect to the construct of interest and different in the absolute level of construct (Klein, Conn, Smith & Sorra, 2001). An example of dispersion construct is the affective climate strength, conceptualized as the degree of within-unit agreement in the members` affective responses. Within-unit agreement (or its opposite, within-unit dispersion) is also a focal construct in Dispersion Theory (Brown & Kozlowski, 1999). This recent theory recognizes that higher-level constructs are originated in individual-level processes and they are developed through social interaction. In Dispersion Theory, unit-level construct would be operationalized as the degree of within-unit agreement. Within-unit agreement would indicate the degree of emergence of higher-level constructs (Brown & Kozlowski, 1999) and it is assumed that "units can be characterized by the extent to which a phenomenon has emerged as a meaningful unit characteristic" (Brown & Kozlowski, 1999, p. 6). Thus, workteams` affective climate can be characterized according to its intensity (high or low affective climate level) and its strength (high or low similarity among team members` affective experiences). To consider both affective climate dimensions is congruent with those authors who state that “group emotion can be examined through a variety of compositional perspective, including the mean emotions of the group members or the degree of emotional variance, or homogeneity, within the group” (Barsade & Gibson, 1998, p.89). Because conceptually both dimensions are different, we expect that they play different roles on team outcomes. To study the consequences Affective Climate 11 of these affective dimensions would provide us with a more complete vision of group affect. 2.1. Affective climate intensity as predictor of team performance Individual affective states, as fundamental components of human experience, have shown to have wide-ranging effects on cognitive processes, attitudes, and behaviors (Brief & Weiss, 2002; Pugh, 2001; Saavedra & Khun, 2000; Staw & Barsade, 1993; Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996). Thus, several authors have argued that worker behavior and productivity are directly affected by employee affect (Ashforth & Humphrey, 1995; Ashkanasy, Härtel & Zerbe, 2000; Fisher &Ashkanasy, 2000; Weiss & Compranzano, 1996). Nevertheless, until recently, little attention has been paid to the consequences of shared affects at work (Barsade, 2001). There is some research examining the relationship between the mean level of affect and various group processes and outcomes (Barsade & Gibson, 1998; Kelly & Barsade, 2001) as organizational spontaneity (George & Brief, 1992) and absenteeism and prosocial behaviour (George, 1990). Some authors have also paid attention to team performance (Barsade et al., 2000; George, 1995; Kelly, 2003; Kelly & Barsade, 2001). George (1995) noted that shared affect may affect to the helping behaviour or prosocial behaviour within the team and the team members` conceptions of their capabilities and their expectations about performance, and, therefore, to team efficacy. Bartel & Saavedra (2000) have asserted that collective affect may help to produce a normative affective aptitude for social situations and may affect members` motivation to attain collective goals (Hackman, 1992). Thus, certain group affects may help to promote congruence in members` attention effort and persistence which may stimulate well-coordinated patterns of behaviour toward collective goals. On the other hand, Barsade et al. (2000) argued that inducing positive Affective Climate 12 mood leads to greater creativity, more efficient cognitive-processing and better use of heuristics in complex decision-making tasks, as well as broadened categories for information sorting and greater flexibility in categorization. The relationship between collective affect and team performance has also been empirically supported by George (1995). She studied whether positive affective tone, operationalized as team mood in 65 sales teams, was related to team performance. Team performance was evaluated by branch managers who received a rating form for each of the teams in their branch included in the study. Her results showed that team positive affective tone was significantly and positively associated with perceived group performance. On the other hand, Totterdell (1999; 2000), examined the moodperformance relationship in cricketer teams. His studies showed that there was a significant association between team’s average happy mood and team’s subjective and objective performance. Thus, team performance was greater when team’s members were happier. Finally, Duffy and Shaw (2000) tested a model of impact of envy in groups in a longitudinal study with 143 student work-teams. They found a negative relationship between intra-team envy and several team outcomes as team satisfaction, absenteeism and team performance ratings by external observers. They also found that group envy led to greater social loafing and less cohesiveness and group potency, which was related to lessened team performance. Considering these theoretical arguments and empirical results, we hypothesize that: Hypothesis 2. Affective climate intensity will have direct effects on team performance. These effects will show a negative sign for group tension and a positive sign for group optimism. Affective Climate 13 2.2. The role of affective climate strength Scholars have shown their interest to examine collective affect through direct consensus models mainly, using the average to represent the group affect (Barsade & Gibson, 1998; Kelly, 2003; Kelly & Barsade, 2001). As Kelly & Barsade (2001) argue, “with regard to future work, it is important to move beyond an investigation of mean levels and to examine indices based on variance and dispersion as well” (p.113). Dispersion constructs, such as affective climate strength, are very scarce within organizational literature (Brown & Kozlowski, 1999; Klein et al., 2001). Studies on the role of personality differences (Barsade et al., 2000), demography diversity (Chatman, Polzer, Barsade & Neale, 1999; Klein et al., 2001; O´Really, Williams & Barsade, 1998; Shaw & Barret-Power, 1998; Sargent & Sue-Chan, 2001; Simons, Pelled & Smith, 1999; Timmerman, 2000; Williams & O´Really, 1998), leader’s charisma homogeneity (Klein & House, 1995), and organizational culture strength (Waldman & Yammarino, 1999) show the important role that similarities and differences among team members have in the different group process and organizational outcomes. Although climate strength has received little attention in studies of work units` climate (González-Romá et al., 2002), recently researchers have started to argue that climate strength could be considered as a valuable theoretical construct to understand work units` outcomes. Direct influence of affective climate strength. There is no clear consensus about how dispersion (or its opposite, homogeneity) influences on performance outcomes, because different dimensions of diversity may have different impacts (Bartel & Saavedra, 2000). Several researchers have argued that diversity within team has positive effects on team outcomes. These studies are based on the idea that the diversity in the Affective Climate 14 abilities, knowledge and aptitudes within the team encourages their outcomes. As Barsade et al. (2000) pointed out, team diversity “enhances the breadth of perspective viewpoints, cognitive resources, experiences, and general problem-solving ability of the team and that diversity can, therefore, help enhance performance” (p. 809). Regarding affective diversity, Barsade and Gibson (1998) point out that low levels of affective diversity, in particular circumstances, may also lead to negative consequences. Thus, those teams composed of members with high negative or positive affective homogeneity, especially when dealing with specific emotions such as anger or euphoria may be unproductive because they may need to be tempered in order for progress to be made. As Barsade & Gibson (1998) argue, “a team comprised of homogeneously positive people…. may contribute to unrealistic euphoria, optimism and groupthink. Conversely, a team comprised of all similarly low energy, pessimistic members could lead to lethargy, lack of productivity, and unrealistic cautions” (p.93). However, there are theoretical arguments and empirical results that show the negative effect of diversity on performance. Thus, as Barsade & Gibson (1998) argue, dispersion creates distance between group members, which influence negatively on various group processes and outcomes as trust, rapport, social integration and communication which in turn have a negative impact on team performance. The similarity (and attraction) created among group members is the most common theoretical rationale for predicting positive effects of group homogeneity. The similarity-attraction paradigm posits that individuals tend to be attracted to those who are similar to them (Green, Anderson & Shivers, 1996). As Barsade et al. (2000) highlighted, similarity causes attraction; so, people prefer to interact with other individuals or groups who have (or are perceived to have) attitudes and values similar to their own. Most of attraction research has been centred on personality variables, Affective Climate 15 attitudes and values. However, several authors argue that affect is a dimension on which people judge similarity to each other as well (Barsade & Gibson, 1998; George, 1996). They pointed out that affective similarity within a group should lead to higher levels of member attraction and cohesion. Thus, team members should feel more trust and rapport to each other, the communication and coordination within the team should be more fluid and they should engage in more cooperative behavior. This should lead to more positive outcomes (Barsade & Gibson, 1998). Empirical evidence has shown the negative effect of team affective dispersion on team processes and performance. Barsade et al. (2000), with a sample of 62 U.S. top management teams, found that positive affective diversity in these teams was negatively associated with team processes such as perceived conflict and cooperativeness. They also found that homogeneous teams in positive affect had higher company financial performance. Thus, we could expect that in high affective homogeneity conditions, team members feel more comfortable with each others` interpersonal interactions, generating more cooperation, communication, trust, social integration, and cohesion. This, in turn, is expected to positively influence group outcomes, so that the greater the affective homogeneity in a team, the greater the team performance. So, we hypothesize that: Hypothesis 3. Affective climate strength in tension and optimism will have positive effects on team performance. Moderator role of affective climate strength. Most of the climate strength studies have stressed that climate strength should be also considered as moderator of the relationship between work units` climate and units` processes and outcomes (Brown & Kowslowski, 1999; González-Romá et al., 2002; Ostroff & Bowen, 2000). As González-Romá et al. (2002) pointed out, “considering that even work units with the Affective Climate 16 same aggregate score on a climate facet can differ in extent within-unit agreement in climate perceptions, … climate strength must be accounted for in studying the relationship between work units` climate and work units` outcomes” (p. 466). The moderator influence of perceptual climate strength has been empirically supported. Scheneider, Salvaggio & Subirats (2002), in a sample of bank branches, found that perceptive climate strength moderated the relationship between unit’s climate and aggregated customer perceptions of service quality. They argued that, when climate for service is high and there is high climate strength, customer perceptions of service quality should be higher than when climate strength is low, because under strong climate conditions, they experience more consistent service. Likewise, in the study developed by González-Romá et al. (2002), results showed that homogeneity in climate’s perceptions (innovation climate and goals orientation climate) enhances the influence of team climate on units` satisfaction and commitment, so that this influence is only observed in teams whose members have homogeneous perceptions of the team. All these studies support that high climate strength would foster the impact of units` climate on units` outcomes. When the within-unit agreement is high, the influence of the climate on criterion variables would be more consistent and uniform, so that those outcomes become more predictable in strong climate conditions. In the same way, we argue that affective climates with high within-unit agreement in which individuals experience similar affective states at work (strong affective climate conditions) diminish the variability of associated performance appraisals. Conversely, in affective climates with low within-unit agreement in members` experiences, the variability of performance appraisals is larger. So, team performance will be more predictable in strong affective climate conditions than in weak ones. Barsade et al. (2000) tested, in an exploratory way, the interaction between mean level trait positive affect (calculated as Affective Climate 17 the average of the team members` trait positive affect scores), and positive affective diversity (degree of heterogeneity in trait positive affect at team level). They found that affective diversity moderated the relationship between teams trait positive affect and cooperation and conflict as team outcomes, so that “for group conflict and cooperativeness, being homogeneous compensated for low trait positive affect, and being high in trait positive affect compensated for being affectively diverse” (p.825). However, the interaction between affective diversity and mean level affective showed no relation to company financial performance. On the basis of this empirical evidence and theoretical rationales, we hypothesize the following: Hypothesis 4. Affective climate strength will moderate the relationship between team’s affective climate intensity and team performance, so that, when climate strength is high (i.e. affective climates with high within-group agreement in members` affective states) the influence of affective climate intensity on team performance will be enhanced and, conversely, when climate strenght is low (low homogeneity in members` affective experiences) the aforementioned influence will be weakened. 3. Sources of performance appraisal Regarding the sources of team performance appraisal, studies reviewed have used only one source of information. However, changes in organizational structures, processes and cultures “have combined to create the conditions where several sources of information have become not only more acceptable but more necessary” (Fletcher, 2001, p.477). Sources of information refer to who provides the information on team Affective Climate 18 processes and outcomes (Tesluk, Mathieu, Zaccaro & Marks, 1997). Usually, the source of performance appraisal is the manager (De Quijano, 1992). However, now, performance appraisal tend to be carried out by several sources: team members (Roberts, 2003), peers (Fletcher, 1997; Valle & Davis, 1999), subordinates (i.e. the appraisal of bosses by their subordinates) (Fletcher, 1997), external observers of the team such as customers or area managers (Hallam & Campbell, 1997; Tesluk et al., 1997) or even the combination of all them as it happens in the “360º (multirater) feedback” (Bozeman, 1997; Fletcher, 2001; Facteau & Craig, 2001; Scott & Einstein, 2001). In this case, the use of multiple sources provides multiple perspectives on team performance in a systematic way from those in good position to observe it (including members, subordinates, customers, as well as supervisors). As Tesluk et al. (1997) argue, “because different sources of information are more qualified to provide assessments on particular aspects of behavior and performance, several sources may be used in combination to provide a more holistic view of team functioning and effectiveness” (p. 205). Each source of information provides relevant and meaningful information. Who appraises will depend of the conception of performance appraisal, the structure of the organization and the quality of relationships within it (Fletcher, 1997). In our study, we use as sources of appraisal the team members (including the manager) and an external evaluator (the area manager). To use both sources has several advantages. Team members are best positioned to provide certain kinds of information. Thus, team members would be used to collect information on team processes and outcomes that are not obvious to outside observers (Tesluk et al., 1997). Team members possess valid, unique and relevant performance information and insight that is unavailable or unobservable by the external rater (Dickinson & McIntyre, 1997). Thus, the quality and quantity of performance appraisal increases leading to a more accurate Affective Climate 19 and valid rating (Roberts, 2003). However, members have a stake in team’s outcomes and might provide biased assessments for variables such as team performance (Tesluk et al., 1997). For it, other type of rater who differs according to their functional relationship to the team could serve as complementary source of information. Thus, we included in this study the appraisal of the area manager. To use only the area manager would have limitations as well, because, although the area manager knows the results of each team in his/her area, he/she may have several teams to deal with, or see them too infrequently to know how they are doing. Consequently, the area manager would not have certain type of information which would be relevant to performance appraisal. Thus, the limitations of an appraiser are compensated for the other appraiser and we can prevent the disadvantages of each source of appraisal. Method Sample The sample consisted of 581 employees, members of 133 workteams from several financial companies of Valencia (Spain). All companies had formally designated work units (teams). A work unit is defined here as a group in which all personnel report directly to the same supervisor and interact to complete unit tasks. Team size ranged from 2 to 14, and the average number of members per teams was 5.6 (s.d. 2.05). The response rate was high (93.3% percent). 54,4% percent of the sample were men and 45,6% women. Regarding subjects` age, 9,5% were below 25 years, 41,1% were between 25 and 35, 25,6% were between 36 and 45, 21,3% between 46 and 55, and, finally, 2,5% were above 55. Concerning academic level, 1,8% percent have primary level, 2,5% percent professional Affective Climate 20 qualification, 34,7% percent High School, 48,8% percent were University Graduates and Doctors. Procedure The human resources departments of several financial firms were contacted to ask for permission to administer the questionnaire in their teams. Once the permission was obtained, supervisors and employees were informed about the study and asked to voluntarily answer the questionnaires. A battery of questionnaires was applied in order to collect data from each work team. Questionnaires application was developed in three different ways. First, a meeting with the workteam was scheduled. During this meeting, which took place in the participants` office, a researcher explained the aims of the study and the instructions to fill the questionnaires. When it was possible, the researcher stayed in the office till participants finished answering the questionnaires; otherwise the researcher came back some days later and picked the questionnaires up. When scheduling a meeting was not possible, questionnaires were sent by mail with a letter that explained the aim of the study and the instructions to fill the questionnaires, and a pre-stamped envelope to return them. The first procedure was the most desirable on, because it allowed the explanation of doubts. However, confidentiality and anonymity of the responses were guaranteed in the three cases. Measures Affective Climate Intensity. It was measured by aggregation of individual scores of the Affective Well-being Scale constructed by Lloret and González-Romá (in press). This scale is composed by two affective climate dimensions, Tension and Optimism. We asked respondents to check “how much of the time in the past few weeks their job Affective Climate 21 had made them feel …”. Participants responded on a 5-point scale which ranged from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much). - Tension. This dimension (tension climate) was measured through three couples of opposed adjectives: Tense vs. Relaxed; Nervous vs. Quiet and Anxious vs. Calm. Subjects should respond to each of these six adjectives. The scores of the items relaxed, quiet and calm were reversed. The alpha coefficient was .90. - Optimism. This dimension was measured through three couples of opposed adjectives: Gloom vs. Cheerful; Pessimist vs. Optimist and Discouraged vs. Animated. The scores of the items gloom, pessimist and discouraged were reversed. The alpha coefficient was .91. Details of scale development, reliability and validity are presented by Lloret and González-Romá (in press). Climate Strength. Two indicators of climate strength were used: one for tension and the other for optimism. Climate strength was operationalized as the degree of within-team agreement on these measures. Within-team agreement was measured by means of the Average Deviation index (ADM(J)). Since this index is a direct measure of within-team variability, the values provided by the ADM(J) index regarding each climate scale were multiplied by –1 so that higher scores represented higher within-team agreement and higher climate strength (González-Romá et al., 2002). Team Performance. Team performance was measured as the responses to the following questions on a 5-point Likert scale: “How do you think your work unit performs?” and “which is the quality of the work that your team achieves?”. The first question is taken from Jehn’s et al. (1999) Members´ Perceived Group Performance Affective Climate 22 Questionnaire. The second one was own elaborated. Team performance was evaluated by team members (members and manager) and by an external rater (area manager). Alpha coefficient for teams’ perceived team performance was .80 and .78 for external evaluators’ perceived team performance. Control Variables. Group size and Team tenure were control variables in this study because the literature on groups has noted that size and tenure are key variables influencing affective reactions and group homogeneity (Jehn, 1995; Kelly, 2003). George (1996) suggested that groups working together over time should come to display similar levels of positive or negative affects. Thus, as this author indicates, in those teams whose members are changing constantly, an affective tone can not develop because the Attraction-Selection-Attrition process would not operate and it could not produce similarity in group. As Barsade et al. (2000) indicate “team tenure becomes an important component or moderator of the similarity-attraction process” (p. 827). In addition, Kelly (2003) indicated that as team size increases, other processes as cohesiveness, norm enforcement decrease and member participation become more disproportionate. As a consequence, one would expect less homogeneity of affect within these teams. Past research also showed that group size influence group performance (Gladstein, 1984; Jehn, 1995). We measured those control variables by asking team managers to obtain information about “Team size" (“How many people are in the team that you manage now, including yourself?”) and “Team Tenure” (“How long does your team take working with the current members”). Affective Climate 23 Data Analysis Descriptive statistics (mean and standard deviation) were obtained and correlation matrix was carried out as previous step to hypothesis contrast. In order to test the hypothesis 1, the Average Deviation Index (Burke, Finkelstein and Dusin, 1999) and Analysis of the Variance (ANOVA) were calculated. The impact of affective climates on perceived performance (Hypothesis 2 to 4) was estimated by a series of hierarchical multiple regression analyses (Cohen & Cohen, 1983). Hierarchical regression analyses were performed to identify the main effects of climate intensity and main and moderator effects of climate strength of the two affective climate dimensions. The independent variables and interaction terms were entered into the regression equation in four successive steps. In step 1, the control variables were entered as a set. In step 2, the predictor variable was entered into the regression equation. Significant effects here would indicate that affective climate intensity had a direct influence on the perceived team performance. In step 3, the moderator variable was entered into the equation. Significant effects would indicate that affective climate strength had a direct influence on perceived team performance. Finally, in step 4, the interaction term was entered into the equation. Significant effects would indicate that affective climate strength moderate the relationship between affective climate intensity and team performance. The usual procedure is to compare the increase of the explained variance (R²) in each step. Each block of steps was made for each one of the two dimensions of affective climate on each criterion variable. In total, four hierarchical regression analyses were performed. We used Z scores as the standardized measure of affective climate, thus avoiding problems of multicolinearity arising from correlations between product terms and their component parts (Sivasubramaniam, Murry, Avolio & Jung, 2002). When Affective Climate 24 evaluating the significance of predicted effects we used one-tailed tests, which are suitable for directional hypotheses (Pelled, Eixenhardt & Xin, 1999). To interpreter each significant interaction, we took an additional analytical step. In this analysis, we examined the functional form of the interactions between independent variable and moderator variable. As indicated Pelled, Eixenhardt & Xin (1999), this method “is appropriate for interactions involving two continuous variables and avoids the information loss associated with median split procedures” (p. 18). First we took a partial derivate from the regression equation to determine mathematically whether the moderated relationship was monotonic or nonmonotonic (for revision, see Schoonhoven, 1981). A nonmonotonic effect is when, in function of portion of the observed range of the moderator variable, the relationship between independent and dependent variable changes its sign, that is, the relationship is negative over a portion of the observed range of the moderator variable and positive over the remainder of its range. A monotonic effect is when the relationship between both variables does not change its sign over the range of the moderator variable. To determine it, we estimate the point² on the range of moderator variable, at which the independent variable has no effect on the dependent variable (i.e. the point of inflection of the partial relation dy/dx³). If this point falls within the observed range of moderator variable, it indicates that the effect of independent variable on dependent variable changes its signs. We then carried out the graphing of the partial derivate from the regression equations (relation dy/dx). Each graph expresses the effect of the independent variable on the criterion variable over the range of the moderator variable, that is, the change in perceived team performance, given a change in affective climate intensity, over the range of affective climate strength. Affective Climate 25 Results All the statistical analyses conducted for testing the study hypotheses were performed at the team level. All variables were assessed at the individual level. In order to meaningfully aggregate individual responses to the group level, differentiation and internal consistency criterion must be demonstrated (James, 1982). Within-team agreement was estimated by means of the Average Deviation index (ADM(J)) of Burke, Finkelstein and Dusin (1999). This index is based on the calculation of the average deviation for each scale item and has several advantages compared to other indexes as interrater agreement index (rwg), developed by James, Demaree and Wolf (1984), which is the most frequently used index, or the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) (for revision see Burke et al., 1999). In order to interpret it, a null response range must be specified. Burke and colleagues (1999) recommend using a null response range equal to or less than 1 when the response scale is a Likert-type 5-point scale. Accordingly, we concluded there was within-unit agreement when the AD M(J) values were equal to or less than 1. This index was calculated for each team on each of the individual level variables (affective work experiences and teams´ perceived group performance). The obtained values were then averaged across the 133 teams. The mean AD M(J) for tension climate was 0.63 (sd=0.21), for optimism climate 0.62 (sd=0.25) and for teams´ perceived group performance was 0.24 (sd=0.16). Thus, we concluded that the level of within-team agreement was sufficient to aggregate scores to the work-unit level Discrimination among teams' scores on each affective climate facet and perceived group performance was investigated by means of a number of one-way ANOVAs. If the variation between workteams' scores is greater than the variation within workteams, then it can be concluded that studied variables relate to the particular teamwork and not Affective Climate 26 to a higher unit level. The observed F values were significant (tension climate: F (132, 447) = 3.07, p<0.01; optimism climate: F (132, 447) = 2.01, p<0.01 and teams´ perceived team performance: F (132, 579) = 2.10, p<0.01) for all three variables they were assessed and they showed that there was significant between-team differentiation (Chan, 1998). Given sufficient within-group agreement and between group differences, participants´ individual responses regarding members´ affective states and perceived performance were aggregated to the group level by calculating the mean value within each group. Descriptive Statistics and Correlations Means, standard deviations, correlations, and reliability estimates are provided in Table 1. The alpha coefficients show sufficient internal consistency in every variable since, without exception, Cronbach´s α meets the criterion of .70 (Nunnaly, 1978). PLEASE INSERT TABLE 1 ABOUT HERE Both affective climate intensity measures (tension and optimism) were significantly and negatively associated (r = -.61, p<.01). Tension climate strength was positively correlated with optimism climate strength (r = .41 p<.01). Examining the relationship between affective climate strength and intensity, optimism climate intensity was positively associated with optimism climate strength (r= .35, p<.01). However, there is not a significantly correlation between tension climate intensity and strength within the team (r=.06, n.s.). The optimism climate intensity was positively correlated with the two perceived performance measures (team members: r = .45, p<.01; and external evaluator: r = .19, Affective Climate 27 p<.05). However, the tension climate intensity was only correlated with members perceived team performance (r = -.36, p< .01). Regarding affective climate strength, a correlation between optimism climate strength and teams` perceived performance was observed (r=.30, p<.01). Positive correlations between the two performance measures could be expected. This would indicate a good internal consistency level of performance measures. However, the external evaluators` perceived performance was not related with the other team performance measure (r=.11, n.s.). Hypotheses Testing 1. Within-team agreement in affective responses. As it indicated above, in order to test Hypothesis 1, the Average Deviation index was computed for each workteam and each affective variable. The average and median ADM(J) values, below the critical point of 1 (Burke et al., 1999) for each affective variable were as follows: tension climate: .63 and .64 and optimist climate: .62 and .60. Discrimination among teams´ scores on each affective variable was investigated by means of one-way ANOVAs. The F ratios obtained for each affective variable were the following: tension climate: F (132, 447) = 3.07, p<0.01; optimism climate: F (132, 447) = 2.01, p<0.01. These results confirmed that shared affective states relate to the particular teamwork and not to a higher unit level. 2. Influence of work teams´ affective climate intensity and strength on team performance In order to test the effects of affective climate intensity and strength on perceived team performance, we performed a hierarchical multiple regression analysis (Cohen & Cohen, 1983). Hypothesis 2 suggested that tension climate intensity would Affective Climate 28 have a negative influence on team performance and optimist climate intensity would have a positive influence on performance. As tables 2 and 3 show, this influence was supported. PLEASE INSERT TABLE 2 ABOUT HERE The results of the regression analyses showed that tension climate intensity negatively predicted the teams` perceived performance (table 2, B= -.33, p< .01), and external evaluators` perceived performance (table 2, B= -.18, p< .05). So, in those teams in which tension affective climate is high, the level of perceived team performance is lower. PLEASE INSERT TABLE 3 ABOUT HERE On the other hand (see table 3) optimism climate intensity had a positive influence on teams` perceived performance (B= .35, p< .01) and external evaluators` perceived performance (B= .24, p< .01). So, the greater optimism climate within the team, the greater will be the perception of team performance. Hypotheses 3 indicated that affective climate strength would have a positive effect on team performance. The results of hierarchical regression analysis showed that optimism climate strength (table 3) predicted the teams` perceived performance (B= .14, p< .05). Thus, the greater degree of homogeneity in the optimism’s affective experiences within the team, the greater will be the teams` perceived performance. Tension climate strength (table 2) did not have significant effects on teams` perceived performance (B=.-.02, n.s.) and external evaluators` perceived performance (B=-.01, n.s.). Affective Climate 29 3. Moderator influence of affective climate strength In order to test the moderator influence of tension and optimism climate strength on the relationship between affective climate intensity and perceived team performance, we carried out moderator regression analyses. Hypothesis 4 proposed that climate strength in tension and in optimism would increase the relationship between climate intensity in tension and optimism, respectively, on team performance. The results of the hierarchical regression analyses showed that the influence of affective climate intensity on team performance was significantly moderated by affective climate strength. The interaction predicted the team performance in two of the four hierarchical regression analyses carried out. Tables 2 and 3 show moderator effect of climate strength on the relationship between optimism climate intensity and teams` perceived team performance (table 3, B= -.12, p< .05), and tension climate intensity and external evaluators` perceived team performance (table 2, B= .16, p< .05). The interactions are displayed in figure 1 and 2. Figures show how the relationship between independent and criterion variable changes (vertical axis) when the value of moderator variable changes (horizontal axis). Partial derivate analyses revealed that the effect of optimism climate intensity on team performance was monotonic over the range of climate strength observed in our sample (i.e. the relationship between independent and criterion variable has a single sign over the observed range of the moderator variable) and the effect of tension climate intensity on team performance was nonmonotonic (i.e. the negative relationship between tension climate and external evaluators` perceived performance changed its sign in a portion of range of the Affective Climate 30 moderator variable). That is, when the homogeneity in tension was large, the relationship had positive sign although its magnitude was low and it was near to zero. PLEASE INSERT FIGURE 1 ABOUT HERE PLEASE INSERT FIGURE 2 ABOUT HERE Graphical displays showed that the effects were weaker for higher levels of tension and optimism climate strength. Hypothesis 4 proposed when climate strength is high, the influence of climate intensity on team performance variable would be high. Contrarily, our results showed that the relationship between the independent variable and the criteria was weaker for the high climate strength. Discussion The aim of the present study was to examine the existence of affective climate within the workteams. Likewise, it analyzed the influence of affective climate, intensity and strength, on team performance as well as the moderator role of affective climate strength in the relationship between workteams affective climate intensity and their performance. Hypothesis 1 indicated that team members have similar affective experiences. Results provide substantial support for it. It was observed that bank professionals belonging to the same team had similar levels of tension and optimism. Further, there were significant differences among workteams` tension and optimism. This discrimination among teams supported that shared affective states could be relate to the particular teamwork and not to a higher unit level. Thus, results confirm the existence of affective climates in workteams. These results are congruent with those obtained by other researchers (González-Romá et al., 2000; Peiró, et al., 2000). As González-Romá Affective Climate 31 et al., (2000) argue, when new concepts appear, “it is necessary to study their validity and usefulness in improving our understanding of organizational behavior” (p.107). Empirical research on the validity of this kind of climate is still scarce and, because of it, we believe our study makes an important contribution to the establishment of this new climate type in the environment of organizational psychology. This study also shows that team affective climate may have important consequences for organizations. Results showed direct effects of affective climate intensity on team performance (hypothesis 2): tension climate intensity had negative effects on external evaluators` perceived performance and teams´ perceived performance. Furthermore, optimism climate intensity had positive effects on external evaluators` perceived performance and teams` perceived performance. These results show that negative and positive affective climate within the team influences, negatively and positively respectively, on performance appraisal carried out by the area manager and team itself. Thus, when affective climate is positive and members feel more cheerful, optimist and animated, external evaluators` and teams` perceived performance will be higher that when affective climate is negative and members feel more tension, nervousness and anxiety. Our results are congruent with others theoretical works which states that team affect has a significant effect on group performance (Barsade & Gibson, 1998; George, 1996; Kelly & Barsade, 2001). Thus, as indicated by Kelly (2003), affective tone is often cited as important factor in many successful groups. The discovered effects show also the same direction as do the effects reported by George (1995), Totterdell (1999; 2000) and Duffy and Shaw (2000). George (1995) found that team positive affective tone would be significantly associated with perceived team performance by external evaluators (branch managers). Similarly, Totterdell`s (1999; 2000) results showed that Affective Climate 32 team’s positive affect positively influences on team’s subjective and objective performance. Finally, Duffy and Shaw (2000) found that negative group affect (envy) was directly and negatively related to group performance. In conclusion, our study offers additional direct evidence about the influence of group affect on team behavior. Hypotheses 3 stated that homogeneity in shared affective experiences within the team would influence on team performance positively. Our results showed that optimism climate strength had a positive influence on teams` perceived performance, so that the greater the affective homogeneity in optimism within the team, the greater teams` perceived performance. Several authors (Barsade & Gibson, 1998; George, 1996) obtained similar results and they argued that members` positive affective similarity would lead positive outcomes for the team, among them a better performance. Moreover, our results are congruent with scarce empirical studies on affective diversity within organizational literature. Barsade et al. (2000) found that homogeneity in positive affect within the team affect positively on company financial performance, so that, the greater homogeneous in teams` positive affect, the greater financial performance. Homogeneity in negative affect did not have any effect on financial performance in Barsade`s et al. (2000) study. We neither found a relationship between negative affectivity diversity and outcome variables. One possible explanation for this may be the fact that, as George (1996) indicated, both dimensions of affect, positive and negative, “are caused by different factors, have differential relationships with behaviors.., and have different consequences for individuals and organizations” (pp. 7879). In conclusion, our results highlight the relevant role affective climate strength plays in workteam performance. As pointed out in the dispersion literature (Brown & Affective Climate 33 Kozlowski, 1999; Klein et al., 2001; Kozlowski & Klein, 2000) to get a complete picture of group dynamics one must take into account dispersion measures as well as mean levels of group variables. Future studies should investigate the impact of diversity in shared affective responses on team functioning and effectiveness. The moderator role of affective climate strength was partially supported (Hypothesis 4). Tension climate strength moderated the impact of tension climate intensity on external evaluators` perceived performance but not on teams` perceived performance. On the other hand, optimism climate strength moderated the influence of optimism climate intensity on teams` perceived performance, but not on external evaluators` perceived performance. These results are congruent with the idea that climate strength affects the predictability of affective climate intensity on team outcomes (Lindell & Brandt, 2000). However, in both cases, an unexpected sign was observed. Tension climate strength weakened the influence of tension climate intensity on external evaluators` perceived performance and optimism climate strength weakened the influence of optimism climate intensity on teams` perceived performance. These moderator effects show the contrary direction that those reported by other authors. In fact, Scheneider et al. (2002) found that high perceptive climate strength fostered the impact of units` climate on customer perceptions of service quality, and the relationship disappeared when climate strength was low. On the other hand, GonzálezRomá`s et al. (2002) results show that units` perceptive climate strength moderated the relationships between the goals orientation and innovation climate and two criterion variables, work satisfaction and organizational commitment, so that these relationships were stronger in strong climate conditions than in weak climate conditions. Finally, Barsade`s et al. (2000) study suggested that teams with low positive affect and high affective homogeneity had levels of cooperativeness and conflict similar to those teams Affective Climate 34 with high positive affect. But teams with low levels of homogeneity and low positive affect were significantly lower in cooperation and higher in conflict. As we mentioned before, there is no clear consensus about how the heterogeneity, or its opposite, influences team’s outcomes. Given the ambiguity in the literature which shows arguments in favour of benefits of heterogeneity and arguments against them, it is possible that homogeneity within the team play a different moderator role for different team processes and outcomes. It may be that climate strength would not always have the same role on the relationships between teams` climate and certain team’s outcomes. As Barsade and Gibson (1998) pointed out, although there is a relationship between team heterogeneity and team processes and outcomes, the nature and direction of that relationship may vary depending on the type of diversity and type of outcomes, and even, as in this case, depending on each type of affect. Thus, our study, partially, supports the influence of affective homogeneity within the team on the magnitude of the relationship between the affective climate and the team’s outcomes. However, it is necessary to continue researching on moderator role of affective climate strength because its role seems to be different for different types of conditions and team outcomes. Regarding different results among both sources of performance appraisal, as we observed before, the two performance measures did not correlate. It could be that external evaluator (area manager) applied different criteria and standard in his/her evaluation than team members did, what would cause those inter-observers differences. As De Quijano (1992) argues, the low correlation among two appraisers` scores does not necessarily demonstrate low reliability of the performance measure. The lack of convergence between different sources is a consistent finding in the empirical literature on performance appraisal systems (Facteau & Craig, 2001; Zammuto, London & Affective Climate 35 Rowland, 1982), which has shown that different rater groups (e.g. team members and area manager) frequently do not agree concerning job performance (Bozeman, 1997). As the most desirable situation, we would hope that the ratings obtained from various sources would display inter-rater agreement. However, as Valle and Davis (1999) point out, there are a series of reasons for that multiple raters may not come to the same conclusions concerning team performance. Among them, these authors suggest the following: organizational level, job knowledge, information processing tendencies or rating styles, rater motivations, and perceptual biases (Harris, 1994). On the other hand, Facteau and Craig (2001) suggest that the failure of observed ratings from different sources to converge may be less a function of the measurement system than of substantive differences between the rater groups (e.g. different rater groups may have different conceptualizations of what constitutes effective performance in a particular job). Bozeman (1997) pointed out that the performance ratings provided by different rater groups are role-related, so that, various rater groups likely evaluate the aspects of the performance that are most relevant to the raters themselves. Baruch and Harel (1993) suggested that different rating sources have different opportunities to observe rated work behavior and, therefore, they have different perspectives. Similarly, Lance, Teachout and Donnelly (1992), noted that the low convergence among different types of rates may be due to the fact that different rater groups could be exposed to only a part of rate behavior. For it, some scholars have asserted that ratings obtained from different rater groups may be valid, even if they do not exhibit high levels of agreement with one another (Bozeman, 1997). As De Quijano (1992) argued, this gives support to the thesis that the really important factor is the evaluator and not the instrument, because there are not such powerful evaluation instruments that can neutralize the huge variance provided by evaluators. However, it is necessary to continue to look for the reliability and Affective Climate 36 validity in inter-rater measures (Valle & Davis, 1999). To provide clear and objective information to the appraisers or to offer them objective standards of performance could help to reduce such variance (Schrader & Steiner, 1996). One concern here might be that the observed relationships may be the result of common method variance due to exclusive use of self-report measures. Thus, a potential limitation of the finding that showed the role of affective climate (intensity and strength) on teams` perceived performance is the fact that the source for all of these variables was the members themselves and this makes the identification of direct and moderator effects more difficult because same source data tend to be correlated. However, as we use data which come from different sources for team performance measure, the potential limitation concerning of these observed relations would be partially mitigated by examining the findings that showed as affective climate, in tension and optimism, has direct effects on external evaluators` perceived performance and affective climate strength moderates those effects. As Schmitt (1994) points out, the use of measures from distinct sources can avoid the effects of this bias. However, this study is not without limitations. First, considering the crosssectional nature of study, we cannot provide conclusions about the causal ordering between the variables. Thus, the results obtained should be interpreted with caution. Further, affective climate is a dynamic construct. In future research, therefore, it is necessary panel designs with various data collections. Thus, we could approach to its dynamic nature. Consideration of different temporal intervals will allow to test causal relationships and to detect changes in dynamic constructs which could have periods of latency and effects with different duration. Second, units sampled for this study met the within-unit agreement criterion for aggregation of unit members` affective states. This means that if individuals within Affective Climate 37 teams have similar affective reactions, there will be relatively low variance in these reactions. To the extent that affective reactions are similar within teams, this reduction in variance or restriction of range will attenuate their consequences (Cohen & Cohen, 1983) within teams. So, our results suffer from restriction of range problems, which have probably attenuated the hypotheses test. However, and although a specific withinunit agreement criterion is met, climate strength had significant direct effects on team performance and moderator effects on the relationship between affective climate intensity and team performance. Third, the sample of teams used in the present study is relatively homogeneous (it only includes bank professionals). This restricts the generalization of our results. Finally, a last limitation would be the use of subjective measures of team performance. Replications with a different kind and more objective measure of team performance may be useful. Furthermore, the appropriate measures of performance could vary across organizations. However, subjective performance measures have been used by studies which have shown the important role of the groups’ emotions in the individual and organizational outcomes (George, 1995; Health & Jourden, 1997; Totterdell, 2000). In conclusion, and these limitations notwithstanding, the results of this study have shown that the team members share affective experiences and that they could be empirically diagnosed. Moreover, our study suggests that team affective climate, in its dimensions of intensity and strength, influences on team performance and affective climate strength is a significant factor to understand the link between team affective climate intensity and team performance. Thus, due to the affective climate’s influence on team outcomes, its nature, antecedents, and consequences at work are important areas for future research. Affective Climate 38 Our study would also show that when examining affects, one needs to take into account not only the mean affective level of each group but also the group's affective diversity. Our data support the importance of the diversity in current researches within work and organizational psychology. Further, it provides some empirical evidence supporting Dispersion Theory of Brown and Kozlowski (1999) and demonstrates that “climate strength” is a meaningful and valid construct and it may have an important role in the understanding of the consequences of work-units` climate. Moreover, our study shows that dispersion constructs should be examined in future research if we want to understand better the factors that are related to team performance (González-Romá et al., 2002). With respect to the more general topic of multilevel research in organizations, affective climate is a multilevel construct and it could be conceptualized and operationalized at different analysis levels within organizations (González-Romá & Peiró, 1999). It would be interesting to examine the relationships between affective climate, at different analysis levels, and performance at different levels as well. As González-Romá and Peiró (1999) indicate, so we could contribute to the realization of organizational behavior as an integrated science. Beyond theoretical interest, results of this study have practical implications. They confirm that affective climate within the team is an important factor which impact on performance. It could help managers make decisions about the factors to consider when deciding how to design their teams. So, positive effects of beneficial affective climate on team performance would be enhanced. Affective Climate 39 References Ashforth, B.E. & Humphrey, R.H. (1995). Emotion in the workplace: a reappraisal. Human Relations, 48, pp. 97-125 Ashkanasy, N.M., Härtel, C.E. & Zerbe, W.J. (2000). Emotions in the workplace. Research, Theory, and Practice. In N.M. Ashkanasy, C.E. Häbel, and W.J. Zerbe (eds.), Emotions in the workplace. Research, Theory, and Practice. (pp. 3-18). London: Quorum Books. Barsade, S.G. (2001). The ripple effect: Emotional contagion in groups. Working Paper, Yale School of Management, Yale University, New Haven, CT. Barsade, S.G. & Gibson, D.E. (1998). Group emotion: a view from top and bottom. In D. Grenfeld, B. Manmix & M. Neak (eds), Research on Managing Groups and Teams (pp.81-102). Stanford, CT: JAI Press Barsade, S.G., Ward, A.J., Turner, J.D. & Sonnenfeld, J.A. (2000). To your heart´s content: a model of affective diversity in top management teams. Administrative Science Quarterly, 45, pp. 802-836 Bartel, C. & Saavedra, R. (2000). The collective construction of work group moods. Administrative Science Quarterly, 45 (2), pp. 1a97-231 Baruch, Y. & Harel, G. (1993). Multi-source performance appraisal: an empirical and methodological note. Public Administration Quarterly, 17 (1), pp.96-111 Bozeman, D.P. (1997). Interrater agreement in multi-source performance appraisal: a commentary. Journal of organizational behavior, 18 (3), pp. 313-316 Brief, A.P. & Weiss, H.M. (2002). Organizational behavior. Affect in the workplace. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, pp. 279-307 Affective Climate 40 Brown, K.G. & Kozlowski, S.W. (1999). Dispersion Theory: Moving Beyond a Dichotomous Conceptualization of Emergent Organizational Phenomena. Klein (Discussant) symposium “New perspectives on Higher Level Phenomena in Industrial/Organizational Psychology” presented at the Fourteenth Annual Meeting of the Society of Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Atlanta G.A. Burke, M.J., Finkelstein, L.M. & Dusig, M.S. (1999). On Average Deviation Indices for Estimating Interrater Agreement. Organizational Research Methods, 2 (1), pp. 49-68 Chan, D. (1998). Functional relationships among constructs in the same content domain at different levels of analysis: A Typology of composition models. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, pp. 234-246 Chatman, J.A., Polzer, J.T., Barsade, S.G. & Neale, M.A. (1999).Being different yet feeling similar: The influence of demographic composition on work processes and outcomes. Administrative Science Quarterly, 43, pp. 749-780 Cohen, J. & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analyses for the behavioural sciences. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. De Quijano, S.D. (1992). Sistemas efectivos de evaluación del rendimiento: resultados y desempeños. Barcelona: PPU De Rivera, J. (1992). Emotional climate: Social Structure and Emotional Dynamics. International Review of Studies on Emotion, 2, pp. 197-218 Dickinson, T.L. & McIntyre, R.M. (1997). A conceptual framework for teamwork measurement. In M.T. Brannick, E. Salas & C, Prince (Eds.), Team performance assessment and measurement, (pp.19-43). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Affective Climate 41 Domagalski, T.A. (1999). Emotion in organizations: main currents. Human Relations, 52 (6), pp. 833-852 Facteau, J.D. & Craig, S.B. (2001). Are performance appraisal ratings from different rating sources comparable?. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86 (2), pp. 215217 Fernández-Dols, J.M. (1998). Estudios sobre clima emocional. In R.A. Baron and D. Bayne (Eds.), Psicología social, (pp.99-101). Madrid: Prentice Hall Fisher, C.D. (1986). Organizational socialization: an integrative review. In K.M. Rowland & G.R. Ferris (Eds.). Research in Personnel and Human Resources Management, vol.4. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, pp. 101-145 Fisher, C.D. & Ashkanasy, N.M. (2000). The emerging role of emotions in work life: an introduction. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, pp.123-129 Fletcher, C. (1997). Performance appraisal in context: organizational changes and their impact on practice. In N.Andersen & P. Herriot (Eds.), International Handbook of Selection and Assessment (pp.567-580). Chichester: Wiley Fletcher, C. (2001). Performance appraisal and management: The developing research agenda. Journal of occupational and organizational psychology, 74, 4, pp. 473-487 George, J. M. & Brief, A.P. (1992). Feeling good-doing: A conceptual analysis of the mood at work-organizational spontaneity relationship. Psychological Bulletin, 112 (2), pp. 310-329 George, J.M (1990). Personality, affect, and behaviour in Groups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75 (2), pp. 107-116 George, J.M. (1995). Leader positive mood and group performance: The case of customer service. Journal applied social psychology, 25, pp.778-794 Affective Climate 42 George, J.M. (1996). Group affective tone. In M. A. West (Ed.), Handbook of work group psychology (pp.77-94). Chichester: John Wiley and Sons. Gladstein, D.L. (1984). A model of task group effectiveness. Administrative Science Quarterly, 29, pp. 499-517 González-Romá, V. & Peiró, J.M. (1999). Clima en las organizaciones laborales y en los equipos de trabajo (Climate in workteams and labor organizations). Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada, 52 (2-3), pp. 269-285. González-Romá, V., Peiró, J.M., Lloret, S., Mañas, M.A. & Muñoz, P. (1996). Influences of workteam climate on individual outcomes: a longitudinal study. Paper presented at the XXVI International Congress of Psychology, 16-21 August, Montreal, Canadá González-Roma, V., Peiró, J.M., Subirats, M., & Mañas, M.A. (2000), The Validity of Affective Workteam Climates. En M. Vartiainen, F. Avallone, & N. Anderson, (Ed.), Innovative Theories, Tools, and Practices in Work and Organizational Psychology (pp. 97-109). Göttingen, Hogrefe & Huber Publishers. González-Romá, V., Peiró, J.M. & Tordera, N. (2002). An examination of the antecedents and moderator influences of climate strength. Journal of applied psychology, 87 (83), pp. 465-473 Green, S.G., Anderson, S.E. & Shivers, S.L. (1996). Demographic and organizational influences on leader-member exchange and related work attitudes. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 66, pp. 203-214 Gump, B.B. & Kulik, J.A. (1997). Stress, affiliation and Emotional contagion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72 (2), pp. 305-319 Affective Climate 43 Hackman, R.J. (1992). Group influences on individuals in organizations. In M.D. Dunnette & L.M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology (2nd ed., pp. 199-267). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press Hallam, G. & Campbell, D. (1997). The measurement of team performance with a standardized survey. In M.T. Brannick, E. Salas & C, Prince (Eds.), Team performance assessment and measurement (pp.155-171). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Harris, M.M. (1994). Rater motivation in the performance appraisal context: a theoretical framework. Journal of Management, 20, pp. 737-56 Hatfield, E., Caccioppo, J.T. & Brapson, M.L. (1994). Emotional Contagion. NY: Cambridge University Press. Health, C. & Jourden, F.J. (1997). Illusion, disillusion, and the buffering effect of groups. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 69 (2), pp. 103-116. Hurley, J.R. (1997). Interpersonal theory and measures of outcome and emotional climate in 111 personal development groups. Group Dynamics, 1, pp. 86-97 James, L. (1982). Aggregation bias in estimates of perceptual measures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67, pp. 219-229 James, L., Dewaree R.G. & Wolf, G. (1984). Estiming within-group interrater reliability with and without responses bias. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, pp. 8598 Jehn, K.A. (1995).A multhimethod examination of the benefits and detriments of intragroup conflict. Administrative Science Quarterly, 40, pp.256-282 Jehn, K., Northcraft, G.B., & Neale, M.A. (1999). Why differences make a difference: A field study of diversity, conflict and performance in workgroups. Administrative Science Quarterly, 44 (4), pp. 741-763 Affective Climate 44 Kavanagh, D.J., O´Halloran, P., Manicavasagar, V., Clark, D., Piatkowska, O., Tennant, C. & Rosen, A. (1997). The family Attitude Scale: reliability and validity of a new scale for measuring the emotional climate of families. Psychiatry Research, 70, pp. 185-195 Kelly, J.R. & Barsade, S.G. (2001). Mood and emotion in small groups and workteams. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 86 (1), pp.99-130 Kelly, J.R. (2003). Mood and emotion in groups. In M. Hogg & S. Tindale (Eds.) Blackweell Handbook in Social Psychology. Vol. 3: Group processes (pp.164-181). Malden: Blackweell Publishing Kirton, M.J. & McCarthy, R.M. (1988). Cognitive climate and organizations. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 61, pp.175-184 Klein, K.J. & House, R.J. (1995). On fire: Charismatic leadership and levels of analysis. Leadership Quarterly, 6, pp.183-198 Klein, K.J., Conn, A.B., Smith, D.B. & Sorra, J.S. (2001). Is every one in agreement? An exploration of within-group agreement in employee perceptions of the work environment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, pp. 3-16 Kozlowski, S.W. & Klein, K.J. (2000). A Multilevel approach to Theory and Research in Organizations: Contextual, Temporal, and Emergent Processes. In K.J. Klein & S.W. Kozlowski (Eds.), A Multilevel Theory, Research, and Methods in Organizations. Foundations, Extensions and New Directions (pp. 3-90). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Lance, C.E., Teachout, M.S. & Donnelly, T.M. (1992). Specification of the criterion construct space: An application of hierarchical confirmatory factor analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, pp. 437-452 Affective Climate 45 Lindell, M.K. & Brandt, C.J. (2000). Climate quality and climate consensus and mediators of the relationship between organizational antecedents and outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, pp. 331- 348. Lloret, A. & González-Romá, V. (in press). How do respondents construe ambiguous response formats of affect ítems?. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Muchinski, P.M. (2000). Emotions in the workplace: the neglect of organizational behavior. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, pp. 801-805 Nunnaly, J.D. (1978). Psychometric theory. NY: McGraww-Fill O´Really, C.A., Williams, K.Y. & Barsade, S. (1998). Group demography and innovation: Does Diversity help?. Research on Managing Groups and Teams, 1, pp. 183-207. Ostroff, C. & Bowen, D.E. (2000). Moving HR to a higher level: HR practices and organizational effectiveness. In K.J., Klein & S.W. Kozlowski (Eds), A Multilevel Theory, Research, and Methods in Organizations. Foundations, Extensions and New Directions (pp. 3-90). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Paez, D., Ruiz, J.I., Gailly, O., Kornblit, A.L., Wiesenfeld, E. & Vidal, C.M. (1997). Clima emocional: su concepto y medición mediante una investigación transcultural. Revista de Psicología Social, 12 (1), pp. 79-98 Peiró, J.M., Gonzalez-Romá, V., Tordera, N. & Belmonte, J. (2000). Emotional climates at work: Determinants of the formation process of collective emotions. Paper presented at the XXVII International Congress of Psychology. Stockholm, Sweden, 23-28 July Affective Climate 46 Pelled, L.H., Eisenhardt, K.M. & Xin, K.R. (1999).Exploring the Black Box: An analysis of work group diversity, conflict and performance. Administrative Science Quarterly, 44 (1), pp.1-28 Pugh, S.D. (2001). Service with a smile: emotional contagion in the service encounter. Academy of Management Journal, 44 (5), pp. 1018-1027 Roberts, G.E. (2003). Employee performance appraisal system participation: A technique that works. Public Personnel Management, 32, 1, pp. 89-94. Saavedra, R. & Khun, S.K. (2000). Affective States in job characteristics theory. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, pp. 131-146 Sargent, L.D. & Sue-Chan, C. (2001). Does diversity affect group efficacy? The intervening role of cohesion and task interdependence. Small Group Research, 32 (4), pp. 426-450 Schmitt, N. (1994). Method bias: the importance of theory and measurement. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 15, pp. 393-398 Scheneider, B., Salvaggio, A.N. & Subirats, M. (2002). Climate Strength: a new direction for climate research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87 (2), pp. 220-229. Schneider, B. (1987). The people make the place. Personnel Psychology, 28, pp. 447-479 Schoonhoven, C.B. (1981). Problems with Contingency Theory: Testing Assumptions Hidden within the Language of Contingency “Theory”. Administrative Science Quarterly, 26, pp. 349-377 Schrader, B.W. & Steiner, D.D. (1996), Common comparison standards: an approach to improving agreement between self and supervisory performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, pp. 813-20. Affective Climate 47 Scott, S.G. & Einstein, W.O. (2001). Strategic performance appraisal in teambased organizations: one size does not fit all. The Academy of Management Executive, 15 (2) pp. 107-116 Sessa, V.I. (1996). Using perspective taking to manage conflict and affect in teams. The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 32 (1), 101-115 Shaw, J.B. & Barret-Power, E. (1998). The effects of diversity a small workgroup processes and performance. Human Relations, 51, 1307-1325 Simons, T., Pelled, L.H. & Smith, K.A. (1999). Making use of difference: Diversity, Debate, and Decision Comprehensiveness in Top Management Teams. Academy of Management Journal, 42, pp.662-673 Sivasubramaniam, N., Murry, W.D., Avolio, B.J. & Jung, D.I. (2002). A longitudinal model of the effects of team leadership and group potency on group performance. Group and Organizational Management, 27 (1), pp. 66-96 Staw, B.M. & Barsade, S.G. (1993). Affect ad performance: A test of the sadderbut-wiser vs. happier-and-smarter hypotheses. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38, pp. 304-331. Sullins, E.S. (1991). Emotional contagion revisited: effects of social comparison and expressive style on mood convergence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17 (2), pp. 166-174 Tesluk, P., Mathieu, J.E., Zaccaro, S.J. & Marks, M. (1997). Task and Aggregation Issues in the Analysis and Assessment of Team Performance. In M.T. Brannick, E. Salas and C, Prince (Eds.), Team performance assessment and measurement (pp.197-224). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Timmerman, T.A. (2000). Racial diversity, age diversity, interdependence, and team performance. Small Group Research, 31 (5), pp. 592-606 Affective Climate 48 Totterdell, P. (1999). Mood scores: Mood and performance in professional cricketers. British Journal of Psychology, 90, pp. 317-332. Totterdell, P. (2000). Catching moods and hitting runs: mood linkage and subjective performance in professional sport teams. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85 (6), pp. 848-859 Totterdell, P., Kellet, S., Teuchamann, K. & Briner, R.B. (1998). Evidence of mood linkage in work groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74 (6), pp. 1504-1515 Valle, M. & Davis, K. (1999). Teams and performance appraisal: Using metrics to increase reliability and validity. Team Performance Management, 5 (3), pp. 238-243 Waldman, D.A. & Yammarino, F.J. (1999). CEO charismatic leadership: Levelsof–management and levels-of-analysis effects. Academy of Management Review, 24, pp. 266-285 Warr, P.B. (1990). Decision latitude, job demands, and employee well-being. Work & Stress, 4 (4), pp. 285-294. Weiss, H.M. & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events story: A theoretical discussion of the structure, causes and consequences of affective experiences at work. In B.M Staw & L.L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior, 18, (pp.174), Greenwinch CT: JSI Press Williams, K.Y. & O´Reilly, C.A. (1998). Demography and diversity in organizations: A review of 40 years of diversity research. In B.M Staw & L.L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in Organizational Behaviour, 20 (pp.77-140). Greenwinch CT: JSI Press Affective Climate 49 Yik, M.S., Russell, J.A. & Barrett, L.F. (1999). Structure of self-report current affect: Integration and beyond. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77 (3), pp.600-619 Zammuto, R., London, M. & Rowland, K. (1982). Organizational and rater differences in performance appraisals. Personnel Psychology, 35, pp. 643-658. Affective Climate 50 Footnotes 1 In this study we have considered as team members the employees without including the manager (leader). Leader was only included in the team performance appraisal. ² This point is calculated with the following formula: x = -b1 / b3, being b1 the predictor coefficient in the regression equation and b3 the interaction coefficient. ³ The relationship between independent and dependent variable over the range of the moderator variable. It is calculated in the following way: dy/dx = b1 + (b3* moderator variable) Affective Climate 51 Table 1. Work-Unit Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations among team level observed variables Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 1. Optimism climate intensity 3.62 .56 (.91) 2. Tension climate intensity 3.25 .64 -.61** (.90) 3. Optimism climate strengthª -.62 .25 .35** -.38** - 4. Tension climate strengthª -.63 ,21 -.13 .06 .41** - 5. Teams´ perceived performance 4.09 .29 .45** -.36** .30** -.03 (.80) 6. External Evaluators´ perceived performance 3.68 .57 .19* .17 .11 .01 .11 *p< .05 two-tailed, ** p< 0.01 two-tailed Cronbach's alpha coefficients are in the correlation matrix diagonal Note. a The Mean shown is for the corresponding Average Deviation index (AD M(J)) 6 (.78) Affective Climate 52 Table 2. Hierarchical Regression Analysis Results: Tension Affective Climate as predictor Dependent Variables Predictor Teams` perceived performance External evaluators` perceived performance Team Size n.s. n.s. Team Tenure n.s. n.s. R2 .01 .01 Tension Climate Intensity -.34** -.18* Step 1 R2 .13 .04 Δ R2 .12** .03* Tension Climate Strenght -.02 -.01 R2 .13 .04 Δ R2 .00 n.s. .00 n.s. Tension Climate Intensity x Strenght .02 .16* R2 .13 .07 R2 .00 n.s. .02* Step 2 Step 3 Step 4 * p≤ .05 Δ ** p≤ .01 One-tailed tests were used for effects predicted in directional hypotheses B are the unstandarized regression coefficients from the significant final stage of the regression analysis. Affective Climate 53 Table 3. Hierarchical Regression Analysis Results: Optimism Affective Climate as Predictor Dependent Variables Predictor Teams` perceived performance External evaluators` perceived performance Team Size n.s. n.s. Team Tenure n.s. n.s. R2 .02 .01 Optimism Climate Intensity .40** .24** R2 .20 .07 R2 .19** .06** Optimism Climate Strenght .14* .05 R2 .22 .07 R2 .02* .00 n.s. Optimism Climate Intensity x Strenght -.12* .02 R2 .24 .07 R2 .02* .00 n.s. Step 1 Step 2 Δ Step 3 Δ Step 4 * p≤ 0.05 Δ ** p≤ 0.01 One-tailed tests were used for effects predicted in directional hypotheses B are the unstandarized regression coefficients from the significant final stage of the regression analysis. Affective Climate 54 Figure Captions Figure 1. Moderating effect of climate strength in tension on the relationship between tension climate intensity and external evaluators` perceived performance. Figure 2. Moderating effect of climate strength in optimism on the relationship between optimism climate intensity and teams` perceived performance. d Ext. Evaluators` Perceived Performance / d Tension C. Intensity Affective Climate 55 ,4 ,2 0,0 -,2 -,4 -,6 -,8 -3 -2 -1 0 1 Tension Climate Strength 2 3 Affective Climate 56 d Teams` Perceived Performance / d Optimism C. Intensity 1,0 ,8 ,6 ,4 ,2 0,0 -3 -2 -1 0 1 Optimism Climate Strength 2 3