The Lomborg Debate and the Nature of Science Education MICHAEL ELLERBROCK Department of Agricultural & Applied Economics Virginia Tech Center for Economic Education Hutcheson Hall 0401 Blacksburg, VA 24061 USA http://www.aaec.vt.edu/aaec/faculty/ellerbrock.asp Abstract: - The worldwide eruption of controversy over Bj ørn Lomborg’s book, The Skeptical Environmentalist [1], focuses debate on the proper measures of environmental quality, use of the scientific method, economic versus engineering frameworks of analysis, validity of data and references, long term modeling, and the need for critical thinking in science education. The debate offers educators a case study in argumentation. Instructional techniques for teaching critical thinking are applied to key aspects of the debate. Key-Words: - Scientific Method, Rhetorical Argumentation, Critical Thinking, Global Environment, Statistics, Economics, Engineering, Sustainable Development, Lomborg. 1 Introduction In his opening remarks at the recent 168th Annual Meeting of the American Association for the Advancement of Science, President Peter Raven warned that Lomborg’s book “demonstrates the vulnerability of the scientific process – which is deliberative and hypothesis driven – to outright misrepresentation and distortion” [2]. He bluntly questions why such a prestigious company would publish the book and why a respected journal like The Economist would defend it. Raven’s comments reflect a tip of the iceberg that has quickly formed in response to Lomborg’s 515 page tome (including 2930 footnotes, plus a 71 page bibliography) in which he argues that the global environment “is generally getting better … there are problems … but we should not act on myths of gloom and doom” as propagated by many environmentalists [3]. During the Spring of 2002, Scientific American ran a series of articles by eminent scientists responding to Lomborg [4-10], including a brief rebuttal from Lomborg [3]. The controversy has ignited a flurry of articles and discussion in the environmental and scientific communities. One database (InfoTrac Onefile) lists 65 responses, both pro and con, in reaction to The Skeptical Environmentalist, some of which criticize and attack Lomborg’s assessment [11-15], some that defend and praise Lomborg [1619], and some that allow him to respond again to his critics [20-22]. What are we to make of this debate? At the least, it raises the epistemological question: how do we humans know anything? What is knowledge? What is truth, wisdom, certainty, plausibility? Is there a hierarchy of facts? The debate generated by Lomborg, a Danish statistician, offers science educators a compelling case study in rhetorical argumentation and the need for critical thinking, the focus of this paper. Instructional techniques for teaching critical thinking are applied to key aspects of the debate. 2 Problem Formulation Problems central to the Lomborg debate include: the proper measures of environmental quality, use of the scientific method, economic versus engineering frameworks of analysis, validity of data and references, challenges of long term modeling, and the legitimacy of various participants and perspectives. To help science educators address the debate in their classrooms, let’s examine some of the key issues and apply principles of critical thinking (Table 1) to the arguments espoused. 2.1 Global Warming Lomborg [3] asserts that the long term rise in global temperature from carbon dioxide will be closer to 1.5 degrees C than 4.5 degrees C, as some have predicted. Stephen Schneider [4] challenges Lomborg on several accounts, perhaps most importantly in Lomborg’s use of a single estimate ($5 trillion) of potential damages from global warming, yet use of a range of estimates ($3-33 trillion) for the cost of implementing climate control policies, possibly an error of Selectivity. Schneider also faults Lomborg for preferring economists’ high estimates of those control costs over the lower and nearzero estimates of some engineers who advocate incentives for consumers and industry to replace inefficient appliances and machines with state-of-the art equipment, possibly another error of Selectivity or an example of Hidden Ideology: economics over engineering. Debates over global warming often center around the choice of baselines on which to make inferences about the past and projections for the future. Regarding Clarity, Schneider criticizes Lomborg for estimating the potential impact of the Kyoto Protocol only into the 21st Century, a short term horizon, and for ignoring that Kyoto is meant to be Flexible and Dynamic, not a one-shot panacea. With regard to scientific probabilities, Schneider objects to Lomborg’s repeated use of the phrase “plausible” without quantifying it in mathematical terms, another possible error of Clarity. When Lomborg emphasizes the role of solar fluctuations in explaining global warming, Schneider faults Lomborg for “the elementary mistake of seeing correlation as causation” [10], a possible Non-Sequitur. With regard to the estimates of damages from global warming, Schneider rejects Lomborg’s reliance solely on the loss of goods and services traded in the marketplace, without including the harm inflicted on the environment itself, possibly reflecting a utilitarian view of nature by Lomborg, which is perhaps an example of Hidden Ideology. Conversely, Lomborg criticizes the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Control for dismissing economic studies that use aggregate measures of costs and benefits, whereas Schneider claims such studies fail to adequately consider important details and categories of effects, an issue possibly reflecting the Fallacy of Composition. Schneider also faults Lomborg for cleverly mixing primary and secondary sources of information and using too many nonscholarly articles, possible violations of the Authority principle. When Lomborg criticizes Schneider for admitting in 1989 that scientists sometimes face a “double ethical bind” in which they need to “offer up scary scenarios” to elicit public financial support, Rennie [10] accuses Lomborg of questioning Schneider’s motives, a possible example of Judgmentalism. 2.2 Energy Resources Whereas Lomborg [1] argues vigorously that the world is not running out of energy resources, which few environmentalists dispute, John Holdren says Lomborg misses their main point that we are running out of environment – the biosphere’s capacity to absorb the consequences and residuals of energy extraction, transportation, transformation and use [5]. In a sense, the sides disagree on defining the problem, a disconnect of mindsets that violates the principles of Framing and Worldviews. Holdren also chides Lomborg for making no distinction between proven versus ultimately recoverable reserves of fossil fuels, a serious discrepancy in terms of what is profitable to extract given today’s technology and prices versus what might be feasible with future technologies and potentially higher prices. Again the interplay of engineering and economics needs to be integrated into the analysis, respecting the principle of Holism. 2.3 Population Impact Whereas Lomborg [1] sees good news in the declining percentage of starving people on earth, John Bongaarts [6] argues that the real news is bad because the number of victims is rising, here again a debate involving Clarity. To make his case for relative measures of human welfare, Lomborg [1, p. 64] argues that a scenario where 750,000 people starve out of a growing population of 2,000,000 is preferable to one where 500,000 starve out of 1,000,000 – a higher percentage. Lomborg facetiously argues that, if absolutes measures were preferable, then a situation where 499,000 starve out of 500,000 would be even better. Bongaarts retorts that the ideal scenario is a stable population with declining numbers of starving people. 2.4 Biodiversity Much of the debate about biodiversity hinges on absolute versus relative measures of change. While Lomborg accepts that the extinction rate has increased to 1500 times the natural rate, he nevertheless emphasizes that “over the next 50 years we might lose 0.7 percent of all species” [3]. Thomas Lovejoy [7] and Rennie [9] blast Lomborg’s logic by analogy: whereas AIDS killed three million people in 2001, that was only 0.05 percent of the world’s population. Toward Clarity, Lovejoy and Rennie ask the reader to consider which measure – absolute or relative – is more important? Lomborg’s retort might be that it is a False Analogy to compare human life with primitive creatures. In an apparent act of Judgmentalism, Lomborg [1] notes that biologists prefer to keep the estimated rates of extinction high because there “are many grants at stake” [10]. When Lomborg asserts that the longest data series reveals little change in world forested area [1, p. 111], Rennie [10] faults him for equating new trees with old trees, perhaps an error of Gross Generalization or the Fallacy of Composition. Similarly, when Lomborg [1, p. 113] quotes U.N. Food and Agriculture Organization figures for tropical deforestation at 0.46 percent per year of total global forest cover, Rennie [10] charges that this is a misleading figure “greatly understating the pressure on tropical forests, where most of the deforestation is taking place,” possibly an error of Selectivity. Lovejoy [7] also berates Lomborg for misrepresenting a statement in Paul Colinvaux’s 1989 text: “As human beings lay waste to massive tracts of vegetation, an incalculable and unprecedented number of species are rapidly becoming extinct.” Lomborg subsequently wrote that, “Colinvaux admits in Scientific American that the rate [of extinction in the tropics] is ‘incalculable’” [10], perhaps abusing the principle of Language. When charged with twisting Colinvaux’s statement, Lomborg refused to recant and asserted that he was “trying to establish the fact that the vast extinction numbers are unsupported” [10]. Also in the quest for Accuracy, Rennie [10] and Schneider [4] fault Lomborg for not defining the term “catastrophic.” 3 Problem Solution Students need to be able to evaluate the validity of arguments and reasoning used in scientific debates. Table 1 presents some common principles for educators to use in developing critical thinking skills in youth and adults, many of which are applicable to key issues in the Lomborg debate and other issues in sustainable development. Table 1. Some Critical Thinking Skills & Strategies in Scientific Thought Tips for the Classroom: Objectivity - Evaluate an argument for its rational merits and weaknesses. Recognize: * Non-Sequiturs - inferences which do not logically follow from the premises or evidence; * Fallacies of Composition - what’s true of the whole is not necessarily true of the parts and vice versa; * False dichotomies – posing dualistic paradigms in which the author wants you to assume that both sides can’t be true, that the problem is simple and you should make a clear choice; * False Analogies – invalid reasoning, comparing “apples to oranges;” * Ill-phrased questions – e.g., “So, when did you stop dumping toxic waste in the river?” Subjectivity - Look beyond strict logic for illogical, rhetorical, poetic, intuitive dimensions. Framing – Ask who framed the question? Who’s missing? How else could it be described? Worldviews - What are the underlying premises and biases being used (consciously or not)? Holism - What facts, ideas, factors, principles or information are omitted? What’s missing? Gross Generalizations – Sweeping statements that hide details. Selectivity – Recognize when partial information is used to ‘prove’ a point. Diversion - What irrelevant information is being used to deflect or disguise the issue? Context - Try to frame the issue within larger “umbrella” issues, i.e., what’s the real issue(s)? Clarity – Define technical terms, measures (absolute vs relative), descriptors (quantitative vs qualitative), time frames (short vs long run), and base lines adopted. Accuracy – Not twisting or spinning others’ statements out of context. Authority – Cite sources of data (primary vs secondary), references used (expert vs lay). Claiming Turf – Fear-based responses to perceived threats, e.g., dismissing “outsiders.” Currency – Use best available information and knowledge. Fairness – Recognize ad hominem (personal) assaults, i.e., killing the messenger for the message. Judgmentalism – Questioning others’ motives. Language - Watch for semantic use of words with double meanings (literal vs figurative) and value-laden terms (implied morality). Hidden Ideology - Rigid schools of thought, often subtle or implied. Sensationalism – Watch for use of words and images for shock value or hyperbole. Listening - Seek to hear the voices of those outside the power structure or the debate. Dissent - Consider the ideas of people with whom you disagree, avoid sycophants. Integration - Seek and assimilate multidisciplinary views about the subject. Paradox – Recognize that seeming opposites can both be correct or true. Process - Decide how to decide. Flexibility - Formulate multiple solutions to a problem, avoid irreversible decisions. Understanding Communicate effectively in all modes of expression. Evaluation – Seek feedback on the effectiveness and outcomes of a decision. Dynamism – Revisit situations in the future. 4 Conclusion The Lomborg debate strikes at the heart of science education. Whereas Lomborg’s critics [6] agree that he is correct in stating that humans as a whole now live longer, are healthier, have fewer children, and possess greater income and wealth [1], they assert that his argumentation and conclusions are seriously flawed, full of Non-Sequiturs and Selected facts. Who’s right? The principle of Paradox reminds us that both sides may be partially correct, at least temporarily. For example, Lomborg says the primary cause of hunger is poverty [1], yet Bongaarts asserts that overpopulation is part of the problem [6]. There is truth in both views. The debate raises many other examples of the need for critical thinking, such as the economic reality of trade-offs: Lomborg rejects the Kyoto Protocol on the grounds that its excessive expenses could otherwise be used to solve our greatest environmental problem – safe drinking water for all [3], thus trying to avoid the error of Diversion. Bongaarts [6] and Rennie [10] accuse Lomborg of making a Gross Generalization when he asserts that China, Egypt, and India have low population densities when measured as a percent of their total respective land base, rather than as a percent of their arable land area. Rennie [10] further accuses Lomborg of violating the principle of Currency when Lomborg criticizes both Lovejoy’s 1980 estimate that 15-20 percent of all species may be lost by now and Norman Myer’s 1979 estimate of the extinction rate at 40,000 species per year, figures no longer used by mainstream scientists, including Lovejoy and Myers. Schneider [4] faults Lomborg on several other grounds: failing to distinguish among different types of probabilities (objective frequentist versus subjective Bayesian), a Clarity error; not understanding the natural science of cloud cover and cosmic radiation, the Authority principle; and, for approaching the subject of sustainable development from an all or nothing paradigm of good news/bad news, rather than as a matter of risk management with hedging on particular issues, a question of Framing. Much of the Lomborg debate centers on the principles of Selectivity and Clarity. Holdren [5] faults Lomborg for “persistently using numbers to two and three figure precision for quantities (U.S. energy wasted, plant photosynthesis) that cannot be known with such accuracy,” perhaps violating the Clarity principle. Lomborg retorts that his critics make ample use of their own preferred facts and figures. Has Lomborg helped or hurt the cause of sustainable development? Whereas some of his critics [4] reject him outright as an outsider, citing Lomborg’s own forthright admission that he is not “an expert as regards environmental problems,” perhaps out of their need for Claiming Turf, others [16-19] welcome Lomborg’s perspective as a fresh challenge to the scientific establishment, thereby exemplifying the principles of Dissent and Integration. If Lomborg’s monumental effort to determine “the real state of the world” forces some radical environmentalists to base their rhetoric on facts rather than fear, then so be it. However, if Lomborg’s assessment reinforces an undue complacency among consumers across the globe, then he may indeed undermine his own optimism. References: [1] Lomborg, B., The Skeptical Environmentalist: Measuring the Real State of the World (Cambridge: University Press, 2001), 515 pp. [2] Russell, S., “Somber Opening to Science Meeting – Warning of Future Environmental Havoc Resulting from Western Society’s Lifestyle,” San Francisco Chronicle, Feb. 15, 2002, A3; sfgate.com/cgi-bin/article.cgi?file=chronicle/archive. [3] Lomborg, B., “The Skeptical Environmentalist Replies,” Scientific American, May 2002, 286(5): 14-15. [4] Schneider, S., “Global Warming: Neglecting the Complexities,” Scientific American, Jan. 2002, 286(1): 62-65. [5] Holdren, J.P., “Energy: Asking the Wrong Question,” Scientific American, Jan. 2002, 286(1): 65-67. [6] Bongaarts, J., “Population: Ignoring Its Impact,” Scientific American, Jan. 2002, 286(1): 6769. [7] Lovejoy, T., “Biodiversity: Dismissing Scientific Progress,” Scientific American, Jan. 2002, 286(1): 69-71. [8] Rennie, J., “Misleading Math about the Earth: Science Defends Itself against The Skeptical Environmentalist,” Scientific American, Jan. 2002, 286(1): 61. [9] Rennie, J., “The Skeptical Environmentalist Replies – Rennie Replies,” Scientific American, May 2002, 286(5): 15. [10] Rennie, J., “A Response to Lomborg’s Rebuttal,” Scientific American: Explore, April 9, 2002, 18pp; www.sciam.com/explorations/2002/041502lomborg/rennie.html, accessed May 20, 2002. [11] Adler, J.H., “Dissident from Denmark,” National Review, April 8, 2002, 18(7): 42. [12] Baker, R., “The Lomborg File: When the Press is Lured by a Contraria’s Tale,” Columbia Journalism Review, March-April 2002, 40(6): 78-79. [13] Burke, T., et al., “Arguments that Don’t Hold Water,” The Guardian, Aug. 20, 2001, G6(2). [14] Editor, “Bjorn Lomborg the Environmental Optimist,” Africa News Service, March 18, 2002, p 1008077u6574. [15] Thornton, J., “How Green Is Our Valley? A Dutch Statistician Insists that Environmentalists Mislead Us – But Who’s Misleading Whom?” Discover, Jan. 2002, 23(1): 81. [16] Bailey, R., “Green with Ideology: The Hidden Agenda behind the ‘Scientific’ Attacks on Bjorn Lomborg’s Controversial New Book, The Skeptical Environmentalist,” Reason, May 2002, 34(1): 38-47. [17] Editor, “Defending Science: The Environment,” The Economist, Feb. 2, 2002, 362(8258): 15-16. [18] Greer, H., “How Data Becomes Dada: A Former Greenie Challenges Doomsayers’ Predictions of Environmental Disaster,” Insight on the News, Feb. 4, 2002, 18(4): 33. [19] Ridley, M., “The Profits of Doom: Matt Ridley Celebrates Bjorn Lomborg, the Environmentalist Brave Enough to Tell the Truth – that the End is not Nigh,” Spectator, Feb. 23, 2002, 288(9055): 10-11. [20] Lomborg, B., “Yes, It Looks Bad, But,” The Guardian, Aug, 15, 2001, G2(3). [21] Lomborg, B., “Running on Empty?” The Guardian, Aug, 16, 2001, G6(2). [22] Lomborg, B., “Why Kyoto Will Not Stop This,” The Guardian, Aug, 17, 2001, G4(2).