PEOPLE WITH ‘FOREIGN-SOUNDING’ NAMES GET FEWER JOB INTERVIEWS Job applicants with ‘foreign-sounding’ names are considerably less likely to be invited for an interview than people with Anglo-Saxon names. That is the central finding of research by Nicolas Jacquemet and Constantine Yannelis, presented at the Royal Economic Society’s 2011 annual conference. This discrimination is of the same size as the often-observed discrimination against African-American names in job applications, suggesting that the discrimination may be due to a general suspicion about differences rather than a particular ethnicity. The study responded to 330 job openings in Chicago, sending three different CVs with names that were Anglo-Saxon sounding, African-American sounding and foreignsounding but without association with any specific minority. The suitability of the last category was determined by a previous survey in Chicago, where 75% of respondents did not even attempt to guess the origin of the foreign name. This experiment found that: Seventy-six resumes with Anglo-Saxon names received a call for interview compared with 52 resumes with an Afro-American name and 54 resumes with a foreign name. This suggests that Anglo-Saxon names are 40% more likely to receive a call back than African-American or foreign names. This discrimination is concentrated in companies in the suburbs, where ‘white flight’ – white families choosing to leave an area once black families begin to move in – has produced areas with a very low ethnic mix. These results suggest that discrimination arises because of a general mistrust of outsiders rather than a mistrust of specific minorities. The authors argue that government policies that target discrimination against specific minorities will miss out on preventing discrimination against others: governments should turn their attention to removing the chance for discrimination of any kind by making all resumes anonymous. More… This paper shows that racial discrimination in the labour market goes well beyond the unequal treatment of specific, clearly identified minorities. The study finds that job applicants with a foreign-sounding name, which is not associated with any particular minority, elicit 40% fewer call-backs compared with identical applicants with Anglo-Saxon sounding names. What is more, the discrimination rate is exactly the same as observed for AfricanAmerican applicants: 40% fewer call-backs than similar resumes with Anglo-Saxon sounding names. The extent of racial discrimination is now well established, and ranges from a 30-50% drop in the likelihood of receiving a positive answer as a result of the perceived ethnicity of the name appearing on the resume. Perhaps the most striking fact is that this result remains the same across countries, geographic regions and ethnic minorities. This may suggest that discrimination arises because of a general mistrust of any individual identified as an outsider of the dominant group, rather than a targeted mistrust against specific minorities – that is, suspicions of dissimilarities rather than differences. To test this assumption, the researchers ran a correspondence test in Chicago. They used three different sets of names: Anglo-Saxon sounding names, African-American sounding names and foreign-sounding names without further association with any specific minority. To assess this last property, they ran a preliminary survey in Chicago, asking respondents their guesses for the ethnic origin of each name shown to them. The foreign-sounding set of names include names for which more than 75% respondents either left the field blank or put a question mark. The researchers responded to 330 job offers, sending three different resumes – one from each set of names – to each employer. They cycle names on the resumes from one answer to the other, so that resumes’ quality is equally distributed across perceived ethnicity of names. The measure of success is the number of call-backs elicited by the applications. This is the case with 76 resumes with an Anglo-Saxon name, 52 resumes with an AfricanAmerican name and 54 resumes with a foreign name. It is not possible to measure how these differences would lead to different job offers, since hiring depends on actual interviews (declined by email following each positive answer). But this is clear evidence of a disadvantage of individuals possessing a nonAnglo-Saxon name in the labour market, since they have to send more than one third more resumes to elicit the same number of interviews. We further check the robustness this result by looking at the discrimination intensity from employers, measured as the number of employers who either call-back or discriminate against only one resume out of the three they received from us. In either case, non Anglo-Saxon applicants are both and equally unfavoured as compared to Anglo-Saxons. The study further investigates the assumption by splitting the results according to the geographic location of the employer. Once call-backs are disaggregated according to whether the employer is in the city proper or the suburbs, it appears the whole discrimination pattern comes from the suburbs, where the ‘white flight’ phenomenon – white families choosing to leave an area once black families begin to move in – has produced very high ethnic homogeneity. In the policy sphere, these results suggest that affirmative action programmes will miss a substantial proportion of those affected adversely by discrimination in targeting explicitly identified ethnic minorities. This should turn attention to programmes that rule out as much name identification as possible in the job matching process, such as mandatory anonymity of resumes. ENDS ‘Indiscriminate Discrimination: A Correspondence Test for Ethnic Homophily in the Chicago Labor Market’ by Nicolas Jacquemet (University Paris 1 Panthéon-Sorbonne & Paris School of Economics) and Constantine Yannelis (Stanford University) Contacts: Nicolas Jacquemet: nicolas.jacquemet@univ-paris1.fr; Cell. +33 6 38 42 25 10 ; Tel. +33 1 44 07 83 66 Constantine Yannelis: yannelis@stanford.edu