Running Head: RTI AND THE STRUGGLING BEGINNING READER Response to Intervention and the Struggling Beginning Reader Lisa Y. Jones East Carolina University READ 6418 RTI AND THE STRUGGLING BEGINNING READER Abstract Response to Intervention is an educational practice that involves providing struggling readers with series of early interventions in order to help them progress. This paper examines research on factors involved in successful literacy intervention at the kindergarten and first grade levels: timing, group size, intervention leader, and literacy skills addressed. It also looks at what might predict a lack of responsiveness as well as implications for instruction. Keywords: Response to Intervention, struggling reader, kindergarten, first grade 2 RTI AND THE STRUGGLING BEGINNING READER 3 In any given year, a teacher has the potential to encounter students with a wide range of reading abilities, some of whom are struggling to be successful. This is true of all grade levels, but is especially true in the earliest grades as students are affected by their level of experience before coming to school. In order to provide effective instruction for these beginning readers, many teachers are turning to a Response to Intervention (RTI) framework. The premise behind RTI is that students’ reading difficulties are recognized early and teachers use a tiered series of increasingly intensive interventions to help them catch up to their on-grade level peers. The interventions often include longer or more frequent instruction in smaller groupings, which are systematic and explicit in nature (Fuchs & Fuchs, 2006). It is a form of dynamic assessment (Gersten & Dimino, 2006), so through consistent monitoring teachers are able to determine if the instruction at each level is helping their struggling readers succeed. Several factors are involved in making the more intensive tiers as effective as possible for early readers, including timing of the intervention, who is providing the instruction, size of the group, and which areas of literacy are being targeted. This paper will explore information and research relating to these factors at the kindergarten and first grade level. Timing of the Intervention Timing can be one key to success when it comes to early literacy intervention. Starting too late can mean missing a valuable window to correct early reading problems before they have gone too far. According to a widely cited study conducted by Juel (1988), a student who is a good reader at the end of first grade is likely to stay a good reader throughout elementary school. However, a student who is struggling at the end of first grade is not likely to catch up. This implies that intervention in kindergarten and first grade RTI AND THE STRUGGLING BEGINNING READER 4 can help to prevent reading difficulties in the future. Several studies have found positive results from reading intervention beginning in kindergarten (O’Connor, Bocian, Sanchez & Beach, 2012; Vadasy & Sanders, 2008; Whiteley, Smith & Connors, 2007). Researchers have determined beginning in kindergarten was preferable to beginning in first grade (O’Connor et al., 2012). Even the time of implementation within the kindergarten year can make a difference as Cooke, Kretlow and Helf (2009) showed when students in a beginning of the year intervention outperformed students starting the reading protocol in mid-year. Furthering the idea of an early start, Wanzek and Vaughn (2010) suggest that intervention beginning in second and third grades is not as successful as earlier implementation due to the fact that the reading difficulties are more complex, and specific research has confirmed the idea (O’Connor, 2012). A contradiction to the concept of starting interventions as early as kindergarten is the fact that looking at these students for reading difficulties can be troublesome. Screening measures used in kindergarten do not tend to be as precise as assessments in the higher grades (Gersten & Dimino, 2006). This could mean false positives and providing reading intervention for students who do not really need it. On the opposite end of the spectrum, kindergarten screenings tend to be based on letter knowledge and phoneme segmentation and may not catch students with reading problems that develop later as reading gets more challenging (O’Connor et al., 2012). These researchers determined that, when it comes to predicting reading difficulties, how a child is reading at the beginning of first grade is more accurate than early kindergarten assessments. Assuming the time during which students will require reading intervention has passed can also cause problems. Gersten and Dimino (2006) suggest that those who RTI AND THE STRUGGLING BEGINNING READER 5 support RTI surmise that when proficiency in reading is attained by the end of first grade it will continue. While that can be true as found by Vadasy, Sanders and Abbott (2008), it is not always the case. A study by Jesson and Limbrick (2014) determined that while a majority of students successfully completing a literacy intervention by the end of first grade continued to be at or near grade level in third, fourth and fifth grade, forty percent were now below grade level. Similarly, O’Conner et al. (2012) found about a third of the students in their study did not present literacy skills low enough to qualify for intervention until second grade. This may mean that students need reading intervention even after the early years. Size of the Intervention Group The number of students working in the same group can have an impact on the success of reading intervention. Small group instruction has been shown to work well when beginning readers require a moderate level of intervention (Cooke et al., 2009; O’Connor et al., 2012). Wanzek and Vaughn (2010) noted as group size increases the effects of the literacy intervention decreases. Individual sessions are commonly used for students requiring a more intense level of reading assistance (Wanzek & Vaughn, 2010), with Whiteley et al. (2007) positing the reason for greater success at the individual level could be due to personal issues, attention span and/or socioemotional factors. However, research by Vadasy and Sanders (2008) has shown that working with students in pairs can be as effective as working with students in a one on one setting, often increasing motivation and enjoyment for the students. Leader of the Intervention RTI AND THE STRUGGLING BEGINNING READER 6 It is generally thought that the people who provide early Tier 2 and Tier 3 interventions should possess high levels of expertise in the field of reading, such as classroom teachers and reading specialists (Fuchs & Fuchs, 2006; Wanzek & Vaughn, 2010). However, research has shown that successful interventions have been led by teacher assistants and tutors (Vadasy & Sanders, 2008; Vadasy et al., 2008). There were several similar characteristics found in these two studies. First of all, the paraprofessionals worked with no more than two students at a time. Vadasy and Sanders (2008) acknowledge that further studies would be required to determine how many students a paraprofessional could successfully handle. The assistants and tutors also received several hours of training on the intervention protocol they would be leading and were subject to fidelity checks and ongoing coaching and modeling from the research staff. Lastly, the intervention programs used were highly scripted, helping to maintain fidelity. One thing pointed out in a study using paraprofessionals to lead interventions was that as students were found to be less responsive, it may have been beneficial to have the reading intervention provided by staff members with a higher level of expertise (Vadasy et al., 2008) . Literacy Components of Intervention Perhaps because it is a known predictor of reading ability, much research has been done on intervention at the kindergarten level involving explicitly-taught phonemic awareness skills, and how it benefits the literacy skills of struggling readers both in the present as well as in future grades. Interventions involving strictly phonemic awareness tend to improve young students’ word identification and fluency (O’Connor et al., 2012) as well as spelling and comprehension (Vadasy & Sanders, 2008; Whiteley et al., 2007). Results of these interventions may only last through the beginning of second grade when RTI AND THE STRUGGLING BEGINNING READER 7 some students begin to show the need for assistance with decoding multisyllabic words and comprehension, skills they may not have needed in the earlier grades (O’Connor et al., 2012). In first grade, interventions tend to involve phonemic awareness as well as other components. Fitting with the research summary by Wansek and Vaughn (2010) in which they noticed that the most effective interventions had students reading text at their reading level along with phonics components, Vadasy et al. (2008) included oral reading practice in the phonemic awareness intervention in their study. They found higher results in decoding, word reading, spelling, comprehension and fluency, all lasting through the end of third grade. Lane, Pullen, Hudson and Konold (2009) included oral reading, manipulative word work and writing, and saw an increase in students’ phonological awareness, sight word knowledge and decoding. They also determined that when word work and writing were removed the intervention was not as successful. Those students did not perform any better than the control group in word recognition or decoding. Predictors of Lack of Responsiveness to Phonological Intervention Several studies have looked into factors that might predict the progress of an early phonological intervention. According to these studies, children who were unresponsive to intervention also had low oral vocabulary skills and letter knowledge (Ouellette and Haley, 2013; Whiteley et al., 2007) as well as receptive language (Vadasy et al., 2008). Ouellette and Haley (2013) determined while alphabetic knowledge was only important in kindergarten, the effects of deficits in oral language continued into first grade. Whiteley et al. (2007) suggest vocabulary intervention for children struggling with phonological awareness at the kindergarten level. RTI AND THE STRUGGLING BEGINNING READER 8 Reflection As the Title I reading specialist in my building, I am typically the “expert” to whom the staff turns when they have a student struggling with reading. Since last year was my first year in the position, I used my best professional judgment to help plan and implement reading interventions across several grade levels. The whole time I had the idea in mind that in the near future I would only be doing interventions at the kindergarten and first grade levels because I would get the students caught up by the end of first grade and they would not need any more intervention. I had read the Juel (1988) research which found good readers at the end of first grade staying good readers and I made remediation by the end of first grade my goal for my students. This was the reason for choosing early grade level Response to Intervention as my paper topic. Now that I have read additional research, I see that is a goal I need to adjust. I would like to help as many struggling readers as I can as early as possible, but also know that I need to work with classroom teachers to engage in the consistent monitoring that will help us catch students who start to have difficulties in the upper grades, either again or for the first time. I also learned that what I thought was the best timing for kindergarten intervention is certainly not best practice. Last year the kindergarten teachers and I decided not to begin literacy interventions for their students until mid-way through the year, after they had time to adjust to routines and instruction. We were thinking they would miss out on vital readiness skills being taught in the classrooms while I was doing pull-out. I now know that these students will get the most benefit from beginning early in the year and there is no reason to hold back. I do not have to make sure they know most of their letters before they go on to anything else. I look forward to seeing the difference this makes next year. RTI AND THE STRUGGLING BEGINNING READER 9 I am the only staff member in my building whose primary purpose is providing literacy intervention. I work with small groups all day long, but even with that type of schedule I can not get to all of the children who need reading assistance. Last year my principal decided I needed some help and she enlisted other people from within the building to work with small groups of struggling readers for part of the day. I trained these tutors in the intervention strategies we use, but never checked in to see if they were following the procedures correctly. I found that to be an important part of the research on using paraprofessionals successfully, that there were fidelity checks and coaching on ways to alter the intervention to fit the students throughout the process in order to ensure a high level of effectiveness. This type of coaching is something on which I can improve in the future. The part of the RTI research I found most enjoyable was reading about the different components used in interventions. It confirmed some of what I have been doing in using a well-balanced series of explicitly taught lessons including both leveled reading and phonics components. One part that was neglected, however, was making the word work truly interactive and manipulative through activities such as word sorts and building words with magnetic letters. The students did much more writing of words than they did interacting with them. With research showing interventions including manipulative word work as more successful than the same intervention without it, I can see this is something that needs to be added. I am hoping to combine the knowledge of how beneficial this can be with instructional techniques learned in the Applied Phonics class I took to make the interventions as effective as possible. RTI AND THE STRUGGLING BEGINNING READER 10 The idea that vocabulary is a predictor of the success of phonological interventions was new to me and I still have lingering questions about using vocabulary intervention to help less responsive students. Whiteley et al. (2007) were very specific in stating interactive storybook reading could be a useful tool, but I would be interested in seeing if other research shows read aloud to be a good use of very limited intervention time. This is a topic I plan to continue exploring in order to determine vocabulary practices that might work well in a small group early intervention setting. As a new interventionist, it was helpful to synthesize the research on Response to Intervention in kindergarten and first grade. It confirmed several intervention practices we were already using, gave me ideas to adjust the timing and components of my early intervention sessions, and caused me to acknowledge that my role as coach is something I should take more seriously. It also led me to a new area of interest, small group vocabulary intervention, which I plan to continue to research in order to make my intervention practices reach as many struggling early readers as possible. RTI AND THE STRUGGLING BEGINNING READER 11 References Cooke, N. L., Kretlow, A. G., & Helf, S. (2009). Supplemental reading help for kindergarten students: How early should you start? Preventing School Failure: Alternative Education for Children and Youth, 54(3), 137-144. doi:10.1080/10459880903492924 Fuchs, D., & Fuchs, L. S. (2006). Introduction to response to intervention: What, why, and how valid is it? Reading Research Quarterly, 41(1), 93-99. doi:10.1598/RRQ.41.1.4 Gersten, R., & Dimino, J. A. (2006). RTI (response to intervention): Rethinking special education for students with reading difficulties (yet again). Reading Research Quarterly, 41(1), 99-108. doi:10.1598/RRQ.41.1.5 Jesson, R., & Limbrick, L. (2014). Can gains from early literacy interventions be sustained? The case of reading recovery. Journal of Research in Reading, 37(1), 102-117. doi:10.1111/1467-9817.12017 Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(4), 437-447. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.80.4.437 Lane, H. B., Pullen, P. C., Hudson, R. F., & Konold, T. R. (2009). Identifying essential instructional components of literacy tutoring for beginning struggling readers. Literacy Research and Instruction, 48(4), 277-297. doi:10.1080/19388070902875173 OConnor, R. E., Bocian, K. M., Sanchez, V., & Beach, K.D. (2012). Access to a responsiveness to intervention model: Does beginning intervention in kindergarten RTI AND THE STRUGGLING BEGINNING READER 12 matter? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(4), 307-328. doi:10.1177/0022219412459354 Ouellette, G. P., & Haley, A. (2013). One complicated extended family: The influence of alphabetic knowledge and vocabulary on phonemic awareness. Journal of Research in Reading, 36(1), 29-41. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01486.x Vadasy, P. F., & Sanders, E. A. (2008). Code-oriented instruction for kindergarten students at risk for reading difficulties: A replication and comparison of instructional groupings. Reading and Writing, 21(9), 929-963. doi:10.1007/s11145-008-9119-9 Vadasy, P. F., Sanders, E. A., & Abbott, R. D. (2008). Effects of supplemental early reading intervention at 2-year follow up: Reading skill growth patterns and predictors. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12(1), 51-89. doi:10.1080/10888430701746906 Wanzek, J., & Vaughn, S. (2010). Tier 3 interventions for students with significant reading problems. Theory Into Practice, 49(4), 305-314. doi:10.1080/00405841.2010.510759 Whiteley, H. E., Smith, C. D., & Connors, L. (2007). Young children at risk of literacy difficulties: Factors predicting recovery from risk following phonologically based intervention. Journal of Research in Reading, 30(3), 249-269. doi:10.1111/j.14679817.2007.00342.x