1 WORK-FAMILY NEGATIVE AND POSITIVE SPILLOVER AND OUTCOMES: DIRECT AND MODERATING EFFECTS Jarrod M. Haar Department of Strategy & Human Resource Management University of Waikato Private Bag 3105 Hamilton New Zealand Phone: +64 7 838 4587 Fax: +64 7 838 4356 E-mail: haar@waikato.ac.nz Anne Bardoel Department of Management Monash University Victoria 3800 Australia Phone: +61 3 9903 2675 Fax: +61 3 9903 2718 E-mail: anne.bardoel@buseco.monash.edu.au Acknowledgements We would like to acknowledge the contribution of the International Work-Family (Project 3535) research team to the theoretical and empirical conceptualization of this research. The team consists of (in alphabetical order): Dr. Zeynep Aycan (Turkey), Dr. Roya Ayman (U.S.), Dr. Anne Bardoel (Australia), Dr. Tripti Pande-Desai (India), Dr. Anat Drach-Zahavy (Israel), Dr. Leslie Hammer (U.S.), Dr. Ting-Pang Huang (Taiwan), Dr. Karen Korabik (Canada), Dr. Donna Lero (Canada)., Dr. Arti Mawardi (Indonseia), Dr. Steven Poelmans (Spain), Dr. Ujvala Rajadhyaksha (India) and Dr. Anit Somech (Israel). 2 WORK-FAMILY NEGATIVE AND POSITIVE SPILLOVER AND OUTCOMES: DIRECT AND MODERATING EFFECTS The work-family literature has focused almost exclusively on negative spillover rather than positive spillover. Further, while other organizational behavior literature has looked to explore moderating effects, there has been no exploration of the interaction effects between work and family dimensions of negative and positive spillover towards outcomes. Using a sample of 398 Australian employees, we found strong support for work-family and familywork negative and positive spillover predicting job satisfaction and psychological distress. In addition, we tested the interaction effects between positive and negative spillover and found a number of significant effects, with findings indicating that respondents with higher positive spillover were able to mitigate the detrimental influence of negative spillover towards job satisfaction and psychological distress. The findings support the notion of testing both negative and positive spillover dimensions towards outcomes from both work and family directions together. The implications for research and organizations are discussed. Keywords: work-family, family-work, negative spillover, positive spillover, outcomes, Australia. 3 INTRODUCTION There is consistently strong evidence that today’s workers face multiple demands on their time. A host of demographic changes including more working mothers, dual career couples and working single parents has lead Frone (2003) to argue that more families now juggle demands of the job and family. Due to these changes, there has been growing attention and interest in work-family conflict, which Greenhaus and Beutell (1985) asserted relates to role pressures from one domain (e.g. work) being incompatible with another domain (e.g. family). Clearly the demands of the modern work environment are increasingly challenging for employees. The detrimental outcomes associated with work-family conflict have been well established, including lower job and life satisfaction (Kossek & Ozeki, 1998). However, there have been recent calls from critics that argue that balancing work and family responsibilities might not always be negative, and that potentially positive outcomes may occur (Greenhaus & Powell, 2006). While empirical study of the positive aspects of work-family domains have only begun to emerge little is understood about how these diametrically opposed conditions (positive and negative aspects) might influence outcomes together. The present study compares the extent of influence from negative and positive spillover dimensions towards established outcomes and the interaction effects of the spillover with each other. This will provide a more complete examination of how spillover influences employee outcomes and allows for greater understanding of what dimensions researchers and employers should focus on. THEORETICAL APPROACHES Negative Spillover Kahn, Wolfe, Quinn, Snoek and Rosenthal (1964) defined role conflict as the “simultaneous occurrence of two (or more) sets of pressures such that compliance with one would make 4 more difficult compliance with the other” (p. 19). Role conflict was originally conceptualised as an incompatibility between competing demands within a role (e.g. work), although conflict between work and other roles (e.g. family) was suggested. Greenhaus and Beutell’s (1985) seminal article on work-family conflict brought the attention strongly towards work and family domains and this has become well established in understanding the issues and problems with balancing these domains. Work-family conflict has been categorised as having three dimensions: time-based, strain-based, and behaviour-based conflict. These approaches highlight the problems employees may face when trying to balance their work and family roles, including lack of time to devote towards both work and family roles, the strains such divisions may cause, and issues associated with behaving differently within each role. Importantly, the direction of conflict can be bi-directional (Grzywacz, Arcury, Marin, Carrillo, Burke, Coates, & Quandt, 2007; Frone, Yardley, & Markel, 1997), with interference between roles coming from the work into family (e.g. coming home late due to work commitments) as well as family into work (e.g. being distracted at work due to having a sick child at home). Grzywacz et al. (2007) stated that “work–family conflict is typically viewed from a demands perspective” (p. 1120), with multiple demands leading to heightened workfamily conflict including social, structural, and psychological demands (Frone et al., 1997). Related to this are role strain and the scarcity approach. Goode (1960) noted that role strain relates to the felt difficulty in performing obligations related to a role, while the scarcity approach suggests that people have a limited quantity resources relating to time, energy and attention. As a result, employees with multiple roles such as work, childcare, eldercare etc. may quickly deplete these resources if not properly allocated, leading to greater conflict. Positive Spillover 5 There have been recent developments to the field that now suggest that work and family roles are not only interdependent but also that the spillover between roles is not always detrimental. While Greenhaus and Beutell’s (1985) conflict perspective has dominated the work-family literature, a number of researchers including Greenhaus have called for an approach that emphasises balancing the negative spillover or conflict perspectives with the positive or enrichment perspectives from the work and family roles (e.g. Greenhaus & Powell 2006; Powell & Greenhaus 2006; Carlson, Kacmar, Wayne & Grzywacz 2006; Wayne, Randel & Stevens 2006; Grzywacz & Marks 2000). While work-family conflict has become the established terminology for the negative spillover between work and family roles, there has been less uniformity towards potentially positive aspects. For example, terms such as positive spillover, enhancement, and facilitation have been offered (Greenhaus & Powell 2006, Powel & Greenhaus 2006, Wayne et al. 2006), while basically arguing the same approach: that the role exchange between work and family isn’t always detrimental and can have enhancing or positive effects. The present study utilizes the terms negative and positive spillover to represent the diametrically opposed influences from the interplay between work and family roles. Grzywacz and Marks (2000) asserted the importance of focusing on positive spillover from both the workplace into family (work-family) and the family into the workplace (familywork). This approach is similar to developments within the work-family conflict literature, where examining conflict originating from both the workplace and the home has become the established norm (e.g. Netemeyer, Boles & McMurrian, 1996). Hanson, Hammer and Colton (2006) defined work–family positive spillover “as the transfer of positively valenced affect, skills, behaviors, and values from the originating domain to the receiving domain” (p. 251). While the work-family literature has been slow in exploring the potential for positive 6 influences from other roles, there have been exceptions in other fields. For example, aspects from one role (e.g. work) have been found to improve other roles (e.g. family) including interpersonal communication and multitasking (Crouter, 1984; Ruderman, Ohlott, Panzer, & King, 2002). Similarly, values such as autonomy and curiosity based in the workplace have influenced the values that employed parents promote in their family (Pearlin & Kohn, 1966). Consequently, there is support for the notion that positive spillover from the workplace and/or the family can influence outcomes relating to work and non-work. The positive influence of work and family roles relates to the enhancement approach, which is an alternative to the scarcity approach. Sieber (1974) suggested that multiple roles could produce positive outcomes through a number of rewards derived from role accumulation. Role privileges relate to benefits and rewards gained by participating in a specific role. For example, enhanced self-esteem might be due to a worker gaining a positive job performance review or through a positive school report from a child. The end result see’s the worker feeling increased self-esteem with potential flow-on benefits to other roles. In a similar vein, status security refers to success from one role buffering the failures from another role. Related to the example above, a positive personal job performance report might buffer the disappointment and anxiety related to a sibling’s poor school report. Similarly, a positive weekend away with one’s family may allow an employee to recast the tension at work with co-workers in a more positive manner, such as reiterating that getting along with people takes effort rather than speaking disparagingly of colleagues. Resources are another reward that can be earned in one role and then utilized by another role. For example, an employee who works long hours and weekends to get a major project finished and earns a monetary bonus might utilize the financial benefit to reward their family with a vacation. Finally, ego gratification relates to personality enrichment where the spillover of skills, attitudes, and perspectives from 7 one role are used to solve problems in another role. For example, a working parent who successfully deals with a problem child might create skills relating to conflict resolution that can be utilized positively in the workplace. Combined, there is theoretical support for the spillover of work and family roles to have positive and negative influences. However, while there has been a plethora of empirical studies on the detrimental outcomes associated with work-family negative spillover, there has been much less empirical attention towards positive spillover. The following section hypotheses outcomes from both positive and negative spillover and builds towards testing potential interaction effects between the spillover dimensions. HYPOTHESES Direct Effects In their meta-analysis, Kossek and Ozeki (1998) found a strong negative relationship between work-family conflict and job satisfaction. They stated the “the relationship between job satisfaction and various w-f conflict measures is strong and negative across all samples: People with high levels of conflict tend to be less satisfied with their jobs” (pp. 141-144). While the relationship between work-family positive spillover and job satisfaction has been tested, there is a dearth of empirical evidence. Researchers have suggested higher work– family positive spillover will be positively linked to greater job satisfaction (Crouter, 1984; Grzywacz, Almeida, & McDonald, 2002). The link between positive spillover and job satisfaction relates to the transfer of positive skills, behaviour and values that in turn lead to improved role performance (Hanson et al., 2006). Hanson et al. (2006) argued that this improved role performance leads to reduced frustration with tasks and improved feelings, which ultimately lead to enhanced role satisfaction, and empirical evidence supporting a 8 positive link between positive spillover and job and family satisfaction were found. Consequently, employees able to positively utilize skills, behaviors and values from the home or workplace should respond with improved job satisfaction. Consequently, we hypothesize that spillover will be related to job satisfaction in positive and negative directions associated with positive or negative spillover. Hypothesis 1: Higher (a) work-family and (b) family-work negative spillover will be related to lower job satisfaction. Hypothesis 2: Higher (a) work-family and (b) family-work positive spillover will be related to higher job satisfaction. Mental health outcomes have been a major focus of both work-family conflict studies and have also been explored in the few positive spillover studies. Strong and consistent links between work-family conflict and psychological distress have been found (Allen, Herst, Bruck, & Sutton, 2000). Major, Klein and Ehrhart (2002) found work-family conflict to be directly related to psychological distress, and Frone (2000) found both work-family and family-work conflict to be related to two psychiatric disorders. In effect, those with greater negative spillover have worse mental health because of the detrimental of balancing the strain of multiple and conflicting roles. Relating to positive spillover, both work-family and familywork dimensions have been found to link with mental health (Grzywacz et al., 2002; Hanson et al., 2006; Hammer, Cullen, Neal, Sinclair, & Shafiro, 2005). It has been argued that positive mental health outcomes associated with positive spillover are due to buffering effects, where reinforced social relationships might buffer the negative events, or through the transference of skills, behaviours, and values, which result in rewards like higher self-esteem, which in turn, buffers against negative events (Hanson et al., 2006). Similarly, Stephens, 9 Franks and Atienza (1997) found higher work-family positive spillover associated with enhanced psychological well being. This leads to our next set of Hypotheses. Hypothesis 3: Higher (a) work-family and (b) family-work negative spillover will be related to higher psychological distress. Hypothesis 4: Higher (a) work-family and (b) family-work positive spillover will be related to lower psychological distress. Interaction Effects of Spillover Perry-Jenkins, Repetti and Crouter (2000) suggested the work-family literature needs to embrace complexity by exploring interaction effects. There is growing recognition that testing interaction effects can clarify and add value to enhance our understanding of established constructs and effects. This approach has been seen in other areas, specifically the organizational justice literature, where studies have begun to explore the moderating role of various organizational justice dimensions upon each other. For example, Spell and Arnold (2007) tested the effects of interpersonal justice and distributive justice on the antecedents and outcomes of procedural justice. Related to this, Graves, Ohlott and Ruderman (2007) criticized the work-family literature for focusing on work aspects rather than non-work aspects (e.g. family) and Graves et al. (2007) noted the importance of understanding the greater roles a person has in their life. Similarly, Bishop, Scott, Goldsby and Cropanzano (2005) noted the need to test effects from one domain (e.g. family) into another (e.g. work), and Ford, Heinen and Langkamer (2007) suggested the influence of non-work factors on job outcomes might be greater than initially evidenced. Further, Greenhaus, Collins and Shaw (2003) highlighted the importance of family influences when they found quality of life was highest for those more satisfied in family than work. 10 Haar (2005) tested the interaction effects of work-family conflict and family-work conflict on each other and found intensification effects from conflict from the other role, such that high levels of both forms of conflict lead to greater reductions in job satisfaction and organizational commitment when both types of conflict were high. When testing the moderating effects of conflict between work-family and family-work domains Haar (2005) stated “conflict occurring in one domain simultaneously as conflict from the other domain might have an intensification effect on job outcomes” (p. 6). While we understand the direct effects of work-family conflict and, to a lesser extent, positive spillover, what we do not have a clear understanding of is how these aspects may influence outcomes together. The reason these interaction effects require greater attention is that we are beginning to understand that employees can experience high levels of positive and negative spillover simultaneously. For example, Demerouti and Geurts (2004) reported 16% of respondents reported relatively high levels of both positive and negative work-family spillover. Consequently, there is increased awareness of, and interest in, how work-family positive and negative spillover influence employees attitudes and behaviors (Demerouti & Geurts, 2004; Frone, 2003). We suggest the argument for testing the interaction effects of both negative and positive spillover dimensions on each other exists in the positive spillover theory. Sieber (1974) suggested that the advantages of role accumulation could outweigh any negative outcomes, resulting in net gratification. As such, positive spillover may buffer the influence of negative spillover. Sieber (1974) asserted that the approach of positive spillover outweighing negative spillover has been widely neglected, and we suggest testing the interactions in this manner allow us to specifically test the ability of positive spillover in buffering detrimental outcomes associated with negative spillover. We argue that testing the four dimensions of work-family and family-work positive and negative spillover will provide insight into the interaction 11 effects of separate domains as suggested above. Consequently, we hypothesize that positive spillover will buffer the detrimental influence of negative spillover towards outcomes for both work-family and family-work dimensions given arguments that positive spillover can influence outcomes over and above negative spillover (Sieber, 1974). Hypothesis 5: (a) Work-family and (b) family-work positive spillover will buffer (moderate) the relationship between work-family and family-work negative spillover and job satisfaction. Hypothesis 6: (a) Work-family and (b) family-work positive spillover will buffer (moderate) the relationship between work-family and family-work negative spillover and psychological distress. METHOD Sample and Procedure Data were collected from two major sources: a private sector bank and a public sector network of employed professionals. Participants were contacted via an email outlining the project and included a link to a web-based survey. Following the suggestions of Kittleson, (1997), follow-up requests to complete the online survey were emailed two weeks later. A total of 1190 employees were emailed and a total of 420 employee surveys were completed on-line. Twenty-two surveys were removed due to missing data, leaving a final data set of 398, for a 33% response rate. Respondents ranged in age from 21 to 63 years, with an average age of 40 years (SD=9.4), 65% were female, 80% were married, and 69% parents. Education was well spread with 30% with high school education, 13% technical college qualification, 24% with a bachelors degree, 15% had a graduate qualification, and 18% with a masters degree or PhD. Overall, respondents worked 45.9 hours per week (SD=11.5). 12 Measures To confirm the separate and opposite dimensions of work-family and family-work positive and negative conflict, items were tested by structural equation modeling (SEM) using AMOS v. 16 (Arbuckle, 1997) to assess the convergent and discriminant validity of the multiple-item measures (Gerbing & Anderson, 1993). Seven goodness-of-fit indexes were utilized to assess the overall measurement model fit (cf. Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1993): the chi-square goodnessof-fit statistic, minimum discrepancy divided by its degrees of freedom (CMIN/DF), the goodness-of-fit index (GFI), the adjusted goodness-of-fit index (AGFI), the comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis coefficient (TLI), and the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) with confidence intervals. The SEM literature suggests a model with acceptable fit is found when the GFI, CFI, TLI are equal to .95 or greater, .90 or greater for the AGFI, .05 or less for the RMSEA and 3.0 or less for CMIN/DF (Bentler & Bonett, 1980; Hu & Bentler, 1999; Carmines & McIver, 1981). The measurement model did fit the data well for a four-factor solution: χ2 (71) = 146.860 (p = .000), GFI = .95, CFI = .96, TLI = .95, AGFI = .96, RMSEA = 0.05 (LO 90 = 0.04 and HI 90 = 0.06), and CMIN/DF = 2.068. To check whether this was the best model based on the items, we re-ran the CFA testing three alternative models, with (1) all items combined for a general positive and negative spillover measure (a one factor model), (2) all work-family and family-work items combined for both negative and positive spillover (a two-factor model), and (3) all work-family items combined (positive and negative) and all family-work items combined (positive and negative) for a different two-factor model. Results for the three alternative models show they are a poor fit for the data. Alternative model 1: χ2 (77) = 1380.259 (p = .000), GFI = .63, CFI = .39, TLI = .28, AGFI = .50, RMSEA = 0.20 (LO 90 = 0.19 and HI 90 = 0.21), and CMIN/DF = 17.925. 13 Alternative model 2: χ2 (76) = 982.901 (p = .000), GFI = .71, CFI = .57, TLI = .49, AGFI = .60, RMSEA = 0.17 (LO 90 = 0.16 and HI 90 = 0.18), and CMIN/DF = 12.933. Alternative model 3: χ2 (76) = 557.603 (p = .000), GFI = .82, CFI = .77, TLI = .73, AGFI = .75, RMSEA = 0.12 (LO 90 = 0.12 and HI 90 = 0.13), and CMIN/DF = 7.337. This reinforces the four factor model of spillover we tested. Work-Family Negative Spillover and Family-Work Negative Spillover were measured with 8items by Carlson, Kacmar and Williams (2000), coded 1=strongly disagree, 6=strongly agree. A sample item for the work-family dimension is “My work keeps me from my family activities more than I would like” (α = .84) and for the family-work dimension is “The time I spend on family responsibilities often interferes with my work” (α = .84). Work-Family Positive Spillover and Family-Work Positive Spillover were measured with 6-items from Grzywacz and Marks (2000), coded 1=never, 5=all of the time. A sample item for the workfamily dimension is “The things you do at work help you deal with personal and practical issues at home” (α = .70) and for the family-work dimension is “The love and respect you get at home makes you feel confident about yourself at work” (α = .66). Job Satisfaction was measured using 2-items by Hackman and Oldham (1975), coded 1=strongly disagree, 6=strongly agree. Items used were “Generally speaking, I am very satisfied with my present job” and “I am generally satisfied with the kind of work I do in my present job”. This measure has a Cronbach’s alpha of .89. Psychological Distress was measured using 9-items by Santor and Coyne (1997), coded 1=none, to a little of the time (less than 1 or 2 days) and 2=a moderate amount of the time (37 days). Questions followed the stem “During the past week…” and a sample item is “you felt depressed”. Two items were reverse coded. A high score indicates higher psychological distress. This measure has a Cronbach’s alpha of .84. 14 Similar to other work-family studies (e.g. Kossek, Colquitt, & Noe, 2001) we controlled for demographic variables: Gender (1=female, 0=male), Marital Status (1=married/cohabitating, 0=single), and Family Size (number of children, 0=no children). Analysis Separate hierarchical regressions were conducted to examine the direct effects of negative spillover on outcomes (Hypotheses 1 and 2), the direct effects of positive spillover on outcomes (Hypotheses 3 and 4), and the interaction effects of these measures on each other (Hypotheses 5 and 6). Step 1 contained the control variables (gender, marital status, and family size). Step 2 contained the work-family and family-work negative spillover measures, while Step 3 contained the work-family and family-work positive spillover measures. We include negative and positive spillover separately in different steps to allow for comparisons between these effects. This will allow for comparisons between negative and positive spillover to be made. To test for moderation, Step 4 held the interaction between work-family and family-work positive and negative spillover. To address issues of multi-collinearity, mean centering of the interaction terms was done (Aiken & West, 1991). RESULTS Descriptive statistics for all the study variables is shown in Table 1. _________ Insert Table 1 about here _________ Table 1 shows that in the majority, the work-family measures are significantly correlated in the expected direction with each other (all r< .30, all p< .01), although there is no evidence of concept redundancy (Morrow, 1983). The exception is family-work negative spillover and 15 work-family positive spillover which is non-significant (r= -.04). The work-family measures are all significantly related to job satisfaction and psychological distress in the appropriate directions (all r > .15, all p >.01). Finally, job satisfaction is significantly correlated with psychological distress (r= .39, p< .01). Results of the hierarchical regressions for the direct and interaction effects of negative and positive spillover (Hypotheses 1 to 6) are shown in Tables 2 and 3. _________ Insert Tables 2 and 3 about here _________ Direct Effects Table 2 shows that work-family negative spillover is significantly related to job satisfaction (ß = -.23, p< .001), as is family-work negative spillover (ß = -.10, p< .1). Similarly, work-family positive spillover is significantly related to job satisfaction (ß = .27, p< .001), as is familywork positive spillover (ß = .11, p< .05). Table 3 shows work-family negative spillover is significantly related to psychological distress (ß = .31, p< .001), as is family-work negative spillover (ß = .20, p< .01). While family-work positive spillover is significantly related to psychological distress (ß = -.15, p< .01), work-family positive spillover is not (ß = -.02, non significant). From the R2 Change figures in Step 2, we see negative spillover accounts for 8% of the variance for job satisfaction (p< .001) and this is similar to that from positive spillover (9%, p< .001). However, towards psychological distress, negative spillover accounts for a large amount of the variance at 18% (p< .001) compared to a more modest 2% (p< .05) from positive spillover. These findings support Hypotheses 1 to three and 4b but not Hypothesis 4a. Interaction Effects 16 Table 2 shows there are significant interaction effects between negative and positive spillover towards job satisfaction, accounting for an additional 3% (p< .05) of variance. Work-family positive spillover had a significant interaction with work-family negative spillover (ß= -.11, p< .05) and family-work negative spillover (ß= -.19, p< .01). This provides strong support for Hypothesis 5a but not 5b. Table 3 shows there are significant interaction effects between negative and positive spillover towards psychological distress, accounting for an additional 3% (p< .05) of variance. Family-work positive spillover had a significant interaction with work-family negative spillover (ß= -.10, p< .1) and work-family positive spillover had a significant interaction with family-work negative spillover (ß= -.13, p< .05). This provides some support for Hypotheses 6a and 6b. To facilitate interpretation of the significant moderator effects, interactions are presented in Figures 1 to 4, with work-family and familywork negative and positive spillover low and high representing points below and above the respective means. _________ Insert Figures 1 and 2 about here __________ Plotting the interaction terms for job satisfaction (Figures 1 and 2) shows similar effects for work-family and family-work negative spillover interactions. When work-family and familywork negative spillover is low, respondents with high work-family positive spillover report higher job satisfaction and these beneficial effects are maintained even when negative spillover from both domains increases to high. While there is a decrease in job satisfaction amongst all respondents, those with high work-family positive spillover report significantly higher job satisfaction at all levels of negative spillover compared to respondents with low work-family positive spillover. _________ 17 Insert Figure 3 about here __________ Plotting the interaction terms for psychological distress (Figure 3) indicates respondents with high family-work positive spillover report significantly lower psychological distress when work-family negative spillover is low. When work-family negative spillover increases to high, all respondents report increased psychological distress, but those with higher familywork positive spillover maintain a significantly lower level of distress than those with low family-work positive spillover. _________ Insert Figure 4 about here __________ Plotting the interaction terms for psychological distress (Figure 4) shows similar levels of psychological distress irrespective of work-family positive spillover levels at low levels of family-work negative spillover. When family-work negative spillover increases to high, all respondents report increased psychological distress, but those with higher family-work positive spillover maintain a significantly lower level of distress than those with low familywork positive spillover. Overall, the regression models were significant: job satisfaction (Total R2 = .25, F = 7.832, p< .001) and psychological distress (Total R2 = .26, F = 7.846, p< .001). Finally, the variance inflation factors (VIF) were examined for evidence of multicollinearity. Experts suggest multicollinearity can be detected when the VIF values equal 10 or higher (Ryan, 1997). However, all the scores were below 1.8, indicating no evidence of multicollinearity unduly influencing the regression estimates. 18 DISCUSSION The present study focused on testing the influence of work-family and family-work dimensions of negative and positive spillover on outcomes. Our use of SEM supported the separation of work-family and family-work directions as well as negative and positive spillover dimensions, which provides us with strong confidence the findings were not due to poor measurement issues. The direct effects from negative spillover were consistent towards both outcomes, which are aligned with the literature which has suggested consistent detrimental effects (e.g. Kossek and Ozeki, 1998). Further, we find support for negative spillover originating from both the work and family domains which confirms testing these effects from both directions. Given the infancy of the positive spillover and lack of large numbers of empirical studies, there is clearly need of more studies to provide a clearer picture of effects. Work-family and family-work positive spillover were both significantly related to job satisfaction which supports the current literature (Grzywacz et al., 2002). Our findings also support positive spillover being linked negatively to psychological distress (Grzywacz et al., 2002; Hanson et al., 2006), although our findings were significant only from the familywork domain. However, it is important to note that the correlations were significant towards psychological distress from both work-family and family-work positive spillover, and thus the significant finding for family-work positive spillover is a result of the regression analysis. Again, these findings encourage a bi-directional approach to testing positive spillover as has been found in the negative spillover literature. The other major focus of the present study was to test the interaction effects and these yielded strong and consistent interaction effects towards job satisfaction and psychological distress. For job satisfaction, the interaction effects were consistent, with respondents with high positive work-family spillover reporting significantly higher levels of job satisfaction at all 19 levels of work-family and family-work negative spillover. This shows that spillover has significant effects on job satisfaction at the direct level of effect for both positive and negative spillover, and when combined, there are additional effects where work-family positive spillover buffers the detrimental effects of negative spillover. The interaction effects towards psychological distress were slightly different from that of job satisfaction, with positive spillover originating from both the work and home buffering negative spillover. Respondents with high positive spillover reported significantly higher levels of job satisfaction and lower levels of distress when negative spillover was high. The results towards psychological distress show that positive spillover can influence this outcome from both the work and family direction towards negative spillover from both the work and family domains. While these influences were not universally influential, it does support the interaction of positive spillover can be from both work-family directions potentially towards both work-family directions of negative spillover. Implications There are a number of major implications from these finding. Firstly, when we compare the R2 Change figures for negative and positive spillover, we see that positive spillover accounts for significant amounts of variance over and above negative spillover. For the job satisfaction model, negative spillover accounts for 8% while positive spillover accounts for an additional 9% of variance after negative spillover has been considered. For psychological distress, the additional amounts of variance from positive spillover is modest (2%) compared to the large amount accounted for by negative spillover (18%). We suggest these findings encourage researchers to test the negative and positive spillover effects together when testing outcomes. While further studies are needed to improve generalizability of these direct effects, these findings do suggest that testing for the effects of negative spillover on their own is limited and 20 may not uncover the entire effects of spillover between the work and family domains. The weighting of positive and negative spillover effects on variance shows positive spillover may be more important than negative spillover towards some outcomes (e.g. job satisfaction), and conversely, of lesser importance (e.g. psychological distress). Given these outcomes have received a lot of attention in the work-family conflict literature our findings suggest the effects of domain spillover on outcomes may be under represented unless both positive and negative spillover from both the work and family domains are included. Consequently, we encourage researchers to explore positive spillover in addition to negative spillover to clarify the consistent effects that spillover may have on outcomes. Further, there are clear methodological advantages in placing negative and positive spillover in separate steps in regression models as this allows us to compare the extent of the effects towards overall variance. This may prove especially beneficial for researchers and organizations if the findings in the present study towards job satisfaction are consistently held, with positive spillover being a greater influencer than negative spillover. In a related manner, testing the interaction effects of positive spillover on negative spillover appears highly warranted. As expected, greater understanding of outcomes associated with work-family spillover was found, which highlights the importance of testing interaction effects and non-work elements supporting such calls in the literature (Ford et al., 2007; Graves et al., 2007; Bishop et al., 2005; Perry-Jenkins et al., 2000). The findings that positive spillover buffered job satisfaction and psychological distress aligns with Sieber’s (1974) assertion that the advantages of role accumulation might outweigh negative influences, and towards these outcomes, there does appear to be net gratification. Consequently, the findings here encourage further testing of the effects of positive spillover buffering negative spillover. While research in positive spillover is only beginning to emerge, we encourage researchers to 21 consider both positive and negative spillover dimensions and how these might interact together. Ultimately, this should provide greater understanding of the effects of spillover between work and family domains, which aligns well with Greenhaus and Powell’s (2006) calls for greater attention to the positive side of work and family. We’d argue that this can be taken one step further through testing the combined (i.e. interaction) effects of negative and positive spillover on outcomes, which should provide a clearer indication of what employees experience simultaneously through their work and family roles. These findings also have implications for practice. This is especially prevalent, when we consider firms have been inundated for decades with the detrimental effects of the workfamily negative spillover. Until recently, firms have been told that the work-family interface results in multiple and consistently negative outcomes, which might be buffered through elements such as supervisor support (e.g. van Daalen, Willemsen & Sanders, 2006). However, our findings support the new thrust of research into positive spillover and provides organizations with evidence that the work-family interface is not always negative, and indeed, that these positive influences may in turn buffer the negative spillover resulting in overall positive fulfilment. Consequently, organizations should work towards understanding, highlighting and promoting positive work and family spillover for employees, for example, reinforcing Sieber’s (1974) rewards such as ego gratification through reinforcing positive skills from the job that might be useful at home. Similarly, organizational rewards targeted at families might provide better work-family spillover, for example, performance rewards such as family vacations or meals our for the family might reinforce the positive links between work and family. As such, the present study provides organizations with evidence that work and home may hold both positive and negative influences on outcomes, and further, that 22 positive spillover might also buffer the negative further enhancing the lives and wellbeing of employees. Limitations An issue with the majority of organizational research is data collection at one point in time, which raises concerns towards common method variance. As undertaken in other work-family research (e.g. Major et al., 2002), we tested for common method variance concerns by conducting Harman’s One Factor Test. The resulting factor analysis (unrotated) resulted in ten factors, the largest accounting for 23% of the variance. Due to the large number of factors, and that no single factor dominated the analysis, there is little evidence of common method variance (Podsakoff & Organ, 1986). Further, the interaction effects like the ones we consider in the present study would not be expected to be as susceptible to method variance as compared to main effects, providing further evidence that common method variance is not an issue (Evans, 1985). Further, the work-family literature has been criticized for homogenous samples (Kossek & Ozeki, 1998) and while our sample was collected from mainly two areas (a bank and a professional network), there was a wide spread amongst respondents in their demographics as well as a good spread in positions (48% managers and 52% non-managers). As such, we suggest the findings represent a good indication of the effects of work-family spillover on this sample of Australian employees, although further studies are needed to solidify our understanding of the direct and interactional effects of positive and negative spillover. Conclusion The present study provides support for assessing the influence of positive and negative spillover on outcomes from both the work-family and family-work directions, and encourages 23 the additional exploration of interaction effects between positive and negative spillover. Clearly, given the infancy of the positive spillover field, further studies are needed to clarify the direct and interaction effects, but we suggest the present study provides strong direction and argument for testing both positive and negative effects together to provide a clearer understanding of the role that the work-family interface plays in the modern workplace. 24 REFERENCES Aiken, L. S. & West, S. G. (1991) Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. Newbury Park: Sage. Allen, T. D., Herst, D. E. L., Bruck, C. S., & Sutton, M. (2000). Consequences associated with work-to-family conflict: A review and agenda for future research. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5, 278-308. Arbuckle, J. L. (1997). AMOS users’ guide version 4.0. Chicago: Smallwaters Corporation. Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588–606. Bishop, J. W., Scott, K. D., Goldsby, M. G., & Cropanzano, R. (2005). A construct validity study of commitment and perceived support variables. A multifoci approach across different team environments. Group and Organisational Management, 30(2), 153-180. Cammann, C., Fichman, M., Jenkins, D., & Klesh, J. (1979). The Michigan Organisational Assessment Questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan. Carlson, D. S., Kacmar, K. M., &Williams, L. J. (2000). Construction and Initial Validation of a Multidimensional Measure of Work–Family Conflict. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 56, 249–276. Carlson, D., Kacmar, K. M., Wayne, J. H., & Grzywacz, J. G. (2006). Measuring the positive side of the work-family interface: Development and validation of a workfamily enrichment scale. Journal of Vocational Behaviour, 68, 131-164. Carmines, E. G. & McIver, J. P. (1981). Analyzing models with unobserved variables. In Bohrnstedt, G.W. & Borgatta, E.F. [Eds.] Social measurement: Current issues. Beverly Hills: Sage. 25 Crouter, A. C. (1984). Spillover from family to work: The neglected side of the work-family interface. Human Relations, 37(6), 425-441. Demerouti, E., & Geurts, S. (2004). Towards a typology of work-home interaction. Community, Work, & Family, 7, 285-309. Evans, M. G. (1985). A Monte Carlo study of the effects of correlated method variance in moderated multiple regression analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 36, 305–323. Ford, M. T., Heinen, B. A., & Langkamer, K. L. (2007). Work and family satisfaction and conflict: A meta-analysis of cross –domain relations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(1), 57-80. Frone, M. R., Yardley, J. K., & Markel, K. S. (1997). Developing and testing an integrative model of the work-family interface. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 50, 145–167. Frone, M. R. (2000). Work-Family Conflict and Employee Psychiatric Disorders: The National Comorbidity Survey. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(6), 888-895. Frone, M. R. (2003). Work-family balance. In J. C. Quick & L. E. Tetrick (Eds.). Handbook of Occupational Health Psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Goode, W. J. (1960). A theory of role strain. American Sociological Review, 25, 483-496. Graves, L. M., Ohlott, P. J., & Ruderman, M. N. (2007). Commitment to family roles: Effects on managers’ attitudes and performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(1), 44-56. Greenhaus, J. H., & Beutell, N. J. (1985). Sources of conflict between work and family roles. Academy of Management Review, 10(1), 76-88. Greenhaus, J. H., Collins, K. M., & Shaw, J. D. (2003). The relation between work-family balance and quality of life. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 63 (3), 510-531. 26 Greenhaus, J. H., & Powell, G. N. (2006). When work and family are allies: A theory of work-family enrichment. Academy of Management Review, 31, 72-79. Grzywacz, J. G., Arcury, T. A., Marin, A., Carrillo, L., Burke, B., Coates, M. L., & Quandt, S. A. (2007). Work–family conflict: Experiences and health implications among immigrant Latinos. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(4), 1119–1130. Grzywacz, J. G. & Marks, N. F. (2000). Reconceptualizing the work-family interface: An ecological perspective on the correlates of positive and negative spillover between work and family. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5(1), 111-126. Grzywacz, J. G., Almeida, D. A., & McDonald, D. A. (2002). Work-family spillover and daily reports of work and family stress in the adult labor force. Family Relations, 51(1), 28-36. Hackman, J. & Oldham, G. (1975). Development of the job diagnostic survey. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60, 159-170. Haar, J. M. (2005). Work-Family conflict predicting job satisfaction and organisational commitment: testing the moderating effects of conflict from both work and family domains. The New Zealand Journal of Human Resources Management, Vol 5, 1-31, retrieved 1 January 2008 at http://www.nzjhrm.org.nz/articles/2005_articles/WFC_FWC_Moderating_NZJHRM_ Feb_2005.doc Hanson, G. C., Hammer, L. B., & Colton, C. L. (2006). Development and Validation of a Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Work–Family Positive Spillover. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 11(3), 249–265. Hammer, L. B., Cullen, J. C., Neal, M. B., Sinclair, R. R., & Shafiro, M. (2005). The longitudinal effects of work-family conflict and positive spillover on depressive 27 symptoms among dual-earner couples. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 10, 138-154. Hu, L. T. & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. Jöreskog, K.G. and Sörbom, D. 1993. Lisrel 8: Structural equations modeling with the SIMPLIS command language. Chicago: Scientific Software International. Kahn, R. L., Wolfe, D. M., Quinn, R., Snoek, J. D., & Rosenthal, R. A. (1964). Organizational Stress. New York: Wiley. Kittleson, M. (1997). Determining effective follow-up of e-mail surveys. American Journal of Health Behavior, 21(3), 193-196. Kossek, E. E., Colquitt, J. A., & Noe, R. A. (2001). Caregiving decisions, well-being, and performance: The effects of place and provider as a function of dependent type and work-family climates. Academy of Management Journal, 44, 29-44. Kossek, E. & Ozeki, C. 1998. Work-family conflict, policies and the job-life satisfaction relation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83(2), 139-149. Major, V. S., Klein, K. J., & Ehrhart, M. G. (2002). Work time, work interference with family, and psychological distress. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(3), 427-436. Morrow, P. (1983). Concept redundancy in organizational research: The case of work commitment. Academy of Management Review, 8(3), 486-500. Netemeyer, R. G., Boles, J. S. & McMurrian, R. 1996. Development and validation of workfamily conflict and family-work conflict scales. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81(4), 400-410. Pearlin, L., & Kohn, M. (1966). Social class, occupational, and parental values: A cross national study. American Sociological Review, 31, 466–479. 28 Perry-Jenkins, M., Repetti, R. L., & Crouter, A. C. (2000). Work and family in the 1990s. Journal of Marriage and Family, 62, 981-998. Podsakoff, P. M. & Organ, D. W. (1986). Self-reports in organisational research: Problems and prospects. Journal of Management, 12(4), 531-544. Powell, G. N. & Greenhaus, J. H. (2006). Think piece: Is the opposite of positive negative? Career Development International, 11(7), 650-659. Ruderman, M. N., Ohlott, P. J., Panzer, K., & King, S. (2002). Benefits of multiple roles for managerial women. Academy of Management Journal, 45, 369-386. Ryan, T. P. (1997). Modern Regression Methods. New York: Wiley. Santor, D. A., Coyne, J. C. (1997). Shortening the CES-D to improve its ability to detect cases of depression. Psychological Assessment, 9, 233-243. Sieber, S. D. (1974). Toward a Theory of Role Accumulation. American Sociological Review, 39(4), 567-578. Spell, C., & Arnold, T. (2007). An appraisal perspective of justice, structure, and job control as antecedents of psychological distress. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 28(6), 729-751. Stephens, M. A. P., Franks, M. M., & Atienza, A. A. (1997). Where two roles intersect: Spillover between parent care and employment. Psychology and Aging, 12, 30–37. van Daalen, G., Willemsen, T, M., & Sanders, K. (2006). Reducing work-family conflict through different sources of social support. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 69, 462476. Wayne, J. H., Randel, A. E. & Stevens, J. (2006). The role of identity and work-family support in work-family enrichment and its work related consequences. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 69(3), 445-461. 29 TABLE 1. Correlations and Descriptive Statistics of the Study Variables Variables 1. Family Size 2. Work-Family Negative Spillover 3. Family-Work Negative Spillover 4. Work-Family Positive Spillover 5. Family-Work Positive Spillover 6. Job Satisfaction 7. Psychological Distress N=398, *p<.05, **p<.01 M 2.1 3.8 2.6 2.7 3.6 4.5 1.3 SD 1.0 1.1 .94 .72 .70 1.2 .29 1 --.07 .02 .06 -.12* .18** -.16** 2 3 4 5 6 7 -.30** -.19** -.16** -.28** .36** --.04 -.21** -.20** .33** -.29** .37** -.16** -.16** -.25** -.39** -- 30 TABLE 2. Regression Coefficients for Job Satisfaction Variables Gender Marital Status Family Size WF Negative Spillover FW Negative Spillover WF Positive Spillover FW Positive Spillover Step 1 Controls .13* .01 .21** Job Satisfaction Step 2 Step 3 Predictors Moderators .14* .07 .03 .05 .20** .19** -.23*** -.10† Step 4 Interactions .07 .04 .19** -.14* -.09† -.18** -.06 .27*** .11* .27*** .08 WF Negative Spillover x WF Positive Spillover WF Negative Spillover x FW Positive Spillover FW Negative Spillover x WF Positive Spillover FW Negative Spillover x FW Positive Spillover R2 change .05** .08*** .09*** Total R2 .05 .13 .22 2 Adjusted R .04 .11 .20 F Statistic 4.546** 7.843*** 10.562*** †p< .1, *p<.05, **p<.01, p< .001. Standardized regression coefficients, all significance tests were single-tailed. -.11* .01 .19** -.02 .03* .25 .22 7.832*** 31 TABLE 3. Regression Coefficients for Psychological Distress Variables Gender Marital Status Family Size WF Negative Spillover FW Negative Spillover WF Positive Spillover FW Positive Spillover Step 1 Controls .02 .07 -.15* Psychological Distress Step 2 Step 3 Predictors Moderators .02 .04 .03 .01 -.13* -.15* .31*** .20** Step 4 Interactions .04 .01 -.15** .27*** .17** .30*** .17** -.02 -.15** -.03 -.11* WF Negative Spillover x WF Positive Spillover WF Negative Spillover x FW Positive Spillover FW Negative Spillover x WF Positive Spillover FW Negative Spillover x FW Positive Spillover R2 change .03† .18*** .02* Total R2 .03 .21 .23 2 Adjusted R .02 .19 .21 F Statistic 2.640† 13.291*** 10.699*** †p< .1, *p<.05, **p<.01, p< .001. Standardized regression coefficients, all significance tests were single-tailed. .07 -.10† -.13* -.01 .03* .26 .23 7.846*** 32 Figure 1. Interaction between Work-Family Negative Spillover & Work-Family Positive Spillover with Job Satisfaction as Dependent Variable 5.2 Job Satisfaction 5.0 4.8 Work-Family Positive Spillover High 4.6 4.4 4.2 Work-Family Positive Spillover Low 4.0 3.8 Work-Family Negative Spillover Low Work-Family Negative Spillover High 33 Figure 2. Interaction between Family-Work Negative Spillover & Work-Family Positive Spillover with Job Satisfaction as Dependent Variable 5.0 Work-Family Positive Spillover High Job Satisfaction 4.8 4.6 4.4 4.2 4.0 3.8 Work-Family Positive Spillover Low 3.6 Family-Work Negative Spillover Low Family-Work Negative Spillover High 34 Figure 3. Interaction between Work-Family Negative Spillover & Family-Work Positive Spillover with Psychological Distress as Dependent Variable Family-Work Positive Spillover Low Psychological Distress 1.4 Family-Work Positive Spillover High 1.3 1.2 1.1 Work-Family Negative Spillover Low Work-Family Negative Spillover High 35 Figure 4. Interaction between Family-Work Negative Spillover & Work-Family Positive Spillover with Psychological Distress as Dependent Variable Psychological Distress 1.5 Work-Family Positive Spillover Low 1.4 1.3 Work-Family Positive Spillover High 1.2 1.1 Family-Work Negative Spillover Low Family-Work Negative Spillover High