Miss America Contestants and the Self: Evidence for Empowerment By Brook Matthews 2 Table of Contents I. Dedication II Introduction A. Description of Topic III. Literature Review A. History of Miss America B. Pageant Boot Camp: Legitimizing the Queen C. Empowerment D. The Male Gaze E. Gender Oppression F. The Self and TST IV. Methodology A. Research Questions B. Measuring the Contestant Self V. Results and Analysis A. Part One – Nature of the Self B. Part Two – Subject Matter C. Part Three – Strength of the Self VI. Conclusion VII. About the Author VIII. Appendix A. Table 4: Tally Sheet for Parts One, Two and Three B. Coding Instructions C. Coding Sheet D. I-statement examples IX. Bibliography 3 Dedication “Empowerers seek to foster growth in others by assisting individuals in finding and using their own power” (Muller, 1994). This work is dedicated to the empowerers in my life; those who have helped me find and use my own power to do things I only dreamed about as a child. I often speak about my first experience as a pageant contestant and what a defining moment that was. For the first time in my life, I truly felt proud of who I was and what I was doing. That experience translated into a philosophy that has guided me ever since: I can accomplish great things by believing in myself and taking a chance. However, it took the encouragement and open-heartedness of the following empowerers to provide the direction and opportunities for me to exercise that philosophy: Dr. Montgomery, who told me four years ago, “Everyone has a story.” She has since assisted me in writing a new chapter in my own story, by spending countless hours reviewing the work you are about to read. With an eagle-eye for recognizing the potential of her students, she works selflessly in helping them achieve their dreams. Dr. Wicks, who has an uncanny way of pushing her students until they are intellectually exhausted and think they can go no further. Just when we think we’ve reached the edge, her understanding of life as a student emboldens us to go one step further than we ever imagined. 4 Bill Matthews, my father, who I have always turned to for advice. He served as an objective critic and has reviewed this work several times, helping me remove my “pageant blinders” so I could see MAO for what it is – warts and all. Rhonda Matthews, my mother and confidant, who has stood beside me every step of the way. She is who I turn to for an ear to gripe at or a shoulder to cry on…Lord knows there were plenty of those moments during the past few months! My “pageant family”: Directors, volunteers and fellow contestants. The experiences I have shared with you are what started me on this “journey of selfexploration.” I hope this work will help us all better understand ourselves and our critics, that we may be able to improve and more effectively defend MAO. I am pleased to share with these people the result of their encouragement and direction. One of the most important lessons learned in writing this is that empowerment is a chain linking people together through the relationships they build. My hope is that following graduation, I will be able to continue the chain of empowerment through the new relationships I build. Seeing the results of such an endeavor will truly be one of life’s “crowning” achievements. 5 Introduction Description of Topic - For more than 80 years, the Miss America pageant has prompted positive and negative emotional responses from contestants and viewers alike, signaling society’s ambivalence “when female beauty is equated with competitiveness and must be judged” (Watson & Martin, p. 123). Protests of the pageant that began in the 1920s led to regulations and changes aimed at the systematic legitimization of the pageant as a way to empower women. Such legitimization has come in the form of rules and contestant contracts, judging procedures, changes in telecast format, and most importantly, the pageant’s position as a scholarship program that empowers women. This study examines the rule changes that led to today’s Miss America (MA) pageant’s main focus – awarding scholarship to women – which the Miss America Organization (MAO) claims sets the program apart from “beauty contests” and gives it a noble raison d’etre. MAO is the world’s largest provider of scholarships to women, awarding more than $40 million each year in scholarship monies to thousands of contestants (Greco, p. 53). While scholarships are the cornerstone of MAO, a focus on personal growth, development and empowerment is also a key component of the program’s mission: “The Miss America Organization is a not-for-profit organization that has maintained a tradition for many decades of empowering American women to achieve their personal and professional goals, while providing a forum for them to 6 express their opinions, talents and intelligence,” (Miss America Mission Statement). Despite the organization’s attempt to focus on scholarship, personal growth, development and empowerment, the pageant has long had a “meat market” reputation in which young women’s bodies are displayed and ogled at during swimsuit competition. “The basic, and base, pageant appeal is and always has been, girl-watching – and the fewer clothes, the better,” (Deford, p. 11). Beauty contests were not uncommon in the years leading up to the first MA pageant. However, what set MA apart from early beauty contests was its inclusion of the bathing suit competition. This early focus on young women in bathing attire proved to be a successful promotional gimmick benefiting Atlantic City resort owners, as over 100,000 spectators gathered to watch (Timeline). It was during this time that, “Producers of even ‘legitimate’ theatrical venues found that the display of women, particularly in scanty attire, was lucrative business,” (Latham, p. 154). Critics charge that “the display” of female bodies, which the MA pageant seems to represent, contributes to the objectification of women. This objectification is said to occur through the pageant’s ability to set up “the gaze,” or “visual inspection of the body,” (Kaschak 1992; Fredrickson & Roberts, p. 175). Stage competition, especially swimsuit competition, is where “the gaze” primarily occurs within the pageant. In addition to objectification during stage competitions, contestants also face the objectifying gaze on a day-to-day basis from other contestants, parents, directors and coaches. Consequently, the meat- 7 market atmosphere of the pageant is said to contribute to the objectification of contestants that may put them at risk for developing psychological problems, including depression, eating disorders and sexual dysfunction (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). However, contestant testimonials such as one made by Miss Nebraska 2000, Jill Pennington, support Fredrickson and Roberts (1997) observation that not all women respond to objectification in the same way: “My participation in the Miss America Program has given me so much; friendships, public speaking skills, memories, scholarships. However, I believe the most valuable benefit for me has been an increased sense of self,” (Pennington, 2001). Maintaining a sense of personal power in the face of objectification is essential to success in the pageant world and some say MA contestants have mastered the art of not letting anyone “rattle their cage.” In doing so, these young women may have found a strength that could be used to help other women overcome the negative impacts of objectification. This paper examines whether MA is accomplishing its mission of empowerment by exploring the phenomenon of personal power among MA contestants in terms of the nature and strength of the self, as well as subject matter they talk about when referring to the self. The lack of research on this topic and the need to improvise quantitative measurement tools mean that this thesis cannot answer this question definitively; instead, this is a pilot exploration. The author acknowledges that the pageant does, in fact, judge physical beauty, which may 8 cause some contestants to feel objectified. However, the study suggests that some contestants read the objectification in the reverse of the first-glance meaning (what some authors call a “subversive reading” that is the opposite of the firstglance meaning), thereby turning their situation into an opportunity for personal growth. The following framework will be used to gauge MA contestant empowerment: Strong Sense of Self=>Personal Power=>Empowerment Many aspects of the pageant process are examined with special attention to questions MA contestants must answer in front of an audience during competition. The next section, the literature review, explains some of the steps taken by MAO to legitimize the pageant, the components of empowerment, and the link between objectification and “the gaze.” 9 Literature Review History of Miss America - Atlantic City resort owners began the pageant in 1921 as a promotional gimmick to extend the summer vacation season. The Inter-City Beauty Pageant, part of Atlantic City’s Fall Frolic, was MA 1921 Margaret Gorman sponsored by several national newspapers and attracted eight contestants. Thousands of onlookers watched as 15-year-old Margaret Gorman of Washington, D.C., was crowned Miss America in a swimsuit. (Timeline) Latham (1995) claims that the pageant acted as a catalyst in the legitimization of public displays of female nudity. At a time when swimwear censorship was strictly enforced, the Miss America pageant sprung up uninhibited by rules applied to beach-goers. Rules included proper skirt length and the donning of tights to cover the lower part of the leg (Latham, p. 152). “They 1921 Swimsuit Line-up, Gorman on far right. (women) may blister their backs and necks if they wish, but not the lower part of their legs,” said Long Island Police Capain, Walter Barriscale, of the restrictions (“Bars Beach Lizards”, New York Times, 27 May 1921). Women who broke the rules faced serious penalties such as arrest and jail time. However, police officers that enforced dress codes on the beach flocked to watch scantily clad contestants model the forbidden swimsuits on the stage. In 1921, the pageant became an 10 “acceptable” display of feminine “nudity” and swimsuit competition became a mainstay of American pageantry. The “display” provided by the pageant proved successful, and by 1923 more than 70 contestants converged in Atlantic City to participate (Timeline). However, the looming depression and negative media attention brought on by, “Charges of fraud, contestants being married, falsifying residences and other factors,” shut the pageant down from 1929 to 1932 (Latham, p. 108; Banner, p. 12). In 1923, “Miss” Boston came to the pageant with her husband and seven month-old baby and “Miss” Alaska, also married, was a resident of New York City (Watson & Martin, p. 107-108). Religious and women’s groups labeled the pageant “indecent” and accused the program of corrupting national morality, as one early protestor stated, “Before the competition, the contestants were splendid examples of innocence and pure womanhood. Afterward, their heads were filled with vicious ideas,” (Transcript). Lenora Slaughter was hired in 1935 to polish the pageant’s reputation. She made changes that improved and protected the pageant’s reputation. Age restrictions were put in place (women ages 18-26); and talent competition was added (Timeline). Slaughter installed the society matron chaperone system that is still in place today to guard contestants from “unscrupulous Executive Director Lenora Slaughter men.” Contestants were also required to sign contracts guaranteeing that they had not committed acts of “moral turpitude.” 11 In 1940, the pageant name was officially changed from “The Showman’s Variety Jubilee” to the “Miss America Pageant” and was moved to its current home in the Atlantic City’s Convention Hall (Watson & Martin, p. 109). Only one contestant was allowed to represent each state; Mu Alpha Sigma, the pageant sorority, was organized; and contestants were limited to a one-time opportunity to compete at the national pageant (Watson & Martin, p. 109). WWII turned Convention Hall into Army barracks, and pageant officials considered postponing the pageant until the war was over (Watson & Martin, p. 109). However, it was decided that the pageant should continue so that it could provide a “positive moment in an otherwise sad time” (Watson & Martin, p. 109). This decision foreshadowed a similar decision made by contestants of the 2001 competition, following the attacks of Sept. 11, 2001. During the war, MA became a symbol of patriotism as demonstrated by MA 1943 Jean Bartel, who turned down a movie offer to sell a record number of war bonds (Timeline). 1944 marked a turning point in MAO history as Lenora Slaughter began gathering sponsors for a pageant scholarship fund and enlisted Junior Chambers of Commerce across the country to sponsor local and state pageants. Bess Myerson, the first and only Jewish MA, was the recipient of the first MAO 1957 MA Parade scholarship in 1945. Slaughter had raised $5,000 from sponsors including Joseph Bancroft & Sons, Catalina Swimwear, F.W. Fitch Company, and the Sandy 12 Valley Grocery Company (Timeline). From that year on, MA became more than a typical American beauty pageant—it became a grassroots scholarship program. The press corps stormed out of AC in 1948, when for the first time in MAO history, the winner was crowned in an evening gown rather than a swimsuit. Movement away from focus on swimsuit competition was accelerated when MA 1951 Yolande Betbeze refused to appear in a swimsuit. Event sponsor Catalina Swimwear was alienated by Betbeze’s decision and dropped its sponsorship of the pageant. Several years later, the company started the for-profit Miss USA and Miss Universe Pageants, in which swimsuit competition accounts for 30 percent of contestant scores. (Timeline) Despite a rift over swimsuits in the late forties, the 1950s was a golden age for MA. With Philco Electronics as a sponsor, MA went live on ABC in 1954 and 27 million viewers tuned in (Timeline). By the end of the decade, Bert Parks was the official pageant emcee, representation all of 50 Pageant Emcee Bert Parks states had been achieved, the viewing audience had tripled and the scholarship fund had grown to $250,000 (Deford, p. 193; Watson & Martin, p. 110). The civil and women’s rights movements of the 1960s led to difficulties for MA. The pageant was declared racist and sexist, and by 1965, MAO Chairman Albert Marks faced “accusations that the pageant was exploitative and degrading to women,” (Watson 1968 Protest 13 & Martin, p. 111). The 1968 pageant was the target of a “zap” action orchestrated by the New York Radical Women’s group. Hundreds of angry women, traveling from as far as California, gathered in front of Convention Hall to protest the pageant, because, as protest organizer Robin Morgan put it, MA represented a “…degrading, mindless-boob-girlie symbol,” (Timeline). Rita Freedman, author of Beauty Bound, said the pageant, “…seemed to epitomize woman’s role as a passive, decorative object,” (vii). During the protest, participants threw curlers, girdles, high heels and bras into the famous “freedom trash can.” Rumors of bra burning were false because protestors were unable to secure a fire permit from the city (Watson & Martin, 111; American Experience). Following the protest, the pageant lost support from sponsor Pepsi-Cola, which said the pageant no longer represented the changing values of American society (Timeline). The company’s observations were reflected by the fact that the number of articles published in newspapers and magazines dropped to 20— fewer than in any other decade (The Readers’ Guide to Periodical Literature; Watson & Martin, p. 111). “The pageant was the antithesis of much of the American culture at that time,” (Watson & Martin, p. 111). Strong criticism from civil rights activists throughout the 1970s led to the quiet elimination of Rule #7 in the MA contract signed by contestants, which required them to be, “in good health and of the white race,” (Timeline). Cheryl Brown, Miss Iowa 1970, was the first black contestant to compete on the national level. In 1980, Miss Washington Doris Hayes and Miss Arkansas Lencola 14 Sullivan became the first black contestants to make Top 10 (Sullivan also became the first black woman to land in the Top Five as fourth runner-up). Vanessa Williams became the first black MA in 1983 and faced opposition from several groups. White supremacists sent death threats, and the Congress of Racial Equality (CORE) issued a statement that her looks were not “black” enough (Watson & Martin, p. 113). Her reign ended in shame when the networks announced that Penthouse Magazine was going print nude pictures of Williams. In July 1984, she relinquished her crown. Another breakthrough for the program occurred when Kaye Lani Rae Rafko, Miss America 1988, became the first MA to dedicate her year of service to a social issue as an advocate for the terminally ill (Timeline). Inspired by her mission, in 1989 MAO began requiring all MA 1988 Kay Lani Rae Rafko contestants on the local, state and national levels to develop a platform issue (Timeline). The requirement marked another turning point in the role of MA— she was no longer just a beautiful girl, but a woman who could use her power to make a difference. The 1990s opened the doors for many different types of contestants, from the crowning of the first disabled MA, Heather Whitestone (deaf), to the diabetic MA 1998 Nicole Johnson. Angela Perez Baraquio, MA 2001, was the first Asian American to win the title. The current MA 2003, Erika Harold, has been described as a “one-woman melting pot,” with a mixture of African American, 15 Native American, Russian, Greek, German, Welsh and English roots (Greco, 2002). Pageant Boot Camp: Legitimizing the Queen – Contestants must successfully compete and win on the local and state levels before competing at the national pageant in Atlantic City. On each level, contestants are scored in swimsuit, evening gown and talent competitions, and a 12-minute interview. They are assigned a composite score, which reflects a contestants’ overall impression on the judges. Each member on a panel of five judges gives each contestant a score ranging from one to 10, with 10 being a perfect score, in each phase of competition. Prior to competing, contestants are required to fill out fact sheets listing activities and accomplishments and write a critical issue statement, or essay, about a platform they would like to promote as a titleholder. A platform is “nothing more than a particular issue that is a significant concern to us and one that we want to dedicate much of our effort to during the 365 days we have our respective jobs,” (Baker, p. C-5). This requirement, enacted in 1989, prompted 500,000 hours of community service completed by contestants in 2002 (Greco, 2002). The paperwork provided by contestants is given to judges as a resource for generating interview questions. Judges are also permitted to ask questions about current events reported in the media. At the national pageant, contestants spend three nights competing in “preliminaries”, which consist of the competitions mentioned above. Divided into three groups – Mu, Alpha and Sigma – contestants compete in one phase of 16 competition per night. Finals are traditionally held on Saturday night, during which time contestants with the highest scores proceed onto further competition. In recent years, MAO has changed the Saturday night format from a strictly Top 10 competition in the 2000 telecast, to a Top 20 competition in the 2001 telecast, and to a Top 15 in the 2002 telecast. Regardless of how competition is narrowed down, contestants who advance are required to compete for a second round in all phases of competition and are eventually narrowed down to a Top Five. These remaining contestants are asked a series of questions similar to those asked during the interview. However, during the 2002 telecast, Top Five contestants were required to answer questions in a format similar to the popular game show, “Who wants to be a millionaire?”. For the purposes of this study, the 1960 and 2000 telecasts employed the traditional Top Five question-and-answer format. MAO has tried to legitimize the pageant by downplaying the focus on physical beauty through changes in the official names and in scoring weights given to all phases of competition, as noted in the “You be the Judge” section of the MA 2002 Competition program book (Greco, 2002). These changes were made to more accurately reflect what MA does during her year of service – speak to civic groups, schools and government assemblies such as Congress. Scoring Breakdown as of 2002 (Greco, 2002): Talent competition (30 percent of preliminary score and 20 percent of final score) is referred to as “Artistic Expression in Talent.” Talent is judged on the following criteria: Technical skill, quality of performance, stage presence, and interest/entertainment 17 value of the performance. Swimsuit competition (10 percent of preliminary and final scores) is referred to as “Lifestyle and Fitness in Swimsuit” and is judged on the following criteria: Lifestyle of good physical, emotional and intellectual health; drive, energy, charisma and expression; and sense of confidence, attractiveness and presence. Evening gown competition (10 percent of preliminary and final scores) is referred to as “Presence and Poise in Evening Wear” and is judged on the following criteria: Compelling charm, presence and personality; and beauty, sense of style, composure and allure. The interview, also known as “Community Achievement in Interview,” accounts for the majority of the preliminary score (40 percent of the score). This competition allows contestants to show their “inner beauty” and intelligence. During finals, a composite score from all events in preliminary competition is carried over and accounts for 40 percent of the score. Scores from two other competitions are also added – “Peer Respect and Leadership” (10 percent) and the “Miss America Quiz” (10 percent). * This weighting system was enacted during the 2002 competition, and does not apply to the telecasts used in this study. Scoring Breakdown, Prior to 2002 (Benedetti, 2000)): The 1960 and 2000 telecasts used a system during preliminaries that weighted talent at 40 percent, interview at 30 percent, evening gown at 15 percent, and swimsuit at 15 percent. A composite score from preliminaries was carried over, accounting for 40 percent of the final score. During finals, Swimsuit accounted for 10 percent, 18 Evening Wear accounted for 10 percent, Talent accounted for 20 percent and the On-Stage Platform Interview accounted for 20 percent. Swimsuit was judged on the following criteria: Overall first impression; Statement of physical fitness and health; Walk, posture, poise and grace; Sense of confidence and comfort; Display of energy, charisma and comfort; and Beauty of face and figure. Evening Wear was judged on the following criteria: Overall first impression; Sense of confidence; Personality and stage presence; Technique (walk, posture, carriage and grace); Appropriateness of evening wear; and Beauty and sense of attractiveness. Talent was judged on the following criteria: Representative’s skill and ability; Interpretive ability; Technical skill level (execution, technique and synchronization); Stage presence (on-stage personality); Totality of all elements (including costume, props, voice, use of body, choreography). On-Stage Platform Interview was judged on the following criteria: Overall first impression; Response to on-stage questions; Personality, beauty and a confident, commanding presence; Independence and a sense of ‘courage of her convictions’; Eloquence of communications; and Sense of commitment as a role model. Public reaction to legitimization in the scoring breakdown: All of these attempts at legitimization have not persuaded the public that physical beauty is secondary to inner beauty at the MA pageant. Viewers of the 1994 pageant were polled about whether the swimsuit competition should be continued and 79 percent of respondents voted in support of the competition, while 21 percent voted against it (Latham, p. 163). In a 2000 pageant poll, 51 percent of viewers 19 who responded reported that they would not watch the pageant if swimsuit competition was discontinued (MA telecast 2000). The results of these polls suggest that in the public mind, MA attempts to take focus away from physical beauty have not succeeded. Reasons for public sentiment that the pageant does judge beauty could be tied the pageant’s history as a bathing beauty contest. Despite public sentiment about the pageant, MA has changed with times. It is arguable that critics’ pressure changed the pageant and resulted in its legitimization and continued acceptability. However, legitimization does not change the fact that contestants are judged, in part, on physical beauty and are thus objectified. Even if the judges did not base scores partly on appearance, many audience members apparently are still dissecting women according to how they look. Contestants are acutely aware of this, as a comment from one contestant clearly demonstrates: “For many of us, this is our first national television debut, and we wanna look good” (MA telecast 2000). Empowerment – While MAO has never officially attempted to define empowerment, a “strong sense of self” and personal power have been directly linked to the process of empowerment. However, some critics claim that the term “empowerment” has a fuzzy definition, leading to its overuse and trivialization (Muller, 1994). Some writers find the value of the term “empowerment” in its lack of definition. “I like the term empowerment because no one has defined it clearly yet; so it gives us a breathing space to work it out in action terms before we have 20 to pin ourselves down to what it means” (Kabeer, p. 436). Julian Rappaport likened the term to U.S. Supreme Court Justice’s statement on obscenity, in that while we may not know exactly what it is, “we know it when we see it” (1984, p. 2; Muller, 1994). Empowerment became a buzzword in educational, psychological and political discourse of the 1980s (Muller, 1994) and has since found its way into the MAO mission statement. Because this study examines the MAO mission, a definition of empowerment and the implications of such a definition must be explored. The debate about the definition arose from “…transliteration from noun (power) to verb (empower),” (Muller, 1994). Inconsistency in use has been further compounded by the term’s use without reference to what one is empowered to do (Muller, 1994). In a case study of six female leaders, Muller defines empowerment as: “The process of acknowledging the latent potential of others and assisting them to act purposefully and assertively to reach their growth potential,” (1994). This definition implies a relationship between one who empowers (a leader) and one who is empowered (a follower). Power is treated as a unit of exchange shared between two entities for the purpose of reaching a common goal. The process of empowerment occurs on the personal, interpersonal and institutional level (Heng, 1995). Personal power involves a “sense of self” as confident, effective and capable; interpersonal power is the ability to affect others; and institutional power 21 is the ability to work with others to improve lives and change social institutions (Heng, 1995). The “power” in empowerment is essentially “the ability to move or produce change” and does not involve domination or “power over,” but rather “power to” and “power with.” “Power to” refers to personal power and “power with” involves interpersonal and institutional power. In order to have “power with,” one must first have “power to.” The idea that “power to” is a prerequisite for “power with” comes from the Heng (1995) study on the emotional subordination of women. Heng (1995) reports, “Most efforts have been directed at improving women’s visible material conditions, their economic livelihoods and participation in decision-making,” (1995). However, the study suggests that to achieve the improvement of women’s visible material conditions, their emotional subordination in the form of gender oppression must be dealt with first. “Power to” implies leadership in terms of the ability to empower, according to the Muller (1994) definition of empowerment. Cantor and Bernay, in their book, Women in Power, The Secrets of Leadership (1992), reduce the leadership involved in empowerment to three basic ingredients. Leadership involves creative MA 2001 Angela Perez Baraquio with President Bush aggression, womanpower and a competent self. Creative aggression is described as the ability to speak out and take initiative. Womanpower is the ability to make a difference for the greater good and a 22 combination of what Cantor and Bernay consider to be the best of male and female qualities: strength, force and nurturance. For this study, competent self is the most important ingredient in the Cantor-Bernay leadership equation. Competent self involves the ability to see “possibilities instead of obstacles” and a “strong sense of self,” similar to what was mentioned by Heng in his definition of personal power (Denmark, 1993). Having a competent self or a “strong sense of self” is vital to personal power and thus empowerment. “Personal power is the basis of all other kinds of empowerment…A loss of personal power is caused by experiences of domination/subordination, oppression, and exploitation,” (Heng, 1995). Issues of domination/subordination, oppression and exploitation come up in many critiques of the MA pageant. Critics argue that the pageant’s emphasis on ideal physical beauty causes MA to become a symbol and vehicle for the disempowerment of women through sexual objectification. Closely linked to objectification is “the male gaze.” The Male Gaze – “Objectification and preoccupation with isolated body parts dehumanizes women, but it also degrades men when they become so obsessed with ankles or breasts that they forget the woman within,” (Freedman, p. 69). Some feminists argue that the pageant perpetuates “the male gaze,” leading to objectification – the notion that the worth of a woman is decided upon the sum of her parts (Watson & Martin, p. 121). Objectification is most often experienced through “the gaze,” or “physical inspection of the body,” (Kaschak, 1992; Fredrickson & Roberts, p. 175). This 23 phenomenon may be directly linked to heterosexual attraction via the Beauty Myth and to the reinforcement of patriarchal dominance. The Beauty Myth is Darwinian in nature, replacing “survival of the fittest” with “survival of the prettiest.” In this construction, all women want to be beautiful, and all men want beautiful women, so it follows that beautiful women are more reproductively successful. This leads to the conclusion that humankind is evolving towards one standard of beauty through sexual selection. Wolf challenged the myth with her contention that beauty “…is not based on evolution, sex, gender, aesthetics or God. It is about men’s institutions and institutional power,” (13). Because Wolf believes that men impose beauty standards, she contends the pageant—which appears to be a celebration of beauty—reinforces the patriarchal suppression of women. Until the 1980s, the MA pageant did appear to support this position, based on body measurement studies done on MA winners. Deford reports that the average contestant of 1971 was 19 years old, stood MA Contestants “weighing in” five feet and six inches tall, weighed 119 pounds and almost always came from the South or the Midwest (Watson & Martin, p.117). The MA pageant demonstrates the gaze in three distinct ways: through depictions in interpersonal encounters (contestant interaction with the emcee), through depictions of objectification (judges watching contestants) and through 24 the creation of the gaze (camera focus on each contestant) on the part of the viewing audience. By requiring contestants to model swimsuits, the pageant is reducing them to objects that can be broken down into breasts, hips, waists and thighs. Critics charge that such objectification leads to eating disorders, depression and sexual dysfunction among women. Sexualized objectification represents a form of gender oppression (Heng, 1995). Gender Oppression – The media are saturated with images of sexualized objectification from MTV’s spring break footage to advertisements touting shampoo. This phenomenon represents a form of gender oppression touched on in the Heng (1995) study. Gender oppression “enables other forms of oppression, such as unemployment, discrimination, sexual violence and trivialization of women’s work and accomplishments,” (Fredrickson & Roberts, p. 174). While not all women respond to objectification the same way, many women who become oppressed by objectification face serious complications that lead to psychological disorders previously mentioned. Those who are oppressed tend to take a viewer’s perspective of the self, leading to what in known as selfobjectification (Fredrickson & Roberts, p. 177). Effective Socialization describes the process of internalization that results in self-objectification (Costanzo, 1992; Fredrickson & Roberts, p. 177). Effective socialization starts with external pressure, such as that provided by media depictions of the benefits of being beautiful. Over time, exposures Preliminary winners from the 1980s 25 to such depictions cause women to equate beauty with power, popularity and success. Under these circumstances, attempting to improve one’s appearance becomes a matter of strategy that will determine how one will be treated. In attending to appearance, women begin inspecting themselves and soon think that the compulsion to improve appearance is natural and in some cases, freely chosen. Because women have no control over their objectification, they learn to be “their own first surveyors” and by doing so, internalize a viewer’s perspective (Fredrickson & Roberts, p. 178). Self-objectification leads women to believe they are sights to be appreciated by others (Fredrickson & Roberts, p. 180). The pressure to meet other’s expectations can lead to chronic self-monitoring and self-consciousness that may deteriorate the sense of self, and cause a special set of psychological circumstances (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). These circumstances include feelings of shame, anxiety, lack of flow and lack of attention to internal bodily states. Feelings of shame are the result of “negative self-evaluations” brought on by self-monitoring (Fredrickson & Roberts, p. 181). The potential for the exposure of shortcomings causes many who feel shame to “shrink away” and become timid as a defense mechanism. However, a “shameful” appearance is difficult to hide or change. Therefore, women who experience shame about appearance because of constant self-monitoring find it difficult to overcome. Anxiety is characterized by ambiguous feelings of danger and threat (Fredrickson & Roberts, p. 182). Women who are objectified can feel anxiety due 26 to appearance. Lack of control over when or where one might be sexually objectified through the gaze, contributes to appearance anxiety. Appearance anxiety is compounded by the fashion industry, because certain clothing styles require constant adjustment to avoid “shamefully” showing too much skin. Women are forced to adjust themselves to avoid shame, while still appearing at ease. Lack of a desirable state called “flow” is caused by objectification. Lack of flow creates a lower quality of life for many women. Flow is described as a situation in which, “a person’s body or mind is stretched to the limits in a voluntary effort to accomplish something difficult and worthwhile,” (Csikszentmihalyi 1990, p. 3). A complete loss of self-consciousness is necessary to achieve maximum flow. Objectification interferes with flow under two circumstances: when attention is called to a woman’s appearance or bodily functions during an activity, or when self-objectification causes a woman to become self-aware during an activity (Fredrickson & Roberts, p. 183). Because flow is a “Prime source of optimal experience,” and signifies “those rare moments when we are truly living, uncontrolled by others, creative and joyful,” it follows that flow increases quality of life through higher productivity and greater enjoyment (Fredrickson & Roberts, p. 183). Objectification is linked to lack of attention to internal bodily states in terms of hunger, pain and arousal. The shame and anxiety that result from selfmonitoring increase the likelihood of dieting, and a woman learns to ignore her hunger. This self-imposed numbness can carry over into other sensations. In 27 addition, energy devoted to self-monitoring is taken away from energy that should be used to identify and attend to internal bodily states. (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997) These emotional circumstances constitute the deterioration of a strong sense of self and personal power necessary in the empowerment process. Women who experience these emotions and circumstances are disempowered by the objectification they face, often in the form of the sexualized gaze. The Self and TST – The self is described as an, “individual’s attitudes (plans of action) toward his/her own mind and body viewed as an object,” (Hickman & Kuhn, 1956, p. 46). Kuhn was a symbolic interactionist who believed that social interactions shape the self through internalized social reflection where “identities, psychological qualities and social relationships form the basis for self-understanding and behavior,” (Grace & Cramer 2002). Measurement of the self among college students has been conducted since 1957 using the TST, or Twenty Statement Test, which was validated in 1964. “The TST reveals the frequency of positive self-evaluations (positive moods, selfesteem and sense of well-being),” (Driver, p. 393). The test requires respondents to list 20 answers to the question, “Who am I?” Answers are then coded according to a four-referential coding scheme (Kuhn & McPartland, 1954); including Type A, or Physical responses (I am blonde.), Type B, or Social responses (I am a Tri-Delta.), Type C, or Reflective responses (I enjoy going to football games.) and Type D, or Oceanic responses (We are part of one sisterhood). 28 The four modes of self are best understood by placing them on a continuum that slides from a concrete sense of self (Type A, or Physical) to an abstract sense of self (Type D, or Oceanic). Studies have shown that sense of self changes from the concrete to the abstract with increased age, and that this change represents a higher level of development in the self-concept (Brinthraupt & Lipka, 1985; Montemayor & Eisen, 1977). Results of TST studies of college students from 1957 and 2001 are important for the purposes of this study because of their close proximity to the telecast years under study and respondents closeness in age to the contestants under study. Hartley (1968) reported findings from a study of University of Iowa students in 1957. The sample included 1,653 students, of which 2 percent indicated Type A responses, 51 percent indicated Type B responses, 31 percent indicated Type C responses and 16 percent indicated Type D responses. Grace & Cramer (2001) reported findings from a sample of 324 Southern Ontario Canadian university students. They predicted a predominance of Type C responses and an increase in Type D responses compared to past research, because of social changes and the proliferation of information technology. Their hypothesis was correct, with Type C responses accounting for 91.3 percent of all responses, showing that, “During the turn of the century, the the nature of self among students continues to be focused on ways of behaving, moods, feelings, preferences and dislikes,” (Grace & Cramer, p. 277). There were no Type A responses, 8.4 percent of Type B responses and .3 percent of Type D responses. 29 Methodology Research Questions – The primary goal of this research is to explore whether the MA pageant competition might empower women. By critics’ definition of the term objectification – the judgment of women on the basis of physical beauty – MA contestants are objectified. However, literature also suggests that not all women react in the same way to being objectified. Are some contestants empowered, despite overt objectification they face during competition? Because the “sense of self” is the root of empowerment according to Heng (1995), research questions focus on the “contestant self” from 1960 and 2000. The “contestant self” is most clearly demonstrated during Top 5 competition, and will be examined in a three-part study designed to answer questions in the following areas: I. Nature of the Self – What is the nature of the self among contestants in the 1960 and 2000 pageants? How does the nature of the contestant self compare to the nature of self as reported among student contemporaries during the same time periods? Did the nature of contestant self change from 1960 to 2000? II. Subject Matter – What subjects do contestants talk about when referring to themselves during Top 5 competition? Has that subject matter changed from 1960 to 2000? III. Strength of the Self – Do contenders maintain a Strong Sense of Self during Top 5 competition? Has the strength of self during Top 5 competition changed from 1960 to 2000? 30 Few studies have been conducted on the MA pageant, and of those, none are quantitative. Because there is no prior research to draw methods from, this study has not been tested for validity and some of the comparisons made may appear to be problematic. However, absent any other data, this study has attempted to find instruments of measurement and comparison data that best fit the task at hand – the exploration of empowerment among MA contestants through the examination of the self, which is most clearly demonstrated during the Top 5 competition. Measuring the contestant self – This study is a three-part analysis of contestant answers to the Top 5 questions during the MA telecasts from 1960 and 2000. The first part uses the four-modal scheme of the TST to quantify the nature of the self among pageant contestants. The second part of the study is a qualitative analysis that explores subject matter themes in contestant answers. The third part of the study is a quantitative analysis of the strength or weakness of the self as demonstrated by contestant answers. Contestant answers were transcribed from video onto note cards and broken down into individual statements, with records kept referencing the year each statement was made. A contestant may have made several statements to prove a point during the course of an answer. For example, in explaining her dedication to raising awareness of heart disease, a contestant could make several statements, including, “Heart disease is the nation’s No. 1 killer,” “It doesn’t discriminate on the basis of race, sex or age,” “and it certainly hasn’t left my 31 family out,” “yet, there are things we can do everyday to prevent it,” “that’s the message I want to convey.” Once answers were broken down into statements, “I-statements” were separated. I-statements included any instance in which a contestant spoke of her own opinions, attitudes, behaviors and feelings. These statements were operationalized by reference to oneself or one’s group, through the use of terms such as “I,” “me,” “we,” “our,” and “us.” The I-statement constituted the unit of analysis used in Parts One, Two and Three of this study. I. Nature of the Self – Two coders coded I-statements according to the four-modal scheme developed by Kuhn (1954). The scheme included Physical, Social, Reflective and Oceanic responses. Physical responses included reference to the self in terms of physical being or body. Social responses included reference to the self in terms of a social context or group. Reflective responses included references to the self in terms of feelings, moods, opinions, attitudes, preferences, dislikes, and behavior. Oceanic responses transcended social context and suggested the self’s interconnectedness with the world in general, rather than with one specific group. The number of responses in each category was divided by the total number of I-statements to provide a percentage of the total, as suggested by Grace & Cramer (2001). Results for the 1960 telecast were compared with results from the 1957 TST study. Results for the 2000 telecast were compared with results from the 2001 TST study. Results for both telecasts were compared to determine if the nature of contestant self changed between the 1960 and 2000 pageants. 32 II. Subject Matter – I-statements were analyzed on the basis of subject material. Two key words were picked out of each statement and put on a separate list. Identification of two key words increased inter-rater agreement. Key words were those that carried the weight of a statement’s meaning; without them, the statement would not have made sense. The statement, “I have faith in my family,” would not make sense without the words “faith” and “family.” Upon completion of the list, words were grouped into broader thematic categories. Each statement was placed in a category based on the word that was used to code it. III. Strength of the Self – I-statements were compiled into two broad categories, Strong Sense of Self and Weak Sense of Self, based on the following five criteria: Mood, Self-Esteem, Sense of Wellbeing, Self-consciousness and Attitude toward Others. Each criterion was rated as positive, neutral or negative. A predominance of positive ratings placed an I-statement in the Strong Sense of Self category. A predominance of negative ratings placed an I-statement in the Weak Sense of Self category. The Strong Sense of Self category was defined as a “greater sense of potency or strength” and was indicated by statements of positive mood, high selfesteem, positive sense of well-being, lack of preoccupation with the self and benevolence toward others (Rushton, 1980). The Weak Sense of Self category was defined as a “lesser sense of potency or strength” and was indicated by statements of negative mood, low self-esteem, negative sense of well-being, preoccupation with the self and hostility toward others (Rushton, 1980). 33 The years of each statement were referenced and number of statements that indicated a Strong or Weak Sense of Self for each year was tallied. A year with more units indicating a Strong Sense of Self was considered moving toward empowerment. A year with more units indicating a Weak Sense of Self was considered moving away from empowerment. 34 Results and Analysis A total of 68 I-statements were used in this study; 29.41 percent (20) were from the 1960 telecast and 70.58 percent (48) were from the 2000 telecast. Inter-rater reliability rates were 91.17 percent, 97 percent and 94.1 percent for Parts One, Two and Three, respectively. These rates were determined by dividing the number of I-statements agreed upon by both coders by the total number of I-statements. Results from all three studies are recorded in Table 4 (Appendix). I. Nature of the Self – What is the nature of the self among contestants in the 1960 and 2000 pageants? How does the nature of the contestant self compare to the nature of self as reported among student contemporaries during the same time periods? Did the nature of contestant self change from 1960 to 2000? This set of questions was answered using the Kuhn (1954) four-modal scheme of the self. This system allowed contestant I-statements to be coded for showing the following self-concepts: Physical, Social, Reflective and Oceanic. Two new categories, Neutral and NA (no agreement), were added to the study because coders were unable to place some I-statements into the four-modal TST coding scheme. I-Statements coded as Neutral did not fall into any category of the fourmodal scheme. I-Statements coded as NA indicate that coders did not agree. Istatements coded as Neutral or NA were excluded from analysis in Part One: Nature of Self, because these categories were not used in the 1957 or 2001 TST. There were no I-statements coded as Neutral or NA for the 1960 telecast. There 35 were four I-statements coded as Neutral and six I-statements coded as NA for the 2000 telecast, leaving 38 I-statements for analysis. The results for each telecast are recorded in Table 1. Table 1 Comparison of MA I-statements and TST answers Part 1 1960 Physical Social Reflective Oceanic Neutral NA Part 1 2000 Physical Social Reflective Oceanic Neutral NA MA Number 0 1 19 0 0 0 N = 20 MA Number 0 3 34 1 4 6 N = 48 College MA TST Percentage Percentage 1957 Study Difference 0% 2% -5% 51% -90.19% 95% 31% 206% 0% 16% -------N = 20 N = 1,653 College MA TST Percentage Percentage 2001 Study Difference 0% 0% 0% 7.89% 8.40% -6.07% 89.47% 91.30% -2% 2.63% 0.30% 776.67% ------N = 38 N = 324 I-statements from the 1960 telecast indicate that the nature of the contestant self was 90.19 percent less Social and 206 percent more Reflective than their contemporaries from the 1957 study. There were minimal or no indications of Physical and Oceanic views of contestants’ nature of self. I-statements from the 2000 MA pageant and the TST from 2001 are more varied in the nature of self represented. While there were no Physical responses for either contestants or students, there were 6.07 percent fewer Social, 2 percent fewer Reflective and 776.67 percent more Oceanic responses among MA 36 contestants compared to TST contemporaries from 2001. While the percentage increase in Oceanic responses is high, it must be taken into account that this percentage only represents one occurrence, which may render this finding insignificant. Inter-pageant rate of change for the nature of self shows that from 1960 to 2000, contestants were 25 percent more likely to identify the nature of the self in terms of social interactions and 25.44 percent less likely to identify the nature of the self in terms of reflection. Contestant I-statements from the 2000 telecast were also less likely to be placed in the traditional four-modal coding scheme of the TST; and 8.33 percent and 12.50 percent of I-statements were placed in the Neutral and NA categories, respectively. While no 1960 I-statements were found to be Oceanic, one Oceanic statement was found in the 2000 telecast. What is the nature of the self among contestants in the 1960 and 2000 pageants? The results from Part One showed that contestants from 1960 and 2000 had a highly Reflective sense of self, with 95 percent and 89.47 percent (percentages exclude I-statements coded as Neutral or NA) of responses, respectively. How does the nature of the 1960 contestant’s self compare to the nature of self reported among college students in 1957? Contestants from the 1960s had an overall more abstract sense of self than their contemporaries. This finding suggests that contestants may have been less likely to view themselves as objects, as shown by the fact that none of their I-statements were coded as Physical, while 2 percent of their contemporaries did give Physical responses to the TST. 37 Because evidence suggests 1960 contestants did not view themselves as objects (self-objectification), they were more likely to have a stronger sense of self, leading to greater personal power and ultimately empowerment. However, an abstract sense of self for contestants in the 1960s stopped at likes, dislikes, moods, behaviors, attitudes and beliefs. There were no Oceanic Istatements, which would have suggested contestants’ ability to connect themselves with the world around them. This finding suggests that while contestants of the 1960s had a more highly developed sense of self in terms of abstraction, that abstraction remained on a lower level. Their contemporaries may have been less likely to use abstractions when describing the self, but when they did, they tended to do so at a higher level with 16 percent of their responses being Oceanic. This finding may be caused by stunted development of the 1960 contestants’ self, or by the fact that only 5 percent of I-statements were Social. Without social interaction, it is difficult to connect oneself with the world at large and espouse a transcendental view of the self, as would be demonstrated by an Oceanic response. How does the nature of the 2000 contestant’s self compare to the nature of self reported among college students in 2001? I-statements of contestants from 2000 more closely resembled TST responses of their contemporaries. Because 2000 I-statements were more complicated, inter-rater agreement was lower and the Neutral category was used more frequently. I-statements lacking inter-rater 38 agreement (NA) or those coded as Neutral were excluded from percentage calculations. Both groups demonstrated a highly developed sense of self through abstract responses represented by the Reflective and Oceanic categories. MA Contestants were slightly less Social and Reflective than their contemporaries. This slight difference, approximately 2 percentage points, could be caused by a higher frequency of Oceanic responses by contestants. Oceanic responses accounted for 2.63 percent of MA contestant responses (N = 38) and only .03 percent of their contemporary’s responses (N = 324), adding up to a 776.67 percent increase in Oceanic responses among contestants compared to their contemporaries. This may appear to be a significant increase, but it represents only one Oceanic response for 2000 MA contestants. Based on percentages, it can be concluded that both 2000 contestants and their contemporaries have a more abstract sense of self, but MA contestants demonstrate that abstraction at a higher level more frequently. Did the nature of the contestant self change from 1960 to 2000? From 1960 to 2000, I-statements became dramatically more Social (57.8 percent increase) and slightly less Reflective (5.82 percent decrease). This indicates that the contestant self has regressed by becoming more concrete over time. However, there were no Physical responses from either telecast, which provides evidence suggesting contestants do not view themselves as objects (self-objectification). Based on this evidence, it can be assumed contestants from both years are able to maintain a strong sense of self, personal power and empowerment. 39 However, the apparent regression towards more concrete conceptualizations of the self from 1960 to 2000 must be explained. The platform requirement has made a significant difference in how contestants approach the interview and on-stage questions, not to mention their perceptions of a titleholder’s role. There was no platform requirement in the 1960s and contestants were not compelled to participate in community service activities. Today’s contestants participate in community service to build their platforms and to use their position as a titleholder to make a difference in their communities. Because the platform and community service activities are central to the interview, 2000 contestants may have been more apt to share their affiliations in their answers to on-stage questions, leading to more I-statements within the Social category. This phenomenon may be a natural outgrowth of community service and the platform requirement or an attempt to impress the judges. Experience with community service and other social interactions may have led to the emergence of Oceanic responses among 2000 contestants. Oceanic responses signal a sense of self that is in harmony with its environment; they provide evidence that the respondent is able to look above one-on-one social interactions by making transcendental statements about the self. Community service may provide the experiences necessary to develop the self at this level. II. Subject Matter – What subjects do contestants talk about when referring to themselves during Top 5 competition? Has that subject matter changed from 1960 to 2000? 40 Based on key word identification, I-statements were placed in eight thematic categories: Situational, Professional, Dealing with Others, Action, As a Contestant, Learning, Appearance, Relationships. The results of Part Two were largely influenced by the Top 5 question format, which changed between the 1960 and 2000 telecasts. The 1960 telecast used a straight question-and-answer session in which each contestant was asked two questions – one of a “frivolous nature” and another of a “more serious nature,” as described by Bert Parks. The 2000 telecast used a combination format involving a roundtable discussion with all contestants and a question-and-answer session with each contestant individually. Results are recorded in Table 2. Table 2 Comparison of MA 1960 and 2000 Key Word Responses Part 2 Situational Professional Dealing with others Action As a Contestant Learning Appearance Relationships Total Total Number Percentage 8 11.76% 4 5.88% 5 7.35% 7 10.29% 5 7.35% 9 13.23% 15 22.05% 15 22.05% N = 68 1960 Number 5 0 0 3 0 0 0 12 N = 20 1960 Percentage 25% 0% 0% 15% 0% 0% 0% 60% 2000 Number 3 4 5 4 5 9 15 3 2000 Percentage 6.25% 8.33% 10.42% 8.33% 10.42% 18.75% 31.25% 6.25% N = 48 Statements from 1960 show little variety in subject matter, with responses in three categories: Situational 25 percent, Action 15 percent and Relationships 60 percent. Statements from 2000 show variety by having responses in all eight categories, with a concentration in Appearance 31.25 percent and Learning 18.75 percent. Rate of change in responses from 1960 to 2000 could be calculated for Difference -75% NA NA 33% NA NA NA -90% 41 the three categories with responses from both years: Situational, Action and Relationships. From 1960 to 2000, Situational showed a 75 percent decrease, Action showed a 33 percent increase and Relationships showed a 90 percent decrease. What subjects do contestants talk about when referring to themselves? The roundtable discussion from the 2000 telecast played a major role in providing I-statements dealing with appearance, which accounted for 22.05 percent of all 68 statements used in this part of the study and 31.25 percent of I-statements from the 2000 telecast. Actress Marie Osmond was the moderator, and she began the discussion with the following question: “Is there a competition look? What is it and how do you answer critics who say it’s outdated?” All five contestants commented on appearance, which explains why the percentage was so high in 2000. Contestants unanimously agreed that there was no “pageant look,” but that contestants did want to look their personal best while representing their states and families on national television. They reported using make-up “to enhance features that make them look their best” and laughed at their early pageant experiences when contestants let hairstylists create “big pageant hair” (2000 MA telecast). The winner, Angela Perez Baraquio, summed up the conversation by noting changes that occurred within herself through pageant competition: “Now, I want to be me, not someone else’s interpretation of me,” (2000 MA telecast). Relationships dominated the question-and-answer session of the 1960 telecast, accounting for 60 percent of all I-statements from that telecast and 22.05 42 percent all I-statements combined. I-statements in this subject area dealt with family, dating and marriage. At the root of Relationships as a subject area was the controversy over “woman’s place” and “proper role” in society. This subject was appropriate for the time period, because traditional gender roles were just beginning to be challenged and the full force of the women’s movement was less than a decade away. Yet, contestants tended to subscribe to patriarchal dominance by espousing that “a young woman’s place is at home with her husband and children” and that an ideal wife “…depends entirely on the viewpoint of the husband,” (1960 MA telecast). One contestant said she would not vote for a female presidential candidate because “women are emotional and unreliable,” rendering them unable to make important decisions. These conservative views were not only consistent with what was acceptable for women at the time, but also with the traditional reputation of the MA pageant itself. The 2000 telecast saw a 90 percent decrease in instances of Istatements dealing with relationships. Furthermore, most of those statements dealt with professional, rather than personal, relationships. This change reflects the shift of the women’s movement from the private to the public sphere. Did the subject matter change from 1960 to 2000? Many of the questions from the 1960 telecast were Situational in nature (15 percent) and attempted set up a scene within which a contestant would react, such as: “You are walking down the runway during swimsuit competition and your high heel breaks off. What would you do?” (1960 MA telecast). By the year 2000, Situational questions had decreased by 75 percent and were replaced with questions about 43 real-life experiences contestants reported on their fact sheets. Many of these questions elicited responses about life lessons that contestants had learned (Learning, 18.75 percent), how they dealt with others (Dealing with Others, 10.42 percent), their experiences or aspirations in the professional world (Professional, 8.33 percent) and their experiences as a pageant contestant (As a Contestant, 10.42 percent). The shift from Situational questions to real-life questions suggests that the pageant has recognized a change in its contestants and a change in American expectations of women. The fact that questions are no longer drawn from fabricated situations may indicate that contestants have experience and interests outside of the domestic realm, and that they are no longer the passive, inexperienced debutantes of the 1960s. There was a 30 percent increase from 1960 to 2000 in I-statements dealing with Actions contestants have taken or performed. This finding is consistent with Part One of this study because these statements tended to also be categorized as Social from the TST four-modal coding scheme. This may be an indication that contestants have become more empowered over time because the ability to “take action” is an important part of personal power. III. Strength of the Self – Do contenders maintain a Strong Sense of Self during Top 5 competition? Has the strength of self during Top 5 competition changed from 1960 to 2000? This set of questions was answered by Part Three of the study, which used positive, negative or neutral evaluations of five indicators to determine if 44 contestant I-statements demonstrated a Strong or Weak Sense of Self. The five indicators were: Mood, Self-esteem, Sense of Well-being, Preoccupation with the Self (or lack thereof), and Attitude toward Others. Similar to Part One, two categories were added to account for statements that did not fit into the Strong/Weak Sense of Self scheme (Neutral) or statements that did not have inter-rater agreement (NA = no agreement). Results are recorded in Table 3. Table 3 Comparison of Strength/Weakness of Self from MA 1960 to 2000 Part 3 Strong Weak Neutral NA 1960 Number 8 9 2 1 N = 20 1960 Percentage 40% 45% 10% 5% 2000 Number 40 4 1 3 2000 Percentage 83.33% 8.33% 2.08% 6.25% Difference 108.32% -81.48% -79.20% 25% N = 48 There was a dramatic shift in contestants’ sense of self between the 1960 and 2000 telecasts. Strong Sense of Self increased by 108.32 percent and Weak Sense of Self decreased by 81.48 percent, Neutral statements decreased by 79.20 percent and NA statements increased by 25 percent. Do MA contenders maintain a Strong Sense of Self? Contestants from 1960 tended to maintain a Weak Sense of Self with 45 percent of responses being Weak and 40 percent of responses being Strong. Contestants from 2000 maintained Strong Sense of Self with 83.33 percent of responses being Strong and 8.33 percent of responses being Weak. 45 Has the strength of the contestant self changed from 1960 to 2000? Part Three may be the most convincing evidence that between the years 1960 and 2000, MA contestants have become more empowered or that the MA pageant emphasizes empowerment of women over physical beauty, which leads to objectification. The empowerment gap widened as instances of I-statements representing a Strong Sense of Self increased (+108.32 percent) while instances of I-statements representing a Weak Sense of Self decreased (-81.48 percent). Neutral I-statements also decreased (-79.20 percent), suggesting that over time, contestants became less ambiguous about how they felt about themselves. However, I-statements about which coders could not agree (NA) increased by 25 percent. This indicates that while there was less inter-rater agreement about the 2000 telecast, there was also less “gray area” in the form of Neutral statements when there was agreement. While Part Three appears to be quite telling, it must be taken into account that I-statements from the 1960 telecast were judged by today’s standards of self and empowerment. As stated earlier, contestant answers from that telecast were indicative of the time period. The fact that the pageant gave contestants a platform to voice their opinions as women is significant. On the other hand, the 1960 contestant responses clearly reinforced patriarchal authority that many feminists have fought hard to overcome. Also, there was evidence that some answers were rehearsed, indicating that the opinions they expressed may not have been their own, but those of their coaches. This evidence includes the lack of natural eye expression and the repetition or 46 stumbling over words, as if memorized, during the Top 5 question-and-answer session. 47 Conclusion This study is a starting point in the exploration of contestant empowerment in the MA pageant. Because this is the first study of its kind, there is significant room for improvement in methodology and for the expansion of research into the impact the pageant has on women in general. The next step in research should move beyond the reporting of the status of self among contestants, as this study does, to the examination of how exactly the contestant self is affected by the pageant process. In the process of dissecting and quantifying contestant answers, some of the depth of meaning behind each I-statement may have been lost. A qualitative study would permit a fuller understanding of the context of I-statements in contestant answers. The four-modal coding scheme from the TST for Part One of the analysis is not a perfect fit for this study, although it is the best instrument that the author could locate. The TST and MAO contestants’ answers were not designed to be compared. However the author chose the TST to compare with the MAO contestants’ answers because they touch on similar, if not exactly the same, dimensions of one of the main variables in the study, how women feel about themselves. The contestant self has larger percentages at higher levels (Reflective and Oceanic) of development than the self of contestant contemporaries in both 1960 and 2000. Higher levels of self development show that contestants have a more abstract view of themselves, leaving them less likely to partake in self- 48 objectification that leads to disempowerment. Ironically, this phenomenon occurs in the face of overt objectification during pageant competition. The MA pageant telecast sets up the male gaze to look at women’s bodies during stage competitions. However, contestants who reach the highest levels of competition are able to maintain a strong sense of self that is vital to empowerment. That strong sense of self allows them to achieve optimal flow that is necessary during pageant competition. This clearly demonstrates Fredrickson & Robert’s observation that, in spite of overt objectification, not all women respond to objectification in similar ways. The pageant competition experience may cause many pageant contestants to become immune to the hazards of objectification. Continuous exposure to objectification could build up “self-armor” that allows contestants to remain uninfluenced by their situation. By looking at the criteria they are judged on, such as “confidence,” “presence,” and “comfort,” we can see that the competition requires contestants to lose self-consciousness and move to a state of “flow” mentioned earlier in the study. Through repeated experience under these circumstances, contestants learn to ignore judgments being made on them by others (this is often done through objectification) and they develop an internal locus of control. Contestants with an internal locus of control may be less likely to worry about uncontrollable circumstances, allowing them to be more at ease in the objectifying pageant environment. Contestants, who try to put themselves in the best possible position to win, spend a great deal of time, energy and in some cases, money, in their pageant 49 endeavors. They learn to avoid comparing themselves to others and focus on what is best for them in each area of competition. Knowledge about what is “best for me” requires self-exploration, which explains the high levels of Reflective and Oceanic responses among 1960 and 2000 contestants, respectively. The most recent competition added to the MA telecast, “Peer Respect and Leadership,” provides evidence that empowerment is an important component of the program. The Top 5 contestants are judged by their peers, as to who best represents the ideals of MA and inspires others to display those same ideals. The ability to inspire others requires community involvement and the ability to reach out to others, two qualities that are often mentioned in definitions of empowerment (“power with” or interpersonal and institutional power). Contestants tend to learn this through platform development. Platform development may explain the shift from Situational questions in the 1960s to questions about “real experiences” contestants have had, in 2000. The program not only expects contestants to have real world experiences, but also recognizes those experiences during on-stage questioning. Having “real experiences” may directly relate to change in the types of relationships contestants talk about when referring to themselves. No longer do they only associate themselves with the family and romantic relationships. Today, they are more likely to speak of professional relationships that may have developed as a result of platform development, as many contestants aspire to build a career around their platforms. 50 By today’s standards, 2000 contestants have a much stronger sense of self than 1960 contestants. Some of this has to do with pageant training, but even more of it has to do with the changes in the roles women are expected to play in society. Women have moved out of the domestic sphere and into public life, where they are independent, professional and educated. Consequently, many women with the highest aspirations gravitate toward pageant competition. Whether paying for a college education, starting a career or something as simple as improving self-confidence, becoming MA is more than a crown to contestants, it is a means to an end. Many critics are leery of the scholarly image MAO is trying to build, because there are ways to award scholarship to young women without objectifying them on-stage. However, the decision made by MAO to continue “the display” through the pageant telecast, is a business strategy. The long-term success of the MA scholarship program is dependent on the annual telecast for ratings to attract sponsors who support the scholarship fund. If the pageant and “the display” were separated, one could not exist without the other, and the positive effects of MAO would vanish. The positive effects include scholarship, dedication to community service and activist leadership on the part of contestants, plus a self-reported feeling among contestants of a “strong sense of self” and personal power. No matter how many scholarships are given away or how the telecast format is changed or what new rules are added to contestant contracts, critics may always be leery of the pageant and MAO may never rid itself of the bathing 51 beauty image. It may better serve the organization to get in touch with its own sense of self, as it requires of contestants. By embracing a colorful history rich in tradition and acknowledging its ability to improve the lives of contestants through scholarship, personal growth, development and empowerment, MAO may find itself on a new level…one on which critics respect rather than rebuke. Then, lessons about the self and personal power learned by MAO and contestants could be used and applied to women in general, who face objectification in more subtle ways on a daily basis, but suffer more greatly as a result. 52 About the Author Brook Matthews, Miss Douglas County 2003 for the state of Nebraska, is a senior at the University of Arkansas. She is an honors student in the JournalismAdvertising/Public Relations sequence. Matthews has conducted this study to resolve issues that have arisen as she competed in MAO contests and to help her understand the often-negative reaction of her professors and peers when they learn she competes. It is her wish that the study might benefit those with partial or erroneous information on what the MAO system is. Also, MAO could benefit from examining how it is perceived and why. Most importantly, she hopes the study will enlighten and empower MA contestants, whose responsibility as titleholders include the ability to defend MAO knowledgably and objectively in the face of criticism. Matthews competed in the Miss Arkansas Pageant in June of 2002 and won the New Comer of the Year Award. She began competing in pageants five years ago and has since won 2nd runner-up at the national Miss United States Teen Pageant, was crowned the University of Arkansas Pledge Queen in 1998 and has funded half of her college education through pageant scholarships. She graduated from high school in Blair, Neb., and has held several titles in Nebraska, including Miss Omaha 2001 and Miss Douglas County 2000 and 2003. She placed top five at the Miss Nebraska Pageant in 2000 and 2001. By competing in Miss America program, Matthews says she has been able to become an advocate for raising heart disease awareness. Matthews is preparing for her next state pageant, Miss Nebraska, which will take place on June 4-7, 2003 in North Platte. 53 Appendix 54 Table 4 Results Tally for Parts One, Two and Three Unit 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Year 1960 1960 2000 2000 1960 2000 1960 Nature Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Sense Weak Weak Weak Weak Weak NA Strong Key Word Relationship Relationship Relationship Situational Situational Appearance Relationship Reflective Category Mother Family Others Students Choice Make-up Disapproval Compete, Pageants 8 2000 Strong 2000 2000 1960 2000 2000 2000 2000 2000 1960 2000 Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Social Reflective Reflective Reflective Identify, Kids Hope, Leave Enjoyed, Living Look good Make-up NA 4-H Interpretation Expect Glad Strong Strong Strong Strong Strong Strong Strong Strong Strong Strong 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 2000 1960 2000 2000 2000 2000 2000 1960 Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Social Strong Neutral Strong Strong Strong Strong Strong Strong 27 28 29 2000 1960 2000 Reflective Reflective NA Teach Order, Milk Comfortable Pageant, Look Look Remember Life From, Carolina Competed, Scholarship Faith, Family Looking, Good As a contestant Dealing with others Professional Actions Appearance Appearance Appearance Actions Learning Situational Relationship Dealing with others Actions Appearance Appearance Appearance Relationship Learning Actions 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 30 31 32 2000 2000 2000 NA NA NA Respect, Students Represent, Family Will, Pediatrician Strong Strong Strong 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 2000 2000 2000 2000 2000 2000 1960 Reflective Reflective Neutral Neutral Neutral Neutral Reflective Tell Beauty Pride Pride Goal Experience Faith, Family Strong Strong Strong Strong Strong Strong Strong Strong Strong Strong As a contestant Relationship Appearance Dealing with others As a contestant Professional Dealing with others Appearance As a contestant Appearance Learning Learning Relationship Agreement Part 1 91.17% Part 2 97% Part 3 94.10% 55 40 2000 Reflective Draw, Line Strong 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 2000 1960 2000 2000 2000 2000 1960 2000 1960 2000 2000 1960 1960 2000 1960 1960 2000 2000 2000 2000 2000 1960 2000 2000 2000 1960 1960 Reflective Reflective NA NA Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Social Reflective Reflective Reflective Oceanic Reflective Social Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Reflective Strong Strong Strong Strong Strong Strong Weak NA Weak Strong NA Weak NA Strong Strong Weak Strong Strong Weak Strong Strong Neutral Neutral Strong Strong Weak Weak 68 2000 Reflective Treat, Equally Care, Women Teacher Unique, Individuals Will Make-up, Learning Faith, Love Goal, Purpose Trust Fun, Gloss Respect Woman Wife Kids, Worked Broke Female Believe Look, Different NA Grew, Farm Decisions React Awkward Profession Learned, Value Family, Smarter Overlook Competing, Pageants Actions Dealing with others Relationship Professional Appearance Actions Learning Relationship Learning Relationship Appearance Appearance Relationship Relationship Professional Situational Situational Appearance Appearance Situational Actions Learning Situational Situational Learning Learning Relationship Relationship Weak As a contestant 56 Coding Instructions READ EACH STATEMENT CAREFULLY AND REMAIN AS OBJECTIVE AS POSSIBLE! Part One: 1. Determine the appropriate “sense of self” each statement represents. “Sense of self”: “individual’s attitudes (plans of action) toward his/her own mind and body viewed as an object” (Hickman & Kuhn, 1956, pg. 46). 3. There are four categories of “self”: 1. Physical: description of self in terms of the body (I am blonde.) 2. Social: description of self in terms of social interaction or involvement (I am a Tri Delta.) 3. Reflective: description of self in terms of feelings, moods, opinions, attitudes, dislikes and preferences (I enjoy being a Tri Delta.) 4. Oceanic: description of self that transcends social interaction (I am at one with nature.) 4. Mark “sense of self” on the back of each card. Part Two: 1. Pick out the two key words from each statement. Key Words: words that are central to the meaning of a statement. Without them, the statement would make no sense. 2. Rank key words in order of importance. The more important word should reveal the most about the speaker’s “self”. “I love my family.” #1) Love – this tells us something about the speaker’s values or feelings #2) Family – this tells us what the speaker values or feels about 3. Write key words on the back of each card, in order of importance Part Three: 1. Evaluate statements for characteristics of a strong or weak “sense of self” 2. Circle your choices on the back of each card 57 Coder #2 Part One: Sense of Self □ Physical □ Social □ Reflective □ Oceanic Part Two: Key Words #1____________________________ #2____________________________ Part Three: Strong/Weak Sense of Self Mood: Positive Self Esteem: High Sense of Wellbeing: High Self-consciousness: Low Attitude toward others: Benevolent Neutral Neutral Neutral Neutral Neutral Negative Low Low High Hostile 58 I believe a woman needs to take care of her husband and children. I imagine the ideal wife depends entirely on the viewpoint of the husband. I think beauty comes in a lot of different forms. 59 Bibliography Allen, A. (1998). Rethinking power. Hypatia, 13(1), 21-41. “Bars Beach Lizards.” (1921, May 27). New York Times, 14. Baker, D. (1997). How we prepare and why we prepare in this manner. Don Baker’s Communications Plus. Bedford, Texas. A1-E3. Banner, L.W. (1983). American Beauty. New York: Knopf. Batwalia, S. (1993). Empowerment of Women in South Asia: Concepts and Practices. New Delhi: FAO-FFHC/AD. Pg 48 Benedetti, E.T. (2000). You be the Judge. The Miss America Pageant 2000. Harry Eaby. 33. Bernay, T., Cantor, D.W. (1992). Women In Power. The Secrets of Leadership. New York: Houghton Mifflin. Brownmiller, S. (1984). Femininity. New York: Linden. Burke, P.J., Tully, J.C. (1977). The Measurement of Role Identity. Social Forces, 55(4), 881-898. Cohen, L.L., Ferguson, M.J., Hyers, L.L., Swim, J.K. (2001). Everyday Sexism: Evidence for Its Incidence, Nature, and Psychological Impact From Three Daily Diary Studies. Journal of Social Issues, 57(1), 31-53. Cohen, L.L., Swim, J.K. (1997). Overt, Covert, and Subtle Sexism. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21(1), 103-119. Costanzo, P.R. (1992). External Socialization and the development of adaptive individuation and social connenction. In D.N. Ruble, P.R. Costanzo, and M.E. Oliveri (Eds.), The Social Psychology of Mental Health (pp. 55-80). New York: Guilford. Cramer, K.L., Grace, S.L. (2002). Sense of Self in the New Millennium: Male and Female Student Responses to the TST. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 30(3), 271-280. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1982). Toward a Psychology of Optimal Experience. In L. Wheeler (Ed.) Review of Personality and Social Psychology. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow. New York: Harper Perennial. 60 Deford, F. (1971). There She Is: The Life and Times of Miss America. New York: Viking. Denmark, F.L. (1993). Women, leadership and empowerment. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 17(3), 343-357. Dennis, J. (2002). The women of SNCC: Struggle, Sexism, and the emergence of feminist consciousness, 1960-66. International Social Science Review, 77(3/4), 185-191. Dominick, J.R., Wimmer, R.D. (2000). Mass Media Research: An Introduction. Sixth Edition. Belmont, California: Wadsworth Publishing. Driver, E.D. (1987). Self-Evaluation and Altruistic Behavior. Journal of Social Psychology, 127(4), 393-395. Fredrickson, B.L., Roberts, T. (1997). Objectification Theory. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21(3), 173-207. Freedman, R. (1986). Beauty Bound. Lexington, MA: Heath. Gallery. Miss America. Retrieved March 28, 2003, from PBS American Experience Web site: http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/amex/missamerica/gallery/index.html Greco, K.L. (2002). Behind the Dreams: Today’s Miss America Organization. Miss America Competition 2002 Program. John Patrick McElwee. 14-15. Greco, K.L. (2002). You be the Judge. Miss America Competition 2002 Program. John Patrick McElwee. 53. Hahn, E.D., Spence, J.T. (1997). The Attitudes Toward Women Scale and attitude change in college students. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 23(1), 17-34. Hartley, W. (1968). Self Conception and Organizational Adaptation. Omaha, Nebraska: Paper presented at the meetings of the Midwest Sociological Association. Heng, C.L. (1995). Women’s empowerment: Addressing emotional subordination. Convergence, 28(3), 78-86. Kabeer, N. (1999). Resouces, Agency, Achievements: Reflections on the Measurement of Women’s Empowerment. Development and Change, 30(3), 435-465. Kaschak, E. (1992). Engendered Lives: A New Psychology of Women’s Experience. New York: Basic Books. 61 Kuhn, M. (1964). Major Trends in symbolic interaction theory in the past twenty-five years. Sociological Quarterly, 5, 61-84. Kuhn, M., and McPartland, T.S. (1954). An empirical investigation of self-attitudes. American Psychological Review, 19, 68-76. Latham, A.J. (1995). Packaging woman: The concurrent rise of beauty pageants, public bathing, and other performances of female nudity. Journal of Popular Culture, 29(3), 149-168. Levin, G., Swift, C. (1987). Empowerment: An emerging mental health technology. Journal of Primary Prevention, 8 (1&2), 71-94. Lu, S. (1990). The development of self-conceptions from childhood to adolescence in China. Child Study Journal, 20(2), 129-138. Matthews, B.A. (2002). A Content Analysis of Advertising from the 1960 and 1980 Miss America Telecasts. University of Arkansas. Fayetteville, Arkansas. Unpublished Manuscript. Miss America Mission Statement. Retrieved October 8, 2002, from the Official Web site of the Miss America Scholarship Competition. Web site: www.missamerica.org Morgan, M.Y., Rhoden, L.J. (1995). Change is White College Women’s Understanding of Sexism: Empowerment through Critical Reflection. NWSA Journal, 7(2), 35-58. Muller, L.E. (1994). Toward an understanding of empowerment: A study of six women leaders. Journal of Humanistic Education and Development, 33(2), 75-83. Pennington, J. (2001). Testimonials. Press Kit. New and Events. Retrieved March 28, 2003, from Offical Web site of the Miss America Scholarship Competition: http://www.missamerica.org/news/presskit/testimonials.asp Rappaport, J. 1984. Studies in Empowerment: Introduction to the issue. In J. Rappaport, C. Swift, & R. Hess (Eds.), Studies in Empowerment: Steps toward understanding and action. New York: Hawthorn. Rappaport, J. (1985). The Power of Empowerment Language. Social Policy, 17(2), 15-21. 62 Riverol, A.R. (1992). Live From Atlantic City: The History of the Miss America Pageant Before, After and in Spite of Television. Bowling Green: Bowling Green State University Popular Press. Rushton, J. (1980). Altruism, Socialization and Society. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, 83-84. Sobel, B. (1931). Burleycue: An Underground History of Burlesque Days. New York: Farrar & Rinehart. Swanson, J.L. (1991). The new sexism: A response to DeVoe. Journal of Counseling and Development, 70(2), 350-353. The Changing Ideal. Special Features. Miss America. Retrieved January 13, 2003, from PBS American Experience Web site: http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/amex/missamerica/sfeature/sf_decades.html Timeline. Miss America. Retrieved January 13, 2003, from PBS American Experience Web site: http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/amex/missamerica/timeline/index.html Transcript. The Film and More. Miss America. Retrieved March 28, 2003, from PBS American Experience Web site: http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/amex/missamerica/filmmore/pt.html Watson, E., Martin, D. (2000). The Miss America Pageant: Pluralism, Femininity, and Cinderella All in One. Journal of Popular Culture, 34(1), 105-127. Wolf, Naomi. (1991). The Beauty Myth. New York: W Morrow, 1991. Reprint ed., New York: Anchor Books, 1992.