Back to Teaching in Higher Education Home Page Teaching of Psychology, Vol. 12, No. 4, December, 1985, pp. 220-222. SomeNorth85.doc FACULTY FORUM Some Northerly Thoughts on Undergraduate Psychology research conferences John J. Furedy University of Toronto R. Cameron McRae Wilfred Laurier University Although we agree with Carsrud, Palladino, Tanke, Aubrecht, and Huber’s (1984) emphasis on the educational importance of undergraduate psychology research conferences (UPRCs), and recognize the value of the information provided by them for prospective organizers, we question the following assumptions: (a) that UPRCs are more favored by and more suited to undergraduate level institutions rather than “larger, research-oriented universities”; (b) that a senior faculty speaker is an essential, or even a desirable, component of a successful UPRC; (c) that speakers should never read their papers; and (d) that practice sessions should only have “reasonable questions” posed to the speaker. In their recent paper, Carsrud et al. (1984) have clearly indicated the educational importance of undergraduate psychology research conferences (UPRCs), and have also provided useful information to those intending to organize these events. There are, however, some assumptions made in their paper that deserve critical commentary. Our comments are made from the perspective of two individuals who have been involved with an annual UPRC that has rotated around universities in Ontario for 15 years. The second author has acted as overall conference coordinator during this period, each author has been the thesis coordinator in his respective department during the same period, and the first author has a special interest in the teaching of undergraduate thesis research (e.g., Furedy & Furedy, 1977). Although we recognize that there are important differences between the United States and Canada, we suggest that it may still be useful to consider some more northerly criticism of certain assumptions in the Carsrud et al. (1984) article in the light of our experience. We now turn to these assumptions. 1. UPRCs are more favored by and more suited to undergraduate level institutions than to “larger, research-oriented universities” (p. 144). In Ontario, many of the larger schools have experimented with deemphasis or deletion of the undergraduate thesis requirement. The rationale, typically, has been that undergraduate research takes 220 up valuable lab space and faculty time that could be better made available to graduate students. However, it turned out that incoming graduate students without formal undergraduate research were not as well-prepared as those who had completed a thesis and presented the results in a conference setting. As a result, not only is the undergraduate thesis a well-supported endeavor in all of Ontario’s universities, but host universities for the conference have ranged from exclusively undergraduate institutions like Brock and Trent to the research powerhouses like Toronto and Western. In terms of support for the conference (space, personnel, funding), the big schools have put on just as good a “show” as the smaller institutions. 2. A senior faculty speaker is an essential, or even a desirable, component of a successful UPRC. At the first few conferences the Ontario group had such speakers, but then abandoned the scheme as being inconsistent with the aim of having undergraduates have their own conference for the interchange of research findings. Quite simply, the students are much more interested in discussing each other’s work (and their respective schools and programs) than in sitting through one more lecture, regardless of how eminent and stimulating the speaker. It’s their day; for once, they are the experts. Moreover, in our view, the focus on an eminent faculty speaker at a UPRC follows a politico-religious or “sophistic” rather than a scientific or “Socratic” model (Furedy & Furedy, 1982). 3. Speakers should never read their papers, a “practice that can be excruciatingly boring for the audience” (p. 145). The absolute ban on reading has been imposed by other American research associations like the Psychonomic Society. but in our view the ban is misguided. When a brief time period is available, and the arguments are at all complex, it is often more clear (and hence less boring!) to read a coherent paper than to try to ad-lib a confusing paper, particularly when the speaker is distraught with stage fright! Of course, the presentation would have to be written for oral rather than journal-style communication and adequately rehearsed (i.e., not read “cold”). Teaching of Psychology 4. Practice sessions should be conducted among “sympathetic peers” who have to ask only “reasonable questions” (p. 145). Practice sessions should, indeed, be conducted among “sympathetic peers” but hardly with restriction to “reasonable questions.” This suggests that UPRCs are more akin to group therapy sessions than sessions for the interchange of research findings. In our experience, more learning takes place and, remarkably, more anxiety is alleviated when the gloves are off. Most student presenters are both able and delighted to deal with “unreasonable” as well as “reasonable” questions. Indeed, because they are free of the professional statusprotection requirements that make many post-graduate conferences such uninformative affairs, it appears that in terms of communication and audience participation, the UPRC sessions are actually superior to those of professional conferences. We recognize that both the assumptions and our opposed positions are matters of informed opinion rather than empirical generalizations based on data. However, that informed opinion, along with that of Carsrud et al., holds that the UPR/UPRC experience is an important, perhaps essential, pant of undergraduate education in psychology. Our main purpose in drawing attention to these issues is to help further the development of the UPRC “ . . . in such a way as to maximize the contribution to the educational process” (Carsrud et al., 1984, p. 141). REFERENCES Carsrud, A. L., Palladino, J. J., Tanke, E. D., Aubrecht, L., & Huber, R. J. (1984). Undergraduate psychology research conferences: Goals, policies, and procedures. Teaching of Psychology, 11, 141-145. Furedy, J. J., & Furedy, C. (1977). Modeling the realities of research experience: Collaboration against common and merciless foes. Teaching of Psychology, 4, 107-110. Furedy, J. J., & Furedy, C. (1982). Socratic versus sophistic strains in the teaching of undergraduate psychology: Implicit conflicts made explicit. Teaching of Psychology, 9, 14-20. NOTE Requests for reprints should be sent to John J. Furedy, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, M5S 1Al Of Data and Informed Opinion: A Reply to Furedy and McRae Alan L. Carsrud Department of Management University of Texas at Austin Joseph J. Palladino University of Southern Indiana The stated purpose of the Furedy and McRae article is to draw attention to certain issues “to help further the development” of undergraduate psychology research conferences (UPRCs). They suggest “it may be useful to consider some more northerly criticism of certain assumptions in our recent article on URRCs (Carsrud, Palladino, Tanke, Aubrecht, & Huber, 1984). Their comments are made from the perspective of individuals who have been involved with a rotating conference. As individuals who have been involved collectively with UPRCs for more than 20 years, we are pleased to hear about the Ontario conference. We will address the points offered by Furedy and McRae in order. 1. Furedy and McRae question the “assumption” that “UPRCs are more favored by and more suited to undergraduate level institutions than to ‘larger, research-oriented universities.’” “ Based on our survey data (which included Vol. 12, No. 4, December 1985 rotating conferences similar to the Ontario conference), we wrote “conferences are typically hosted by smallto-medium-size institutions that emphasize undergraduate education” (p. 143). We also reported that “surveys of undergraduate psychology research conferences . . . have consistently indicated that most of the participants come from small or medium size institutions” (p. 144). It may be regrettable that larger institutions typically do not support UPRCs more than they do, but it is a fact, not an assumption. For example, the 1985 Calendar of Undergraduate Research Conferences published in the APA Monitor listed 16 conferences in the United States. Only three of the sponsoring institutions could be classified as “larger, research-oriented universities.” Furedy and McRae’s comments are based on a single rotating conference and they provide no data on the participation of students from institutions of 221 various sizes. The parsimonious explanation for differing views here may be the difference in the sample sizes used as a basis for generalization and not cultural or educational differences between the United States and Canada. 2. Furedy and McRae question the “assumption” that “a senior faculty speaker is an essential . . . component of a successful UPRC.” Once again, based on our survey we re ported that all of the responding conferences invited featured speakers. We did not write that “a senior faculty speaker” is an essential component of a conference. Furedy and McRae state that “students are much more interested in discussing each other’s work” than listening to a speaker. Certainly, others involved with undergraduate psychology conferences (e.g., Anderson & Rosenfeld, 1983) write favorably about guest speakers and students’ reactions to them. We do not accept Furedy and McRae’s characterization of the involvement of speakers in UPRCs as “politicoreligious” activity. We prefer to think of these speakers as appropriate professional role models. 3. Furedy and McRae question the “assumption” that “speakers should never read their papers.” They argue that it is better “to read a coherent paper than to try to ad-lib a confusing paper” or read a paper “cold,” and we whole heartedly agree. We did not suggest that students ad-lib their presentations, but espoused “coaching and practice” in preparation. Our advice is quite similar to the advice given in the Publication Manual of the American Psychological Asso ciation (1983): “Do not read your presentation. Reading a paper usually induces boredom and can make even the best research sound second-rate. Instead, tell your audience what you have to say, just as you would in conversation” (p. 193). 4. Finally, Furedy and McRae question the “assumption” that practice sessions should be conducted among “sympathetic peers,” who “have to ask only ‘reasonable questions.’ “ It is important to note that our full statement was: “Asking several reasonable questions at the end of the practice session also helps as this will mimic what typically occurs at most of these conferences” (p. 145). Furedy and McRae characterize our portrayal of UPRCs as “more akin to group therapy sessions than sessions for the interchange of research findings.” This “assumption” is not consistent with the data provided in Carsrud (1975) and Carsrud (1984). Perhaps our description does seem like “group therapy” compared to the approach that views research as “collaboration against common and merciless foes” (Furedy 222 & Furedy, 1977, p. 107). We suggested that conference organizers try to make students’ participation a good “first experience” and mentioned a number of ways to alleviate students’ anxieties. Coaching, preparation, and practice can all help to achieve our goals for UPRCs. Perhaps even PhD presentations at regional and national conventions could benefit from such an approach. We hope that this exchange serves to encourage other faculty members both here and abroad to become actively involved in UPRCs and in research designed to characterize their organization and structure and to evaluate their effects. REFERENCES American Psychological Association. (1983). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Anderson, D., & Rosenfeld, P. (1983). Letting form follow function: A multipurpose model for undergraduate psychology conferences. Teaching of Psychology, 10, 204206. Carsrud, A. L. (1975). Undergraduate psychology conferences: Is good research nested under Ph.D.s? Teaching of Psychology, 2, 112-114. Carsrud, A. L. (1984). Graduate student supervision of undergraduate research: Increasing research opportunities. Teaching. of Psychology, 11,203-205. Carsrud, A. L., Palladino, J. J., Tanke, E. D., Aubrecht, L., & Huber, R. J. (1984). Undergraduate psychology research conferences: Goals, policies, and procedures. Teaching of Psychology, 11, 141-145. Furedy, J. J., & Furedy, C. (1977). Modeling the realities of research experience: Collaboration against common and merciless foes. Teaching of Psychology, 4, 107-110. Furedy, J. J., & McRae, R. C. (1985). Some northerly thoughts on undergraduate psychology research conferences. Teaching of Psychology, 12(4), 220-221. NOTES 1. We thank our colleagues for their encouragement in writing this response. 2. On July 1, 1985, Indiana State University Evansville became the University of Southern Indiana, an independent state university. 3. Requests for reprints should be sent to Alan L. Carsrud, Department of Management, CBA 4.202, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712. Teaching of Psychology