This article appeared in Clinical Psychology, 2004, issue 34, pp 4-10 Beck never lived in Birmingham: Why CBT may be a less useful treatment for psychological distress than is often supposed Paul Moloney and Paul Kelly, Solihull & Birmingham Mental Health NHS Trust This article suggests that CBT has a long way to go before addressing the difficulties of typical NHS patients attending clinical psychology services. distress encountered by CBT therapists working in public services, and to the consequent need for therapeutic approaches that emphasise social action over the exploration of putative internal psychological spaces. In recent years, a growing number of central government agencies and therapeutic psychologists have argued that Cognitive Behaviour Therapy (CBT) should be the therapy of choice for mental health practitioners – and particularly for those working in the time-limited and pressured circumstances of primary care services. This position is supported by claims that CBT may be the most effective of all therapies, that it is well founded in clinical outcome research and in the findings of cognitive science and, finally, that CBT offers a collaborative and inevitably empowering way of helping people in distress (e.g. Beck, 1995; Fenell, 1997). In this paper we wish to question all of the above claims. We suggest that the current popularity of CBT may at least equally reflect the needs and values of the mental health professions, and of those political and social institutions that help to shape their aims and activities. The paper will begin with a brief discussion of the nature of CBT as practised within the NHS, and will then move on to a critical examination of the approach from three perspectives. These will include, first, a body of psychological research that may cast doubt on the conceptual basis of the CBT approach; second, that section of the psychotherapy outcome research literature that bears on the effectiveness of CBT; and third, the mental health epidemiological literature, which may point to the social origins of much of the The scope and nature of CBT In the UK, CBT has enjoyed a strong historical association with the profession of clinical psychology, particularly in NHS outpatient settings (Clegg, 1998; Pilgrim & Treacher, 1992). After a recent period of therapeutic eclecticism within clinical and counselling psychology, CBT may be returning as the preferred approach for most therapeutic psychologists working in the public health services, as supported by recent government planning for mental health care (Department of Health, 2001; NHS Executive, 1996). In terms of both theory and application, the term ‘Cognitive Behaviour Therapy’ encompasses a group of perhaps 15 approaches, in which the link between practice and theoretical foundations may vary widely (Chadwick et al., 1996; Boyle, 2002). The origins of CBT are likewise multistranded, and include an (arguably incompatible) mixture of applied learning theory, the more accessible aspects of the psychodynamic tradition, selected elements of laboratory-based cognitive psychology and pragmatic clinical experience (Beck & Weishaar, 1989; Hawton et al., 1989; Willis & Sanders, 1997). In general, CBT practitioners tend to share a view of the human condition as a product of the four interacting elements of cognition, physiology, behaviour and emotion. Perhaps for most CBT therapists, the treatment of distress will entail a process of enabling the client to change ‘unhelpful’ beliefs and behaviours by means of a series of structured exercises. This process is based upon the key assumptions that the client’s more accessible thoughts – and the deeper patterns or ‘schemas’ that are said to underlie them – will mediate their reactions to events, and that these thoughts can be readily examined and then modified so as to yield therapeutic change (Davidson, 2000; Padesky, 1994; Trower & Casey, 1989). Conceptual difficulties with CBT Perhaps one of the strongest arguments in favour of CBT is that the therapy is seen as being derived from a scientifically valid body of knowledge based within the discipline of cognitive science. However, this claim may be challenged on a number of different levels. As already noted, CBT is based on the idea that cognitive processes are fundamental in the origin and amelioration of personal distress (Beck, 1995; Beutler & Guest, 1989). Yet, as some reviewers have recognised, evidence for this idea is equivocal at best (Bracken, 2002; Cromby & Standen, 1998; Godsi, 1998; Hughes, 1997). For example, although there are indications that depressed people may say negative things more frequently (or quickly) than non-depressed individuals, this may actually reveal little about any causal relationship between cognition and emotion. Such a process could easily reflect the effect of aversive environments having primed many individuals to more readily access pessimistic beliefs about themselves and their world. Likewise, the finding that successful completion of laboratory tasks by depressed individuals can elevate their mood may offer little support for the claim that this has been achieved by the direct falsification of the person’s underlying negative beliefs (e.g. Beck, 1995). A more reasonable interpretation of this work may be that the person’s basic belief repertoire includes positive dimensions that are activated by positive experiences, and that a given individual’s prevailing negativity may be far more an outcome of the kinds of negative experiences that they have recently been undergoing (Erwin, 1996; Fancher, 1995). However, we would argue that perhaps the key flaw within most models of CBT lies in the poor fit between the concepts of mind offered by many writers in the field (e.g. Hawton et al., 1989; Padesky, 1994) and those offered within other branches of psychology. For example, the wider perspective of both historical and cross-cultural research indicates large variations in the way in which human beings have customarily understood the causes of their thoughts and actions and behaviours, in turn suggesting that currently accepted Western views on the nature of the self need have no special claim to validity (Crook, 1980; Gray, 2002; Sue & Sue, 1990). In the context of clinical problems, there is some evidence that the self-abnegating language commonly found among depressed Westerners may be much less common among other cultural groups, including South East Asians and Africans (Marsella, 1981; Littlewood & Lipsedge, 1997). One reason for this difference may be that, for depressed non-Westerners, the explanatory repertoires offered by their cultures are less likely to promote those expressions of guilt and responsibility that derive from Western Christianity and the Protestant work ethic (Chan, 1990; Sue & Sue, 1990). Conversely, when seeking to account for the likely origins of their own experiences of ill health, contemporary Westerners may be inclined to discount the effects of those social and material adversities with which they may be struggling (Blaxter, 2003; 1997; Cornwell, 1984). Indeed, the practice and theory of CBT seems to be premised upon the quintessentially Western idea of our being able to scrutinise and then modify our own thinking, although it is sometimes unclear whether this Cartesian notion of an internal observer should be regarded as a metaphor or a reality (see Baars, 1997). Nevertheless, a wide variety of research suggests that the process of introspecting into the causes of our thoughts, feelings and actions may often be inaccurate and misleading, and to an extent that seldom seems to be recognised within the cognitive and behavioural therapies (e.g. Beck, 1995; Willis & Sanders, 1997). In the field of neuropsychology, for example, the well-known experiments conducted with people whose brains have been surgically divided have shown that the explanations that they give for the causes of their feelings and actions can be blatantly in error from the standpoint of an external observer, yet completely compelling for the individuals concerned (Gazzaniga, 1993; McKay, 1980). These results agree with a number of similar findings in other branches of clinical neurology, which suggest that even for physically normal individuals, there is no necessary link between beliefs about the sources of subjective experiences and of conduct on the one hand, and their demonstrable neurological, bodily and environmental underpinnings on the other (Claxton, 1999; 1996; Dennett, 1991; Parfitt, 1987). Indeed, neuroscientists increasingly view the brain as a set of parallel systems without any central controller or ‘Cartesian theatre’ in which thoughts may be viewed and then manipulated in the way that many CBT writers seem to imply (Blackmore, 2001; Damassio, 1994; Dennett, 1991; Norrentranders, 1998). The results of several decades of social psychological research seem to complement this picture. Here, researchers have consistently shown that our degree of insight into the likely reasons for our thoughts and behaviours can be surprisingly limited, and that we may instead habitually rely upon a priori (and often erroneous) causal theories in order to explain ourselves to ourselves and to one another (Caldini 1994; Nisbett & Wilson, 1980; Wegner, 2002; Wilson, 2002). Overall then, the notions of consciousness, introspection and deliberation that are central to the theory and practice of CBT seem to match poorly with current knowledge of the mind. This incongruence may underline both how the therapeutic task of finding a cognition that putatively causes other thoughts or feelings may be far from straightforward, and also how the sensations of viewing and controlling our thoughts within CBT may have little connection with the webs of social and material influence that may in reality shape much, and perhaps all, of our experience and conduct (Smail, 2001; Smillensky, 2000; Wegner, 2002). CBT and the psychotherapeutic outcome research literature: A critique Despite the above difficulties with the theoretical bases of CBT, there seems to be a growing consensus within the mental health field that the effectiveness of this approach is well supported by clinical research (Department of Health, 2001; Roth & Fonagy, 1996). This research appears to provide valid evidence, based largely upon randomised control trials (RCTs), of the effectiveness of CBT in the treatment of many forms of distress. However, a thorough review of the literature reveals that this claim may lack firm support, for a number of reasons. To begin with, many RCTs involving CBT have included inadequate control groups for comparison purposes – usually individuals who remain on a waiting list or receive an unconvincing form of pseudotherapy, delivered with limited commitment by the researchers (Bolsover, 2002; Holmes 2002; Mair, 1992). Second, CBT has indeed sometimes compared well with other therapies in a number of trials involving selected research populations, which may often comprise middle class university students and therapeutic practitioners who are strongly convinced of the efficacy of the CBT approach (Dawes, 1994; Mair, 1992). The situation may be different when comparisons are made within inner city community-based clinical settings, characterised by hard-pressed clinicians serving populations that experience high levels of social and economic deprivation (cf. Hagan & Donnison, 1999; Richards, 1995). Here, comparisons of CBT with other psychological therapies have suggested that the former may offer little or no significant additional benefit in the treatment of such problems as alcohol and drug abuse, depression, chronic anxiety, and behavioural and emotional disturbance (Dawes, 1994; Dineen, 1999; Eisner, 2000; Epstein, 1995; Elkin et al, 1994; Hemmings, 2002; Leff et al., 2000; Sandell et al., 2000; Sanders & Tudor, 2001). A third challenge to the evidence base for the effectiveness of CBT consists in the large body of comparative clinical outcome literature that has accumulated over the last half century. This has convincingly shown that, for a wide range of clinical problems, such effectiveness as psychotherapy does have may bear scant relation to the therapist’s theoretical position, extent of professional training or alleged expertise (Dawes, 1994; House, 2003; King-Spooner, 1995; Spinelli, 2001). These startling conclusions seem to have passed almost unremarked in the professional training literature. Indeed, particularly in the field of CBT, it seems to emphasise the acquisition of ever more refined clinical skills (see Proctor, 2002). Finally, it is perhaps worth noting that even strong proponents of CBT recognise that this treatment, like all other psychotherapies, will significantly help only about two-thirds of all recipients, even under ideal research trial conditions (Bergin & Garfield, 1994; Smith & Glass, 1977; Tarrier, 2002). Both in the professional literature and in discussions of clinical practice that we have witnessed, this commonplace observation rarely seems to prompt any searching discussion as to why this might be so or as to what alternative forms of help might be offered. Yet this issue is clearly an important one, when lack of clinical improvement can all too readily be attributed to a failure of motiv on the client’s part (Pilgrim, 1997; Smail, 2001; Willoughby, 2002). An alternative view: social inequalities as the fundamental determinants of personal distress In contrast to this individualised view of the genesis of human difficulties, many writers and researchers in the mental health field have highlighted the importance of toxic social influences in the origins of personal distress. There are abundant indications that the incidence and severity of a range of familiar psychological disturbances (including anxiety, depression, psychosis, substance abuse and self-harm) are linked to the cumulative effects of widespread social and economic inequalities (Bruce et al., 1992; Godsi, 1998; Mirowsky & Ross, 1989; Prilleltensky et al., 2000; Wilkinson, 2001). Critical psychologists have described these processes by reference to the operation of varieties of social power, which may work to profoundly (and negatively) shape the identity and self-efficacy of the least privileged individuals – and indeed of wide swathes of the population during times of economic and political upheaval (Bordieu, 1984; Smail, 1993; Stoppard, 2000; Wilkinson, 1996). In the context of the practice of CBT, this analysis suggests that, for the distressed person, any attempt to modify their ‘negative’ thoughts will have little capacity to beneficially change their psychological state in the long term, beyond that individual’s power to alter the landscape of social and material influences in which they are embedded (Franzblau & Moore, 2001; Smail, 2001; Wilkinson, 1996). However, these kinds of observations have at best been embraced with ambivalence by the mental health professions. Perhaps because they have always had to operate in political and institutional climates that have favoured individualised and technical or treatmentoriented approaches to distress, of which CBT may be a paradigmatic example (Ferudi, 2003; Hansen et al., 2003). Conclusion As the title of this paper suggests, our overall argument is that although aspects of CBT may be helpful – particularly those parts that encourage the client to confront the environmental causes of their distress where this is possible – it may none the less be the case that, overall, the theory and practice of CBT can be seen as effective only if viewed from the standpoint of those in positions of socio-economic privilege. For the majority of clients seen by psychologists in areas of relative deprivation (such as many parts of Birmingham, for example), the emphasis of CBT on alleviating distress through challenging thoughts may be profoundly misleading for the client and for the therapist alike, and may indeed constitute a poor reflection of their shared clinical experience. In the long run, this emphasis may also serve to obstruct the growth of a therapeutic psychology that seeks to reflect the experiences of ‘ordinary’ people, to place their distress firmly in a social and material context, and perhaps to help them to find ways of making that context a more tolerable one in which to live. Acknowledgements We would like to thank Guy Holmes and David Smail for their helpful comments on early drafts of this paper. References Baars, B. (1997). In the Theatre of Consciousness: The workspace of the mind. New York: Oxford University Press. Beck, A. (1995). Cognitive Therapy: Basics and beyond. New York: Guilford Press. Beck, A. T & Weishaar, A. T. ( 1989) Cognitive therapy. In, A. Freeman, K. Simon, L. Beutler & H. Arkowitz (Eds.) Comprehensive Handbook of Cognitive Therapy. New York: Plenum Press. Clinical Psychology 34 – February 2004 Bergin, A.E. & Garfield, S.L. (Eds.) (1994) Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behaviour Change. New York: John Wiley. Beutler, L. & Guest, P. (1989). The role of cognitive change in psychotherapy. In A. Freeman, K. Simon, L. Beutler & H. Arkowitz (Eds.) Comprehensive Handbook of Cognitive Therapy. New York: Plenum Press. Blackmore, S. (2001) State of the art: Consciousness. The Psychologist, 14, 10, 522-525. Blaxter, M. (2003). Biology, social class and inequalities in health: Their synthesis in ‘health capital’. In, Williams, j. Burke, L. & Bendelow, G.A. (Eds) Debating Biology: sociological reflections on health, medicine and society. London. Routledge. Blaxter, M. (1997). Whose fault is it? People’s conceptions of the reasons for health Inequalities. Social Science and Medicine, 44, 747-756. Bolsover, N. (2002). Commentary: The ‘evidence’ is weaker than claimed. British Medical Journal, 324, 298-301. Bordieu, P. (1984). Distinction: A social critique of the judgement of taste. London. Routledge. Boyle, M. (2002). Schizophrenia: A scientific delusion? (second Edition). London. Routledge. Bracken, P. (2002). Trauma: Culture, meaning and philosophy. London: Wurr. Brewin, C. (1988) Cognitive Foundations of Clinical Psychology. London: Earlbaum. Bronfrenbrenner, U. (1977) Towards an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychologist, 32, 513-531. Bruce, M. Takeuchi, D. & Leaf, P. (1992). Poverty and psychiatric status: Longitudinal evidence from the New Haven epidemiological catchment area study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 5, 470–474. Caldini, R. B. (1994). Influence: The psychology of persuasion. New York: Morrow.