Neurocognitive Impairments Richard S. E. Keefe, Ph.D. Charles E. Eesley, B.S. Dr. Richard Keefe Associate Professor Duke University Box 3270 Duke University Medical Center Durham, NC 27710 USA Fax: 1-919-684-2632 richard.keefe@duke.edu Charles E. Eesley, B.S. Duke University Box 3270 Duke University Medical Center Durham, NC 27710 USA 919-471-0054 eesle001@mc.duke.edu 1 Neurocognitive Impairments Chapter I. What is the cognitive impairment in schizophrenia? A. Cognition as a Core Feature of Schizophrenia B. Profile and Magnitude of Cognitive Impairment Associated with Schizophrenia 1. 7 divisions of cognition II. What is the natural history of neurocognitive impairment? A. Deficits in children at risk for schizophrenia and follow-back studies B. The course of cognitive impairment from prodrome to end-stage illness. Prodrome Studies First Episode of Psychosis Studies Cognitive Changes in Patients Recovering from Acute Exacerbation Longitudinal Change in Cognitive Function Cognitive Functioning in Elderly Patients C. Summary III. Relationship of neurocognitive impairment to positive, negative and extrapyramidal symptoms. A. Pseudospecificity B. Cross-sectional studies. C. Longitudinal studies. IV. Clinical importance of neurocognitive impairment. A. Cross-sectional correlations with function B. Relationship of Neurocognitive Impairment to Functioning V. Conclusion 2 SECTION I. Cognitive Impairment in Schizophrenia Cognition as a Core Feature of Schizophrenia Cognitive impairment associated with schizophrenia is now viewed as a potential psychopharmacological target for treatment (Hyman and Fenton 2003). While cognition is not a formal part of the current diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, volume IV (DSM-IV-TR 2000) includes seven references to cognitive dysfunction in the description of the disorder. Diagnostic and scientific experts have increasingly expressed the idea that neurocognitive impairment is a core feature of the illness, and not simply the result of the symptoms or the current treatments of schizophrenia. Profile and Magnitude of Cognitive Impairment Associated with Schizophrenia Severely impaired performance on cognitive tests is the strongest evidence for the importance of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. In several cognitive domains, the average impairment can reach 2 standard deviations below the healthy control mean (Harvey and Keefe 1997; Heinrichs and Zakzanis 1998; Saykin et al. 1991). While only about 27% of patients with schizophrenia (and 85% of the general population) are not rated as “impaired” by clinical neuropsychological assessment (Palmer 1997), these patients tend to have the highest levels of premorbid functioning (Kremen 2000), and demonstrate cognitive functioning that is considerably below what would be expected of them based upon their premorbid levels and the educational level of their parents. Recent analyses of data from 150 patients with schizophrenia suggest that up to 98% of patients perform more poorly on cognitive tests than would be predicted by their parents level of education (Keefe, Eesley, and Poe submitted for publication). In addition, comparisons of monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia suggest that almost all affected twins perform worse than their unaffected twin on cognitive tests (Goldberg et al. 3 1990). Therefore, it is likely that almost all patients with schizophrenia are functioning below the level that would be expected in the absence of the illness. Neurocognitive tests often assess more than one domain of functioning, and many tests do not fit neatly into a single domain. Thus, descriptions of the profile of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia have varied across literature reviews. The domains that are most consistently cited as being severely impaired in schizophrenia are verbal memory, executive functions, attention or vigilance, verbal fluency, and motor speed (Harvey and Keefe 1997). Deficits in social cognition also appear to be severe, but this is a relatively new area of research, with fewer studies to support this finding (Pinkham et al. 2003). The opinion of a group of experts who served on the Neurocognition Subcommittee for the Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia (MATRICS) Project is that the most important domains of cognitive deficit in schizophrenia are: working memory, attention/vigilance, verbal learning and memory, visual learning and memory, reasoning and problem solving, speed of processing, and social cognition (Green et al., in preparation www.matrics.ucla.edu). The most striking aspect of the profile of cognitive deficits in patients with schizophrenia is that so few cognitive functions remain similar to healthy controls (Harvey and Keefe 1997; Saykin et al. 1994). In fact, an estimate of the severity of neurocognitive impairment in patients with schizophrenia, based upon an examination of the most methodologically sound studies completed, suggests that many important cognitive functions are in the severely impaired range (2 to 3 standard deviations below the normal mean) or moderately impaired range (1 to 2 standard deviations below the normal mean). A review and meta-analysis of 204 studies shows a consistent and stable difference between patients with schizophrenia (N = 7420) and healthy controls (N = 5865) in a wide range of domains of cognitive functioning (Heinrichs and Zakzanis 4 1998). Examples of tests that measure the most important components of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia are briefly reviewed here. Attention/Vigilance. Many neurocognitive tests require vigilance functions, even if the test itself is not a measure of “pure” vigilance. Vigilance refers to the ability to maintain attention over time. A standard vigilance test used in many studies is the Continuous Performance Test, or CPT. The “A-X” version of the CPT, requires that patients attend to a series of letters presented one at a time on a computer screen at a rate of one per second. Patients respond with a button-press on the computer mouse or keyboard each time an “A” is followed by an “X”. This type of CPT reveals severe vigilance impairments in patients with schizophrenia (Cornblatt and Keilp 1994). Impairments in vigilance can result in difficulty following social conversations, inability to follow important instructions regarding treatment, therapy or work functions, and simple activities like reading or watching television become labored or impossible. Reviews of the literature have suggested that vigilance deficits in patients with schizophrenia are related to various aspects of outcome, including social deficits, community functioning, and skills acquisition (Green 1996; Green 2000). Verbal Learning and Memory. Several different types of abilities involved in memory functioning include, but are not limited to, abilities associated with learning new information, retaining newly learned information over time, and recognizing previously presented material. In general, patients show larger deficits in learning than in retention. The findings for recognition are more equivocal, with most studies suggesting relatively mild deficits (Calev 1984; Saykin et al. 1991) yet large deficits have also been reported (Mohamed et al. 1999). The tests used to measure learning typically involve the ability to learn lists of words or written passages. Verbal 5 list learning tasks usually require the patient to listen to 12 to 16 words, then immediately recall as many of the words as possible. Using the California Verbal Learning Test, normal controls can recall about 8 of 16 words after the first trial; patients with schizophrenia can recall only about 5 (Paulsen et al. 1995). After 5 consecutive trials of the same word list, most controls can recall at least 13 of the words, while patients with schizophrenia, on average, can recall only 9. Thus, patients are not only impaired in their ability to immediately recall verbal material, they are also impaired compared to controls in their ability to learn over time. Patients are also impaired in recalling more interesting verbal material, such as stories (Hoff et al. 1992). Much empirical evidence points to the connection between verbal memory impairment and social deficits in patients with schizophrenia (Green 1996). Visual Learning and Memory. Since visual information is not as easily expressed as verbal information, fewer tests sensitive to the deficits of schizophrenia have been developed, and this area of cognitive function has generally been found not to be as impaired as verbal memory (Heinrichs and Zaksanis 1998). Most tests require subjects to draw figures from memory or to indicate which among an array of figures was previously presented. Studies of the relationship between poor visual memory and functional outcome have yielded mixed findings. Visual memory has been found to correlate modestly with employment status (Gold et al. 2003), job tenure (Gold et al. 2002), psychosocial rehabilitation success (Mueser et al. 1991), social functioning (Dickerson et al. 1999), quality of life ratings (Buchanan et al. 1994), and strongly with functional capacity (Twamley et al. 2003). Other studies have reported no significant correlations (Addington and Addington 2000; Addington et al. 1998; Ertugrul and Ulug 2002; Velligan et al. 2000). 6 Reasoning and Problem-solving. While there are numerous tests of reasoning and problem solving, the most well-known and most frequently utilized in schizophrenia research is the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Heaton 1981). In this test, patients are given a deck of cards with various numbers of colored shapes on them, and are asked to match their cards to four “key” cards that have shapes on them that differ by color, form, and number. The first principle to which the subject needs to learn to sort the cards is color. After a patient demonstrates that he has learned that color is the correct sorting principle, the principle changes to form without warning. Repeated sorting attempts by the previously correct principle are referred to as perseverations, and patients with schizophrenia, like patients with frontal lobe damage, often make many of these errors. In fact, the very poor performance of patients with schizophrenia on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Goldberg et al. 1987), and the reduced activity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during performance of this test (Weinberger et al. 1987) led to widespread pursuit of the hypothesis of frontal hypoactivation in schizophrenia. It is important to note, however, that the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test measures a variety of cognitive functions and is not a “pure” measure of executive functions (Keefe 1995) The rules of society and the workplace change regularly, and success in these arenas is often measured by one’s ability to adapt to changes. Patients with schizophrenia who are impaired on measures of executive functions have difficulty adapting to the rapidly changing world around them. Speed of processing. Many neurocognitive tests require subjects to process information rapidly and can be compromised by impairments in processing speed. A standard example of this type of task is the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Digit Symbol Test (Wechsler et al. 1997). Each numeral (1 through 9) is associated with a different simple symbol. Subjects are required to 7 copy as many of the symbols associated with the numerals as possible in 90 seconds. This nonspecific cognitive impairment has been found to correlate with a variety of clinically important features of schizophrenia, such as daily life activities (Evans et al. 2003), job tenure (Gold et al. 2002), and independent living status (Brekke et al. 1997). It is also sensitive to medication side effects such as somnolence and extrapyramidal symptoms (Galletly et al. 2000). Reduced processing speed can impair ability to keep in step with the task-oriented jobs that are frequently held by patients with schizophrenia. Increased response latency in social settings may hamper social relationships. Verbal Fluency. Most cognitive assessments in treatment studies of schizophrenia have included measures of verbal fluency as a separate domain of functioning (Harvey and Keefe 2001; Keefe et al. 1999; Meltzer and McGurk 1999). Most of these tests measure either phonological fluency (also referred to as letter fluency) or semantic fluency. Phonological fluency refers to a patient’s ability to produce as many words as possible beginning with a particular letter within for instance 60 seconds. Semantic fluency refers to the ability to produce words within a particular meaning-based category, such as “vegetables.” Not only do schizophrenia patients produce fewer words than normal controls, but they often produce inappropriate examples, such as examples of fruits instead of vegetables. Impaired verbal fluency can damage functioning in social and vocational settings by making communication difficult and awkward. Immediate / Working Memory. Immediate memory refers to the ability to hold a limited amount of information "on-line" for a brief period of time (usually a few seconds). Repeating a string of digits (Digits Forwards) is an example of immediate memory. The definition of working memory, on the other hand, is more complex and varies across studies. Some investigators 8 consider working memory to be synonymous with immediate memory, whereas others describe that it should require some manipulation of the information being held on-line. For example, repeating a series of digits in the reverse order than they were presented (Digits Backward) requires an active manipulation because the information needs to be both held on-line and then subsequently re-ordered. Patients typically show deficits on both tasks. These visual tasks involve keeping a visuospatial stimulus, such as a dot on a computer screen, or series of visual objects in mind while working on a related or unrelated task (reviewed in Keefe 2001). Working memory has been described by various authors as a core component of the cognitive impairment in schizophrenia (Goldman-Rakic 1994; Keefe 2001; Silver et al. 2003), and is related to functional outcomes such as employment status (McGurk and Meltzer 2000), and job tenure (Gold et al. 2002). Much of the clinical relevance of working memory deficits in schizophrenia comes from strong correlations that working memory measures have with a variety of other cognitive domains impaired in schizophrenia, such as attention, planning, memory (reviewed in Keefe 2001), and intelligence (Baddeley 1992), as well as the advanced understanding of the neuroanatomy of working memory functions in human and non-human primates. This neuroanatomical work has suggested that neural circuitry that includes prefrontal cortical regions mediates aspects of working memory functions (Callicott et al. 1999; GoldmanRakic 1987), and that this circuitry may be impaired in schizophrenia (Callicott et al. 1999). Social Cognition. Theory-of-mind skills and social perception have been the general focus of the literature on social cognition in schizophrenia. Theory-of-mind is the ability to infer about another’s intentions and/or to represent the mental states of others. Individuals with schizophrenia perform poorly on measures of theory-of-mind abilities (Corcoran, Mercer, and Frith 1995; Drury, Robinson, and Birchwood 1998; Sarfati et al. 1997). The evidence regarding 9 whether impairments in theory-of-mind skills are independent of a general cognitive deficit is mixed (Doody et al. 1998; Pinkham et al. 2003). Facial affect recognition and social cue perception are the two general areas into which studies of social perception in schizophrenia can be broken down. Reviews of the literature on facial affect recognition (Morrison, Bellack, and Mueser 1988; Penn et al. 1997) suggest that individuals with schizophrenia have stable deficits on tests of facial affect perception compared with healthy controls and psychiatric controls, and that perception of negative emotions and fear may be particularly impaired (Addington and Addington 1998; Edwards et al. 2001; Gaebel and Wolwer 1992). Tests of social cue perception use more dynamic stimuli that require multiple sensory modalities, such as watching videotapes of persons interacting. Patients with schizophrenia show consistent impairments on these tasks (Bell, Bryson, and Lysaker 1997; Corrigan, Davies-Farmer, and Stolley 1990). In particular, they have more difficulty discerning other individuals’ goals and intentions than what they are wearing or saying. A variety of reports have described the strong relationship between cognitive function and social deficits in schizophrenia (reviewed by Trumbetta and Meuser 2000). Even so, there is growing evidence that social cognition is related to social impairments in schizophrenia, even after controlling for performance on neurocognitive tasks (Penn et al. 1996). Summary Experts in cognition and schizophrenia have come to a clear consensus that cognitive impairment is a core feature of the illness. The profile of deficits is broad, severe, and is likely present in most if not all patients. Neurocognitive impairment has clear clinical relevance, as cognitive impairment interferes with the everyday lives of patients in various important ways, from limiting social relationships to reducing the likelihood of employment. 10 SECTION II. Natural History of Neurocognitive Impairment Evidence shows that the time course of the development of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia patients appears to follow a predictable pattern. Some deficits may be present in childhood, followed by a decline in cognitive function prior to the first episode. The severity of neurocognitive impairments becomes even more severe once psychosis develops. The long-term stability of neurocognitive impairment over time is not clear, but evidence for progression in non-elderly patients is lacking. Deficits in Children at Risk for Schizophrenia and Follow-back Studies Individuals who are genetically vulnerable to schizophrenia have notable cognitive impairments (Cornblatt and Keilp 1994), while individuals who are examined with cognitive assessments before they develop schizophrenia are found to have impairments in a variety of areas (Davidson et al. 1999). High-risk studies of children with one or two biological parents with schizophrenia (Cornblatt et al. 1999) have suggested that attention deficits can predict which children will develop schizophrenia in the future. Several studies have utilized the “follow-back” method of identifying adult patients with schizophrenia, and then linking these patients with their records of cognitive assessments performed while they were children or adolescents (Fuller et al. 2002; Jones et al. 1994; Davidson et al. 1999). In the UK National Survey of Health and Development study, data from 5,362 people indicated that children who went on to develop schizophrenia as adults differed significantly from the general population in a wide range of cognitive and behavioral domains (Jones et al. 1994). Low educational test scores in verbal, non-verbal, and mathematics/arithmetic at all ages assessed were significant risk factors. Similar findings were generated from a population-based study that investigated the risk of schizophrenia in a sample 11 of 50,000 18-year-old males conscripted into the Swedish Army between 1969 and 1970 (David et al. 1997). In the U.S., scores from grades 4, 8, and 11 on the Iowa Tests for 70 children who later developed schizophrenia show no significant differences from controls at grades 4 and 8. However, for children who developed schizophrenia, test scores dropped significantly between grades 8 and 11, corresponding to the onset of puberty (Fuller et al. 2002). Yet limitations of this study including small sample size and a non-random sample restrict the generalizability of these results. Several studies have utilized a link between the Israeli Draft Board Registry and the National Psychiatric Hospitalization Case Registry. Israeli law requires that all adolescents between the ages of 16-17 undergo pre-induction assessment to determine their intellectual, medical, and psychiatric eligibility for military service. This assessment is compulsory and is administered to the entire, unselected, population of Israeli adolescents. It includes individuals who will be eligible for military service, as well as those who will be excused from service based on medical, psychiatric, or social reasons. The results suggest that cognitive functions are significantly impaired in those adolescents who are later hospitalized for schizophrenia. These deficits thus precede the onset of psychosis in young people destined to develop schizophrenia, and along with social isolation and organizational ability, cognitive deficits are a significant predictor of which young people will eventually develop a psychotic disorder (Davidson et al. 1999). One study from this series examined the cognitive performance of 44 patients with a first episode of schizophrenia who had previously undergone cognitive assessment as part of their registration with the Israeli Draft Board. The stability of the deficits in these patients suggests that most of the cognitive impairment seen occurs prior to the first psychotic episode (Caspi et al. 12 2003). These follow-back studies consistently demonstrate that cognitive impairment precedes the onset of illness in schizophrenia and schizoaffective illness. Prodrome Studies Cognitive deficits are also found in individuals who are identified as being at “ultra-high” risk (Yung and McGorry 1996) for schizophrenia by virtue of their family history of schizophrenia and/or the manifestation of mild signs and symptoms consistent with the prodromal symptoms of schizophrenia. While several research groups are gathering data on this question, results have only recently begun to be reported (Brewer et al. 2003; Hawkins et al. 2004). Preliminary data also suggests that olfactory identification deficits predict which individuals at ultra-high risk will develop schizophrenia (Brewer et al. 2003). First Episode of Psychosis Studies Once psychosis develops, cognitive deficits are severe (Mohamed et al. 1999; Bilder et al. 2000; DeLisi et al. 1995; Hoff et al. 1999; and Stirling et al. 2003). Patients with a first episode of schizophrenia who have never taken antipsychotic medication, already exhibit cognitive impairment (Brickman et al, in press; Mohamed et al. 1999; Saykin et al. 1994). Motor functions and language functions may be more mildly impaired prior to medication treatment (Brickman et al, in press). Initial treatment with first-generation antipsychotic medications does little to change these cognitive deficits (Bilder et al. 2000). Cognitive Changes in Patients Recovering from Acute Exacerbation Several studies have investigated the longitudinal course of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia by repeatedly assessing cognition in samples that combine first episode patients and those who have had an acute exacerbation of their symptoms. Patients recovering from an acute exacerbation of illness do not appear to demonstrate substantial changes in the severity of 13 their cognitive impairment despite clear improvements in symptoms with treatment (Hughes et al. 2003; Nopoulos et al. 1994; Gold S. et al. 1999; Sweeney et al. 1991). Longitudinal Change in Cognitive Function There are many age-related changes in cognitive functioning in the normal population, especially in motor speed and memory functions (Moss and Albert 1988). The magnitude of change in long-term memory and ability to access and utilize previously learned material is more modest. Reading and vocabulary are tests of “old learning” therefore baseline functioning can be estimated from performance on these tests which show minimal aging-related changes. The possibility of detecting cognitive decline with aging is diminished due to floor effects on numerous tests. The types of cognitive functions that change with normal aging, such as new learning and recall, attention, and processing speed are related to the deficits most commonly observed in schizophrenia patients early in life. Thus, aging-related changes are likely to affect those functions that are already severely impaired in adults with schizophrenia (Harvey 1999).The notion of neurodegeneration in schizophrenia has been controversial. Evidence has been presented suggesting that schizophrenia is a neurodegenerative process, and some have concluded that schizophrenia is progressive (Lieberman 1999). Supportive evidence for this theory derives from the greater cognitive impairment reported in chronic patients compared with first episode patients (Saykin et al. 1994). However, with a few notable exceptions (Bilder et al. 1992; Davidson et al. 1995; O’Donnell et al. 1995), cross-sectional studies generally have not found evidence of increased cognitive impairment in association with duration of illness, and older non-elderly patients do not manifest greater impairment than younger patients (Goldberg et al. 1993b; Heaton et al. 1987; Hyde et al. 1994; Zorrilla et al. 2000). Definitive answers to questions regarding possible progression of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia must come 14 from longitudinal studies. The available studies, however, have a variety of methodological limitations such as small or unrepresentative samples, no controls, limited assessment, or relatively short follow-up periods. One recent longitudinal study traced 111 first-episode schizophrenia patients over 10-12 years and finding that Visuospatial function while spared in the first episode may deteriorate over time while executive deficits do not (Stirling et al. 2004). These longitudinal studies of neurocognitive impairment suggest that it is a stable, enduring feature of the illness, with very little change in chronic patients between assessment periods of up to 5 years duration (Heaton et al. 2001; Rund 1998) Cognitive Functioning in Elderly Patients There is some evidence that neurocognitive impairment in schizophrenia may worsen over time in at least a subgroup of elderly patients with schizophrenia. In older schizophrenic patients with a lifetime of poor functional outcome, prominent cognitive impairments resembling dementia have been reported (Arnold et al. 1995; Davidson et al. 1995; Harvey et al. 1997). Based upon cross-sectional studies, elderly patients with schizophrenia appear to show some decline in cognitive function toward the end of life. However, this decline may be restricted to those patients who had an early onset of illness followed by a lifetime of poor functioning (Heaton et al. 1994; Hyde et al. 1994; Jeste et al. 1995; Zorrilla et al. 2000). Some of the inconsistency of these results may derive from the subject selection processes in these studies. Finally, cognitive decline in some elderly patients with schizophrenia has been found to be associated with tardive dyskinesia (Waddington and Youssef 1996) and neurological dysfunction (Goldstein and Zubin 1990). Summary Several studies have demonstrated that neurocognitive impairment is present in a mild 15 form prior to the onset of psychosis in young people destined to develop schizophrenia. Neurocognitive impairment is severe in patients who have experienced their first psychotic episode, even before antipsychotic treatment is initiated. While some early phase patients may demonstrate a slight improvement in neurocognitive impairment with treatment, many patients do not improve at all. Following this early phase, neurocognitive impairment appears to be remarkably consistent, even in the presence of positive and negative symptom change, although few longitudinal studies have had follow-up periods of longer than 5 years. The course of neurocognitive impairment in elderly patients in uncertain, but some studies suggest that patients with the most chronic courses of illness may manifest a further, albeit gradual, cognitive decline in the latest years of life. Early treatment with second generation antipsychotics could potentially improve long term cognitive and functional outcome and change the longitudinal course of illness described above. We will return to this topic of a greater cognitive enhancing effect of second generation antipsychotics in the section entitled Impact of Second Generation Antipsychotics on Cognition. III. Relationship of Neurocognitive Impairment to Schizophrenia Symptoms Overview and Introduction of Pseudospecificity This section will address the relationship between neurocognitive impairment and symptoms. If neurocognitive deficits were the result of the symptoms of the illness, then the deficits would disappear when the symptoms do. However, this is usually not the case in patients with schizophrenia. In contrast to patients with psychotic bipolar illness whose performance on cognitive tests may improve when their psychotic symptoms remitted, the performance of the patients with schizophrenia does not change when psychosis remits (Harvey et al. 1990). 16 The impact of first generation antipsychotics on cognition is very weak (Blyler and Gold 2000). The recalcitrance of neurocognitive impairment in the context of substantial symptom improvement is one of the most compelling lines of evidence of the independence of these symptom domains. However, recent studies investigating second-generation antipsychotics raise an important question about the potential “pseudospecificity” of cognitive improvement: If second-generation antipsychotics improve cognition and symptoms, is the cognitive improvement explained by the improvement in symptoms? While substantial improvements in symptoms and mild improvements in cognition may be found with some second-generation drugs, the correlations between these two domains of improvement have largely been found not to be statistically significant (Bilder et al. 2002). Cross-sectional Studies of the Relation to Symptoms of Schizophrenia Neurocognitive deficits are largely separate from both the positive and negative symptoms of the disorder. Cross-sectional relationships between neurocognition and positive symptoms, particularly for hallucinations and delusions, are usually quite weak (Addington and Addington 2000; Hughes et al. 2003; Strauss 1993). Generally, the variance shared by these variables is less than 10 percent. Relationships tend to be slightly stronger between neurocognition and negative or ‘deficit’ symptoms (Addington and Addington 2000; Buchanan et al. 1997; Hughes et al. 2003; Tamlyn et al. 1992). Positive Symptoms It has been repeatedly reported that neurocognitive ability is not strongly correlated with severity of psychotic symptoms in patients with schizophrenia (Addington et al. 1991; Bilder et al. 1985; Strauss 1993). In acute exacerbation, the severity of positive symptoms has been found to be significantly correlated with better performance on select domains of cognitive function. 17 Although some exceptions exist, such as isolated reports of significant correlations of positive symptoms with working memory (Bressi et al. 1996; Carter et al. 1996), source monitoring (Keefe et al. 2002), and auditory distractibility (Walker and Lewine 1988), the overall trend is for general neurocognitive impairment not to be correlated with positive symptoms. This low correlation across various patient samples, including first episode (Mohamed et al. 1999), chronic (Addington, Addington, and Maticka-Tyndale 1991; Tamlyn et al. 1992), and elderly (Davidson et al. 1995) samples, suggests that positive symptoms are clearly not the sole cause of the cognitive impairment found in patients with schizophrenia. The low reliability of positive symptom assessment is a crucial factor (Strauss 1993). The subjective report of a psychotic patient may not reflect the true level of the patient’s psychosis. Another factor may be that the exclusion of patients who are too psychotic to be tested may serve to weaken any potential correlation between the severity of psychosis and neurocognitive impairment. Finally, those patients who have more intact cognitive abilities may be better able to recall and express their internal state including detailed delusions and hallucinations. These patients would thus receive higher scores on positive symptom rating scales, resulting in a reduction in any potential correlation between positive symptoms and neurocognitive impairment. Clearly, there are limitations to the assessment of positive symptoms, and the absence of cross-sectional correlations between positive symptoms severity and neurocognitive impairment is complex. Negative Symptoms Cognitive dysfunction is significantly correlated with various types of negative symptoms (Addington, Addington, and Maticka-Tyndale 1991; Cuesta et al. 1995; Morris et al. 1995; Strauss 1993; Summerfelt et al. 1991; Tamlyn et al. 1992). The greater variance shared between 18 neurocognition and negative symptoms compared with positive symptoms may result from measurement overlap. For instance, the neurocognitive variable verbal fluency and the negative symptom variable poverty of speech both measure the speed at which a patient generates speech. A patient who generates speech at a slow rate will do so during a test of verbal fluency as well as in an interview during which he is being rated for poverty of speech. Empirical studies suggest that motor functions are strongly correlated with negative symptoms (Cuesta et al. 1995; Manschreck et al. 1985), job success (McGurk et al. 2003) and outcome (Bilder et al. 1985). Deficient motor skills are represented in both the negative symptom and the cognitive dysfunction domain, since symptoms such as blunted affect and motor retardation are observational measures of motor functioning (Alpert 1985; Andreasen 1989). Thus, impaired motor skills in many ways lie at the core of negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Whether the negative symptom of reduced motivation underlies the poor performance of patients with schizophrenia on cognitive tests is controversial. Though monetary reinforcement has been shown to improve performance on effortful cognitive tests such as the WCST in some studies (Summerfelt et al. 1991), especially in less difficult tasks, others have not (Green et al. 1990). Increases in pupil size are associated with increased cognitive processing demands (Granholm et al. 1996). Therefore, pupillary response can measure engagement in a task. If the cognitive deficits of schizophrenic patients were due to lack of interest or motivation, their pupillary response would be low throughout the period of cognitive assessment. Only during high processing conditions do patients have abnormal pupillary responses, suggesting they put forth a normal amount of effort when being given cognitive tests, yet their decreased processing capacity leads them to be unable to engage in difficult tasks (Granholm et al. 1997). 19 On the contrary, cognitive deficits may cause reduced motivation. Individuals with cognitive deficits are less likely to be motivated to have goals and pursue them (reviewed by Deci and Flaste 1996). Patients with neurocognitive impairment are likely to be met with failure if they attempt to pursue employment, social, and even recreational avenues that require cognitive skill. Repeated failures are likely to cause discouragement and reduced motivation in people with schizophrenia. In sum, a cross-sectional correlation of negative symptoms and neurocognitive impairment has been consistently reported, but the magnitude of this relation is modest. Formal Thought Disorder Deficits in semantic memory may lie at the heart of the cognition—thought disorder relation (Elvevag et al. 2002). This argument has recently been supported by empirical data suggesting that the difference between semantic fluency (the number of names of animals produced in 60 seconds) and phonological fluency (the number of words beginning with “F” produced in 60 seconds), an indication of the severity of the impairment of the “semantic network” in schizophrenic patients, predicted the severity of their formal thought disorder (Goldberg et al. 1998). Thus, a patient’s ability to have verbal information available (referred to as “semantic priming”) may be the most important cognitive factor in formal thought disorder. Affective symptoms Since many patients with schizophrenia also report depressed mood (Jin et al. 2001) if not a full depressive disorder, and since depression is associated with some cognitive impairment (Goldberg et al. 1993a), the role of depressed mood in neurocognitive impairment is important. Very few studies have examined this relationship directly. Furthermore, the distinction between depressive symptoms and negative symptoms is sometimes difficult for raters and clinicians to 20 make (McKenna et al. 1989; Goldman et al. 1992), yet factor analyses suggest that they are separate dimensions of schizophrenia (Lindenmayer et al. 1995; Van der Does et al. 1995). Higher depression scores were significantly correlated with worse verbal memory task performance, which remained even when psychomotor retardation and processing speed performance were controlled statistically (Brebion et al. 2001). Thus, depression may influence the association between negative symptoms and cognitive impairments, and it may also have a direct deleterious effect on some aspects of neurocognitive impairment. In conclusion, the cross-sectional correlations between neurocognitive impairment and symptoms are weaker than might be expected, and vary depending upon symptom domain. The consistency of this finding strongly supports the idea that neurocognitive impairment is not caused by psychosis. Longitudinal Studies The relative independence of neurocognitive impairment and symptoms in crosssectional studies appears to be supported by longitudinal studies. When patients with schizophrenia are successfully treated with first-generation antipsychotics, the severity of their symptoms is substantially reduced (Lieberman et al. 2003). However, if neurocognitive function improves with first-generation antipsychotic treatment, the effects are small (Blyler and Gold 2000; DeLisi et al. 1995; Gold S. et al. 1999; Hoff et al. 1999). It is important to emphasize here that the stability of neurocognitive impairment occurs in the context of frequent variability of the symptoms of schizophrenia, especially the positive symptoms (Bilder et al. 2000). For example, in a study of patients tested during a psychotic exacerbation and then again when symptoms had improved considerably one to two years later, cognitive function was found to be very stable over time (Nopoulos et al. 1994). In a longitudinal 21 study over a one-year interval, cognitive and negative symptoms were correlated at each of the assessments, but there was no predictive relationship between these variables over time (Harvey et al. 1996). If negative symptoms were somehow causing poor performance on cognitive tests, then a longitudinal relationship between these variables would have been expected. Second, when the differential relationship between cognitive and negative symptoms and adaptive functioning deficits has been examined, cognitive impairments have been found to be more strongly correlated with functional deficit than negative symptoms (Harvey et al. 1996; Harvey et al. 1998). Third, in those patients whose functional status worsens over time, changes in cognitive impairment predicts the level of change while changes in negative symptoms do not (Harvey et al. 1998). These data suggesting a variable relationship of symptoms and neurocognitive impairment provide yet another line of support for the relative independence of these domains. Another way to investigate the longitudinal relationship between neurocognitive impairment and symptoms is to calculate the correlation between change in neurocognitive impairment and symptom change. This approach addresses the issue of ‘pseudospecificity’. Several studies have performed such analyses and though results are mixed, the findings have generally suggested that symptom change does not contribute substantially to cognitive change (Addington, Addington and Maticka-Tyndale 1991; Hoff et al. 1999). Impact of Second Generation Antipsychotics on Cognition Patients with schizophrenia were treated with excessive doses of conventional antipsychotic medication for years. This may have had direct adverse cognitive effects and led clinicians to use anticholinergic medications to control side effects. Lowering dosages of conventional antipsychotic medications has some modest cognitive benefit (Seidman, Pepple, 22 and Faraone 1993) which suggests that use of excessive doses may have led to some of the failures of conventional medications to improve cognition (Harvey et al., in press). When compared with first generation antipsychotics, second generation antipsychotics have some cognitive benefits (Harvey and Keefe 2001; Keefe et al. 1999). This issue is still controversial as many of the studies are methodologically limited. For instance, many comparisons of the cognitive benefit of second generation and first generation antipsychotics include excessive doses of first generation antipsychotic medication, which may unfairly inflate the benefits attributable to second generation antipsychotics by confusing them with dose effects. Further, there is evidence that unlike first generation antipsychotics, second generation medications may allow patients to benefit from practice-related improvements (Harvey et al. 2000). However, recent studies of first episode patients on lower doses of first generation antipsychotics suggest that second generation antipsychotics have a greater cognitive enhancing effect (Keefe et al, in press; Harvey et al, submitted). Summary In conclusion, the literature on neurocognitive impairment consistently demonstrates that cognitive impairment is not caused by the symptoms of schizophrenia. Most impressive is the zero-to-mild cross-sectional correlations between neurocognitive impairment and the positive symptoms of the illness. While other aspects of schizophrenia, such as negative symptoms, appear to correlate with neurocognitive impairment, there is as much or more evidence supporting the idea that neurocognitive impairment causes negative symptoms as there is evidence that negative symptoms cause neurocognitive impairment. While there is limited evidence of a relationship between changes in neurocognitive impairment and changes in symptoms over time, there is certainly no clear evidence that improvements in neurocognitive 23 impairment are caused by improvements in symptoms. In fact, most data point to the relative independence of these two targets of treatment in schizophrenia. IV. Clinical Importance of Neurocognitive Impairment. Cross-sectional Correlations with Function Although the literature on neurocognitive impairment suggests that it is not strongly correlated with symptoms, it has proven to be consistently related to a variety of other important aspects of the illness, such as social functioning (Liberman, Mueser, and Wallace 1986; Spaulding et al. 1986), functional impairments, unemployment (Brekke et al. 1997; Lysaker and Bell 1995; McGurk and Meltzer 2000; Velligan et al. 2000), quality of life (Fujii and Wylie 2003), relapse prevention (Fenton, Blyler, and Heinssen 1997; Jarboe and Schwartz 1999), medical status and economic cost (Knapp 1997; Sevy and Davidson 1995). Neurocognitive impairment also has considerable power to predict functional status years later. Relationship of Neurocognitive Impairment to Functioning Functional outcome in schizophrenia is difficult to define and measure. The three types of functional outcome that most studies of neurocognitive deficits have examined are: community (social and occupational) outcome, the ability to solve simulations of interpersonal interactions, and success in psychosocial rehabilitation programs. Strong support for associations between key areas of neurocognition and functional outcome are offered by two reviews of the literature (Green 1996; Green et al. 2000). The connection between actual community functioning and cognition has been weaker than the strong link between laboratory measures of community functioning or skills acquisition and cognition (Green 1996). In a one-year follow-up study of patients with first episode 24 psychosis, the size of the correlations between the number and quality of actual social relations and various cognitive measures were found to be in the .25 to .35 range (Malla et al. 2002). Across the studies reviewed by Green (2000), all of the key neurocognitive constructs (secondary memory, immediate memory, vigilance, and executive functioning / card sorting) had significant relationships to functional outcome and effect sizes in the medium range. Cognitive impairments are also correlated with deficits in the performance of specific skills critical for independent living (Evans et al. 2003; Patterson et al. 2001). Neurocognitive Impairment and Unemployment Several studies of cognitive performance and vocational functioning have been conducted in rehabilitation settings (Bell and Bryson 2001; Lysaker and Bell 1995). Ratings of work behavior/performance are related to baseline scores on verbal memory tests and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Additionally, improvement in patient work performance in a 6 month work rehabilitation program was predicted by baseline performance on various cognitive tests. A number of positive findings have been reported in studies examining correlates of competitive employment (Beiser et al. 1994; Brekke et al. 1997; Velligan et al. 2000). Patients enrolled in school full-time or holding competitive employment show superior performance across measures of working memory, sustained attention, problem solving, and episodic memory when compared to unemployed patients (McGurk and Meltzer 2000), with scores of part-time workers falling between the other two groups. In addition, vocational functioning is significantly associated with performance on speed of processing tasks such as Trailmaking A and B (McGurk et al. 2003). McGurk and Meltzer (2000) evaluated the relationship of cognitive functioning and work status and concluded that neurocognitive performance plays a more important role than clinical 25 symptoms in the ability of patients with schizophrenia to work. The implication is that patients with schizophrenia who have higher levels of cognitive impairment may require greater amounts of vocational support than those with lower levels of impairment (McGurk et al. 2003). Neurocognitive Impairment and Quality of Life Quality of life is often defined by the quality of social, occupational, and interpersonal aspects of life and is related to cognitive function. Some evidence suggests that reductions in quality of life are more strongly associated with cognitive deficits than other symptomatic features of the illness. Specifically, the relationship between subjective experience and social functioning has been shown to be mediated by executive functioning (Brekke, Kohrt, and Green 2001). Patients have less realistic impressions of their social functioning if they also have more severe executive deficits. The need for anticholinergic treatments is also associated with reductions in quality of life. Reductions in quality of life and anticholinergic medications may be related to the EPS that triggers anticholinergic medication prescriptions, nonetheless anticholinergic medications reliably reduce cognitive functioning as well. In patients with schizophrenia, anticholinergic medications impair attention and memory functions (Spohn and Strauss 1989). These impairments can induce reductions in subjective quality of life. One study of patients with schizophrenia that examined the relationship between coping abilities and cognitive dysfunction suggested that more severe executive and memory deficits are related to decreased use of coping mechanisms (Wilder-Willis 2002). Fujii and Wylie (2003) studied the long-term effects of neurocognition on quality of life in patients with severe schizophrenia. They found that neurocognition does, in fact, have long- 26 term predictive validity for quality of life and that therapeutics targeting neurocognition could improve quality of life in this population. Neurocognitive Impairment and Relapse Prevention Cognitive functions have been shown to be the strongest predictors of patients' ability to manage medications (Jeste et al. 2003). Cognitive deficits contribute to patterns of medication mismanagement that are associated with poor adherence and risk of relapse (Fenton, Blyler, and Heinssen 1997; Jarboe and Schwartz 1999). In one study, memory impairment was the best predictor of partial compliance (Donohoe et al. 2001). Patients performing poorly in medication management tests also had poor global scores on a dementia inventory (Patterson et al. 2002). Decreased medication compliance in schizophrenic patients has been shown to be related to poor performance on tests of attention and visual memory (Jarboe and Schwartz 1999). Neurocognitive Impairment and Medical co-morbidity Neurocognitive Impairment is also related to medical co-morbidities in schizophrenia. Deficits in organization (executive skills) directly affects patients’ ability to seek treatment for medical problems. In elderly patients with schizophrenia, cognitive and functional impairments predicted the later incidence of new-onset medical problems, while medical problems did not predict the subsequent worsening of cognitive and self-care deficits (Friedman et al. 2002). Inability of patients with schizophrenia to reduce damaging habits such as smoking has been correlated with deficits in memory and attention (Buchanan, Holstein, and Breier 1994; George et al. 2000). Thus, cognitive impairments were shown to directly effect new-onset medical problems in older patients. The Costs of Neurocognitive Impairment 27 A major factor in the costs (direct and indirect) associated with schizophrenia is cognitive impairment (Sevy and Davidson 1995). Factors leading to the increased cost include loss of ability for self-care, level of inpatient and outpatient care needed, and loss of productivity (for both patient and caretaker). Although cognitive impairment by itself is rarely a reason for acute hospitalization of a patient with schizophrenia, it may contribute to the overall length of hospitalization. It may lead to early admission to nursing homes or long-term care facilities in the case of elderly patients. V. Conclusion A consensus has developed amongst experts in cognition and schizophrenia that cognitive impairment is a core feature of the illness. The course of neurocognitive impairment follows a characteristic pattern and following the early phase, is consistent even when positive and negative symptoms change. Consistent with the demonstration that cognitive impairments are not a result of symptoms of schizophrenia, on the whole studies show the independence of these two targets for treatment. Long term functional and cognitive outcome as well as the longitudinal course of the illness could potentially improve with early treatment using second generation antipsychotics. Functional status years later can be predicted with considerable accuracy with the extent of neurocognitive impairments. The relationship has been established between cognition and many other facets of schizophrenia from social functioning to unemployment to relapse prevention. Indeed, improved understanding and treatment of neurocognitive impairments in schizophrenia holds considerable promise for the field and for the lives of patients with schizophrenia. 28 References Addington J, Addington D: Neurocognitive and social functioning in schizophrenia: a 2.5 year follow-up study. Schizophr Res 44(1):47-56, 2000 Addington J, Addington D: Facial affect recognition and information processing in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophr Res 32:171-181, 1998 Addington J, McCleary L, Munroe-Blum H: Relationship between cognitive and social dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 34(1-2):59-66, 1998 Addington, J, Addington, D, Maticka-Tyndale, E: Cognitive functioning and positive and negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 5(2):123-134, 1991 Alpert M: The signs and symptoms of schizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry 26(2):103-112, 1985 American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition, Text Revision. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Publishing, Incorporated, 2000 Andreasen NC: Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Br J Psychiatry 155(Suppl 7):53-58, 1989 Arnold SE, Gur RE, Shapiro RM, et al: Prospective clinicopathologic studies of schizophrenia: accrual and assessment of patients. Am J Psychiatry 152(5):731-7, 1995 Baddeley A: Working memory. Science 255(5044):556-9, 1992 29 Bell M, Bryson G, Lysaker P: Positive and negative affect recognition in schizophrenia: a comparison with substance abuse and normal control subjects. Psychiatry Res 73:73-82, 1997 Bell MD, Bryson G: Work rehabilitation in schizophrenia: does cognitive impairment limit improvement? Schizophr Bull 27:269-279, 2001 Beiser M, Bean G, Erickson D, et al: Biological and psychosocial predictors of job performance following a first episode of psychosis. Am J Psychiatry 151:857-863, 1994 Bilder RM, Goldman RS, Volavka J, et al: Neurocognitive effects of clozapine, olanzapine, risperidone, and haloperidol in patients with chronic schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Am J Psychiatry 159(6):1018-28, 2002 Bilder RM, Goldman RS, Robinson D, et al: Neuropsychology of first-episode schizophrenia: initial characterization and clinical correlates. Am J Psychiatry 157(4):549-59, 2000 Bilder RM, Lipschutz-Broch L, Reiter G, et al. Intellectual deficits in first-episode schizophrenia: evidence for progressive deterioration. Schizophr Bull 18(3):437-48, 1992 Bilder RM, Mukherjee S, Rieder RO, Pandurangi AK: Symptomatic and neuropsychological components of defect states. Schizophr Bull 11(3):409-19, 1985 Blyer CR, Gold JM: Cognitive effects of conventional antipsychotics: another look, in Cognitive Functioning in Schizophrenia: Characteristics, Correlates, and Treatment. Edited by Sharma T, Harvey P. Oxford University Press, 2000, pp 241–265 Brebion G, Gorman JM, Malaspina D, et al: Clinical and cognitive factors associated with verbal memory task performance in patients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 158(5):75864, 2001 30 Brekke JS, Kohrt B, Green MF: Neuropsychological functioning as a moderator of the relationship between psychosocial functioning and the subjective experience of self and life in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 27:697-708, 2001 Brekke JS, Raine A, Ansel M, Lencz T: Neuropsychological and psychophysiological correlates of psychosocial functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 23:19-28, 1997 Bressi S, Miele L, Bressi C; Astori S; et al: Deficit of central executive component of working memory in schizophrenia. New Trends in Experimental & Clinical Psychiatry 12(4):243252, 1996 Brewer WJ, Wood SJ, McGorry PD, et al: Impairment of olfactory identification ability in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis who later develop schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 160(10):1790-4, 2003 Brickman A, Buchsbaum M, Bloom R, et al: Neuropsychological Functioning in First-Break, Never-Medicated Adolescents with Psychosis. J Nerv Ment Dis (in press) Buchanan RW, Strauss ME, Breier A, et al: Attentional impairments in deficit and nondeficit forms of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 154(3):363-70, 1997 Buchanan RW, Holstein C, Breier A: The comparative efficacy and long-term effect of clozapine treatment on neuropsychological test performance. Biol Psychiatry 36:717-725, 1994 Calev A: Recall and recognition in chronic nondemented schizophrenics: Use of matched tasks. J Abnorm Psychol 93(2):172-177, 1984 Callicott JH, Mattay VS, Bertolino A, et al: Physiological characteristics of capacity constraints in working memory as revealed by functional MRI. Cereb Cortex 9(1):20-6, 1999 31 Caspi A, Reichenberg A, Weiser M, et al: Cognitive performance in schizophrenia patients assessed before and following the first psychotic episode. Schizophr Res 65(2-3):87-94, 2003 Carter C, Robertson L, Nordahl T, et al. Spatial working memory deficits and their relationship to negative symptoms in unmedicated schizophrenia patients. Biol Psychiatry 40(9):9302, 1996 Ciompi L: The natural history of schizophrenia in the long term. Br J Psychiatry 136:413-20, 1980 Corcoran R, Mercer G, Frith CD: Schizophrenia, symptomatology and social influence: investigating "theory of mind" in people with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 17:5-13, 1995 Cornblatt B, Obuchowski M, Roberts S, et al: Cognitive and behavioral precursors of schizophrenia. Dev Psychopathol 11(3):487-508, 1999 Cornblatt BA, Keilp JG: Impaired attention, genetics, and the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 20(1):31-46, 1994 Corrigan PW, Davies-Farmer RM, Stolley MR: Social cue recognition in schizophrenia under variable levels of arousal. Cognit Ther Res 14:353-361, 1990 Cuesta MJ, Peralta V, Caro F, de Leon J: Schizophrenic syndrome and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test dimensions. Psychiatry Res 58(1):45-51, 1995 David AS, Malmberg A, Brandt L, et al: IQ and risk for schizophrenia: a population-based cohort study. Psychol Med 27(6):1311-23, 1997 32 Davidson M, Reichenberg A, Rabinowitz J, et al: Behavioral and intellectual markers for schizophrenia in apparently healthy male adolescents. Am J Psychiatry 156(9):1328-35, 1999 Davidson M, Harvey PD, Powchik P, et al: Severity of symptoms in chronically institutionalized geriatric schizophrenic patients. Am J Psychiatry 152(2):197-207, 1995 Deci EL, Flaste R: Why We Do What We Do: Understanding Self-Motivation. New York, NY, Penguin Books, 1996 DeLisi LE, Tew W, Xie S, et al: A prospective follow-up study of brain morphology and cognition in first-episode schizophrenic patients: preliminary findings. Biol Psychiatry 38(6):349-60, 1995 Dickerson F, Boronow JJ, Ringel N, Parente F: Social functioning and neurocognitive deficits in outpatients with schizophrenia: a 2-year follow-up. Schizophr Res 4;37(1):13-20, 1999 Donohoe G, Owens N, O'Donnell C, et al: Predictors of compliance with neuroleptic medication among inpatients with schizophrenia: a discriminant function analysis. Eur Psychiatry 16(5):293-8, 2001 Doody GA, Gotz M, Johnstone EC, et al: Theory of mind and psychoses. Psychol Med 28(2):397-405, 1998 Drury VM, Robinson EJ, Birchwood M: "Theory of mind" skills during an acute episode of psychosis and following recovery. Psychol Med 28:1101-1112, 1998 Edwards J, Pattison PE, Jackson HJ, Wales RJ: Facial affect and affective prosody recognition in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 48:235-253, 2001 Elvevag B, Weickert T, Wechsler M, et al: An investigation of the integrity of semantic boundaries in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 53(3):187-98, 2002 33 Ertugrul A, Ulug B: The influence of neurocognitive deficits and symptoms on disability in schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 105(3):196-201, 2002 Evans JD, Heaton RK, Paulsen JS, et al: The relationship of neuropsychological abilities to specific domains of functional capacity in older schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychiatry 53: 422-430, 2003 Fenton WS, Blyler CR, Heinssen RK: Determinants of medication compliance in schizophrenia: empirical and clinical findings. Schizophr Bull 23:637-651, 1997 Friedman JI, Harvey PD, McGurk SR, et al: Correlates of change in functional status of institutionalized geriatric schizophrenic patients: focus on medical co-morbidity. Am J Psychiatry 159:1388-1394, 2002 Fujii DE, Wylie AM: Neurocognition and community outcome in schizophrenia: long-term predictive validity. Schizophr Res 59(2-3):219-23, 2003 Fuller R, Nopoulos P, Arndt S, et al: Longitudinal assessment of premorbid cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia through examination of standardized scholastic test performance. Am J Psychiatry 159(7):1183-9, 2002 Gaebel W, Wolwer W: Facial expression and emotional face recognition in schizophrenia and depression. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 242:46-52, 1992 Galletly CA, Clark CR, McFarlane AC, Weber DL: The effect of clozapine on the speed and accuracy of information processing in schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol 24:8 1329-1338, 2000 George TP, Ziedonis, DM, Feingold A et al.. Nicotine transdermal patch and atypical antipsychotic medications for smoking cessation in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 157:1835-1842, 2000 34 Gold JM, Wilk CM, McMahon RP, et al: Working memory for visual features and conjunctions in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 112(1):61-71, 2003 Gold JM, Goldberg RW, McNary SW, et al: Cognitive Correlates of Job Tenure Among Patients With Severe Mental Illness. Am J Psychiatry 159:1395-1402, 2002 Gold S: Longitudinal study of cognitive function in first-episode and recent-onset schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 156(9):1342-8, 1999 Goldberg TE, Aloia MS, Gourovitch ML, et al: Cognitive substrates of thought disorder, I: The semantic system. Am J Psychiatry 155(12):1671-1676, 1998 Goldberg TE, Gold JM, Greenberg R, et al: Contrasts between patients with affective disorders and patients with schizophrenia on a neuropsychological test battery. Am J Psychiatry 150(9):1355-62. 1993a Goldberg TE, Hyde TM, Kleinman JE, Weinberger DR: Course of schizophrenia: neuropsychological evidence for a static encephalopathy. Schizophr Bull 19(4):797-804, 1993b Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR, Berman KF, et al: Further evidence for dementia of the prefrontal type in schizophrenia? A controlled study of teaching the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Arch Gen Psychiatry 44(11):1008-14, 1987 Goldberg TE, Ragland JD, Torrey EF, Gold JM, et al: Neuropsychological assessment of monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 47(11):1066-1072, 1990 Goldman RS, Tandon R, Liberzon I, Greden JF: Measurement of depression and negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Psychopathology 25(1):49-56, 1992 35 Goldman-Rakic PS: Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 6(4):348-57, 1994 Goldman-Rakic PS: Circuitry of the frontal association cortex and its relevance to dementia. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 6(3):299-309, 1987 Goldstein G, Zubin J: Neuropsychological differences between young and old schizophrenics with and without associated neurological dysfunction. Schizophr Res 3(2):117-26, 1990 Granholm E, Morris SK, Sarkin AJ, et al: Pupillary responses index overload of working memory resources in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 106(3):458-67, 1997 Granholm E, Asarnow RF, Sarkin AJ, Dykes KL: Pupillary responses index cognitive resource limitations. Psychophysiology 33(4):457-61, 1996 Green MF, Kern RS, Braff DL, Mintz J: Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the "right stuff"? Schizophr Bull 26:119-136, 2000 Green MF: What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry 153:321-330, 1996 Green MF, Ganzell S, Satz P, Vaclav JF: Teaching the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test to schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 47(1):91-2, 1990 Gur RE, Maany V, Mozley D, et al: Subcortical MRI volumes in neuroleptic-naive and treated patients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 155(12):1711-1717, 1998 Harvey PD, Green MF, Keefe RSE, Velligan DI: Cognitive function in schizophrenia: Its role in the definition and evaluation of effective treatments for the illness. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry (in press). Harvey PD, Keefe RSE: Studies of cognitive change in patients with schizophrenia following novel antipsychotic treatment. Am J Psychiatry 158(2):176-184, 2001 36 Harvey PD, Moriarty PJ, Serper MR, Schnur E, Lieber D: Practice-related improvement in information processing with novel antipsychotic treatment. Schizophr Res 46(2-3):13948, 2000 Harvey PD: Treatment of Cognitive Deficits in Elderly Schizophrenic Patients. in Improving Cognitive Function in the Schizophrenic Patient. Edited by Keefe RSE. Science Press, 1999 Harvey PD, Howanitz E, Parrella M, et al: Symptoms, cognitive functioning, and adaptive skills in geriatric patients with lifelong schizophrenia: a comparison across treatment sites. Am J Psychiatry 155:1080-1086, 1998 Harvey PD, Keefe RSE: Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia and implications of atypical neuroleptic treatment. CNS Spectr 2:1-11, 1997 Harvey PD, Lombardi J, Leibman M, et al: Cognitive impairment and negative symptoms in geriatric chronic schizophrenic patients: a follow-up study. Schizophr Res 22(3):223-31, 1996 Harvey PD, Keefe RS, Moskowitz J, et al. Attentional markers of vulnerability to schizophrenia: Performance of medicated and unmedicated patients and normals. Psychiatry Res 33(2):179-188, 1990 Hawkins KA, Addington J, Keefe RS, et al: Neuropsychological status of subjects at high risk for a first episode of psychosis. Schizophr Res 67(2-3):115-22, 2004 Heaton RK, Gladsjo JA, Palmer BW, et al: Stability and course of neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58(1):24-32, 2001 Heaton R, Paulsen JS, McAdams LA, et al: Neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenics. Relationship to age, chronicity, and dementia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 51(6):469-76, 1994 37 Heaton RK: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test manual. Odessa, FL, Psychological Assessment Resources, 1981 Heinrichs RW, Zakzanis KK: Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology 12:426-444, 1998 Ho BC, Alicata D, Ward J, et al: Untreated initial psychosis: relation to cognitive deficits and brain morphology in first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 160(1):142-8, 2003 Hoff AL, Sakuma M, Wieneke M, et al: Longitudinal neuropsychological follow-up study of patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 156(9):1336-41, 1999 Hoff AL, Riordan H, O'Donnell DW, et al: Neuropsychological functioning of first-episode schizophreniform patients. Am J Psychiatry 149(7):898-903, 1992 Hughes C, Kumari V, Soni W, et al: Longitudinal study of symptoms and cognitive function in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1;59(2-3):137-46, 2003 Hyde TM, Nawroz S, Goldberg TE, et al: Is there cognitive decline in schizophrenia? A crosssectional study. Br J Psychiatry 164(4):494-500, 1994 Hyman SE, Fenton WS: What Are the Right Targets for Psychopharmacology? Science 299:350351, 2003 Jarboe KS, Schwartz SK: The relationship between medication noncompliance and cognitive function in patients with schizophrenia. J Am Psychiatr Nurses Assoc 5:S2-S8, 1999 Jeste SD, Patterson TL, Palmer BW, et al: Cognitive predictors of medication adherence among middle-aged and older outpatients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 63(1-2):49-58, 2003 Jeste DV, Harris MJ, Krull A, et al: Clinical and neuropsychological characteristics of patients with late-onset schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 152(5):722-30, 1995 38 Jin H, Zisook S, Palmer BW, et al: Association of depressive symptoms and functioning in schizophrenia: A study of older outpatients. J Clin Psychiatry 62:797–803, 2001 Jones P, Rodgers B, Murray R, Marmot M: Child development risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet 344(8934):1398-402, 1994 Keefe RSE, Seidman, LJ, Christensen, BK, et al: Comparative effect of atypical and conventional antipsychotic drugs on neurocognition in first-episode psychosis: A Randomized double-blind trial of olanzapine versus haloperidol. Am J Psychiatry (in press). Keefe RS, Arnold MC, Bayen UJ, et al: Source-monitoring deficits for self-generated stimuli in schizophrenia: multinomial modeling of data from three sources. Schizophr Res 57(1):51-67, 2002 Keefe RSE: Neurocognition. in Current Issues in the Psychopharmacology of Schizophrenia. Edited by Breier A, Tran PV, Herrera J, Bymaster F, Tollefson G. Baltimore: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Publishers, 2001, pp 192-208 Keefe, RSE: The assessment of neurocognitive treatment response and its relation to negative symptoms in schizophrenia. in Negative Symptom and Cognitive Deficit Treatment Response in Schizophrenia. Edited by Keefe RSE, Mcevoy JP. Washington: APA, 2000, pp 85-110 Keefe RSE, Silva SG, Perkins DO, Lieberman JA: The effects of atypical antipsychotic drugs on neurocognitive impairment in schizophrenia: A review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull 25:2 201-222, 1999 Keefe RSE: The contribution of neuropsychology to psychiatry. Am J Psychiatry 152(1):6-15, 1995 39 Knapp M: Costs of schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 171:509-518, 1997 Kremen WS, Seidman LJ, Faraone SV, et al: The paradox of normal neuropsychological function in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 109:743-752, 2000 Lieberman JA, Tollefson G, Tohen M, et al: Comparative efficacy and safety of atypical and conventional antipsychotic drugs in first-episode psychosis: a randomized, double-blind trial of olanzapine versus haloperidol. Am J Psychiatry 160(8):1396-404, 2003 Lieberman JA: Is schizophrenia a neurodegenerative disorder? A clinical and neurobiological perspective. Biol Psychiatry 46(6):729-39, 1999 Liberman RP, Mueser KT, Wallace CJ: Social skills training for schizophrenic individuals at risk for relapse. Am J Psychiatry. 1986 Apr;143(4):523-6, 1986 Lindenmayer JP, Grochowski S, Hyman RB: Five factor model of schizophrenia: replication across samples. Schizophr Res 14(3):229-34, 1995 Lysaker P, Bell M: Work rehabilitation and improvements in insight in schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis 183:103-106, 1995 Malla AK, Norman RMG, Manchanda LT: Symptoms, cognition, treatment adherence and functional outcome in first-episode psychosis. Psychol Med 32(6):1109-1119, 2002 Manschreck TC, Maher BA, Waller NG, et al: Deficient motor synchrony in schizophrenic disorders: clinical correlates. Biol Psychiatry 20(9):990-1002, 1985 McGurk SR, Mueser KT, Harvey PD, et al: Cognitive and symptom predictors of work outcomes for clients with schizophrenia in supported employment. Psychiatr Serv 54(8):1129-35, 2003 McGurk SR, Meltzer HY: The role of cognition in vocational functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 45:175-184, 2000 40 McKenna PJ, Lund CE, Mortimer AM: Negative symptoms: relationship to other schizophrenic symptom classes. Br J Psychiatry 7:104-7, 1989 Meltzer HY, McGurk SR: The effects of clozapine, risperidone, and olanzapine on cognitive function in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 25(2):233-55, 1999 Mohamed S, Paulsen JS, O'Leary D, et al: Generalized cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: a study of first-episode patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 56(8):749-54, 1999 Morris RG, Rushe T, Woodruffe PW, Murray RM: Problem solving in schizophrenia: a specific deficit in planning ability. Schizophr Res 14(3):235-46, 1995 Morrison RL, Bellack AS, Mueser KT: Deficits in facial-affect recognition and schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 14:67-83, 1988 Moss M, Albert M: Geriatric Neuropsychology, Edited by Albert M, Moss M. New York, Guilford Press, 1988 Mueser KT, Bellack AS, Douglas MS, Wade JH: Prediction of social skill acquisition in schizophrenic and major affective disorder patients from memory and symptomatology. Psychiatry Res 37(3):281-96, 1991 Nopoulos P, Flashman L, Flaum M, Arndt S, Andreasen N: Stability of cognitive functioning early in the course of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 14(1):29-37, 1994 O'Donnell BF, Faux SF, McCarley RW, et al: Increased rate of P300 latency prolongation with age in schizophrenia. Electrophysiological evidence for a neurodegenerative process. Arch Gen Psychiatry 52(7):544-9, 1995 Palmer BW, Heaton RK, Paulsen JS, et al: Is it possible to be schizophrenic yet neuropsychologically normal? Neuropsychology 11:437-446, 1997 41 Patterson TL, Lacro J, McKibbin CL, et al: Medication management ability assessment: results from a performance-based measure in older outpatients with schizophrenia. J Clin Psychopharmacol 22:11-19, 2002 Patterson TL, Goldman S, McKibbin CL, et al: UCSD performance-based skills assessment: development of a new measure of everyday functioning for severely mentally ill adults. Schizophr Bull 27:235-245, 2001 Paulsen JS, Heaton RK, Sadek JR, et al: The nature of learning and memory impairments in schizophrenia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1(1):88-99, 1995 Penn DL, Corrigan PW, Bentall RP, et al: Social cognition in schizophrenia. Psychol Bull 121:114-132, 1997 Penn DL, Spaulding W, Reed D, Sullivan M: The relationship of social cognition to ward behavior in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 20:327-335, 1996 Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Perkins DO, Lieberman J: Implications for the neural basis of social cognition for the study of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 160(5):815-824, 2003 Rund BR: A review of longitudinal studies of cognitive functions in schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Bull 24(3):425-35, 1998 Sarfati Y, Hardy-Bayle MC, Nadel J, Chevalier JF, Widloecher D: Attribution of mental states to others by schizophrenic patients. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 2:1-17, 1997 Saykin AJ, Shtasel DL, Gur RE, et al: Neuropsychological deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 51(2):124-31, 1994 Saykin AJ, Gur RC, Gur RE, et al: Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia. Selective impairment in memory and learning. Arch Gen Psychiatry 48(7):618-24, 1991 42 Seidman LJ, Pepple JR, Faraone SV: Neuropsychological performance in chronic schizophrenia in response to neuroleptic dose reduction. Biol Psychiatry 33:575-584, 1993 Sevy S, Davidson M: The cost of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 17(1):13, 1995 Silver H, Feldman P, Bilker W, Gur RC: Working Memory Deficit as a Core Neuropsychological Dysfunction in Schizophrenia Am. J. Psychiatry 160:1809-1816, 2003 Spaulding WD, Storms L, Goodrich V, Sullivan M: Applications of experimental psychopathology in psychiatric rehabilitation. Schizophr Bull 12(4):560-77, 1986 Spohn HE, Strauss ME: Relation of Neuroleptic and anticholinergic medication to cognitive functions in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 98, 478-486, 1989 Stirling J, White C, Lewis S, et al: Neurocognitive function and outcome in first-episode schizophrenia: a 10-year follow-up of an epidemiological cohort. Schizophr Res 65(23):75-86, 2003 Strauss ME: Relations of symptoms to cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 19(2):215-31, 1993 Summerfelt AT, Alphs LD, Funderburk FR, et al: Impaired Wisconsin Card Sort performance in schizophrenia may reflect motivational deficits. Arch Gen Psychiatry 48(3):282-3, 1991 Sweeney JA, Haas GL, Keilp JG, Long M: Evaluation of the stability of neuropsychological functioning after acute episodes of schizophrenia: One-year followup study. Psychiatry Res 38(1):63-76, 1991 Tamlyn D, McKenna PJ, Mortimer AM, et al: Memory impairment in schizophrenia: its extent, affiliations and neuropsychological character. Psychol Med 22(1):101-15, 1992 43 Trumbetta, Susan L; Mueser, Kim T: Social functioning and its relationship to cognitive deficits over the course of schizophrenia. in Negative Symptom and Cognitive Deficit Treatment Response in Schizophrenia. Keefe RSE, Mcevoy JP, editors. Washington: APA, 2001 Twamley EW, Doshi RR, Nayak GV, et al: Generalized cognitive impairments, ability to perform everyday tasks, and level of independence in community living situations of older patients with psychosis. Am J Psychiatry 159(12):2013-20, 2003 Van der Does AJ, Willem, Dingemans PMAJ, Linszen DH, Nugter MA, et al. Dimensions and subtypes of recent-onset schizophrenia: A longitudinal analysis. J Nerv Ment Dis 183(11):681-687, 1995 Velligan DI, Bow-Thomas C, Mahurin RK, et al: Do specific neurocognitive deficits predict specific domains of community function in schizophrenia? J Nerv Ment Dis 188:518-524, 2000 Waddington JL, Youssef HA: Cognitive dysfunction in chronic schizophrenia followed prospectively over 10 years and its longitudinal relationship to the emergence of tardive dyskinesia. Psychol Med 26(4):681-688, 1996 Walker E, Lewine RJ: The positive/negative symptom distinction in schizophrenia. Validity and etiological relevance. Schizophr Res 1(5):315-28, 1988 Weinberger DR: Implications of normal brain development for the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 44(7):660-9, 1987 Wechsler D: Weschsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation, 1997 Wilder-Willis KE, Shear PK, Steffen JJ, Borkin J: The relationship between cognitive dysfunction and coping abilities in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 55(3):259-67, 2002 44 Yung AR, McGorry PD: The prodromal phase of first-episode psychosis: Past and current conceptualizations. Schizophr Bull 22(2):353-370, 1996 Zorrilla LT, Eyler, Heaton RK, et al: Cross-sectional study of older outpatients with schizophrenia and healthy comparison subjects: No differences in age-related cognitive decline. Am J Psychiatry 157(8):1324-1326, 2000 45 Table 1. Cognitive Impairments in Schizophrenia and Their Severitya Severe Impairments (2 to 3 SD below the meanb) Serial learning Executive functioning Vigilance Motor speed Verbal fluency Moderate Impairments (1 to 2 SD below the mean) Distractibility Delayed recall Visuo-Motor skills Immediate memory span Working memory Mild Impairments (0.5 to 1 SD below the mean) Perceptual skills Delayed recognition memory Confrontation naming Verbal and full-scale IQ No Impairment Word recognition reading Long-term factual memory a The estimated average severity scores are corrected on the basis of age and relative education level. The “mean” refers to the average level of performance of normal individuals who are similar in age and educational attainment. FROM: Harvey and Keefe, CNS Spectrums, 1997 b 46 Course of Cognitive Impairment in Schizophrenic Patients Using Typical Neuroleptics 0 Normal ? –1 Standard Deviations Psychosis-Free Patients –2 –3 Premorbid Onset Initial Therapy 2 Years 20 Years After Start After Onset of Therapy 10 Figure 1. Course of Cognitive Impairment in Schizophrenic Patients Using Typical Neuroleptics. From: Keefe RSE: Neurocognition. in Current Issues in the Psychopharmacology of Schizophrenia. Breier A, Tran PV, Herrera J, Bymaster F, Tollefson G, eds. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Publishers. Baltimore, 2001 47