Supplementary information Behavioral analysis Object-based attention test. The object-based attention test was conducted to assess the attentional function as described previously (Alkam et al, 2011). In the first habituation phase, mice were individually subjected to a single habituation session of 10 min, during which they were exposed to both empty chambers. In the second acquisition phase, mice were subjected to a single 3-min session, during which five floor-fixed objects (A, B, C, D, and E) were placed separately in the exploration chamber. Mice were allowed to explore the objects in the exploration chamber. Two of the five objects were randomly chosen (e.g., object A and object D) and the time the mice spent on exploring these objects was recorded as the object-exploration time. Upon completion of the training session, all five objects, but not the mouse, were immediately withdrawn from the exploration chamber. In the third phase retention, which immediately followed the acquisition phase, object A was replaced from the exploration chamber into the test chamber in the position analogous with its original position in the exploration chamber. A novel object F was placed in the test chamber in the position analogous with the original position of object D in the 1 exploration chamber. The mouse, which had not been moved from the exploration chamber, was immediately allowed to enter the test chamber and explore the two objects: the familiar object A and the novel object F. A recognition index was expressed as the ratio (TF × 100) / (TA + TF), where TA and TF were the times spent on objects A and F during the retention phase, respectively. Marble-burying behavior test. The marble-burying behavior test was conducted to assess the anxiety and compulsive behavior as described previously (Alkam et al, 2013; Deacon, 2006). On test day, 12 glass marbles were evenly spaced on flattened sawdust in one half of the cage in the presence of the mouse. Marble burying behavior was expressed as the number of marbles buried at least two-thirds deep in a 15-min test period. Elevated plus-maze test. The elevated plus-maze test was conducted to assess the anxiety level as described previously (Alkam et al, 2013; Miyamoto et al, 2002). Recorded parameters consisted of the frequency of entry into open and closed arms in a 5-min test period. These data were used to calculate the percentage of open arms entries [i.e., (open arms entries / open and closed arms entries) × 100]. Fear conditioning test. 2 The auditory fear conditioning test was performed to assess the cognitive function and flexibility as described previously (Mouri et al, 2010; Niwa et al, 2011). On day 1 (conditioning phase), the conditioned stimulus was a 30 s tone (85 dB), and the unconditioned stimulus was a 5 s foot-shock (0.8 mA) that terminated simultaneously with the tone. This procedure was repeated four times at 1 min intervals. On day 2 (extinction phase), mice were placed in neutral cages, and after a lapse of 3 min, a 1 min tone (85 dB) was played six times at 1 min intervals without foot-shock. On day 3 (recall phase), the tone was played three times to test for recall of extinction learning (Milad and Quirk, 2002). Some of the mice underwent the recall phase (day 3) without undergoing the extinction phase (day 2) (No-Ext groups; No-Ext control and No-Ext PNE). Time spent freezing was automatically assessed by fear-conditioning video software (MED-VFC2-SCT-M; Med Associates Inc, USA) during the conditioning phase (Anagnostaras et al, 2010). Latent inhibition test. The latent inhibition (LI) test was performed to assess the attentional shifting and cognitive flexibility as previously reported (Kuroda et al, 2011; Vuillermot et al, 2012). For the pre-exposure phase, the pre-exposed (PE) group received a 10-tone stimulus (85 dB, 30-sec tones with random intervals), whereas the non-pre-exposed (NPE) group 3 received no stimulus during an equivalent period. In the conditioning phase commenced immediately after the pre-exposure phase, a 30-sec tone stimulus was delivered to both the PE and NPE groups, and during the last 1 sec of each tone stimulus, foot shocks of 0.3 mA were simultaneously delivered as unconditioned stimuli. This procedure was repeated two or four times at 180-sec intervals. LI was measured after two or four pairings of the conditioned stimulus with the unconditioned stimulus (2CS-US or 4CS-US, respectively). Twenty-four hr after the conditioning phase, the conditioned tone stimulus was played continuously for 3 min, during which the time of conditioned freezing was evaluated. 4 Reference Alkam T, Hiramatsu M, Mamiya T, Aoyama Y, Nitta A, Yamada K, et al (2011). Evaluation of object-based attention in mice. Behav Brain Res 220: 185-193. Alkam T, Kim HC, Hiramatsu M, Mamiya T, Aoyama Y, Nitta A, et al (2013). Evaluation of emotional behaviors in young offspring of C57BL/6J mice after gestational and/or perinatal exposure to nicotine in six different time-windows. Behav Brain Res 239: 80-89. Anagnostaras SG, Wood SC, Shuman T, Cai DJ, Leduc AD, Zurn KR, et al (2010). Automated assessment of pavlovian conditioned freezing and shock reactivity in mice using the video freeze system. Front Behav Neurosci 4. Deacon RM (2006). Digging and marble burying in mice: simple methods for in vivo identification of biological impacts. Nat Protoc 1: 122-124. Kuroda K, Yamada S, Tanaka M, Iizuka M, Yano H, Mori D, et al (2011). Behavioral alterations associated with targeted disruption of exons 2 and 3 of the Disc1 gene in the mouse. Hum Mol Genet 20: 4666-4683. Milad MR, Quirk GJ (2002). Neurons in medial prefrontal cortex signal memory for fear extinction. Nature 420: 70-74. Miyamoto Y, Yamada K, Noda Y, Mori H, Mishina M, Nabeshima T (2002). Lower 5 sensitivity to stress and altered monoaminergic neuronal function in mice lacking the NMDA receptor epsilon 4 subunit. J Neurosci 22: 2335-2342. Mouri A, Noda Y, Shimizu S, Tsujimoto Y, Nabeshima T (2010). The role of cyclophilin D in learning and memory. Hippocampus 20: 293-304. Niwa M, Matsumoto Y, Mouri A, Ozaki N, Nabeshima T (2011). Vulnerability in early life to changes in the rearing environment plays a crucial role in the aetiopathology of psychiatric disorders. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 14: 459-477. Vuillermot S, Joodmardi E, Perlmann T, Ogren SO, Feldon J, Meyer U (2012). Prenatal immune activation interacts with genetic Nurr1 deficiency in the development of attentional impairments. J Neurosci 32: 436-451. 6