cheney__seyfarth-oxford_handbook

advertisement
The Evolution of a Cooperative Social Mind
Dorothy L Cheney1 and Robert M. Seyfarth2
1. Department of Biology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104
2. Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104
email: cheney@sas.upenn.edu
Word count:
Text: 12,273
Complete MS: 16,814
Oxford Handbook of Comparative Evolutionary Psychology
edited by J. Vonk & T. Shackelford
Keywords: cooperation, contingent cooperation, reproductive success, social relationships, social
cognition
1
1
Abstract
2
It has long been hypothesized that the demands of establishing and maintaining social
3
relationships in complex societies place strong selective pressures on cognition and intelligence.
4
What has been less clear until recently is whether these relationships, and the skills they require,
5
confer any reproductive benefits, and whether such benefits vary across individuals. During the
6
last few years, much progress has been made in resolving some of these questions. There is now
7
evidence from a variety of species that animals are motivated to establish close, long-term bonds
8
with specific partners, and that these bonds enhance offspring survival and longevity. The
9
cognitive and emotional mechanisms underlying such cooperation, however, are still not
10
understood. It remains unclear, for example, whether animals keep track of favors given and
11
received, and whether they rely on memory of past cooperative acts when anticipating future
12
ones. Although most investigations with captive primates have indicated that cooperation is
13
seldom contingency-based, several experiments conducted under more natural conditions suggest
14
that animals do take into account recent interactions when supporting others. Moreover, while
15
interactions within dyads are often unbalanced over short periods of time, pairs with strong
16
bonds have strongly reciprocal interactions over extended time periods. These results suggest
17
that the apparent rarity of contingent cooperation in animals may not stem from cognitive
18
constraints. Instead, animals may tolerate short-term inequities in favors given and received
19
because most cooperation occurs among long-term reciprocating partners.
20
2
21
22
h. 1 Introduction
Many species of animals, including in particular many non-human primates, live in large
23
social groups composed of both kin and nonkin, with whom they both cooperate and compete. It
24
has long been hypothesized (e.g. Jolly 1966; Humphrey 1976; reviewed by Cheney & Seyfarth
25
2007) that the demands of establishing and maintaining social relationships in these complex
26
societies should place strong selective pressures on cognition and intelligence. What has been
27
less clear until recently is whether these relationships, and the skills they require, confer any
28
reproductive benefits, and whether such benefits vary across individuals. Indeed, doubts persist
29
about whether non-human primates even have the cognitive capacity or motivation to maintain
30
long-term relationships.
31
During the last few years, much progress has been made in resolving some of these
32
questions, although many remain unanswered. In particular, the cognitive and emotional
33
mechanisms underlying cooperation are still not understood. It remains unclear, for example,
34
whether animals keep track of favors given and received, and whether they rely on memory of
35
past cooperative acts when anticipating future ones. Some problems are methodological, arising
36
from the difficulties of testing cooperation experimentally under natural conditions. Others stem
37
from the different results obtained from observations of wild animals as opposed to those living
38
in captivity (see also Boesch, this volume).
39
In this chapter, I first briefly summarize recent research on non-human primates’
40
knowledge of other individuals’ social relationships, intentions, and motivations. I then describe
41
several studies suggesting that the ability to maintain long-term social relationships confers
42
significant reproductive benefits. Finally, I discuss some of the many outstanding questions
43
regarding the function of cooperation in non-human primates and the cognitive mechanisms that
3
44
may underlie them. I restrict my discussion to species that exhibit relatively low reproductive
45
skew, including in particular Old World monkeys and apes. Although many callitrichid species
46
exhibit high levels of cooperation, this cooperation is largely obligate and functions primarily to
47
aid the reproduction of the breeding pair. My focus here instead is on species where individuals
48
vary in the extent of their cooperation and the strength of their social bonds, with the aim of
49
examining whether such variation is correlated with some measure of reproductive success.
50
51
52
h1. The recognition of other individuals’ social relationships
Over the past 30 years considerable progress has been made in the study of social cognition
53
in primates and, to a lesser extent, non-primates. A variety of observational and experimental
54
studies, conducted primarily on Old World monkeys, have provided evidence that animals
55
recognize other individuals’ social relationships and dominance ranks, and that they use this
56
knowledge, for example, when reconciling with opponents and their families and when soliciting
57
alliance partners.
58
59
60
h2. The recognition of other individuals’ social relationships
In some species, natural patterns of aggression and reconciliation suggest that monkeys
61
have some knowledge of the close relations that exist among matrilineal kin (i.e. close
62
associates). For example, an individual who has just been involved in an aggressive interaction
63
with another will often redirect aggression by attacking a third, previously uninvolved individual
64
(rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta: Judge 1982, 1991; Japanese macaques, M. fuscata: Aureli et
65
al. 1992; vervet monkeys, Chlorocebus aethiops: Cheney and Seyfarth 1986, 1989). Similarly,
66
Perry et al. (2004) found that capuchin monkeys (Cebus capuchinus) preferentially solicited
4
67
allies who both out-ranked their opponents and had a social relationship with them that was
68
closer than their relationship with the opponent.
69
Field playback experiments provide additional evidence that monkeys recognize other
70
animals’ kin relations. In one experiment conducted on wild chacma baboons (Papio hamadryus
71
ursinus), two unrelated females heard a call sequence that mimicked a fight between two other
72
individuals. When the apparent combatants were unrelated to the subjects, they showed little or
73
no reaction. However, when each of the two opponents was closely related to the two subjects,
74
subjects were significantly likely to look at each other. Furthermore, the dominant subject was
75
more likely to seek out the subordinate subject and supplant her in the half hour that followed
76
these trials than in the half hour that followed trials that had not involved the two subjects’
77
relatives. In other words, the females behaved as if they recognized that a conflict between their
78
families had occurred and had temporarily disrupted their relationship (Cheney and Seyfarth
79
1999).
80
Many primates reconcile with an opponent by touching, hugging, or behaving in a friendly
81
way towards the opponent after aggression (Cords 1992; de Waal 1996). In baboons,
82
reconciliation often takes the form of a grunt given by the aggressor to her victim (Cheney et al.
83
1995b; Silk et al. 1996; Cheney & Seyfarth 1997). The grunts of a close relative of the aggressor
84
can also function as a proxy to reconcile opponents. Playback experiments have shown that
85
victims of aggression are more likely to tolerate their opponent’s proximity in the hour after the
86
dispute if they have heard the grunt of their opponent’s relative than if they have heard the grunt
87
of a more dominant individual belonging to a different matriline (Wittig et al. 2007a). Such kin-
88
mediated reconciliation can succeed only if the victim recognizes the relationships that exist
89
among other group members (see also Das 2000; Judge and Mullen 2005). Conversely, if the
5
90
victim hears the threat-grunt of her opponent’s relative shortly after aggression, she is more
91
likely to avoid her opponent and other members of her opponent’s matriline (Cheney and
92
Seyfarth 2007; Wittig et al. 2007b).
93
The experiments on baboons’ responses to kin-mediated reconciliation and vocal
94
alliances support the view that baboons recognize other females’ matrilineal kinship relations.
95
This is not, however, to say that baboons treat all the members of a matriline as equivalent.
96
Although they recognize that close kin can serve as proxies for each other, they nonetheless
97
distinguish among the different members of a matriline. Hearing a ‘reconciliatory’ grunt from an
98
opponent’s relative changes females’ disposition toward the opponent and that relative, but less
99
so toward other members of the opponent’s matriline (Cheney and Seyfarth 2007; see Rendall et
100
al. 1996 for experiments demonstrating the recognition of individuals and of the close relations
101
among matrilineal kin in rhesus macaques).
102
Similarly, among chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) uninvolved ‘bystanders’ will sometimes
103
direct friendly behavior toward the victim of an aggressive dispute. In one study, the probability
104
that a bystander would engage in such behavior depended primarily on the strength of the bond
105
between the bystander and the victim: the stronger their bond, the more likely that such
106
‘consolation’ would occur (Kutsukake & Castles 2004; see also de Waal & Aureli 1996). In
107
another study, however, the bystander was most likely to direct friendly behavior toward the
108
victim if the bystander had a close relationship with the aggressor (Wittig 2010). This
109
‘consolation’ gesture increased the likelihood that the aggressor and victim would tolerate each
110
other’s proximity in the near future. This effect disappeared if the relationship between the
111
bystander and the aggressor was relatively weak. Here again, victims acted as if they recognized
112
the close bond (or lack of it) between the bystander and the aggressor. As a result, they treated
6
113
the bystander’s friendly behavior as a proxy for reconciliation only if the bystander was a close
114
associate of the aggressor.
115
The ability to recognize the social relationships that exist among others appears to have
116
been favored by natural selection because it allows an individual to predict, for example, who is
117
likely to support an opponent in an aggressive alliance. This skill is essential in any species
118
where triadic interactions are common (Harcourt 1988). Recognition of other animals’ close
119
associates may also help individuals to form and maintain social relationships. While the
120
evolutionary benefits of this kind of social knowledge seem clear, however, the underlying
121
mechanisms are less well understood. Considered in isolation, the recognition of other animals’
122
close kin relations could be accomplished through simple associative mechanisms. The
123
recognition of matrilineal kin, however, does not occur in isolation: matrilineal kin relations are
124
embedded in a network of short- and long-term bonds that vary among individuals according to
125
age, rank, reproductive state, and many other variables. Whether social knowledge under natural
126
conditions, in all its simultaneous manifestations, can be explained by simple theories of
127
association remains an open issue.
128
129
130
h2. The recognition of other individuals’ dominance ranks
Like the bonds among matrilineal kin, linear, transitive dominance relations are a
131
pervasive feature of behavior in many primate groups. A linear rank hierarchy might emerge
132
because individuals simply take note of who is dominant and who is subordinate to themselves –
133
an egocentric view of the world, but one that nonetheless would result in a linear, transitive rank
134
order. Alternatively, individuals might also distinguish among the relative ranks of others. If rank
135
were determined by a physical attribute like size, recognizing other individuals’ ranks would be
7
136
easy. Among most primates, however, there is little relation between rank and size, condition, or
137
age. As a result, the problem is considerably more challenging.
138
There are hints from their behavior that monkeys do recognize other individuals’ relative
139
dominance ranks. In a meta-analysis of 14 different species, including New and Old World
140
monkeys, Schino (2001) found consistent evidence that high-ranking females received more
141
grooming and were groomed by more different individuals than lower-ranking females. These
142
data suggest a general preference for grooming high-ranking individuals, but they fall short of
143
showing that each animal recognizes the relative ranks of others. In subsequent papers, Schino et
144
al. used a within-subject regression analysis to test the hypothesis that each individual distributed
145
grooming among others in direct relation to their relative ranks. They found a significant rank
146
effect in Japanese macaques (Schino et al. 2007) but no such effect in capuchins (C. apella;
147
Schino et al. 2009).
148
Knowledge of other individuals’ rank relations, like the knowledge of other individuals’
149
kin relations, is also evident in patterns of recruitment and coalition formation. Female vervets,
150
macaques, and baboons, for example, almost always support the higher-ranking of two
151
opponents when forming alliances (vervets: Cheney 1983; Seyfarth 1980; rhesus macaques: de
152
Waal 1991; Japanese macaques: Chapais 2001). Animals do not simply recruit any higher-
153
ranking individual: male bonnet macaques (M. radiata), for example, solicit relatively lower-
154
ranking allies when threatening very low-ranking opponents and higher-ranking allies when
155
threatening higher-ranking ones. Because rank relations among male bonnet macaques change
156
often, males appear to be monitoring carefully the relative ranks of potential opponents (Silk
157
1993, 1999).
8
158
These observational studies are supported by field experiments. In one study, female
159
chacma baboons heard a sequence of vocalizations mimicking an interaction that violated the
160
female dominance hierarchy. The sequence consisted of a series of grunts originally recorded
161
from a lower-ranking female combined with a series of fear barks originally recorded from a
162
higher-ranking female. As a control stimulus, subjects heard the same anomalous sequence of
163
calls but with the inclusion of the grunts of a third female who out-ranked both of the other
164
individuals. Supporting the view that baboons recognize other individuals’ dominance ranks,
165
subjects looked in the direction of the loudspeaker for significantly longer durations when they
166
heard the sequence that violated the dominance hierarchy (Cheney et al. 1995a).
167
The recognition of other individuals’ dominance ranks has also been documented among
168
adult male chacma baboons, whose aggressive contests often involve loud ‘wahoo’ calls
169
(Kitchen et al. 2003; Fischer et al. 2004). In this experiment, subjects heard wahoo sequences
170
that mimicked a contest between either adjacently ranked or disparately ranked males. They
171
responded significantly more strongly to playback of a wahoo contest between males of disparate
172
ranks than to playback of a contest between males of adjacent ranks (Kitchen et al. 2005). The
173
result is particularly striking because, like the ranks of male bonnet macaques, the ranks of male
174
baboons change often.
175
The recognition of other individuals’ rank relations, like the recognition of other animals’
176
matrilineal kin relations, requires by itself no special skills in learning and intelligence beyond
177
those well-documented in laboratory studies of classical conditioning. In nature, however,
178
recognition of other animals’ ranks does not occur on its own – it must necessarily be integrated
179
into a complex matrix of other social relations. We are only beginning to understand how this is
180
achieved.
9
181
Having found that chacma baboons recognize the close bonds among matrilineal kin and
182
individual dominance ranks, Bergman et al. (2003) tested whether individuals integrated their
183
knowledge of other individuals’ kinship and rank to recognize that the female dominance
184
hierarchy is composed of a hierarchy of families (that is, sub-groups of closely bonded females).
185
Rank relations among adult female baboons are generally very stable over time, with few rank
186
reversals occurring either within or between families. When rare reversals do occur, however,
187
their consequences differ significantly depending on who is involved. If, for example, a female
188
rises in rank above her sister, the reversal affects only the two individuals involved; the family’s
189
rank relative to other families remains unchanged. However, a rare rank reversal between two
190
females from different matrilines is potentially much more momentous, because it can affect
191
entire families, with all the members of one matriline rising in rank above all the members of
192
another.
193
Bergman et al. (2003) played sequences of calls mimicking rank reversals to subjects in
194
paired trials. In one set of trials, subjects heard an apparent rank reversal involving two members
195
of the same matriline. In the other set, the same subject heard an apparent rank reversal involving
196
the members of two different matrilines. As a control, subjects also heard a fight sequence that
197
was consistent with the female dominance hierarchy. As before, listeners responded with
198
apparent surprise to call sequences that appeared to violate the existing dominance hierarchy.
199
Moreover, between-family rank reversals elicited a consistently stronger response than did
200
within-family rank reversals (Bergman et al. 2003). Subjects acted as if they were classifying
201
individuals simultaneously according to both kinship and rank. The classification of individuals
202
simultaneously according to two different criteria has also been documented in an observational
203
study of Japanese macaques (Schino et al. 2006).
10
204
205
h2.Recognition of more transient social relations
206
Bonds among matrilineal kin and a linear, transitive female dominance hierarchy are
207
components of monkey social structure that typically remain stable for many years. It is not
208
surprising, therefore, that primate social cognition has been most well documented in these two
209
domains. There is growing evidence, however, that primates also recognize and monitor more
210
transient social bonds. Just this kind of monitoring seems to occur in multi-male groups of
211
chacma baboons, where males form sexual consortships with adult females during the time when
212
they are most likely to ovulate. Sexual consortships constitute a form of mate guarding, and
213
typically involve the highest-ranking male. When a consortship has been formed, lower-ranking
214
males can only gain mating opportunities by taking advantage of very temporary separations
215
between a female and her consort to mate ‘sneakily’.
216
To test whether subordinate males monitor sexual consortships for such opportunities,
217
Crockford et al. (2007) used a two-speaker playback experiment to simulate a temporary
218
separation between the consort pair. One speaker played the consort male’s grunt, to signal his
219
location. The other, located approximately 40 meters away, played the female’s copulation call,
220
to signal that she was mating with another male and that further mating opportunities might be
221
available. Subordinate males responded immediately to the apparent separation between the
222
female and her consort by approaching the speaker playing the female’s call. By contrast, when
223
the same playback was repeated a few hours after the consortship had ended, subordinate males
224
showed no interest. Apparently, they already knew that the consortship had ended and the
225
information was therefore redundant. Thus, males appear to monitor the status of these transient
11
226
consort relationships very closely, even though they typically last for only a few days (see Smuts
227
1985 for similar data on animals’ recognition of male-female ‘friendships’ in baboons).
228
229
h2. Social cognition in non-primates
230
Many non-primate species display examples of social cognition that rival those found in
231
monkeys and apes. When competing over access to females, male dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
232
form dyadic and triadic alliances with specific other males, and allies with the greatest degree of
233
partner fidelity are most successful in acquiring access to females (Connor et al. 1999, 2001).
234
These observations suggest that opponents may recognize the bonds that exist among others and
235
selectively retreat when they encounter rivals with a long history of cooperation. Other hints of
236
such recognition are seen in spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta). Hyenas will often redirect
237
aggression toward another, previously uninvolved individual after a fight. Observations suggest
238
that these forms of aggression are selectively directed against a relative of the former opponent
239
(Engh et al. 2005).
240
A variety of species also show evidence of recognizing other individuals’ dominance
241
ranks. Like many monkeys, hyenas (Engh et al. 2005), lions (Panthera leo; Packer and Pusey
242
1982), horses (Equus equus; Feh 1999), and dolphins (Connor & Mann 2006) intervene
243
selectively on behalf of the higher-ranking animal when forming a coalition. Hyenas also seem
244
to make transitive inferences about other individuals’ dominance ranks (Engh et al. 2005). The
245
ability to engage in transitive inference seems to have evolved independently in several species
246
with linear dominance hierarchies. Pinyon jays (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) provide a good
247
example. These birds live in stable flocks of 50 to 500 individuals, each containing individuals
248
that are linked by kinship and arranged in a linear dominance hierarchy. Elegant experiments by
12
249
Paz-y-Miño et al. (2004) have shown that jays use transitive inference to calculate their own
250
dominance status relative to that of a stranger they have observed interacting with their group-
251
mates.
252
Striking examples of social cognition have even been found in monogamous birds (e.g.
253
Peake et al. 2002) and fish (Oliveira et al. 1998; Grosenick et al. 2007), where individuals
254
‘eavesdrop’ on competitive interactions and remember the identities of winners and losers. These
255
data suggest that there is no simple causal relation between large group size and the knowledge
256
of other animals’ relations.
257
258
259
h1. Recognition of intentions and knowledge
Although it now seems clear than many animals recognize other group members’
260
relationships and dominance ranks, we still know little about whether they imbue these
261
relationships with emotions and motives, as humans do. In the more than 30 years since Premack
262
and Woodruff (1978) posed the question “Does the ape have a theory of mind?” much progress
263
has been made in the study of mental state attribution in animals. Many questions, however,
264
remain unresolved (see Smith et al., this volume).
265
266
267
h2. The recognition of motives and intent
Several lines of evidence suggest that many animals routinely attribute simple mental
268
states, like intentions and motives, to others. This ability is particularly evident in their
269
vocalizations, when animals must make inferences about the intended recipient of someone
270
else’s calls. Monkey groups are noisy, tumultuous societies, and an individual could not manage
271
her social interactions if she interpreted every vocalization she heard as directed at her.
13
272
Inferences about the directedness of vocalizations are probably often mediated by gaze direction
273
and relatively simple contingencies. Even in the absence of visual signals, however, monkeys are
274
able to make inferences about the intended recipient of a call based on their knowledge of a
275
signaler’s identity and the nature of recent interactions. For example, when female chacma
276
baboons were played the ‘reconciliatory’ grunt of their aggressor within minutes after being
277
threatened, they behaved as if they assumed the call was directed at themselves, as a signal of
278
benign intent. As a result, they were more likely to approach their former opponent and to
279
tolerate their opponent’s approaches than after hearing either no grunt or the grunt of another
280
dominant female unrelated to their opponent (Cheney and Seyfarth 1997). Call type was also
281
important, because subjects avoided their recent opponent if they heard her threat-grunt rather
282
than her ‘reconciliatory’ grunt (Engh et al. 2006b). By contrast, if subjects heard a female’s
283
threat-grunt shortly after grooming with her, they ignored the call and acted as if they assumed
284
that the female was threatening another individual. Thus, baboons use their memory of recent
285
interactions to make inferences about the caller’s intention to communicate with them.
286
In some cases, these inferences are complex and indirect, and call upon baboons’
287
knowledge of the kinship relationships of other group members. For example, when female
288
baboons were played the threat-grunts of their aggressor’s relative soon after being threatened,
289
they avoided members of their aggressor’s matriline. In contrast, when they heard the same
290
threat-grunts in the absence of aggression, they ignored the call and acted as if they assumed that
291
the call was directed at someone else (Wittig et al. 2007b). Similarly, as already mentioned,
292
when subjects heard the ‘reconciliatory’ grunt of their aggressor’s relative after a fight, they were
293
more likely to approach both their aggressor and the relative whose grunt they had heard (Wittig
294
et al. 2007a). They did not do so, however, if they had heard the ‘reconciliatory’ grunt of
14
295
another, unrelated female. Here again, subjects behaved as if they believed that a grunt from
296
their aggressor’s relative must be directed at them, as a consequence of the fight. What is
297
especially interesting in these experiments is that subjects inferred that they were the target of the
298
vocalization even though they had not recently interacted with the signaler, but with her relative.
299
They could only have done so if they recognized that close bond that existed between the two
300
females.
301
In primates, faces and voices are the primary means of transmitting social signals, and
302
monkeys recognize the correspondence between facial and vocal expressions (Ghazanfar &
303
Logothetis 2003). Presumably, visual and auditory signals are somehow combined to form a
304
unified, multi-modal percept in the mind of a monkey. In a study using positron emission
305
tomography (PET), Gil da Costa et al. (2004) showed that when rhesus macaques hear one of
306
their own species’ vocalizations, they exhibit neural activity not only in areas associated with
307
auditory processing but also in higher-order visual areas, including STS. Auditory and visual
308
areas also exhibit significant anatomical connections (Poremba et al. 2003).
309
Ghazanfar et al. (2005) explored the neural basis of sensory integration using the coos
310
and grunts of rhesus macaques as stimuli. They found clear evidence that cells in certain areas of
311
the auditory cortex are more responsive to bi-modal (visual and auditory) presentation of
312
species-specific calls than to unimodal presentation. Although significant integration of visual
313
and auditory information occurred in trials with both vocalizations, the effect of cross-modal
314
presentation was greater with grunts than with coos. The authors speculate that this may occur
315
because grunts are usually directed toward a specific individual in dyadic interactions, whereas
316
coos tend to be broadcast generally to the group at large. The greater cross-modal integration in
15
317
the processing of grunts may therefore have arisen because, in contrast to listeners who hear a
318
coo, listeners who hear a grunt must determine whether or not the call is directed at them.
319
In sum, when deciding “Who, me?” upon hearing a vocalization, monkeys must take into
320
account the identity of the signaler (who is it?), the type of call given (friendly or aggressive?),
321
the nature of their prior interactions with the signaler (were they aggressive, friendly, or
322
neutral?), and the correlation between past interactions and future ones (does a recent grooming
323
interaction lower or increase the likelihood of aggression?). Learned contingencies doubtless
324
play a role in these assessments. But because listeners’ responses depend on simultaneous
325
consideration of all of these factors, this learning is likely to be both complex and subtle.
326
327
328
h2. Inferences about others’ knowledge
Although baboons and other monkeys may be able to recognize other individuals’
329
intentions when inferring whether or not they are the target of another individual’s call, there is
330
less convincing evidence that they also take into account their audience’s knowledge or beliefs
331
when producing or assessing calls. Both monkeys and apes give alarm calls, for example,
332
without any apparent recognition of whether listeners are ignorant or already informed about the
333
presence of a predator (reviewed by Cheney and Seyfarth 2007). Similarly, although the ‘food
334
calls’ of capuchin monkeys (Gros-Louis 2004) and the pant hoots of chimpanzees (Clark &
335
Wrangham 1994) attract others to food, signalers show no evidence of recognizing whether their
336
audience is already aware of the presence of food. To provide another example, chacma baboons
337
often give ‘contact’ barks when separated from others. When several individuals are calling
338
simultaneously, it often appears that they are answering each other’s calls in order to inform
339
others of the group’s location. Playback experiments suggest, however, that baboons call
16
340
primarily with respect to their own separation from the group, not their audience’s. They
341
‘answer’ others when they themselves are separated, and they often fail to respond to the calls of
342
even their offspring when they themselves are in close proximity to other group members
343
(Cheney et al. 1996; Rendall et al. 2000). In this respect, the vocalizations of monkeys and apes
344
are very different from human speech, where we routinely take into account our audience’s
345
beliefs and knowledge during conversation.
346
The extent to which animals attribute knowledge, ignorance, and beliefs to others is
347
controversial. It is now well established that many animals are highly attentive to other
348
individuals’ direction of gaze. In particular, domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are adept at using
349
gaze or gestures to determine which of two locations has food. When presented with a human or
350
another dog informant, they reliably choose the location where the informant is looking,
351
pointing, or orienting (e.g. Hare et al. 1998; Hare & Tomasello 1999; Miklosi & Topal 2004).
352
Indeed, in one direct comparative experiment dogs were more accurate than chimpanzees in their
353
ability to use communicative cues like pointing, gazing, and reaching to locate food (Brauer et al.
354
2006b). In addition to using other individuals’ direction of gaze to gain information, dogs often
355
go out of their way to make eye contact with others before attempting to communicate with
356
them, and they appear to be sensitive to whether a person is attentive or inattentive (Gacsi et al.
357
2004).
358
Some investigators have suggested that animals’ attentiveness to gaze direction is an
359
indication that animals recognize what other individuals can and cannot see and hence what they
360
can and cannot know. Rhesus macaques, for example, are more likely to attempt to steal food
361
from a human whose eyes are averted than from one whose eyes are not (Flombaum & Santos
362
2005), and captive chimpanzees are more likely to approach food that a competitor cannot see
17
363
than food it can see (Hare et al. 2000). Similarly, when potential competitors are present, ravens
364
(Corvus corax) and scrub jays (Aphelocoma californica) are more likely to cache food in sites
365
that are out of view or hidden behind barriers than in more open sites (reviewed by Clayton this
366
volume; Bugnyar & Heinrich 2005; Bugnyar & Kotrschal 2002; Emery et al. 2004; Dally et al.
367
2005).
368
These results are certainly consistent with the interpretation that animals recognize the
369
relationship between seeing and knowing. However, they are also consistent with a simpler
370
interpretation that posits that animals use gaze direction to assess not other individuals’
371
knowledge, but rather their intentions. As a result, they recognize, for example, that other
372
individuals are motivated to defend food that they are looking at, and less likely to defend food
373
in which they show no interest.
374
Some recent experiments have attempted to avoid this confound by eliminating the
375
possibility that subjects are responding only to their rival’s direction of gaze when choosing
376
among food items. Kaminski et al. (2008) presented chimpanzees with the choice of three
377
buckets, two of which contained food. The first bucket was baited in the presence of both the
378
subject and the rival. The second bucket was baited in the presence only of the subject. In the test
379
condition, the subject’s view of the apparatus was blocked, while the rival was allowed to choose
380
first. In the control condition, the subject chose first. When subjects chose first, they were as
381
likely to choose the bucket that their rival had seen baited as the one he had not. However, when
382
they chose second, they were more likely to choose the bucket that their rival had not seen
383
baited, suggesting they inferred that the rival would have chosen the bucket that he had seen
384
baited. In other words, they acted as if they recognized what their rival knew, based on what he
385
had seen.
18
386
To date, most studies of animals’ ‘theory of mind’ have been conducted on captive
387
animals, using paradigms and rewards determined by human experimenters. It is to be hoped that
388
future investigations will attempt to address these questions under more natural conditions, on
389
the animals’ own terms. Until such experiments are conducted, we can only speculate about the
390
selective forces that might favor the evolution of a theory of mind, and its function in social
391
interactions.
392
393
394
h1. The function of cooperation, social relationships, and a social mind
If knowledge of other individuals’ relationships and mental states is adaptive, it should be
395
possible to identify correlations between social knowledge and reproductive success. Although
396
this has not yet proved impossible, there is growing evidence that animals benefit from social
397
bonds, that they are motivated to form social bonds, and that there is individual variation not
398
only in the strength and consistency of social bonds but also that this variation is correlated with
399
reproductive success.
400
Female monkeys form the strongest and most enduring social bonds with close kin
401
(reviewed by Silk 2007). Among wild female baboons, for example, the strongest bonds are
402
formed between mothers and daughters, the next strongest bonds with maternal sisters, and less
403
close bonds with less closely related individuals (Silk et al. 2006a, b; 2010a). Females form the
404
strongest social bonds with those that groom them most equitably, and those that groom most
405
equitably have the most enduring social bonds (Silk et al. 2006a, 2006b, 2010a). A similar
406
pattern characterizes wild male chimpanzees (Mitani 2009b). Indeed, a number of studies have
407
demonstrated that primates balance grooming exchanges with long-term partners; although
408
grooming within dyads is often unbalanced over short periods of time, pairs with strong bonds
19
409
have strongly reciprocal grooming relations over extended time periods (chimpanzees: Gilby &
410
Wrangham 2008; Gomes et al. 2009; capuchins: Schino et al. 2009, anubis baboons (P.h.
411
anubis): Frank & Silk 2009). Even females who lack close kin form close relationships with a
412
few specific females, though these are often not as enduring as those formed among kin. This is
413
true also of male chimpanzees. Although males form the strongest and enduring bonds with
414
maternal brothers (Langergraber et al. 2007; Mitani 2009b), close bonds are not limited to kin,
415
and even unrelated females chimpanzees often establish and maintain close bonds with specific
416
other females (Langergraber et al. 2009).
417
Close bonds also appear to be correlated with reproductive success. Female yellow
418
baboons (P. h. cynocephalus) that are more socially integrated into their groups experience
419
higher infant survival than females who are less socially integrated (Silk et al. 2003b). Similarly,
420
female chacma baboons who maintain strong bonds with other adult females have higher
421
survivorship among their offspring than females with weaker bonds (Silk et al. 2009).
422
Furthermore, females who maintain the closest and most consistent bonds experience greater
423
longevity (Silk et al. 2010b). These effects are independent of dominance rank, suggesting that
424
strong and consistent bonds may partially offset any fitness loss due to low dominance rank.
425
Importantly, the fact that most females’ partner changes are not due to the death of the partner
426
suggests that some females may be more skilled or more motivated than others in maintaining
427
relationships with preferred partners over time. Positive correlations between sociality and
428
reproductive success have also been documented in unrelated wild female horses (Cameron et al.
429
2009), indicating again that the strength of an individual’s social relationships exerts a stronger
430
effect on reproductive success than the mere presence of kin. These findings also parallel
20
431
evidence from humans showing that social integration enhances longevity and health (e.g.
432
Berkman & Glass 2000; Cacioppo & Hawkley 2003; Smith & Christakis 2008).
433
The factors that underlie the correlation between strong social bonds and fitness are not
434
yet understood, and there is some uncertainty about the direction of the causal links between
435
social bonds and fitness. One causal factor may be related to stress. When rats (Rattus
436
norvegicus) and other rodents are housed in isolation, they become hypervigilant and fearful of
437
new stimuli (Cavigelli & McClintock 2003; Cavigelli et al. 2006; reviewed by Cheney &
438
Seyfarth 2009). Fearfulness early in life is associated with greater reactivity to stressful events
439
later in life and earlier age at death. Socially isolated females have more exaggerated
440
glucocorticoid responses to every day stressors, and are much more likely to develop mammary
441
cancers than group-housed females (McClintock et al. 2005). Similarly, prolonged social stress
442
impairs the immune system of female long-tailed macaques (M. fasicularis; Shively et al. 2005),
443
but affiliative interactions with group members partially offset these deleterious effects (Cohen et
444
al. 1992; Gust et al. 1994).
445
The quality of social relationships may influence females’ ability to cope with the
446
challenges of daily life. For example, female house mice (Mus musculus), which often share
447
nests with other females and rear their pups communally, reproduce more successfully when they
448
are allowed to choose their nestmates than when nestmates are assigned randomly (Weidt et al.
449
2008). Rat sisters with well-balanced affiliative relationships exhibit lower glucocorticoid levels,
450
fewer tumors, and higher survival rates than those with less well-balanced relationships (Yee et
451
al. 2008). Female chacma baboons display marked increases in glucocorticoid levels when a
452
preferred social partner dies (Engh et al. 2006a). In the same population, females experience
453
lower glucocorticoid levels when their grooming networks are more focused, and females with
21
454
more focused grooming networks show less pronounced responses to various stressors, including
455
the immigration of potentially infanticidal males (Crockford et al. 2008; Wittig et al. 2008).
456
Similar effects have been observed in semi-free-ranging rhesus macaque females, who exhibit
457
lower glucocorticoid levels in months when their grooming networks are less diffuse (Brent et al.
458
2010).
459
460
461
h1. The mechanisms underlying cooperative behavior
Despite the reproductive benefits of strong cooperative relationships, the mechanisms
462
that motivate individuals to form such bonds are still far from well understood. Female baboons,
463
for example, do not groom only with close kin and those with whom they share a close social
464
bond; they also groom less regularly with other females. When a close partner dies, they may
465
attempt to establish a close bond with a previously infrequent partner. We hypothesize that
466
knowledge of other individuals’ relationships guides the formation of new relationships, but this
467
hypothesis has not yet been tested. Indeed, we still know little about whether or how animals
468
keep track of their social relationships, of cooperative and non-cooperative interactions, or of
469
favors given and received.
470
471
472
h2. The mechanisms underlying social interactions
There continues to be debate about the psychological mechanisms that underlie animals’
473
social interactions and relationships. Because we have no direct evidence that animals can plan
474
or anticipate the benefits that might derive from a long-term relationship, a number of
475
investigators have argued that animals’ cooperative interactions are motivated only by short-term
476
rewards, such as the opportunity to handle an infant or gain access to food. According to these
22
477
arguments, social interactions are not founded on long-term patterns of affiliation but are based
478
instead on short-term by-product mutualism or biological markets motivated by the likelihood of
479
immediate reward (Noe & Hammerstein 1994). For example, much cooperative behavior in
480
nonhuman primates occurs in the form of low cost services like grooming and alliance support
481
against lower-ranking opponents (Widdig et al. 2000; Watts 2002; Schino 2007). Because these
482
alliances function primarily to reinforce the status quo, it has been suggested that they might
483
more properly be regarded as a form of mutualism. Similarly, when a female grooms another in
484
order to gain access to her infant, she may simply be engaging in a short-term negotiation with a
485
trading partner who controls a desirable commodity (Barrett et al. 1999, 2003; Fruteau et al.
486
2009; Henzi & Barrett 2002).
487
There is no doubt that many social interactions vary with current conditions. Several
488
studies have shown, for example, that female baboons often groom lactating females to obtain
489
immediate access to their infants (Seyfarth 1976; Altmann 1980; Frank and Silk 2009; Henzi &
490
Barrett 2002; Silk et al. 2003a). Female baboons are particularly likely to reconcile after conflicts
491
with mothers of newborns, as reconciliatory behavior facilitates infant handling (Silk et al.
492
1996). Similarly, males groom estrous females at higher rates than pregnant or lactating females,
493
and subordinate individuals groom dominant individuals in apparent exchange for tolerance at
494
food sites (de Waal 1997; Ventura et al. 2006). In an experiment directly testing the hypothesis
495
that grooming in vervet monkeys is motivated in part by the expectation of immediate reward,
496
Fruteau et al. (2009) manipulated a food container in such a way that it could only be opened by
497
one low-ranking female. Consistent with biological market theory, the rate at which the female
498
subsequently received grooming from others increased significantly. This initial gain, however,
23
499
decreased after a second subordinate female was allowed to open the container. Thus, grooming
500
appeared to be adjusted according to the relative value of the provider.
501
The view that many social interactions are based on the current value of commodities and
502
the supply of alternative trading partners is not necessarily inconsistent with evidence indicating
503
that others reflect long-terms patterns of affiliation. Female baboons, for example, form long-
504
term bonds with only a small number of other females; many of their other social interactions
505
may well be initiated or maintained by interactions that depend on the current value of
506
commodities. Nevertheless, their long-term bonds can endure for years despite short-term
507
fluctuations in behavior due, for example, to the birth of infants. Moreover, grooming often
508
occurs in the absence of an immediate reward, and it is seldom evenly balanced between partners
509
within single bouts. Finally, despite short-term asymmetries, non-human primates form the
510
strongest bonds with those individuals with whom they have the most balanced and reciprocal
511
grooming interactions over extended periods of time (see above).
512
513
514
h2. The mechanisms underlying reciprocity and contingent cooperation
During the last decade, there has also been increasing skepticism about the relevance of
515
reciprocity in the social interactions of animals. Because most cooperative interactions like
516
grooming occur between long-term partners (often kin) for whom any single altruistic act may be
517
relatively insignificant, many investigators are now convinced that the sort of reciprocal altruism
518
first proposed by Trivers (1971) may be both rare and fragile in nature (e.g. Hammerstein 2003;
519
Clutton-Brock 2009). Although there is limited experimental and correlational evidence that
520
animals sometimes rely on memory of recent interactions when behaving altruistically toward
521
others, interpretation has been complicated by a paucity of convincing examples, the absence of
24
522
important controls in some early tests, and a number of experimental studies seeming to indicate
523
that animals lack the cognitive or empathetic ability to sustain contingent cooperative exchanges.
524
As originally formulated by Trivers (1971; see also e.g. Stephens 1996; Gurven 2006;
525
West et al. 2007; Schino & Aureli 2009), reciprocal altruism occurs when the donor of an
526
altruistic act incurs an immediate cost but receives delayed benefits when the recipient
527
reciprocates the altruistic act at some future time. For reciprocal altruism to evolve, individuals
528
must have a high probability of meeting again, and they must be able to detect or avoid cheaters.
529
Reciprocal altruism can be distinguished from mutualism, in which both participants receive
530
immediate benefits that are greater than any immediate costs, and from kin selection, in which
531
the donor gains inclusive fitness benefits despite incurring costs. Because the costs and benefits
532
of many altruistic acts are difficult to quantify, I will here use the term ‘contingent cooperation’
533
rather than ‘reciprocal altruism’ to describe altruistic behavior whose occurrence is contingent
534
upon a specific previous altruistic act. This definition is agnostic with respect to the precise costs
535
and benefits of the altruistic behavior; it posits only that a given supportive act by individual A
536
toward B is causally dependent upon a previous supportive act by B toward A.
537
538
h3. Cognitive constraints on contingent cooperation
539
Some of the skepticism about the relevance of contingent cooperation in animals’
540
interactions stems from doubts about whether animals possess the requisite cognitive abilities to
541
sustain such cooperation. These include the ability to remember specific interactions, to delay
542
reward, to track favors given and returned, to plan and anticipate future outcomes, and to
543
distinguish between cooperators and defectors (e.g. Stevens & Hauser 2004; Stevens et al. 2005;
544
Barrett et al. 2007; Henzi & Barrett 2007; Hauser et al. 2009). Many of these objections may not
25
545
be valid. As already described, for example, a number of playback experiments have
546
demonstrated that female baboons’ behavior is influenced by memory of a recent interactions
547
with specific individuals and their relatives (Cheney & Seyfarth 1997; Engh et al. 2006b; Wittig
548
et al. 2007a, b). The extent to which this memory is explicit is as yet unknown.
549
Other purported cognitive limitations can also be questioned. There is now a large
550
literature on animals’ numerical discrimination abilities, suggesting that quantity assessments
551
and counting are widespread across many taxa (reviewed by Shettleworth 2010). Like children,
552
nonhuman primates appear to rely on two distinct core systems for representing quantities: one
553
for identifying small absolute numbers and another for estimating and comparing larger amounts
554
(Feigenson et al. 2005). In macaques, specific neurons in the PFC appear to be tuned to
555
particular small numbers (Nieder et al. 2002). Rhesus macaques are also able to sum the number
556
of sounds they hear and the number of objects they see, indicating that the ability to match and
557
add quantities is cross-modal and extends across different currencies (Jordan et al. 2008).
558
Similarly, although many tests with non-human primates have suggested a general failure to
559
delay rewards beyond very short periods of time (e.g. Evans & Beran 2007; Ramseyer et al.
560
2006), there appears to be considerable inter-individual variation in self-imposed delayed
561
gratification (Beran 2002; Pele et al. 2010). Moreover, the ability of primates and other animals
562
to delay gratification in contexts that do not involve food rewards remains largely untested. Thus,
563
contingent cooperation in animals is not necessarily constrained by the inability to delay reward
564
or to quantify past cooperative acts.
565
It has also been assumed that animals are not capable of contingent cooperation because
566
it demands the ability to anticipate future interactions. Leaving aside for the moment the question
567
of whether mental projections of future outcomes are necessary to sustain contingent
26
568
cooperation, the assumption that animals are unable to anticipate events that have not yet
569
occurred may not be valid. There is a long history in experimental psychology of tests
570
demonstrating that many animals – including pigeons, rats, and nonhuman primates – accurately
571
and predictably anticipate future rewards and outcomes (reviewed by Shettleworth 2010). Both
572
the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala play important roles in delay discounting and reward
573
anticipation in animals (Miller & Wallis 2003; Churchwell et al. 2009; Crystal 2009).
574
Furthermore, although non-human primates may lack the ability to reflect explicitly and
575
consciously about their past and future interactions (reviewed by Cheney & Seyfarth 2007), they
576
do appear to have some mental access to their own experiences (reviewed by Smith et al., this
577
volume). Indeed, the ability to monitor uncertainty and assess knowledge appears to be highly
578
adaptive, especially when an animal finds itself in a novel predicament.
579
It is also doubtful that non-human primates are unable to distinguish cooperators from
580
non-cooperators. In tests conducted in captivity that require two individuals to work together to
581
obtain a food reward, both capuchin monkeys and chimpanzees are more likely to cooperate with
582
partners with whom rewards are shared more equitably (de Waal & Davis 2003; Melis et al.
583
2006b; Melis et al. 2009; reviewed by Silk & House, this volume). Chimpanzees also recognize
584
which partners are most effective (Melis et al. 2006a), and they show a limited ability to increase
585
their rate of cooperation with partners who have cooperated with them in the past (Melis et al.
586
2008). They are also able to some extent to resolve conflicts of interests when working together
587
to achieve a common goal (Melis et al. 2009; see Silk & House for more discussion).
588
589
h3. Emotional constraints on contingent cooperation
27
590
In humans, inequity aversion, social tolerance, and the motivation to engage in joint
591
activities are important catalysts for cooperative behavior. The degree to which non-human
592
primates are motivated by these emotions, however, remains unclear (reviewed by Silk & House,
593
this volume). Some experiments have suggested that capuchins and chimpanzees reject food
594
offered by humans if a rival is receiving a better reward (Brosnan & de Waal 2003; Brosnan et
595
al. 2005; Takimoto et al. 2010). Other experiments, however, have failed to replicate these
596
results, suggesting that the food rejections are due to frustration at seeing, but not obtaining, a
597
preferred food item rather than to perceived inequity. Indeed, in some cases, seeing a rival eating
598
preferred food actually increases the likelihood that subjects will accept less preferred food
599
(Brauer et al. 2006a, 2009; Dubreuil et al. 2006). In captivity, chimpanzees seem generally
600
indifferent to inequitable outcomes to themselves and others. In two experiments in which
601
chimpanzees had the opportunity to deliver food to a partner at no cost to themselves, subjects
602
showed no evidence of other-regarding behavior (Silk et al. 2005; Jensen et al. 2006; but see
603
Lakshminarayanan & Santos 2008). They did not behave spitefully or withhold food from their
604
partner; they simply ignored their partner’s returns. Similarly, in ultimatum games, chimpanzees
605
appear to behave more rationally than humans, accepting inequitable returns to themselves if no
606
alternatives are available (Jensen et al. 2007a).
607
It has also been argued that a lack of social tolerance may contribute to the low levels of
608
cooperation achieved by chimpanzees in many experiments. Bonobos (Pan paniscus) achieve
609
higher levels of success in some cooperative tasks than do chimpanzees, seemingly because their
610
willingness to share rewards with their partners prompts continued cooperation (Hare et al.
611
2007). One explanation for this greater tolerance is that adult bonobos retain the characteristics
612
of juveniles, because juvenile chimpanzees also tend to be more cooperative than adult
28
613
chimpanzees (Wobber et al. 2010). It remains unclear, however, whether bonobos also show
614
higher degrees of cooperation and tolerance for partners under natural conditions, in contexts
615
where the nature of the task and its rewards are not determined by humans. It is not known, for
616
example, whether bonobos show higher levels of cooperation than chimpanzees when hunting, or
617
whether they share their kills more equitably. Similarly, it is not at all apparent whether bonobos
618
ever engage in any behavior that is as cooperative and potentially costly as chimpanzees’
619
patrolling behavior (Watts & Mitani 2001; Mitani et al. 2010; see below), or if they do, whether
620
they are more likely than chimpanzees to share risks equitably.
621
Taken together, the results of these experiments suggest that cooperation in animals may
622
be sustained by qualitatively different mechanisms than it is in humans. Indeed, experiments
623
explicitly designed to compare the behavior of children and young chimpanzees suggest that
624
humans may be uniquely motivated to engage others’ attention, to empathize with others, and to
625
share their intentions, emotions, and knowledge (Tomasello et al. 2005; Warneken & Tomasello
626
2009; Silk & House; this volume). It is also possible, however, that inequity aversion may be less
627
universal in humans than is often supposed. Surveys of people living in societies that lack large-
628
scale religions and economic markets have tended to reveal a general indifference to unfair
629
outcomes (Henrick et al. 2010), suggesting that what is often regarded as a species-specific
630
prosociality in humans is not entirely the result of innate psychological mechanisms (see Boesch,
631
this volume; Silk & House, this volume).
632
633
634
635
h2. How rare, in fact, is contingent cooperation?
Most primates live in groups that include both kin and nonkin, with whom they both
cooperate and compete. Because individuals do not derive inclusive fitness benefits from
29
636
cooperating with nonkin, investigators have typically postulated that any form of cooperation
637
among nonkin must be maintained either by mutualism or by some form of contingent
638
cooperation from which individuals derive long-term benefits while tolerating short-term
639
inequities. For several reasons, however, it has proved difficult to investigate contingent
640
cooperation under natural conditions. First, in the absence of experiments, it is almost impossible
641
to determine whether a given altruistic act is causally dependent upon a specific prior interaction.
642
Second, many altruistic acts occur in different currencies –such as grooming, alliance support, or
643
food sharing – whose relative values are difficult to calibrate or quantify. Moreover, even
644
altruistic acts that occur in the same currency may not carry equal value to each participant. In
645
species that form dominance hierarchies, for example, a low-ranking individual may value
646
alliance support from a more dominant partner more highly than vice versa. As a result, he may
647
willing to provide substantially more alliance support to the dominant partner than he receives in
648
return and still regard the relationship as reciprocal. Given these tautological assumptions, almost
649
any relationship can be termed reciprocal. Finally, the degree to which interactions are regarded
650
as reciprocal may also be a function of the time scale under consideration. As already mentioned,
651
grooming exchanges within single bouts are often unbalanced and asymmetrical. Nonetheless,
652
over longer periods of time partners with close social bonds exhibit a high degree of reciprocity
653
in their grooming interactions.
654
655
h3. Observational evidence
656
Correlations between grooming and alliance support have been documented in a variety
657
of primates (reviewed in Silk 2007). In a meta-analysis involving 14 different primate species,
658
Schino (2007) found a weak but highly significant correlation between grooming and alliances
30
659
among female non-human primates over extended periods, but little evidence that alliance
660
support is motivated specifically by recent grooming bouts (see also Schino et al. 2007). For
661
example, in one year-long study of Japanese macaques, individuals formed most coalitions with
662
preferred grooming partners, but there was no evidence that a recent grooming bout increased the
663
probability of support over the short term (Schino et al. 2007). Monkeys appeared to choose
664
alliance partners on the basis of long-term partner preferences rather than on memory of specific
665
interactions. These observations suggest that grooming and alliance support have evolved as a
666
system of long-term, low-cost reciprocity (Schino & Aureli 2009) rather than one of short-term,
667
contingency-based exchange.
668
In contrast, de Waal (1997) reported that captive chimpanzees were more likely to share
669
food with individuals who had recently groomed them than with those that had not. Here again,
670
however, the effect of prior grooming appeared to be influenced by the nature of the relationship
671
between the two individuals. For pairs that seldom groomed, sharing was contingent upon recent
672
grooming; for pairs that regularly groomed at high rates, sharing was less influenced by recent
673
interactions.
674
Similarly, wild male chimpanzees reciprocate grooming, alliances, and meat-sharing with
675
specific long-term partners. Although exchanges are often asymmetrical within dyads over short
676
time periods, they become more evenly balanced over longer periods of time, and they are not
677
simply a byproduct of association frequency or genetic relatedness (Mitani 2006, 2009a; see
678
Boesch, this volume). Cooperation also involves the exchange of services in different
679
currencies, with males reciprocating grooming for support, and support for meat, for example.
680
The most costly cooperative behavior shown by male chimpanzees occurs in the form of
681
boundary patrols and inter-group aggression. Chimpanzees make sometimes lethal incursions
31
682
into the territories of neighboring communities (Nishida et al. 1985; Watts & Mitani 2001;
683
Wilson & Wrangham 2003; Mitani et al. 2010; Boesch this volume). These incursions are highly
684
risky, because a small party or lone individual is vulnerable to a fatal attack if a larger party is
685
encountered; therefore, they cannot be undertaken alone. Although it remains unclear whether
686
patrols are planned, they do appear to involve some degree of shared intentionality and a high
687
degree of mutual support. Little is known about the mechanisms that motivate chimpanzees to
688
initiate and participate in these highly cooperative and potentially costly activities. It is not
689
known, for example, whether chimpanzees take into consideration memory of another
690
individual’s behavior during previous patrols when deciding whether or not to recruit or join him
691
in a patrol. Whether cooperation in this context is more, or less, contingent upon memory of
692
previous events, remains unclear.
693
In sum, most observational studies suggest that cooperation under natural conditions is
694
generally not contingent upon specific recent events. Instead, reciprocal exchanges tend to
695
emerge gradually among regular partners over repeated interactions and longer time scales,
696
despite not being balanced over short periods of time (Mitani 2006, 2009a; reviewed by Schino
697
& Aureli 2009).
698
699
h3. Experimental evidence
700
It is difficult if not impossible to demonstrate through observation alone that a given
701
cooperative act is contingent upon a specific previous act. Experiments have typically been
702
conducted on captive animals, relying on single food exchanges as evidence for reciprocity.
703
These have usually yielded negative results. For example, in one set of experiment with captive
704
chimpanzees, subjects were given a choice of cooperating with either an individual who had
705
previously helped them or one that had not (Melis et al. 2008). Although there was some
32
706
evidence that subjects increased their cooperation with the more helpful partner, this effect was
707
relatively weak. Similarly, in another experiment explicitly designed to test whether cooperation
708
was contingent upon a specific recent interaction, Brosnan et al. (2009) found no evidence that
709
chimpanzees were more likely to provide food to a partner if that partner had previously
710
provided food to them (see also Yamamoto & Masayuki 2010). Melis et al. (2006b) suggest that
711
chimpanzees may be capable of contingent reciprocity, but that long-term partner preferences
712
that develop over repeated interactions may override the decisions that chimpanzees make on the
713
basis of immediate exchanges and rewards. It is also possible, however, that the lack of
714
convincing evidence for contingent cooperation in tests with captive animals results in part from
715
the stringent standards set by these experiments, which have typically required proof of equal
716
back-and-forth exchanges of single currency food rewards whose amounts and timing are
717
determined by humans. These requirements may have set the bar unrealistically high, leading
718
investigators to underestimate the extent to which a recent cooperative interaction may motivate
719
animals to cooperate again.
720
Several investigations conducted under more natural conditions, and without the distraction
721
of food rewards, provide more positive evidence for the reciprocal exchange of cooperative
722
behavior. Unfortunately, however, interpretation is complicated by the lack of important controls
723
in early experiments and the lack of follow-up studies to correct for these confounds. For
724
example, in the well-known study of vampire bats (Desmodus rotundus: Wilkinson 1984) most
725
reciprocal exchanges of blood occurred among close kin. Furthermore, although some
726
individuals regularly exchanged blood with unrelated partners, it was not clear whether any
727
specific act of regurgitation was contingent upon a specific recent donation. The same was true
728
in Packer’s (1977) classic study of reciprocal alliance formation in wild baboons: although males
33
729
supported those individuals who most supported them, cooperative behavior may have emerged
730
not as a result of memory of specific previous alliances but through bonds that developed
731
gradually over extended periods of time.
732
A recent investigation of mobbing behavior in pied flycatchers (Ficedula hypoleuca)
733
provides more convincing evidence for contingent cooperation (Krams et al. 2008). In this
734
experiment, subjects had the opportunity to help one of two neighbors in mobbing an owl. One
735
of these neighbors had recently helped the subjects to mob an owl at their own nest box, while
736
the other had been prevented from doing so by the experimenters. Subjects were significantly
737
more likely to help previous supporters than apparent defectors, suggesting that cooperative
738
behavior was contingent upon memory of each of the neighbor’s behavior. However, the
739
possibility that the birds’ behavior might have been influenced by any recent interaction with
740
their neighbors – not just a supportive one – was not addressed.
741
This confound was also present in Seyfarth and Cheney’s (1984) playback experiment on
742
wild vervet monkeys. Although subjects were more attentive to the recruitment call of an
743
unrelated female after grooming with her than after no interaction, it remained unclear whether
744
subjects might have been equally responsive after any interaction with her, including even
745
aggression.
746
Subsequently, Hemelrijk (1994) demonstrated that grooming increased the probability of
747
actual alliance support in an imaginative experiment with captive long-tailed macaques. In this
748
experiment, three females were temporarily isolated from the rest of the group, and one of the
749
two higher-ranking females was dabbed with a sticky mixture of seeds and syrup to elicit
750
grooming. After 10 minutes, the experimenters provoked aggression by feeding a desirable tidbit
751
to the lowest-ranking of the three females and then observed whether the bystander supported the
34
752
primary aggressor. Hemelrijk found that bystanders were more likely to provide support after
753
being groomed by the aggressor than after no grooming had occurred, suggesting that support
754
was based on memory of the prior grooming interaction.
755
Recently, we conducted a playback experiment with wild chacma baboons that attempted
756
to control for some of the confounds complicating earlier experiments (Cheney et al. 2010). In
757
the test condition, a subject was played the recruitment call of another female at least 10 minutes
758
after she had groomed with that female and then separated without any further interactions. This
759
playback was designed to mimic a context in which the former grooming partner was threatening
760
another individual and soliciting aid. Each subject’s responses were compared to her responses in
761
two control conditions. The first control was also conducted after the subject and the same
762
female had groomed and then separated for at least 10 minutes. In this case, however, no
763
playback was conducted. This control was designed to test whether a recent friendly interaction
764
might simply motivate the subject to approach her partner again, even in the absence of any
765
solicitation for support. In the second control, the same female’s threat-grunts were played to the
766
same subject at least 10 minutes after the subject had threatened that female. This control was
767
designed to test whether subjects’ responses to a recruitment call were primed by any prior
768
interaction, not just a friendly one. On the assumption that a recent cooperative interaction would
769
exert a stronger influence on females with weaker social bonds than those with stronger social
770
bonds (de Waal 1997; Schino & Aureli 2009), we predicted that nonkin would be more likely
771
than kin to show different responses across conditions. Kin selection theory predicts in any case
772
that contingency-based altruism should be less common among kin than among nonkin. Indeed,
773
in one study of captive Japanese macaques kin were never observed to support each other in the
774
half hour after grooming, even when they had the opportunity to do so (Schino et al. 2007).
35
775
Results provided some support for delayed contingent cooperation among unrelated
776
individuals. Hearing the recruitment call of a recent grooming partner caused subjects to move in
777
the direction of the loudspeaker and approach their former partner. When the subject and her
778
partner were close kin no such effect was observed. Importantly, subjects’ responses were not
779
influenced by any type of recent interaction, because subjects only responded to their former
780
partner’s recruitment call after grooming, and not after aggression. Similarly, their responses
781
were not prompted only by the motivation to resume a friendly interaction, because prior
782
grooming alone did not elicit approach. Instead, subjects were most likely to approach their
783
former grooming partner when they had also heard her recruitment call. Thus, females’
784
willingness to attend to the recruitment calls of other individuals appeared to be motivated at
785
least in part by memory of a specific friendly interaction.
786
In sum, several factors may interact to motivate contingent cooperation in animals under
787
natural conditions: the strength of the partners’ social relationship, the nature of their recent
788
interactions, and the opportunity to reengage in some form of cooperative behavior. Animals
789
appear to possess many of the cognitive abilities thought to be essential for the emergence of
790
contingent cooperation, if only in rudimentary form. Nonetheless, such cooperation appears to be
791
much more rare than the high rates of non-contingent cooperation that develops among kin and
792
long-term partners.
793
794
795
h2. The avoidance of non-cooperators
If cooperation depends on the memory of previous behavior, why do animals seldom
796
avoid or punish cheaters and free-loaders? In captive tests, for example, chimpanzees will
797
continue to work with non-cooperators despite receiving inequitable returns (Melis et al. 2006b,
36
798
2009). And while they retaliate against an individual who steals food from them, but they do not
799
attempt to punish those who obtain disproportionate rewards, and they are not spiteful (Jensen et
800
al. 2007b; Melis et al. 2009).
801
Under natural conditions, too, free-loaders appear to be tolerated. To provide two
802
examples, individual lionesses vary predictably in their participation in territorial conflicts. In
803
playback experiments that simulated the approach of an aggressive intruder, some females
804
consistently advanced toward the source of the calls, while others consistently lagged behind and
805
avoided the potential cost of a conflict (Heinsohn & Packer 1995). Advancers were aware of the
806
laggards’ behavior, because they often looked back at them; nonetheless, they did not avoid or
807
punish them. It is possible that advancers tolerate laggards because they derive inclusive fitness
808
benefits through the laggards’ survival and reproduction. Laggards may also cooperate in other
809
currencies, such as hunting. It is also possible, however, that lions do not have the cognitive
810
ability to recognize laggards as free-loaders, with the result that laggards are able to exploit
811
advancers.
812
Similarly, male chimpanzees do not participate equally in boundary patrols. As with
813
lions, some individuals are allowed to reap the benefits of territorial integrity and even expansion
814
without incurring any costs (Mitani 2006; Mitani et al. 2010). Mitani (2006; 2009a) offers
815
several, as yet untested, explanations for chimpanzees’ tolerance of free-loaders. First, the
816
benefits of patrolling may be greater for some individuals than others. Perhaps, for example,
817
patrolling is a costly signal that enhances an individual’s dominance or access to females.
818
Second, patrolling may yield indirect fitness benefits in the form of enhanced survival and
819
reproduction of close kin. Thus, males with more kin in the community may engage in higher
820
rates of patrolling. Finally, like the lions just mentioned, chimpanzees may lack the cognitive
37
821
capacity to foster or infer deceptive intent. If true, animals may well not be capable of achieving
822
the sort of contingent cooperation manifested by humans, which is sustained in part by inequity
823
aversion and sensitivity to envy, spite, and deception (Jensen et al. 2007b).
824
This last objection, however, only denies the possibility for human-like contingent
825
cooperation in animals; it does not rule it out entirely. The detection of cheaters does not in
826
principle require the ability to impute complex mental states like deception to others. It could
827
arise through relatively simple associative processes, by which animals learn to avoid individuals
828
whose presence is associated with a negative experience. Indeed, mental state attribution may be
829
irrelevant to the emergence of contingent cooperation in animals. Schino and Aureli (2009; see
830
also Schino et al. 2007) have argued that the focus on cognitive constraints in discussions of
831
contingent cooperation is misguided, and confuses proximate and ultimate explanations for
832
behavior. Altruistic behaviors may be favored by natural selection because of the subsequent
833
benefits they confer, but what motivates animals to behave altruistically are the previous benefits
834
they have received. In this view, the accumulation of multiple, altruistic exchanges over time
835
causes animals to form partner-specific emotional bonds that motivate future altruistic behavior.
836
In contrast to cooperation in humans, therefore, contingent cooperation in animals may be
837
mediated by relatively simple proximate mechanisms based on the memory of previous
838
interactions rather than the expectation of future reward. Thus, reciprocity may be maintained by
839
a kind of partner-specific ‘emotional book-keeping’ (de Waal 2000; Schino & Aureli 2009) that
840
permits long-term tracking of multiple partners and facilitates cooperation in different behavioral
841
currencies. The resulting bonds that develop between preferred partners may motivate future
842
positive interactions without the need for explicit tabulation of favors given and returned, or
843
calculations of anticipated benefits (de Waal 2000; Aureli & Schaffner 2002; Aureli & Whiten
38
844
2003). For unrelated females who interact at low rates, a single grooming bout may temporarily
845
elevate a female’s positive emotions toward her partner sufficiently above baseline to influence
846
her immediate interactions with her. In contrast, grooming and support among females with close
847
bonds (who are also usually kin) should be less subject to immediate contingencies and less
848
influenced by single interactions.
849
Finally, it is important to emphasize that while the absence of punishment in animals
850
may derive in part from cognitive constraints, it is likely that a strict accounting of services given
851
and received is maladaptive in stable societies where individuals establish close bonds with
852
others and interact regularly in a variety of contexts. Indeed, although the cognitive constraints
853
that supposedly limit the occurrence of contingent cooperation in animals is often contrasted
854
with humans’ sensitivity to inequitable exchanges, human friendships are rarely contingency-
855
based. Numerous studies have shown that people seldom keep tabs of past costs and benefits in
856
interactions with regular partners (reviewed by Silk 2003). Although people become resentful
857
and dissatisfied when exchanges within a friendship are consistently unbalanced, tallying of
858
favors given and received are typically reserved for strangers and infrequent associates. It seems
859
probable that the relative rarity of contingent cooperation in animals stems less from the inability
860
to remember and keep track of interactions than from a similar tolerance of short-term inequities
861
among long-term partners.
862
863
864
h1. Future directions
We are only beginning to understand the many functions of cooperative behavior in
865
animals and the cognitive and emotional mechanisms that underlie them. There have been only a
866
handful of direct experimental tests of contingent cooperation under natural conditions, and we
39
867
as yet do not understand how supportive, reciprocal relationships emerge from single interactions
868
that are often asymmetrical. Similarly, there have as yet been few attempts to document the
869
reproductive benefits of cooperation and strong social bonds. Here, I highlight three of many
870
possible foci for future research.
871
872
h2 Cognition
873
The recognition of other individuals’ relationships can in principle be achieved through
874
relatively simple associative processes. At the same time, however, there is increasing evidence
875
that animals attribute simple mental states like motives and perspective to others. It therefore
876
seems possible that monkeys and apes – and perhaps also animals – may imbue knowledge of
877
others’ relationships with motives and emotions. In other words, they may not just recognize that
878
A associates with B, but also that A likes B. What, if anything, might this additional layer of
879
social knowledge buy them?
880
I have argued that contingent cooperation does not require complex cognition, such as the
881
ability to detect cheaters or to plan future cooperative acts based on memory of previous ones.
882
Nonetheless, some animals may engage in such mental activities. Non-human primates have
883
some capacity to access their knowledge states and to assess their certainty or uncertainty. Smith
884
et al. (this volume) suggest that metacognition permits animals to weigh alternative strategies in
885
novel contexts, and that it may serve as a precursor to reading others’ minds, as well. Although
886
playback experiments indicate that baboons remember the nature of specific interactions with
887
specific individuals, the extent to which this memory is explicit or episodic remains to be
888
determined. Similarly, some forms of cooperative behavior in animals – the boundary patrols of
889
chimpanzees in particular – are highly suggestive of shared intentionality, planning, and episodic
40
890
memory. To date, however, these cognitive abilities have been examined only under captive
891
conditions, in tests whose rules and constraints are determined by humans. A challenge for future
892
research will be to devise experimental means to examine mental state attribution and
893
metacognition under more natural conditions, in contexts where behavior has the potential to
894
influence reproductive success.
895
896
h2. Personality
897
Recent evidence from baboons has indicated that females vary in the strength and
898
stability of their social relationships, and that this variation contributes significantly to individual
899
variation in reproductive success. The fact that some females fail to maintain the same partners
900
over time also suggests that some individuals may be less skilled or motivated than others at
901
establishing and maintaining bonds. Although the proximate mechanisms underlying these
902
individual differences are not yet understood, they may well be related to personality traits
903
associated with attributes like anxiety, caution, and confidence. For example, in female primates
904
there tends to be no correlation between stress and dominance rank or number of kin. Instead,
905
glucocorticoid levels are more strongly influenced by the size and stability of a female’s social
906
network. Females tend to have lower glucocorticoid levels in months when their grooming
907
network is focused rather than diffuse (reviewed by Cheney &Seyfarth 2009; see also Brent et al.
908
2010).
909
These results emphasize that personality traits may exert a stronger influence on social
910
relationships and reproductive success than more obvious attributes like dominance rank. Some
911
individuals, for example, may be more adept than others at recruiting allies, reconciling with
912
others, and assessing the strength and stability of others’ relationships. Whatever the cause,
41
913
results point to the need for a stronger focus in future research on individual difference in
914
personality traits.
915
Personality traits are influenced not only by genetic factors but also by environmental
916
factors that affect gene expression. In both humans and rhesus macaques, for example, a specific
917
polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene is associated with deficits in neurobiological
918
functioning and poor control of aggression (Suomi 2007). Monkeys carrying the deleterious
919
‘short’ allele are less willing than monkeys homozygous for the ‘long’ allele to gaze directly at
920
the faces and eyes of conspecifics, and they react more adversely to images of dominant animals
921
(Watson et al. 2009). Similarly, mothers carrying short versions of the allele are more likely to
922
be abusive, and both they and their infants exhibit higher cortisol levels (McCormack et al. 2009;
923
Maestripieri et al. 2009).
924
Indeed, maternal effects have been shown to have a profound impact on offspring
925
growth, dominance, HPA axis, and personality in a variety of species (reviewed by Onyango et
926
al. 2008; Jablonka & Raz 2009). For example, egg composition in birds is strongly influenced by
927
food availability, and these maternal effects can influence phenotypic expression across multiple
928
generations (Lindstrom 1999). Similarly, in female rodents social isolation and low levels of
929
maternal care during infancy reduce the expression of hypothalamic estrogen receptors, which in
930
turn results in decreased maternal behavior in these females as adults. These epigenetic effects
931
may persist across generations (Champagne & Mashmoodh 2009). In baboons, the adolescent
932
sons of dominant mothers exhibit significantly lower glucocorticoid levels than the sons of
933
subordinate mothers (Onyango et al. 2008). This effect persists beyond that age when males have
934
begun to out-rank females and suggests that maternal influences from infancy and perhaps even
935
gestation can have profound effects on the organization of the HPA axis. These influences may
42
936
stem in part from patterns of maternal rejection and protectiveness, which has long been known
937
to have long-term influences on offspring behavior (e.g. Hinde 1974).
938
Thus, genetic variation affecting factors such as serotonin and oxytocin reactivity,
939
anxiety, and social reward may influence the strength and stability of an individual’s social
940
bonds, which in turn exert epigenetic effects in offspring. Differences in personality traits may
941
well explain some of the individual variation in cooperative behavior.
942
943
944
h2. The integration of field and laboratory studies
There is currently some disconnect between results obtained in experiments with captive
945
animals and observations derived from field observations. For example, experiments
946
investigating contingent cooperation in captive animals have consistently yielded negative
947
results, while several conducted under more natural conditions have yielded more positive
948
results. In captivity chimpanzees seem relatively indifferent to inequitable outcomes to others
949
and themselves and fail to reciprocate favors in back-and-forth exchanges (Silk & House, this
950
volume). In the wild, however, chimpanzees exchange grooming, alliances, and meat-sharing
951
with specific long-term partners (Mitani 2006, 2009a). Although exchanges over the short term
952
are often asymmetrical, reciprocity emerges gradually over repeated interactions. In captivity,
953
tasks that require cooperation are easily disrupted by disparities in the participants’ dominance
954
ranks, the size of the rewards, and the degree to which rewards can be monopolized. In contrast,
955
under natural conditions chimpanzees not only share meat (if inequitably) but also regularly
956
participate in risky boundary patrols that are obligately cooperative. These discrepancies point to
957
the need both for more detailed investigations of cooperation in the wild and, in captivity, for
958
experiments that carry ecological and behavioral validity.
43
959
References
960
Altmann, J. (1980). Baboon Mothers and Infants. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
961
Aureli, F., Cozzolino, R., Cordischi, C., & Scucchi, S. (1992). Kin-oriented redirection among
962
963
964
965
Japanese macaques: An expression of a revenge system? Animal Behaviour, 44, 283-291.
Aureli, F., & Schaffner, C.M. (2002). Relationship assessment through emotional mediation.
Behaviour, 139, 393-420.
Aureli, F., & Whiten, A. (2003). Emotions and behavioral flexibility. In D. Maestripieri (Ed.),
966
Primate Psychology (pp. 289-323). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
967
Barrett, L., Henzi, S.P., Weingrill, A., Lycett, .JE., & Hill, R. (1999). Market forces predict
968
grooming reciprocity in female baboons. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B,
969
266, 665-670.
970
971
972
Barrett, L., Henzi, P., & Dunbar, R. (2003). Primate cognition: From ‘what now?’ to ‘what if?’
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 494-497.
Barrett, L., Henzi, P., & Rendall, D. (2007). Social brains simple minds: Does social complexity
973
really require cognitive complexity? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of
974
London B, 362, 561-575.
975
Beran, M.J. (2002). Maintenance of self-imposed delay of gratification by four chimpanzees
976
(Pan troglodytes) and an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus). Journal of General Psychology,
977
129, 49-66.
978
979
Bergman, T. J., Beehner, J. C., Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R.M. (2003). Hierarchical
classification by rank and kinship in baboons. Science, 302, 1234-1236.
44
980
Berkman, L.F., & Glass, T. (2000). Social integration, social networks, social support, and
981
health. In L.F. Berkman, & I. Kawachi (Eds.), Social Epidemiology (pp. 137–173). New
982
York: Oxford University Press.
983
984
985
Brauer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello M. (2006a). Are apes really inequity averse? Proceedings of the
Royal Society of London B, 273, 3123–3128.
Bräuer, J., Kaminski, J., Riedel, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006b). Making inferences about
986
the location of hidden food: Social dog - causal ape. Journal of Comparative Psychology,
987
120, 38-47.
988
989
990
991
Brauer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Are apes inequity averse? New data on the tokenexchange paradigm. American Journal of Primatology, 71, 175-181.
Brent, L.J.N., Semple, S., Dubuc, C., Heistermann, M., & MacLarnon, A. (2101). Social capital
and physiological stress levels in free-ranging adult female rhesus macaques.
992
Brosnan, S.F., & de Waal F.B.M. (2003). Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature, 425, 297–299.
993
Brosnan, S.F., Schiff, H.C., & de Waal, F.B.M. (2005). Tolerance for inequity may increase with
994
social closeness in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 272,
995
253–258.
996
Brosnan, S.F., Silk, J.B., Henrich, J., Mareno, M.C., Lambeth, S.P., & Schapiro, S.J. (2009).
997
Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) do not develop contingent reciprocity in an experimental
998
task. Animal Cognition, 12, 587-597.
999
Bugnyar, T., & Heinrich, B. (2005). Ravens, Corvus corax, differentiate between knowledgeable
1000
and ignorant competitors. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 272, 1641-1646.
1001
1002
Bugnyar, T., & K. Kotrschal. (2002). Observational learning and the raiding of food caches in
ravens, Corvus corax : Is it “tactical deception”? Animal Behaviour, 64, 185-195.
45
1003
Cacioppo, J.T., & Hawkley, L.C. (2003). Social isolation and health, with an emphasis on
1004
underlying mechanisms. Perspectives in Biological Medicine, 46, S39-S52.
1005
Cameron, E.Z., Setsaas, T.H., & Linklater, W.L. (2009). Social bonds between unrelated females
1006
increase reproductive success in feral horses. Proceedings of the National Academy of
1007
Sciences USA, 106, 13850-13853.
1008
Cavigelli, S.A., & McClintock, M.K. (2003). Fear of novelty in infant rats predicts adult
1009
corticosterone dynamics and an early death. Proceedings of the National Academy of
1010
Sciences USA, 100, 16131-16136.
1011
1012
1013
Cavigelli, S.A., Yee, J.R., & McClintock, M.K. (2006). Infant temperament predicts life span in
female rats that develop spontaneous tumors. Hormones and Behavior, 50, 454-462.
Champagne, F.A., & Mashmoodh, R. (2009). Genes in context: Gene-environment interplay and
1014
the origins of individual differences in behavior. Current Directions in Psychological
1015
Science, 18, 127-131.
1016
1017
1018
Chapais, B. (2001). Primate nepotism: What is the explanatory value of kin selection?
International Journal of Primatology, 22, 203-229.
Cheney, D.L. (1983). Extra-familial alliances among vervet monkeys. In R.A. Hinde (Ed.),
1019
Primate Social Relationships (pp. 278-286). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific.
1020
Cheney, D.L., & Seyfarth, R.M. (1986). The recognition of social alliances among vervet
1021
1022
1023
1024
1025
monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 34, 1722-1731.
Cheney, D.L., & Seyfarth, R.M. (1989). Reconciliation and redirected aggression in vervet
monkeys. Behaviour, 110, 258-275.
Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R.M. (1997). Reconciliatory grunts by dominant female baboons
influence victims’ behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 54, 409–418.
46
1026
1027
1028
1029
1030
1031
1032
Cheney, D.L., & Seyfarth, R.M. (1999). Recognition of other individuals’ social relationships by
female baboons. Animal Behaviour, 58, 67–75.
Cheney, D.L., & Seyfarth, R.M. (2007). Baboon Metaphysics: The Evolution of a Social Mind.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Cheney, D.L., & Seyfarth, R.M. (2009). Stress and coping mechanisms in female primates.
Advances in the Study of Behavior, 39, 1-35.
Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R.M., & Silk, J.B. (1995a). The responses of female baboons to
1033
anomalous social interactions: Evidence for causal reasoning? Journal of Comparative
1034
Psychology, 109, 134–141.
1035
Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R.M., & Silk, J.B. (1995b). The role of grunts in reconciling opponents
1036
and facilitating interactions among adult female baboons. Animal Behaviour, 50, 249-
1037
257.
1038
1039
1040
Cheney D.L., Seyfarth R.M., & Palombit, R. (1996). The function and mechanisms underlying
baboon ‘contact’ barks. Animal Behaviour, 52, 507–518.
Cheney, D.L., Moscovice, R., Heesen, M., Mundry, R. & Seyfarth, R.M. (2010). Contingent
1041
cooperation in wild female baboons. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
1042
USA, 107, 9562-9566.
1043
Churchwell, J.C., Morris, A.M, Heurtelou, N.M., & Kesner, R.P. (2009). Interactions between
1044
the prefrontal cortex and amygdala during delay discounting and reversal. Behavioral
1045
Neuroscience, 123, 1185-1196.
1046
1047
Clark, A. P., & Wrangham, R.W. (1994). Chimpanzee arrival pant-hoots: Do they signify food or
status? International Journal of Primatology, 15, 185–205.
47
1048
1049
1050
Clutton-Brock, T.H. (2009). Cooperation between non-kin in animal societies. Nature, 462, 5157.
Cohen, S., Kaplan, J.R., Cunnick, J.E., Manuck ,S.B., & Rabin, B.S. (1992). Chronic social
1051
stress, affiliation, and cellular immune response in nonhuman primates. Psychological
1052
Sciences, 3, 301-304.
1053
1054
1055
Connor, R.C., Heithaus, R.M., & Barre, L.M. (1999). Superalliance of bottlenose dolphins.
Nature, 371, 571-572.
Connor, R.C., Heithaus, R.M., & Barre, L.M. (2001). Complex social structure, alliance stability,
1056
and mating access in a bottlenose dolphin ‘super-alliance’. Proceedings of the Royal
1057
Society of London B, 268, 263-267.
1058
Connor, R.C., & Mann, J. (2006). Social cognition in the wild: Macchiavellian dolphins? In S.
1059
Hurley & M. Nudds (Eds.), Rational Animals (pp. 329-370). Oxford: Oxford University
1060
Press.
1061
1062
1063
1064
1065
Cords, M. (1992). Post-conflict reunions and reconciliation in long-tailed macaques. Animal
Behaviour, 44, 57-61.
Crockford, C., Wittig, R.M., Seyfarth, R.M. & Cheney, D.L. (2007). Baboons eavesdrop to
deduce mating opportunities. Animal Behaviour, 73, 885-890.
Crockford, C., Wittig, R.M., Whitten, P.L., Seyfarth, R.M. & Cheney, D.L. (2008). Social
1066
stressors and coping mechanisms in wild female baboons (Papio hamadryas ursinus).
1067
Hormones and Behavior, 53, 254-265.
1068
1069
Crystal, J.D. (2009). Elements of episodic-like memory in animal models. Behavioural
Processes, 80, 269-277.
48
1070
1071
1072
1073
1074
1075
1076
1077
1078
1079
1080
1081
1082
Dally, J.M., Emery, N.J., & Clayton, N.S. (2006). Food-caching western scrub jays keep track of
who was watching when. Science, 312, 1662-1665.
Das, M. (2000). Conflict management via third parties. In F. Aureli & F.B.M. de Waal (Eds.),
Natural Conflict Resolution (pp. 263-280). Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
de Waal, F.B.M. (1991). Rank distance as a central feature of rhesus monkey social organization:
A sociometric analysis. Animal Behaviour, 41, 383-395.
de Waal, F.B.M. (1996). Conflict as negotiation. In W. McGrew, L. Marchant, & T. Nishida
(Eds.), Great Ape Societies (pp. 159-172). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
de Waal, F.B.M. (1997).The chimpanzee’s service economy: Food for grooming. Evolution and
Human Behavior, 18, 375–386.
de Waal, F.B.M. (2000). Attitudinal reciprocity in food sharing among brown capuchin
monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 60, 253–261.
deWaal, F.B.M., & Aureli, F. (1996). Consolation, reconciliation, and a possible difference
1083
between macaques and chimpanzees. In A.E. Russon, K.A. Bard, & S.T. Parker (Eds.),
1084
Reaching into Thought: The Minds of the Great Apes (pp 80-110). Cambridge:
1085
Cambridge University Press.
1086
1087
1088
de Waal, F.B.M., & Davis, J.M. (2003). Capuchin cognitive ecology: Cooperation based on
projected returns. Neuropsychologia, 41, 221-228.
Dubreuil, D., Gentile, M.S., & Visalberghi, E. (2006). Are capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)
1089
inequity averse? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 273, 1223-1228.
1090
Emery, N.J., Dally, J.M., & Clayton, N.S. (2004). Western scrub-jays (Aphelocoma californica)
1091
use cognitive strategies to protect their caches from thieving conspecifics. Animal
1092
Cognition, 7, 37-43.
49
1093
Engh, A.L., Siebert, E.R., Greenberg, D.A., & Holekamp, K. (2005). Patterns of alliance
1094
formation and postconflict aggression indicate spotted hyenas recognize third party
1095
relationships. Animal Behaviour, 69, 209-217.
1096
Engh, A.L., Beehner, J.C., Bergman, T.J., Whitten, P.L., Hoffmeier, R.R., Seyfarth, R.M., &
1097
Cheney, D.L. (2006a). Behavioural and hormonal responses to predation in female
1098
chacma baboons (Papio hamadryas ursinus). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London
1099
B, 273, 707-712.
1100
1101
1102
1103
1104
1105
1106
1107
Engh, A.E., Hoffmeier, R.R., Cheney, D.L., & Seyfarth, R.M. (2006). Who me? Can baboons
infer the target of vocalisations? Animal Behaviour, 71, 381-387.
Evans, T.A., & Beran, M. (2007). Delay of gratification and delay maintenance by rhesus
macaques (Macaca mulatta). Journal of General Psychology, 134, 199-216.
Feh, C. (1999). Alliances and reproductive success in Camargue stallions. Animal Behaviour, 55,
705-713.
Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 8, 307-314.
1108
Fischer, J., Kitchen, D. M., Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D.L. (2004). Baboon loud calls advertise
1109
male quality: Acoustic features and their relation to rank, age, and exhaustion. Behavioral
1110
Ecology and Sociobiology, 56, 140-148.
1111
1112
1113
1114
Flombaum, J. I., & Santos, L.R. (2005). Rhesus monkeys attribute perceptions to others. Current
Biology, 15, 447-452.
Frank, R.E., & Silk, J.B. (2009). Impatient traders or contingent reciprocators? Evidence for the
extended time-course of grooming exchanges in baboons. Behaviour, 146, 1123-1135.
50
1115
Fruteau, C., Voelkl, B., van Damme, E., Noe, R. (2009). Supply and demand determine the
1116
market value of food providers in wild vervet monkeys. Proceedings of the National
1117
Academy of Sciences USA, 106, 12007-12012.
1118
Gacsi, M., Miklosi, A., Varga, O., Topal, J., & Csanyi, V. (2004). Are readers of our face readers
1119
of our mind? Dogs (Canis familiaris) show situation-dependent recognition of human’s
1120
attention. Animal Cognition 7, 144-153.
1121
1122
Ghazanfar, A., & Logothetis, N. (2003). Facial expressions linked the monkey calls. Nature, 423,
937-938.
1123
Ghazanfar, A.A., Maier, J.X., Hoffman, K.L., & Logothetis, N. (2005). Multisensory integration
1124
of dynamic faces and voices in rhesus monkey auditory cortex. Journal of Neuroscience,
1125
25, 5004-5012.
1126
Gil da Costa, R., Braun, A., Lopes, M., Hauser, M.D., Carson, R.E., Herskovitch, P., & Martin,
1127
A. (2004). Toward an evolutionary perspective on conceptual representation: Species-
1128
specific calls activate visual and affective processing systems in the macaque.
1129
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 101, 17516-17521.
1130
Gilby, I.C., & Wrangham, R.W. (2008). Association patterns among wild chimpanzees (Pan
1131
troglodytes schweinfurthii) reflect sex differences in cooperation. Behavioral Ecology
1132
and Sociobiology, 62, 1831-1842.
1133
1134
1135
1136
Gomes, C.R., Mundry, R., & Boesch, C. (2009). Long-term reciprocation of grooming in wild
West African chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 276, 699-706.
Gros-Louis, J. (2004). The function of food-associated calls in white-faced capuchin monkeys,
Cebus capuchinus, from the perspective of the signaler. Animal Behaviour, 67, 431-440.
51
1137
1138
1139
1140
1141
Grosenick, L., Clement, T.S., & Fernald, R. (2007). Fish can infer social rank by observation
alone. Nature, 446, 102-104.
Gurven, M. (2006). The evolution of contingent cooperation. Evolutionary Anthropology, 47,
185-192.
Gust, D.A., Gordon, T.P., Brodie, A.R., & McClure, H.M. (1994). Effect of a preferred
1142
companion in modulating stress in adult female rhesus monkeys. Physiology and
1143
Behavior, 55, 681-684.
1144
Hammerstein, P. (2003). Why is reciprocity so rare in social animals? A Protestant appeal. In P.
1145
Hammerstein (Ed.), Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation (pp. 83-94).
1146
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
1147
Harcourt, A.H. (1988). Alliances in contests and social intelligence. In R.W. Byrne & A. Whiten
1148
(Eds.), Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in
1149
Monkeys, Apes, and Humans (pp. 72-84). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
1150
Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (1999). Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use human and conspecific
1151
social cues to locate hidden food. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 1-5.
1152
Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Chimpanzees know what conspecifics
1153
1154
1155
do and do not see. Animal Behaviour, 59, 771-785.
Hare, B.A., Melis, A.P., Woods, V., Hastings, S., & Wrangham, R.W. (2007). Tolerance allows
bonobos to outperform chimpanzees on a cooperative task. Current Biology, 17, 619-623.
1156
Hauser, M., McAuliffe, K., & Blake, P. (2009). Evolving the ingredients for reciprocity and
1157
spite. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 364, 3255-3266.
1158
1159
Heinsohn, R., & Packer, C. (1995). Complex cooperative strategies in group-territorial African
lions. Science, 269, 1260-1262.
52
1160
1161
1162
Hemelrijk, C.K. (1994). Support for being groomed in long-tailed macaques, Macaca
fascicularis. Animal Behaviour, 48, 479-481.
Henrich, J., Ensminger, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J.C.,
1163
Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich, N., Lesorogol, C., Marlowe, F., Tracer, D., & Ziker, J.
1164
(2010). Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment.
1165
Science, 327, 1480-1484.
1166
1167
1168
1169
Henzi, S.P., & Barrett, L. (2002), Infants as a commodity in a baboon market. Animal Behaviour,
63, 915-921.
Henzi, S.P., & Barrett, L. (2007). Coexistence in female-bonded primate groups. Advances in the
Study of Behavior, 37, 107-132.
1170
Hinde, R.A. (1974). Biological Bases of Human Social Relationships. New York: McGraw-Hill.
1171
Humphrey, N.K. (1976). The social function of intellect. In P. Bateson & R.A. Hinde (Eds.),
1172
Growing Points in Ethology (pp. 303-318). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
1173
Jablonka, E., & Raz, G. (2009). Trangenerational epigenetic inheritance: Prevalence,
1174
mechanisms, and implications for the study of heredity and evolution. Quarterly Review
1175
of Biology, 84, 131-176.
1176
Jensen, K., Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). What’s in it for me? Self-regard precludes
1177
altruism and spite in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 273,
1178
1013-1021.
1179
1180
Jensen, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007a). Chimpanzees are rational maximizers in an
ultimatum game. Science, 318, 107–109.
1181
Jensen, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007b). Chimpanzees are vengeful but not spiteful.
1182
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104, 13046-13050.
53
1183
Jolly, A. (1966). Lemur social behavior and primate intelligence. Science, 153, 501-506.
1184
Jordan, K.E., MacLean, E.L., & Brannon, E.M. (2008). Monkeys match and tally quantities
1185
1186
1187
1188
1189
1190
1191
1192
1193
1194
across senses. Cognition, 108, 617-625
Judge, P. (1982). Redirection of aggression based on kinship in a captive group of pigtail
macaques. International Journal of Primatology, 3, 301.
Judge, P. (1991). Dyadic and triadic reconciliation in pigtailed macaques (Macaca nemestrina).
American Journal of Primatology, 23, 225-237.
Judge, P., & Mullen, S.H. (2005). Quadratic post-conflict affiliation among bystanders in a
hamadryas baboon group. Animal Behaviour, 69, 1345-1355.
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Chimpanzees know what others know, but not
what they believe. Cognition, 109, 224-234.
Kitchen, D.M., Seyfarth, R.M., Fischer, J., & Cheney, D.L. (2003). Loud calls as an indicator of
1195
dominance in male baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus). Behavioral Ecology and
1196
Sociobiology, 53, 374-384.
1197
Kitchen, D.M., Cheney, D.L., & Seyfarth, R.M. (2005). Male chacma baboons (Papio
1198
hamadryas ursinus) discriminate loud call contests between rivals of different relative
1199
ranks. Animal Cognition, 8, 1-6.
1200
Krams, I., Krama, T., Igaune, K., & Mand, R. (2008). Experimental evidence of reciprocal
1201
altruism in the pied flycatcher. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 62, 599-605.
1202
Kutsukake, N., & Castles, D.L. (2004). Reconciliation and post-conflict third-party affiliation
1203
among wild chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Primates, 45, 147-165.
1204
1205
Lakshminarayanan, V., & Santos, L.R. (2008). Capuchin monkeys are sensitive to others’
welfare. Current Biology, 18, R999-R1000.
54
1206
Langergraber, K.E., Mitani, J.C., & Vigilant, L. (2007). The limited impact of kinship on
1207
cooperation in wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA,
1208
104, 7786-7790.
1209
Langergraber, K.E., Mitani, J.C., & Vigilant, L. (2009). Kinship and social bonds in female
1210
chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology, 71, 840-851.
1211
Lindstrom, J. (1999). Early development and fitness in birds and mammals. Trends in Ecology
1212
1213
and Evolution, 14, 343–348.
Maestripieri, D., Hoffman, C.L., Anderson, G.M., Carter, C.S., & Higley, J.D. (2009). Mother–
1214
infant interactions in free-ranging rhesus macaques: Relationships between physiological
1215
and behavioral variables. Physiology and Behavior, 96, 613-619.
1216
McClintock, M.K., Conzen, S.D., Gehlert, S., Masi, C., & Olopade, F. (2005). Mammary cancer
1217
and social interactions: Identifying multiple environments that regulate gene expression
1218
throughout the life span. Journal of Gerentology B, 60, 32-41.
1219
McCormack, K., Newman, T.K., Higley, J.D., Maestripieri, D., & Sanchez, M.M. (2009).
1220
Serotonin transporter gene variation, infant abuse, and responsiveness to stress in rhesus
1221
macaque mothers and infants. Hormones and Behavior, 55, 538-547.
1222
1223
1224
1225
1226
1227
1228
Melis, A.P., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2006a). Chimpanzees recruit the best collaborators.
Science, 311, 1297-1300.
Melis, A.P., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2006b). Engineering cooperation in chimpanzees:
tolerance constraints on cooperation Animal Behaviour, 72, 275-286.
Melis, A.P., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Do chimpanzees reciprocate favours? Animal
Behaviour, 76, 951–962.
Melis, A.P., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Chimpanzees coordinate in a negotiation game.
55
1229
Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 381-392.
1230
Miklosi, A., & Topal, J. (2004.) What can dogs teach us? Animal Behaviour, 67, 995-1004.
1231
Miller, E.K., & Wallis, J.D. (2003). The prefrontal cortex and executive brain functions. In L.R.
1232
Squire, F.E. Bloom, J.L. Roberts, M.J. Zigmond, S.K. McConnell, & N.C. Spitzer (Eds.),
1233
Fundamental Neuroscience, 2nd Edition (pp. 1353-1376). New York: Academic Press.
1234
Mitani, J.C. (2006). Reciprocal exchange in chimpanzees and other primates. In P.M. Kappeler
1235
& C.P. van Schaik (Eds.), Cooperation in Primates and Humans: Mechanisms and
1236
Evolution (pp. 107-119). Berlin: Springer Verlag.
1237
1238
1239
1240
1241
1242
1243
1244
1245
Mitani, J.C. (2009a). Cooperation and competition in chimpanzees: Current understanding and
future challenges. Evolutionary Anthropology, 18, 215-227.
Mitani J.C. (2009b). Male chimpanzees form enduring and equitable social bonds. Animal
Behaviour, 77, 633-640.
Mitani, J.C., Watts, D.P., & Amsler, S.J. (2010). Lethal intergroup aggression leads to territorial
expansion in wild chimpanzees. Current Biology, 20, R-507-R508.
Nieder, A., Freedman, D.J., & Miller, E.K. (2002). Representation of the quantity of visual items
in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science, 297, 1708-1711.
Nishida, T., Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M., Hasegawa, T., & Takahata, Y. (1985). Group extinction and
1246
female transfer in wild chimpanzees in the Mahale National Park, Tanzania. Zietschrift
1247
für Tierpsychologie, 67, 284–30.
1248
Noe, R., & Hammerstein, P. (1994). Biological markets: supply and demand determine the effect
1249
of partner choice in cooperation, mutualism, and mating. Behavioral Ecology and
1250
Sociobiology, 35, 1-11.
56
1251
Oliveira, R.F., McGregor, P.K., & Latruffe, C. (1998). Know thine enemy: Fighting fish gather
1252
information from observing conspecific interactions. Proceedings of the Royal Society of
1253
London B, 265, 1045-1049.
1254
Onyango, P.O., Gesquiere, L.R., Wango E.O., Alberts S.C., & Altmann J. (2008). Persistence of
1255
maternal effects in baboons: mother's dominance rank at son's conception predicts stress
1256
hormone levels in sub-adult males. Hormones and Behavior, 54, 319-324.
1257
Packer, C. (1977). Reciprocal altruism in Papio anubis. Nature, 265, 441–443.
1258
Packer, C., & Pusey, A.E. (1982). Cooperation and competition within coalitions of male lions:
1259
1260
1261
1262
Kin selection or game theory? Nature, 296, 740-742.
Paz-y-Miño, G., Bond, A.B., Kamil, A.C., & Balda, R.P. (2004). Pinyon jays use transitive
inference to predict social dominance. Nature, 430, 778-782.
Peake, T.M., Terry, A.M.R., McGregor, P.K., & Dabelsteen, T. (2002). Do great tits assess rivals
1263
by combining direct experience with information gathered by eavesdropping?
1264
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 269, 1925–1929.
1265
1266
1267
1268
Pele, M., Dufour, V., Micheletta, J., & Thierry, B. (2010). Long-tailed macaques display
unexpected waiting abilities in exchange tasks. Animal Cognition, 13, 263-271.
Perry, S., Barrett, H.C. & Manson, J. (2004). White-faced capuchin monkeys show triadic
awareness in their choice of allies. Animal Behaviour, 67, 165-170.
1269
Poremba, A., Malloy, M., Saunders, R.C., Carson, R.E., Herskovitch, P., & Mishkin, M. (2004).
1270
Species-specific calls evoke asymmetric activity in the monkey’s temporal poles. Nature,
1271
427, 448-451.
1272
1273
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral
and Brain Sciences, 4, 515-526.
57
1274
Ramseyer, A., Pele, M., Dufour, V., Chauvin, C., & Thierry, B. (2006). Accepting loss: the
1275
temporal limits of reciprocity in brown capuchin monkeys. Proceedings of the Royal
1276
Society of London B, 273, 179-184.
1277
1278
Rendall, D., Rodman, P.S., & Emond, R.E. (1996). Vocal recognition of individuals and kin in
free-ranging rhesus monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 51, 1007-1015.
1279
Rendall, D., Cheney, D.L., & Seyfarth, R.M. (2000). Proximate factors mediating ‘contact’ calls
1280
in adult female and their infants. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114, 36-46.
1281
1282
1283
1284
1285
1286
1287
1288
1289
Schino, G. (2001). Grooming, competition and social rank among female primates: A metaanalysis. Animal Behaviour, 62, 265-271.
Schino, G. (2007). Grooming and agonistic support: A meta-analysis of primate reciprocal
altruism. Behavioral Ecology, 18, 115-120.
Schino, G., & Aureli, F. (2009). Reciprocal altruism in primates: Partner choice, cognition, and
emotions. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 39, 45-69.
Schino, G., Tiddi, B., & Polizzi di Sorrentino, E. (2006). Simultaneous classification by rank and
kinship in Japanese macaques. Animal Behaviour, 71, 1069-1074.
Schino, G., Polizzi di Sorrentino, E., & Tiddi, B. (2007). Grooming and coalitions in Japanese
1290
macaques (Macaca fuscata): Partner choice and the time frame of reciprocation. Journal
1291
of Comparative Psychology, 121, 181-188.
1292
1293
1294
1295
Schino, G., Di Giuseppe, F., & Visalberghi, E. (2009). Grooming, rank, and agonistic support in
tufted capuchin monkeys. American Journal of Primatology, 71, 101-105.
Seyfarth, R.M. (1976). Social relationships among adult female baboons. Animal Behaviour, 24,
917-938.
58
1296
1297
1298
1299
1300
1301
Seyfarth, R.M. (1980). The distribution of grooming and related behaviours among adult female
vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 28, 798-813.
Seyfarth, R.M., & Cheney, D.L. (1984). Grooming, alliances, and reciprocal altruism in vervet
monkeys. Nature, 308, 541-543.
Shettleworth, S. (2010). Cognition, Evolution, and Behavior, 2nd edition. New York: Oxford
University Press.
1302
Shively, C.A., Register, T.C., Friedman, D.P., Morgan, T.M., Thompson, J., & Lanier, T. (2005).
1303
Social stress-associated depression in adult female cynomolgus monkeys (Macaca
1304
fascicularis). Biological Psychology, 69, 67-84.
1305
1306
1307
1308
1309
Silk, J.B. (1993). Does participation in coalitions influence dominance relationships among male
bonnet macaques? Behaviour, 126, 171-189.
Silk, J.B. (1999). Male bonnet macaques use information about third-party rank relationships to
recruit allies. Animal Behaviour, 58, 45-51.
Silk, J.B. (2003). Cooperation without counting: The puzzle of friendship. In P. Hammerstein
1310
(Ed.), Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation (pp. 37-54). Cambridge, MA: MIT
1311
Press.
1312
1313
1314
1315
Silk, J.B. (2007). The strategic dynamics of cooperation in primate groups. Advances in the
Study of Behavior, 37, 1-41.
Silk, J.B., Cheney, D.L., & Seyfarth, R.M. (1996). The form and function of post-conflict
interactions between female baboons. Animal Behaviour, 52, 259–268.
1316
Silk, J.B., Rendall, D., Cheney, D.L., & Seyfarth, R.M. (2003a). Natal attraction in adult female
1317
baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus) in the Moremi Reserve Botswana. Ethology, 109,
1318
627-644.
59
1319
1320
1321
1322
1323
Silk, J.B., Alberts, S.C., & Altmann, J. (2003b). Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant
survival. Science, 302, 1231-1234.
Silk, J.B., Alberts, S.C., & Altmann, J. (2004). Patterns of coalition formation by adult female
baboons in Amboseli, Kenya. Animal Behaviour, 67, 573-582.
Silk, J.B., Brosnan, S.F., Vonk, J., Henrich, J., Povinelli, D.J., Richardson, A.S., Lambeth, S.P.,
1324
Mascaro, J., & Schapiro, S.J. (2005). Chimpanzees are indifferent to the welfare of
1325
unrelated group members. Nature, 437, 1357-1359.
1326
Silk, J.B., Altmann, J., & Alberts, S.C. (2006a). Social relationships among adult female baboons
1327
(Papio cynocephalus). I. Variation in the strength of social bonds. Behavioral Ecology
1328
and Sociobiology, 61, 183-195.
1329
Silk, J.B., Altmann, J., & Alberts, S.C. (2006b). Social relationships among adult female
1330
baboons (Papio cynocephalus). II: Variation in the quality and stability of social bonds.
1331
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 61, 197-204.
1332
Silk, J.B., Beehner, J.C., Bergman, T.J., Crockford, C., Engh, A.L., Moscovice, L.R., Wittig,
1333
R.M., Seyfarth, R.M., & Cheney, D.L. (2009). The benefits of social capital: Close social
1334
bonds among female baboons enhance offspring survival. Proceedings of the Royal
1335
Society of London B, 276, 3099-3104.
1336
Silk, J.B, Beehner, J.C., Bergman, T.J., Crockford, C., Engh, A.L., Moscovice, L.R., Wittig,
1337
R.M., Seyfarth, R.M., & Cheney, D.L. (2010a). Female chacma baboons form strong,
1338
equitable, and enduring social bonds. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 64,
1339
Silk, J.B., Beehner, J.C., Bergman, T.J, Crockford, C., Engh, A.L., Moscovice, L.R., Wittig, R.,
1340
Seyfarth, R.M., & Cheney, D.L. (2010b). Strong and consistent social bonds enhance the
1341
longevity of female baboons. Current Biology, 20,
60
1342
1343
Smith, K.P., & Christakis, N.A. (2008). Social networks and health. Annual Review of Sociology,
34, 405-429.
1344
Smuts, B. (1985). Sex and Friendship in Baboons. New York: Aldine.
1345
Stephens, C. (1996). Modeling reciprocal altruism. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science,
1346
1347
1348
1349
1350
1351
1352
1353
47, 533 -551.
Stevens, J.R., Cushman, F.A., & Hauser, M.D. (2005). Evolving the psychological mechanisms
for cooperation. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 36, 499-518.
Stevens, J.R., & Hauser, M.D. (2004). Why be nice? Psychological constraints on the evolution
of cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 60-65.
Suomi, S.J. (2007). Risk, resilience, and gene x environment interactions in rhesus monkeys.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 52-62.
Takimoto, A., Kuroshima, H., & Fujita, K. (2010). Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) are
1354
sensitive to others’ reward: An experimental analysis of food-choice for conspecifics.
1355
Animal Cognition, 13, 249-261.
1356
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing
1357
intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675-691.
1358
1359
1360
Trivers, R.L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 3557.
Ventura, R., Bonaventura, M., Koyama, N.F., Hardie, S., & Schino, G. (2006). Reciprocation
1361
and interchange in wild Japanese macaques: Grooming, cofeeding, and agonistic support.
1362
American Journal of Primatology, 68, 1138-1149.
1363
1364
Warneken, F. & Tomasello, M. (2009). Varieties of altruism in children and chimpanzees.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 397-402.
61
1365
Watson, K.K., Ghodasra, J.H., & Platt M.L. (2009). Serotonin transporter genotype modulates
1366
social reward and punishment in rhesus macaques. PLoS ONE 4(1):e4156. Epub 2009
1367
Jan 14.
1368
1369
1370
1371
1372
Watts, D.P. (2002). Reciprocity and interchange in the social relationships of wild male
chimpanzees. Behaviour, 139, 343-370.
Watts, D.P., & Mitani, J.C. (2001). Boundary patrols and intergroup encounters in wild
chimpanzees. Behaviour, 138, 299-327.
Weidt, A., Hofmann, S.E., & Konig, B. (2008). Not only mate choice matters: Fitness
1373
consequences of social partner choice in female house mice. Animal Behaviour, 75, 801-
1374
808.
1375
1376
1377
1378
West, S.A., Griffin, A.S., & Gardner, A. (2007). Evolutionary explanations for cooperation.
Current Biology, 17, R661-R672.
Widdig, A., Streich, W.J., & Tembrock, G. (2000). Coalition formation among wild male
barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus). American Journal of Primatology, 50, 37-51.
1379
Wilkinson, G. (1984). Reciprocal food sharing in vampire bats. Nature, 308, 181-184.
1380
Wilson, M., & Wrangham, R.W. (2003). Intergroup relations in chimpanzees. Annual Review of
1381
1382
Anthropology, 32, 363-392.
Wittig, R.M. (2010). Function and cognitive underpinnings of post-conflict affiliation in wild
1383
chimpanzees. In E.V. Lonsdorf, S.R. Ross, T. Matsuzawa, & J. Goodall (Eds.), The Mind
1384
of the Chimpanzee: Ecological and Experimental Perspectives (pp. xx-xx). Chicago:
1385
University of Chicago Press.
62
1386
Wittig, R.M., Crockford, C., Wikberg, E., Seyfarth, R.M., & Cheney, D.L. (2007a). Kin-
1387
mediated reconciliation substitutes for direct reconciliation in female baboons.
1388
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 274, 1109-1115.
1389
Wittig, R.M., Crockford, C., Seyfarth, R.M., & Cheney, D.L. (2007b). Vocal alliances in chacma
1390
baboons, Papio hamadryas ursinus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 61, 899-909.
1391
Wittig, R.M., Crockford, C., Lehmann, J. Whiten, P.L., Seyfarth, R.M., & Cheney, D.L. (2008).
1392
Focused grooming networks and stress alleviation in wild female baboons. Hormones
1393
and Behavior, 54, 170-177.
1394
1395
1396
Wobber, V., Wrangham, R., & Hare, B. (2010). Bonobos exhibit delayed development of social
behavior and cognition relative to chimpanzees. Current Biology, 20, 226-230.
Yamamoto, S., & Tanaka, M. (2010). The influence of kin relationship and reciprocal context on
1397
chimpanzees' other-regarding preferences. Animal Behaviour, 79, 595-602.
1398
Yee, J.R., Cavigelli, S.A., Delgado, B., & McClintock, M.K. (2008). Reciprocal affiliation
1399
among adolescent rats during a mild group stressor predicts mammary tumors and
1400
lifespan. Psychosomatic Medicine, 70, 1050-1059.
1401
1402
63
Download