Applied Vision Association and Colour Group one-day meeting on Animal Vision. 09.30 - 17.30. Thursday, 21st September Department of Experimental Psychology. University of Bristol. REGISTRATION: http://www.hls.dmu.ac.uk/ava/meetings/animalvision.html . Programme 09.30 – 10.00 Coffee 10.00 – 10.45. Mike Land (Sussex). The eye movements of animals and men. 10.45 – 11.15. Rob Harris (Sussex). Visual navigation and memory recall by ants. 11.15 – 11.45. Martina Wicklein (UCL). Strategies for colour learning in bees. 11.45 – 12.15. Hannah Rowland (Liverpool). The cryptic value of countershading in lepidopteran larvae. 12.15 – 13.45. Lunch & Posters 13.45 – 14.15. Christian Wehrhahn (Tübingen). S-cones contribute to brightness perception in primates. 14.15 – 14.45 Jules Davidoff (Goldsmiths). Cross-species differences in colour categorisation. 14.45 – 15.15. Tom Troscianko (Bristol). Bird and primate colour constancy 15.15 – 15.45 Tea 15.45 – 16.15 Olle Håstad (Uppsala). Phylogenetic distribution of colour vision systems among birds. 16.15 – 17.00. Doekele Stavenga (Groningen). Butterfly coloration and colour vision. 17.00 – 17.30. Ulrike Siebeck (Queensland). Ultraviolet communication in reef fish. 1 Abstracts PLUMAGE COLOUR VARIATION AND ASSORTATIVE MATING IN THE CRIMSON ROSELLA (PLATYCERCUS ELEGANS) RING SPECIES COMPLEX Mathew Berg1, Leonie Raijmakers2, Raoul Ribot1, Leo Joseph3, Kate Buchanan4 and Andy Bennett1 1 School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol,; 2 Animal Ecology Group, University of Groningen, The Netherlands; 3 Australian National Wildlife Collection, CSIRO, ACT, 2911, Australia; 4 School of Biosciences, Cardiff University. Ring species are rather unusual phenomena formed by a ring-like arrangement of intergrading populations, with distinct terminal forms that rarely interbreed in sympatry. Such cases offer valuable insights into the process of speciation by showing how small differences between often sympatric populations can lead to species-level divergence. The crimson rosella complex (Platycercus elegans) of south eastern Australia has been proposed as one such system. It consists of a ring of populations that vary dramatically in plumage coloration, culminating in terminal forms of deep crimson red or dull yellow. To date it is unknown whether selective processes maintain this pattern, but sexual selection leading to assortative mating is one possibility. Here, we describe the pattern of plumage colour variation that occurs throughout the range. Furthermore, we studied the relationship between plumage coloration and pairing decisions in the highly variable intermediate adelaidae form (just east of Adelaide) and the zone of sympatry between the terminal flaveolus and elegans forms (south west of Canberra). Our data suggests that rosellas do not mate assortatively based on plumage coloration in these populations. Thus, at this stage it does not appear that assortative mating reinforces plumage colour variation, and hence population divergence, in this system. Instead, ecological factors or population histories may be largely responsible for the current pattern of plumage colour diversity in the crimson rosella complex. CROSS-SPECIES DIFFERENCES IN COLOUR CATEGORISATION Jules Davidoff1 , Joël Fagot2, Julie Goldstein1, and Alan Pickering1 1. Goldsmiths’ College, University of London, UK; 2. CNRS-INCM Marseille, France Berlin and Kay (1969) found systematic restrictions in the colour terms of the world’s languages and were inclined to look to the primate visual system for their origin. As the visual system does not provide adequate neurophysiological discontinuities to give natural colour category boundaries, and recent evidence points to a linguistic origin (Davidoff, Davies & Roberson, 1999), a new approach investigated the controversial issue of the origin of colour categories. Baboons and humans were given the same task of matching to sample colours that crossed the green/blue boundary. The data and consequent modelling were remarkably clear-cut. All human participants matched our generalisation probe stimuli as if to a sharp boundary close to the midpoint between their training items. Despite good colour discrimination, none of the baboons showed any inclination to match to a single boundary but rather responded with two ‘boundaries’ close to the training stimuli. Furthermore, the human colour category boundary between green and blue was shown to promote superior recognition in a crosscategory task in humans, but not in baboons. However, despite the category advantage for humans, psychophysically determined colour discrimination thresholds were very similar for the two species. The data give no support to the claim that colour categories are explicitly instantiated in the primate colour vision system. Berlin, B. & Kay, P. (1969) Basic color terms: Their universality and evolution. Berkeley: University of California Press. Davidoff, J., Davies, I. & Roberson, D. (1999) Colour categories of a stone-age tribe. Nature 398, 203-204 SENSORY ECOLOGY AND ANIMAL WELFARE - THE EFFECT OF FLICKER FROM FLUORESCENT LIGHTS ON THE WELFARE OF EUROPEAN STARLINGS Jennifer E Evans1, Innes C Cuthill1, Andrew T D Bennett1 & Katherine L Buchanan2 1 School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol. BS8 1UG.; Cardiff School of Biosciences, University of Cardiff, Cardiff. CF10 3TL. 2 Animals’ sensory capabilities are frequently tuned to the characteristics of the habitats in which they evolved. However, if animals are brought into captivity, the environmental parameters that surround them are changed, often negatively impacting on their welfare. As a case study, we present a series of experiments examining how the flicker emitted from conventional, lowfrequency (LF; which flicker at 100Hz) fluorescent lights affects the welfare of captive European starlings. We found that starlings show a significant preference for high frequency (HF; which flicker at 100kHz, a level imperceptible to any animal) over LF fluorescent lights, indicating they can detect a difference between these two lights and find HF light the least aversive condition. Starlings also experience higher incidence of myoclonus (mild muscular spasms) when viewing LF lights. In addition, long and short-term exposure to LF light affects both behavioural and physiological indicators of stress. Further experiments studied the effect of flicker rate on visually mediated tasks. During mate choice, females ranked males consistently under HF, but not under LF conditions. In foraging experiments, birds differed in their foraging habits under the two lighting types. Together, these experiments indicate that starlings can detect a difference between HF and LF lights, with LF lights negatively impacting not only on their welfare, but also on visually mediated behaviours. PHYLOGENETIC DISTRIBUTION OF COLOUR VISION SYSTEMS AMONG BIRDS Olle Håstad, University of Uppsala. We have no idea of what birds looks like to other birds. Still much research within the fields of animal ecology and evolution has been based on observations of bird plumage colouration and the effects of colour manipulations on bird behaviour. The interpretation of these studies are entirely dependent on our understanding of avian colour perception. To make matters even more interesting, microspectrophotometry studies on bird retinas has revealed that two distinct colour vision systems are present in birds, one with a short-wavelength sensitivity biased toward violet (VS) and the other biased toward ultraviolet (UVS). In the last years information from molecular studies has given us a more detailed picture of the basic patterns of vision system distribution among birds, in turn enabling us to infer some effects this variation has on the sexual signalling and prey detection of birds. 2 UNUSUAL FEATURES IN THE SEQUENCES OF THE RH1 AND RH2 OPSINS IN THE ROSELLA PARROT PLATYCERCUS ELEGANS Ben Knott*, Wayne L. Davies**, Mathew Berg*, James K. Bowmaker**, Andrew T.D. Bennett*, David M. Hunt** *Ecology of Vision Lab, School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol. Woodland Road. Bristol BS8 1UG; **Institute of Ophthalmology, 11-43 Bath Street, London, EC1V 9EL Rosella parrots Platycercus elegans form a ring species with three main intraspecific colour morphs: a crimson form, yellow form, and a highly variable adelaide form intermediate between the previous two. Sensory drive theories of sexual selection predict that the extreme colour variation within the species make the rosella an ideal potential candidate for intraspecific variation in visual pigments, hitherto never reported in birds. Previous work shows birds possess five visual pigments: LWS, RH1, RH2, SWS1 and SWS2, and these basic sequences were needed from the rosella before making comparisons between morphs. Eyes were obtained from three wild intermediate morphs and retinal RNA extracted. The RH1 opsin showed considerable extension at the 5` end, 25 amino acids beyond the expected position of the stop codon when compared to birds and other vertebrates. Sequencing of the RH2 opsin produced an expected sequence, plus a sequence with an unrecognised final exon. The change in sequence occurring at the known exon boundary suggests and alternative splicing event has occurred, in which the normal exon 5 has been missed and an exon from further down the DNA strand has been spliced in its place in messenger RNA. This is the first known case of alternative splicing in an opsin. Since all spectral tuning sites are located in exons 1 to 4, the above anomalies may not affect opsin function to any significant degree. However, further work is needed to test this hypothesis explicitly. THE EYE MOVEMENTS OF ANIMALS AND MEN Michael Land. University of Sussex Most of our dealings with the visual world are mediated by brief fixations, with gaze being shifted up to four times a second by saccadic eye movements during which we are blind. This 'saccade and fixate' strategy is not limited to primates that need to move their high resolution foveas around the scene, but also occurs in most vertebrates, many insects, crustaceans and even cephalopod molluscs. The underlying reason for this ubiquitous behaviour seems to be the avoidance of motion blur, although there may be other benefits as well. A few animals - sea snails, mantis shrimps, and jumping spiders - do break the general rule, and move their eyes continuously across the surroundings. These exceptions all have in common that their retinas are linear strips, a few receptors wide. Rather than having a two-dimensional retina, they use time to provide the missing image dimension. RELATIVE EYE SIZE AND ECOLOGY IN ELASMOBRANCH FISHES Thomas J. Lisney* and Shaun P. Collin be coastal, often benthic, rays and sharks. Active benthopelagic and pelagic species, which prey on active, mobile prey also have relatively larger eyes than more sluggish, benthic elasmobranchs that feed on more sedentary prey such as benthic invertebrates. THE CRYPTIC VALUE OF COUNTERSHADING IN LEPIDOPTERAN LARVAE Hannah Rowland. University of Liverpool Of the many traits seen in cryptic prey animals, countershading (darker pigmentation on those surfaces exposed to the most lighting) is one of the common, and paradoxically one of the least well understood. Countershading has been hypothesised to enhance crypsis by shadow-obliteration, in which lighter colouration on the undersides compensates for increased shadow in these regions, thus reducing detection by visually hunting predators. We tested the hypothesis that there is an optimal level of contrast between upper and lower surfaces which facilitates self-shadow concealment and therefore enhances crypsis. We report two experiments with artificial prey presented to individual free living birds. In the first experiment, artificial prey with different levels of contrast were presented on green boards to individual blackbirds (Turdus merula) and robins (Erithacus rubecula) In the second experiment a second set of lighter prey with the same level of contrast were presented on lighter green boards. In the first experiments an intermediate level of contrast in countershaded prey provided significantly more protection from predation than other levels of contrast and controls. ULTRAVIOLET COMMUNICATION IN REEF FISH Ulrike E. Siebeck. Vision Touch and Hearing Research Centre, School of Biomedical Sciences, University of Queensland, Australia In contrast to their freshwater and deep-sea counterparts, coral reef fish are extremely colourful. Many possess ultraviolet (UV) colour patterns invisible to humans and many predatory fish. This study set out to study the behavioural significance of these patterns. In the first experiment, the response of Pomacentrus amboinensis males towards a choice of two conspecific intruders was tested. One intruder was presented in a UV-transmitting (UV+) container and the other in a UV-absorbing (UV-) container. Territory owners attacked intruders viewed though UV+ filters significantly more vigorously than those seen through UV- filters, revealing that the males are sensitive to UV light and that they modified their behaviour based on the visibility of UV patterning. In the second experiment, the response of Pomacentrus amboinensis males was tested towards a conspecific intruder and a heterospecific intruder possessing similar general appearance but a markedly different UV facial pattern. Conspecifics were preferentially attacked in UV+ conditions but not under UVconditions. Taken as a whole, the two experiments strongly support the hypothesis that UV light has an important impact on behaviour. Communication in the UV may represent an effective solution to the age-old evolutionary conflict between being conspicuous to conspecifics and inconspicuous to predators. Vision Touch and Hearing Research Centre, School of Biomedical Sciences, University of Queensland, Australia. email: tom_lisney@hotmail.com Variation in relative eye size was investigated in a sample of 46 species of elasmobranchs (sharks and rays). To get a measure of eye size relative to body size, eye axial diameter was scaled with body mass using least-squares linear regression, using both raw species data, where species are treated as independent data points, and phylogenetically independent contrasts. Residual values calculated for each species calculated using the regression equations describing these scaling relationships, were then used as a measure of relative eye size. Relative eye size varies considerably in elasmobranchs, although sharks have significantly larger eyes than rays. The sharks with the relatively largest eyes are oceanic species; either pelagic sharks that move between the epipelagic (0-200m) and ‘upper’ mesopelagic (200-600m) zones, or benthic and benthopelagic species that live in the mesopelagic (200-1000m) and, to a lesser extent, bathypelagic (1000-4000m) zones. The elasmobranchs with the relatively smallest eyes tend to BUTTERFLY COLORATION AND COLOUR VISION. D.G. Stavenga. Department of Neurobiophysics, University of Groningen, the Netherlands A comparative survey of wing coloration of pierid butterflies, using UV photography and reflectance spectrophotometry, reveals that two quite different methods of sexual dichroism have been developed among pierid butterflies. The colourful wing patterns are presumably tuned to the color vision properties of the butterflies. Butterfly compound eyes are composed of numerous ommatidia. All ommatidia contain 9 photoreceptor cells. The basic groundplan of butterfly eyes, encountered in the nymphalids, exists of three types of randomly arranged ommatidia, with either 1 UV and 1 blue receptor, or 2 UV, or 2 blue receptors. In Pieridae, the green receptors have greatly diversified by spectrally selective filters, resulting in two different types of red receptors, which greatly 3 improves red discriminability. The set of short-wavelength receptors is expanded by multiplication of the short-wavelength rhodopsins, presumably for sexual discrimination of the wing colors. In Papilionidae a similar diversification occurred, with multiplication of the long-wavelength rhodopsins, as well as application of different spectral filters; presumably all to the benefit of colour vision. COLOUR VISION AND ILLUMINATION NOISE IN PRIMATES AND BIRDS Tom Troscianko, P George Lovell, C Alejandro Párraga, Roland Baddeley, Michelle To, David Tolhurst. Universities of Bristol & Cambridge. There is considerable evidence that the properties of primate colour vision are optimised to allow foraging for food. The data on which these conclusions are based are the reflectance spectra of fruit and leaves, and measures of the spectral composition of the illumination. However, the 3D structure of natural scenes containing leaves results in considerable variation in the intensity and chromatic content of light reflected from different elements of each scene. We present data which show that the absorption spectra of cones in primates and passerine birds have an effect on the degree to which this illumination noise is encoded by the visual system. In particular, the close spacing of primate L and M cones results in a greater degree of invariance to shadows and illumination than the equivalent spacing of these cones in passerine birds. The results point to a trade-off between (a) achieving greater chromatic resolution, and (b) immunity from illumination noise. INDIVIDUAL STRATEGIES FOR COLOUR LEARNING IN BUMBLE BEES Martina Wicklein, Institute of Ophthalmology, UCL, London It is well known that bumblebees have a labour division in the hive and that foragers can be mainly foraging for pollen, water or nectar and therefore be divided into distinct task groups. Here we explore if bumblebees could also become specialists for foraging under different illumination conditions. We trained bumblebees to forage in an artificial flower meadow consisting of 64 flowers (8 different flower colours) presented under two different illuminants. Only one flower colour was rewarded (target) and the task of the bees was to learn to find the targets under both illuminants. In the first condition the bees encountered a situation akin to a dappled illumination, both illuminants were present simultaneously. In the second condition the illuminants were presented sequentially. Over the course of training we preformed regular tests to assess how well the bees were able to find the correct targets under the two training illuminants and what strategies they used to find the targets. We found that under the first, simultaneous, illumination condition the forager population split up into two groups, each forager group specializing for one of the illuminants, using a different strategy to find the target. Interestingly the split into two specialized forager groups only happens under the simultaneous training regime, not when the bees were trained to both illuminants sequentially. The main implication of this is that whether bees spilt into two specialized forager groups does not depend on the identity of the illuminants, but the temporal and spatial arrangement of the illumination. Given that primate vision has a particularly enhanced ability to ignore shadows and changes in the colour of the illumination, we present data to show that human observers are able to ignore such changes when making judgments about the degree of similarity of natural scenes. S CONES CONTRIBUTE TO BRIGHTNESS PERCEPTION IN PRIMATES Christian Wehrhahn. Max-Planck-Institut, Tubingen, Germany The perception of brightness in animals or humans is currently not well understood, as demonstrated by our inability to explain illusions that rely on mid and/or high level mechanisms. Although animal studies have shown the activation of neurons in the early visual pathways whose functional properties correlate with the perception of brightness, the components of a “brightness pathway” (in analogy to the motion pathway) have not yet been described. Perception of brightness has been explored in psychophysical experiments in monkeys. Rhesus monkeys, trained to discriminate the brightness of two test fields, also succeeded in performing the tasks of brightness induction and White's effect (Huang et al. 2002). In all three tasks, the performance of monkey observers was similar to that of human observers. This indicates that monkeys perceive surface brightness in a way comparable to humans. So far, only retinal contributions to the perception of brightness have been successfully investigated. Results related to the contribution of S cones to brightness perception have been reported. In physiological experiments in anaesthetized monkeys, responses of V1 neurons to S cone isolating stimuli showed that S cones account for about 10% of the total amplitude of modulation (Chatterjee & Callaway 2003). For human observers, flicker fusion experiments in 73 color normal human observers indicated that on average S-cone input accounts for about 5% of the total brightness signal. 4